Neuropsychologia, Vol, ~ Pergamon PU: S0028 3932(97)00032 8 35. No. 8, pp. 11175 1085. 1997 i: 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved Printed in Grea! Britain 1/028 3932/97 $17.00+0.00 Lexical and semantic processing in the absence of word reading: Evidence from neglect dyslexia ELISABETTA LADAVAS,*?CARLO UMILTA~ and DANIELA MAPELLI+§ *Dipartimento di Psicologia, Universitfi di Bologna, Bologna, Italy; ~Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, UniversitS, di Padova, Padova, Italy; §Ospedale 'I Fraticini', Firenze, Italy (Received 15 Januw:v 1996; accepted 24 January 1997) Abstract--Nine patients with left-sided neglect and nine matched control patients performed three tasks on horizontal (either normal or mirror-reversed) letter strings. The tasks were: reading aloud, making a lexical decision (word vs non-word), and making a semantic decision (living vs non-living item). Relative to controls, neglect patients performed very poorly in the reading task, whereas they performed nearly normally in the lexical and semantic tasks. This was considered to be a dissociation between direct tasks, rather than a dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge. The explanation offered for the dissociation is in terms of both a dual-route model for reading aloud and a degraded representation of the letter string. ~i~,, 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd Key Words: neglect dyslexia; visual attention; lexical/semantic access; reading routes. Introduction a patient with left visual neglect. The patient could not read aloud words presented in the left hemifield, nor judge their lexical status or semantic content. He could not even detect the presence of strings of letters in that hemifield. However, response to a word in the right visual field was faster when the word was preceded by a brief presentation of an associated word in the neglected left hemifield. In other words, he indirectly showed associative priming caused by words presented in the neglected hemifield, even though he was unable directly to process any attributes of the stimuli in that same hemifield. Berti et al. [5] documented this type of dissociation in a patient with neglect dyslexia. In reading regular words and color words (i.e. a direct reading task), the patient made the usual pattern of neglect errors on the left side. However, when asked to name the colors in which color words were written (i.e. a Stroop-like task that indirectly tests reading), naming time was found to be affected by the meaning of those words he was not able to read correctly. In the above examples, the use of the term 'implicit" (or 'covert') is appropriate because it refers to the nature of the processing that is tapped by indirect tasks. The use of this term is instead inappropriate when it refers to tasks in which the experimental procedure implies direct, explicit judgements, rather than refering to the type of processing [27, 31, 43]. Several examples of dissociation between direct tasks can be found in the literature on neglect. Volpe et al. [39] Much current neuropsychological research concerns demonstrations of 'unconscious', 'implicit' or 'covert' information processing. These terms are all used to denote cases where information processing appears to proceed more or less normally, despite the absence of awareness of the results of this processing (see, e.g. reviews in [27] and [38]). Although the essence of m a n y of these experiments is the indirectness of the testing procedure, an important distinction should be made between types of task and types of process (e.g. [27, 31, 43]). The use of terms like 'covert' or 'implicit' is better reserved to refer to the nature of the processinq. In contrast, to characterise tasks, the term 'indirect' is used. A direct task directly enquires about a given ability, and performance is dependent on that ability. In an indirect task, assessment of the ability in question is incidental and the task ostensibly measures something else. In the case of neglect dyslexia, implicit knowledge was shown through indirect testing by Lfidavas et al. [20], for t Address for correspondence: Dipartimento di Psicologia, Universitfi di Bologna, Viale Berti-Pichat, 5, 40127 Bologna, Italy; tel.: 39 51 242030; fax: 39 51 243086; e-mail: ladavas(a: psibo.unibo.it. §? Daniela Mapelli is now at the Dipartimento di Psicologia, Universitfi di Trieste. 1075 1076 E. Lfidavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia tested patients with extinction with pairs of visual stimuli (i.e. drawings of common objects or threeqetter words), one in each hemifield. On each trial, the patients were required to perform two direct tasks, that is to name the stimuli and to judge whether they were same or different. The patients performed poorly in naming left-visual-field stimuli, but performed much above chance (between 88 % and 100% correct) in the same~different matching task. This outcome is interesting because performing the samedifferent task no doubt requires processing left-sided stimuli, which the patients were unable to name. However, it is important to point out that both tasks, the one in which the patients failed (naming) and the one in which the patients performed correctly (matching), required a direct response. In contrast, in the studies by Berti et al. [5] and Lfidavas et al. [23] the direct response was modulated by information that was not directly assessed. The dissociation between direct identification of leftvisual-field stimuli and cross-field same-different matching was replicated by several authors who studied neglect (see [12], for review). For example, Berti et al.'s study [4] aimed to determine the level of processing to which extinguished stimuli were encoded. They also used pairs of pictures that were physically different but depicted either the same objects from different views, or different exemplars of objects belonging to the same category. The patient performed nearly correctly in matching stimuli across fields, even though the stimulus on the left could not be recognised. These results showed that the extinguished stimuli may be coded at least at the category level. Marshall and Halligan [26] reported a patient with neglect, who showed a consistent preference for a picture of a normal house over a picture of the same house with flames coming from the left side, in spite of her inability to say whether the two stimuli were the same or different. The authors proposed that neglect stimuli are processed at least to the degree that they can evoke a preference response. Up to now, however, a dissociation between two direct tasks has not be documented for neglect dyslexia, even though it might be expected on the basis of what is known about this syndrome. Patients with neglect dyslexia (see, e.g. [29]) typically make errors in reading the left side (i.e. the beginning) of words and non-words after right parietal lesion. They omit some of the letters or insert letters that are not present. More often, however, they substitute some or all of the letters on the left side. Interestingly, often the number of erroneously substituted letters matches the number of letters actually present in the letter string (also, see [1 l, 40]). This finding shows that the patient has some knowledge about the letters present on the left side, Perhaps on the left there is an impaired visuo-spatial representation that conveys sufficient information about word length. The fact that errors tend to be more numerous for non-words than for words (also see [2, 3]) seems to suggest that lexical information, too, may in part be preserved in neglect dyslexia. The aim of the present study was to document the dissociation between direct tasks in the case of neglect dyslexia. A group of dyslexic patients were presented with a letter string and they were asked to perform three tasks, that is to read aloud the letter string, to judge its lexical status (word vs non-word), and (if it was a word) to judge its semantic category (living vs non-living). Every task required a direct response, but, as will be seen, dissociations emerged. These were because the patients were able to classify correctly as words or non-words and to denote as living or non-living items, letter strings that they could not read. Basically, three explanations have been offered for the dissociation between explicit and implicit processing [12, 32, 42]. The first is that an information processing system is intact but has been disconnected from other brain systems necessary for showing the type of explicit recognition tapped by direct tasks. The second proposes that there may be two brain systems capable of processing the relevant information, only one of which, however, can mediate explicit recognition through direct tasks. The third explanation is that implicit recognition reflects the residual processing capabilities of a partially damaged brain system. The lower quality information conveyed by the damaged system is enough for supporting performance in indirect tasks but not for supporting performance in direct tasks. When the dissociation occurs between two direct tasks, the explanation that invokes the disconnection of an intact system from another system (or other systems) that supports performance in direct tasks is of course not applicable, both tasks being direct in nature. The explanation that invokes two independent systems is applicable, on condition, however, that it is modified by proposing that both systems can support performance in direct tasks, even though only one of them mediates explicit recognition. The third explanation (that is the one based on the degraded input from a partially damaged system) is very easily applicable. One has only to add that the degraded information is sufficient for executing the direct task in which the patient performs correctly, but is insufficient for executing the direct task in which the patient fails. In the experiment to be described here, words and non-words were presented horizontally, either normally arranged or mirror-reversed. This was done for testing a proposal by Caramazza and Hillis [6, 7]. They reported that in some cases neglect affects the highest internal representation of a word, which, in their view, is always canonical, that is horizontal and arranged from left to right, irrespective of how the word is presented. Method Subjects" Two groups selected from the inpatient population of the 'I Fraticini' Hospital in Florence were tested: an experimental and E. Lfidavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia 1077 Table 1. Characteristics of the neglect patients (PI P9) and control patients (CI-C9). For the cancellation tests the number of correct responses are reported. The highest score is 51 for the letters and 17 for the bells. R = right hemisphere; D = diencephalon: F = frontal lobe; P = parietal lobe: T = temporal lobe Cancellation tests Case Sex/Age P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 C6 C7 C8 C9 M,68 M,74 F,71 F,88 M,77 F,69 F,53 F,72 M,67 M,65 M,70 F,80 M,71 M,75 M,66 M,80 F,55 F,67 Education (yrs) Onset of Illness (mths) Locus lesion 17 17 8 13 8 13 5 8 5 5 13 8 8 5 17 13 8 8 3 14 2 2 9 2 4 4 3 5 3 14 2 3 2 2 4 3 RTD RD RPT RFP RFPT RFD RPTD RTD RPD RT RPD RD RD RD RFTP RD RPT RTP a control group. The experimental group consisted of nine neurological patients without visual field deficit,t each with a severe left visual neglect, neglect dyslexia for words and nonwords, and hemiplegia or severe hemiparesis contralateral to the lesion. The nine patients were selected from a larger population of 34 patients with severe left visual neglect, without intellectual and psychiatric disorders. The characteristics of each subject group are outlined in Table 1. The control group consisted of nine inpatients with right hemispheric lesions without visual neglect or visual field deficits. The presence/absence of horizontal visual neglect was assessed by a number of tests. In the current study, only some of them are mentioned. Others, such as drawing from memory, drawing from a sample, and reading sentences, will not be reported. For the present purposes, the relevant tests were: (1) to cross out ' H ' s in a structured array of letters [10]; (2) to cross out lines of a given orientation that were displayed among lines of many different orientations [1]; and (3) to cross out bells in a display of drawings of several objects [15]. Neglect patients who omitted more than 60% of the stimuli on the left in each test were included in the experimental group and they were then tested for neglect dyslexia. Few patients with visual neglect showed reading errors for words, whereas most of them manifested the deficit for nonwords. Because the aim of the present study was to explore the t In the present study patients with visual field deficits were not included in the experimental group because the presence of visual sensory deficits renders the interpretation of the results unclear. When these patients are tested, a lack of response for stimuli presented on the contralesional space may be due to a visual sensory deficits or to a specific impairment in spatial representation. Therefore, we believe that, in order to study spatial representational disorders, it is better to test patients without sensory deficits, although it is true that most of the neglect patients, but not all, have visual field deficits. . Letters . . . . L R 0 I~ I~ 1 II 38 2I 0 II 45 51 51 51 50 46 45 47 49 11 15 6 16 6 45 41 5 6 46 51 50 51 51 46 47 48 48 Bells . . k R 0 0 0 1 0 10 0 0 0 7 7 6 7 6 7 6 7 7 4 5 5 12 5 16 10 9 5 17 17 16 17 17 16 12 16 17 degree of preservation of lexical and semantic access in neglect patients, the investigation was confined only to patients who showed neglect dyslexia for words. Patients who made more than 20% of errors of the neglect dyslexia type on both words and non-words were then studied. Control patients who omitted less than 5% of the stimuli on either side were included in the control g r o u p Stimulus and procedure The stimuli were 78 letter strings, the length of which could be 7 letters (6 stimuli), 8 letters ( 16 stimuli), 9 letters (22 stimuli), 10 letters (22 stimuli), or I 1 letters (12 stimuli). Each letter was printed on upper case Palatino style (18pt). Half of the stimuli were Italian words (39 stimuli) and the other half (39 stimuli) were legal non-words obtained by substituting one (6 stimuli) or two (33 stimuli) vowels. For the non-words, the substituted vowels were equally located on the left and on the right side of the stimulus. All the words were concrete and denoted either living or non-living items (18 and 21, respectively). C o m p o u n d words were not used. The stimuli, located in the centre of the page (A4 format), were written horizontally, either normally or mirror-reversed. Each letter string was presented twice: once in the normal horizontal orientation, and once in the horizontal mirror-reversed orientation. They were shown one at the time by means of a moveable window, that is a window that concealed all the letter strings except the one that was currently shown to the patient. In the reading task, the patients were instructed to read the letter string aloud. In both the lexical and the semantic tasks they were instructed to make a decision about whether the letter string was a word or a non-word (lexical decision) and whether the word denoted a living or a non-living item (semantic decision). Each patient attended three experimental sessions in 1078 E. Lfidavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia 3 consecutive days.t Order of task was determined randomly and independently for each patient. Before performing the reading task, the patient was explicitly told that 50% of the letter strings were words and 50% were non-words, and that half of the stimuli were normal and half were mirror-reversed. Before performing the semantic task, the patient was told that only words would be shown, and that half denoted living items and half non-living items. The patient was also told that there was no time pressure for providing the response. ,oo N e g l e c t patients 90 W = Word NW = Non-word 80 L = Living 70 NL = Non-living 60 50 40 Results In the analyses of variance (ANOVAs) to be reported below, the dependent variable was accuracy, as indexed by error rate. In the case of the reading task, omitting or misreading one or more letters was considered to be an error for the whole letter string. For the lexical and semantic tasks, responses indicating an incorrect categorisation were considered errors. The probability that a word or a non-word is read completely without error (i.e. without omitting or misreading a single letter) is much lower than the 50% chance rate that applies to the lexical and semantic tasks. It must be pointed out, however, that the confounding between performance at chance level and type of task would be very damaging if the patients had the tendency to misread single letters in the string. Instead, our patients' errors were much more often omissions than substitutions (91% vs 9% overall, for either words or non-words). Substitution errors were those in which the patients produced the same number of letters as in the letter string but erroneously substituted one or more letters. Thus, considering the different distribution of omissions and substitutions, it does not seem likely that they tried to guess the letter they could not read. Perhaps, the fact that errorless reading was less likely than making a correct lexical decision or a correct semantic decision might explain why controls were more accurate in the lexical and semantic tasks than in the reading task (see results below). Two A N O V A s were performed on errors (also see Figs 1 and 2, and Table 2). All factors were within-subjects factors, except for the one concerning group. A first A N O V A was performed only on words. In it the factors were: group (patients vs controls), task (reading, lexical decisions or semantic decision), and stimulus orientation (normal vs mirror-reversed). The Bonferroni correction for the number of sources of variability yielded alpha--0.007. The main effects of group, task, and the interaction group x task, were significant: [F(I, 16)= 99.3, P<0.0001, F(2,32)= 119.2, P<0.0001], and [F(2,32)= 100.5, P < 0.0001 ], respectively. Errors were more numerous for patients than for con- 30 20 10 0 NW W NW L e x i c a l task L NL S e m a n t i c task Fig. 1. Percentage of errors committed by the neglect patients in the three tasks (reading, lexical decision, and semantic decision). trois (29.5% vs 4.3%), and for the reading than for either the lexical or the semantic task (36.9% vs 8.6% and 5.3%). The interaction showed that the difference in accuracy between patients and controls was much greater in the reading task (67.9% vs 5.9% errors, P<0.001) than in the lexical task (11.9% vs 5.3%, n.s.) or the semantic task (8.9 % vs 1.8 %, P < 0.01 ). Normally oriented words produced fewer errors than mirror-reversed words in reading, lexical and semantic tasks both in neglect patients (65.5 vs 70.3, 6.5 vs 17.3, 5.7 vs 12.1, respectively) and normal subjects (4.5 vs 7.4, 4.2 vs 6.5, 1.1 vs 2.6, respectively), However, none of the sources that included the factor stimulus orientation was significant. The second A N O V A was performed on responses to both words and non-words. The inclusion of non-words Control patients 100 90 W = Word 80 NW = Non-word 70 L = Living NL = Non-living 60504030- 10 o t Subsequently, the patients were tested in additional experimental sessions with the same stimuli arranged vertically. These data will be reported elsewhere. W R e a d i n g task w NW R e a d i n g task w NW L e x i c a l task L NL S e m a n t i c task Fig. 2. Percentage of errors committed by the control patients in the three tasks (reading, lexical decision, and semantic decision). E. L/tdavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia 1079 Table 2. Percentage of errors committed by each neglect patient (P l-P9) in the three tasks (reading:, lexical decision, and semantic decision) Reading task Case PI P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 P7 P8 P9 X Lexical task Semantic task Word Non-word Word Non-word Living Non-living 64 73 56.3 74.3 84.5 93.5 65.3 43.5 56.4 67.9 93.5 87.1 83.3 93.5 97.4 94.8 74.3 92.3 93.5 89.9 16.6 12.7 3.9 21.7 15.3 17.9 6.3 10.2 2.5 11.9 1.2 I 1.5 12.7 10.2 16.6 25.5 8.9 15.3 7.6 12.2 l1 13.8 2.5 2.7 32 8.3 16.5 24.9 11 13.6 0 7.1 0 0 4.7 7.1 9 9.5 0 4.2 rendered it necessary to eliminate the results for the semantic decision, which concerned only words. The factors were: group, task (reading vs lexical decision), lexical status (word vs non-word), and stimulus orientation. With the Bonferroni correction, alpha---0.003. The main effects of group [F(1,16)=145.7, P<0.0001], task [F(1,16) = 694.6, P<0.0001], and lexical status [F(1,16)=36.9, P<0.0001] were all significant. Also the interaction group x task [F(1,16)= 359.8, P < 0.0001] was significant. The second analysis showed that the results of the first one also held true when non-words were included. Patients made more errors than controls (45.5% vs 9.9%), and more so in the reading task (78.9% vs 15.4%, P < 0 . 0 0 1 ) than in the lexical task (12% vs 4.5%, P<0.01). For either group, the reading task proved to be more difficult than the lexical task (47.1% vs 8.2% errors). Normally oriented non-words produced fewer errors than mirror-reversed non-words in the reading task but not in the lexical task, both in neglect patients (87.4 vs 92.6, 14.2 vs 10.2) and control subjects (20.8 vs 29, 4.5 vs 2.8). However, none of the sources that included the factor stimulus orientation was significant. Another A N O V A was performed in the reading task to establish whether errors occurred more often on the left than on the right side of the letter string. Side was determined with reference to egocentric space for both normal and mirror-reversed letter strings. Thus, the letters at the begining of the letter string were coded as being on the left for normal strings and as being on the right for mirror-reversed strings. In the case of strings with an uneven number of letters, the central letter was not considered. In this ANOVA, the dependent variable was the percentage of omitted or misread letters on each side of the letter string. The factors were: group, lexical status, stimulus orientation, and side (left or right). With the Bonferroni correction, alpha=0.003. The significant sources that included the factor side were the main effect side [F(1,16) = 110.50, P < 0.001] and the interaction group x side [F(1,16)= 103.50, P<0.001]. Errors were more numerous on the left than the right side ~29. 1% vs 5.8%), but this asymmetry was present in the patient group (55.7% vs 9.9%) and absent in the control group (2.6% vs 1.8%). Neglect patients made more errors on the left than on the right side in all reading conditions, i.e. when the task required to read normally oriented words (50.3 vs 5.4), mirror-reversed words (52.3 vs 12.1), normally oriented non-words (61.9 vs 7.0) and mirror-reversed non-words (58.2 vs 15.0). None of the sources including stimulus orientation was significant. Controljor guessing and other alternative strategies The patients performed much better in the lexical and semantic decisions than in the reading task. An obvious possibility is that the letters reported in the reading task, although incomplete and incorrect, were enough for allowing a good guess about the lexical status and the semantic category of the letter string. That is to say, the patients might have used successfully a guessing strategy based on the outcome of whatever they were able to read. If this were the case, normal subjects should also be able to make correct lexical and semantic guesses when presented with the patients' verbal productions in the reading task. To control for this possibility, we proceeded as follows. The letter strings produced by each patient in the reading task were written on a piece of paper and were presented to four independent judges. Different judges were used for each patient, for a total of 36 judges. They were matched to the patients for age, sex and years of schooling. The controls were told that the letter string was an error produced by a patient when trying to read a word (or a non-word) and that the letters produced were mostly based on the right part of the word (although they may not be an entirely correct rendering of that part of the word). They were also given a number of examples. The task was two-Ibid. In the first session, the judges were asked to guess whether the letter string belonged to E. Lfi,davas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia deficit always affects one part of the word, regardless of the way the letter strings are presented. In the current study, the deficit might have affected the letters on the left side for normally arranged words and the letters on the right side for mirror-reversed words. The results showed that mirror-reversed stimuli, which are probably more difficult to be processed, produced more errors than normally oriented stimuli, although the difference was never significant and that left-sided errors were frequent for either normal or mirror-reversed stimuli. The third ANOVA, in which side (left or right) was included as a factor, is the relevant one for testing Caramazza and Hillis's hypothesis. It showed that the errors occurred always on the left side for either normal or mirrorreversed words. That is to say, neglect affected the egocentric left side of the words, rather than the left side of their canonical representations. It must be pointed out, however, that this finding does not confute Caramazza and Hillis's [6, 7] hypothesis, because in their model neglect can occur at each of three levels of representation, and that form of neglect dyslexia that affects the egocentric, stimulus-centered, representation is by far the most frequent one. The most interesting finding of the present study was that neglect patients performed much better in the lexical and semantic tasks than in the reading task. Actually, they performed very close to controls in the first two tasks, whereas their performance in the reading task was grossly impaired. These task-related differences cannot be attributed to the fact that there was a trend for word stimuli to produce word responses (either correct or wrong) and for non-word stimuli to produce non-word responses (either correct or wrong). In fact, patients produced non-word responses irrespective of the lexical status of the letter string. The simplest way to explain the task-related differences is by making the reasonable assumption that reading is a much more difficult task than either a lexical or a semantic decision. This is because the probability of being correct by adopting a guessing strategies is far lower in reading than it is in the other two tasks. This interpretation was addressed in the present study by asking normal controls to use the patients' responses to make lexical and semantic decisions. Because the controls could not produce correct lexical and semantic decisions on the basis of the patients' responses, it was concluded that successful guessing strategies on the patients' part was unlikely. One might however argue that the patients have some partial information and this information may help in the lexical and semantic tasks but may be insufficient for reading. Partial information about letters on the left can interact with later top down levels of processing and may suffice for performing the lexical semantic tasks but may be insufficiently precise to allow correct identification of the individual letters. This would be consistent with a computational model proposed by Mozer and Behrmann [28]. The important point is that in the control experiment aimed at testing the guessing interpretation, normal sub- 1081 jects were shown only the patients' final output, in which the degraded information on the left was missing. Thus, there was no basis for the higher t o p - d o w n processes to clean up degraded information. Even though the above account is in principle tenable, we believe it cannot be applied to the performance of the patients in the present study. Mozer and Behrmann's [28] model was proposed to explain reading pertbrmance of patients with mild neglect dyslexia, for whom most of the information on the left side of the words is still available, though in a degraded form. The patients described in this study, instead, had a severe neglect dyslexia, as attested by the fact that in the case of four- to five-syllable words, they reported only the one or two syllables on the right side. An interpretation in terms of guessing strategies, based on the degraded information, in the lexical and semantic tasks is perhaps applicable to twoto three-syllable words. In contrast, in our data there was no indication whatsoever that accuracy in the lexical and semantic tasks depended on word length. A further indication that a guessing strategy, based on the available partial information, was not used comes from the fact that lexical and semantic decisions were equally accurate for normal and mirror-reversed letter strings. When the words were mirror-reversed, the letters that were available at the explicit level belonged to the beginning of the word. Considering that the stem is the most important part of a word, guessing should have been more effective with mirror-reversed than with normally oriented words. None of the ANOVAs supported this prediction. In addition, it should be kept in mind that in the present study legal non-words were derived from words by substituting one or two vowels. Therefore, non-words were very similar to real Italian words. If one assumes that top down processing played a major role in cleaning up degraded inlbrmation, and considering that the lexicon contains only words, then the prediction is that the patients should produce more word than non-word incorrect responses, and incorrect word responses should be similar to the word from which the non-word was derived. In contrast, lexicalisation errors were extremely rare (see Table 4) and the incorrect word responses were of the omission type. An anonymous reviewer has suggested another interpretation that since in the reading task patients were told to expect word and non-word stimuli, the readingaloud response may follow some kind of silent analysis, performed on the letter string in order to decide whether it was a word or a non-word. The implicit lexical decision about the stimulus might have produced a rapid decay of the perceptual representation already degraded on the left side. This, in turn, might have produced reading errors confined on the left side of the letter string. This interpretation can be rejected in view of a recent study by Lfidavas el al. [22] in which it has been shown that patients wilh neglect dyslexia, showing preservation of lexical/semantic judgements for words they could not 1082 E. L~davas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia originally read, were able to read the word aloud following the lexical/semantic processing of the word. For the same reason, Luck et al.'s findings [24] (showing that words presented during the attentional blink are analysed to the point of meaning extraction, even though the extracted meaning cannot be reported 1-2 sec later), are not relevant for the interpretation of the present results. After having discarded the simplest interpretations, we proceed by taking into consideration alternative explanations that are based on the assumption that the present results reflect a true dissociation between different mechanisms used by patients in the different tasks. All the three tasks were direct, but performance was much better in two of them than in the third. What we have found, therefore, is a dissociation between direct tasks rather than between implicit and explicit knowledge (see [27, 31, 43]). A possible explanation of this dissociation might be in terms of the residual processing capabilities of a partially damaged brain system (e.g. [12]). In neglect patients, the visual representation of the letter string is impaired [2, 3]. Consequentely, an inappropriate representation within the visual word-form system might be activated [40]. This faulty access to the visual word-form system would cause reading errors but would allow correct lexical and semantic discriminations, which can be carried out with a weaker or noisier output from the orthographic system than that required for reading aloud. Note that this account is different from the one we have discussed, and rejected, above. The latter was mainly based on the notion that top-down processes clean up the degraded information and because of that differently affect the probability of being correct in the three tasks. The probability of being correct would be much higher in the lexical and semantic tasks than in the reading task. The explanation we are considering now maintains that a failure to identify or read the letter string is to be attributed to a decreased level of activation within the system that normally subserves explicit identification [34]. It can be noted in passing that this view is compatible with a cascade-type model of reading processes [25]. Weak input from an impaired word-form system could allow sufficient activation of the corresponding lexical and semantic representations to activate other representations by spreading activation, but not enough to inhibit the competing possibilities which an explicit identification requires. The degraded output of the orthographic system may be sufficient to support the correct category discrimination without being sufficient for the identification of the specific item itself. This is similar to the argument put forward by Farah [12] to explain some related neglect phenomena, and is essentially analogous to the explanation given for the performance of some semantic access and pure alexic patients who can carry out lexical decisions and perform category judgements at well above chance levels on words they cannot read aloud or identify [10, 35, 41]. An alternative interpretation of the dissociation may be found by making recourse to the classical dual-route model of reading aloud (e.g. [8, 33]). According to this model, a written letter string can be read by a non-lexical phonological route that implies grapheme-to-phoneme conversion, and by a lexical route that implies direct access to the mental lexicon through the visual representation of words. We propose that, due to the neglect deficit, our patients had a degraded visual representation of the letter string ([2, 3]). The representation was degraded because the component letters were poorly differentiated or because their relative order within the string was partly lost ([18, 30]). This degraded representation did not allow correct reading. Contrary to usual, errors were of the missed variety, rather than of the substitution type. A possible explanation for that is that neglect was very severe and did not allow the processing of the word length, which usually produces substitution errors. Also, errors were typically non-words (see Table 4). This can be considered as evidence that in the reading task our patients made use of the non-lexical phonological route, perhaps because lexical routes are useless for non-words. The low percentage of lexicalisation errors may be conceived as evidence that lexical routes were seldom used by our patients when the task required reading aloud. If they had used the addressed phonology, obtained through the lexical routes, and considered that non-words differed by just one or two vowels from real words, one should have expected to find many lexicalisation errors in response to non-words. That is, the patients should have read most non-words as words. This prediction was clearly contradicted by the results (see Table 4). Patients showed a strong bias to produce non-words in response to either word or non-word stimuli, although the bias seems stronger for non-word than word targets. In conclusion, it would seem likely that our patients used the non-lexical phonological procedure in the reading task. In contrast, it would seem that they, when asked to make lexical and semantic categorisation tasks, made use of the lexical routes, which could still operate even though the representation of the letter string was degraded. One question that needs to be addressed is why the non-lexical phonological route is more affected by neglect disorders than the lexical routes. It may be possible that the use of the lexical routes requires, but also leads to, a broadening of the attentional focus. In contrast, the use of the phonological route, due to the existence of multiple perceptual units within the same display, leads to a greater bias to the right by comparison with the single perceptual unit that is the input to the lexical routes. Evidence in favour of this interpretation comes from studies that showed that the lexical status of the letter string influences the distribution of visual attention [13, 36, 37]. The Farah et al.'s study [13] showed that in a letter colour naming task, neglect patients were more accurate at naming the colours on the left when the letter string E. L~tdavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia was a word than when it was a non-word. In addition, when the same patients were asked to mark the centre of a line that was presented underneath the letter string, the spatial distribution of attention was more symmetrical for words than for non-words. Sieroff and Posner [37] showed that, in normal subjects, when attention was cued to the left or right of foveal-centered letter strings, responses were greatly biased by the direction of attention for strings of consonants and not for orthographically regular words. The authors argued that chunking the letters within a visual word does not require attention because a visual word is processed as a single perceptual item. If a word constitutes a whole, it is not necessary for attention to scan the component letters. In contrast, orthographically irregular strings are comprised of many independent letters, and attention is necessary for scanning them. This reasoning was used by Sieroff et al. [36] to explain the well-known effect that neglect patients make fewer errors in reading words as compared to nonwords. In addition, the notion that the size of attentional focus depends on task demands is compatible with the results of many studies on normals, in which it has been shown that, unlike naming latency [14], latency for semantic category decisions is unaffected by word length [16], and with the distinction between object-based and locationbased attentional effects [12, 19], whereby object representations to which attention can be allocated may include such abstract objects as words, that is the input to the lexical routes. In conclusion, two alternative explanations have been offered to explain the dissociation between reading task and lexical and semantic categorisation tasks. The first is analogous to the explanation given for related phenomena in semantic access dyslexia and pure alexic patients, who can carry out lexical decisions and perform category judgements at well above chance level on words they cannot read aloud or identify [9, 17, 35, 41]. The other explanation is in terms of different reading routes, which require, but also lead to, different patterns of spatial attention over the degraded representation of the letter string. The lexical routes lead to an attentional focus that encompasses the single perceptual unit, whereas the phonological route leads to a bias of the attentional focus to the rightmost position of multiple perceptual units within the display. The attentional bias may play an important role in determining the extremely poor performance in the reading aloud task, as has been shown in many other perceptual tasks [21, 23]. Note that the interpretation based on the dual-route model differs from the quality of representation account that was put forward, among others, by Farah [12], which was discussed above. In Farah's account, neglect is assumed to result in a poor-quality representation that provides degraded information to higher level processes. This poor-quality representation is held to be sufficient to subserve tasks that require binary decisions (e.g. lexical decisions. Jiving/non-living judgements, same~different 1083 matchings of letters or objects), but would be insufficient to subserve multiple-choice naming. Even though the findings of the current study are no doubt consistent with both accounts, the two proposals are logically distinguishable because they make different predictions (see [22]). The degraded-information account predicts that performance of neglect patients should deteriorate as the semantic task becomes more difficult. In contrast, the dual-route account predicts that the level of performance in the semantic task should not depend on the difficulty of the semantic discrimination. This issue was directly addressed in a recent study by L'fidavas et al. [22], in which neglect patients performed a living/nonliving semantic decision task, a semantic categorial task, and a semantic inferential task. The living/non-living task was identical to the one used in the present study. In the categorial task, a visual target word was shown and was followed by two acoustically presented words, only one of which belonged to the same category as the visual target. Both, however, belonged to the broader living or non-living category (e.g. camel: elephant or fir-tree). The inferential task was very similar except that the two members of the pair belonged to the same semantic category, but only one of them was related to the target by means of a common feature (penguin: tuxedo or tracksuit). In either the categorial or the inferential task, the patient was asked to make a decision about which item was more related to the target, The results showed that performance was normal in every task, and, in accordance with the prediction of the dualroute hypothesis, it did not depend on the size of the category that was tapped by each task. In view of the results of the study by Ladavas et al. [22], we prefer to explain the findings of the current study according to the dual-route model, even though, in themselves, they can also be interpreted on the basis of the poor-quality information hypothesis. Acknowledgements--We are indebted to Barbara Cosi and Cristina Dazzi for collecting part of the data. E.L. and C.U. were supported by grants from MURST. C.U. was also supported by CNR through contract no. 9300752.PF41. References 1. Albert, M. L. A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 23, 658-664, 1973. 2. Behrmann, M., Moscovitch, M., Black, S. E. and Mozer, M. Perceptual and conceptual mechanisms in neglect: Two contrasting case studies. Brain 113, 1163-1183, 1990. 3. Behrmann, M., Moscovitch, M. and Mozer, M. C. Directing attention to words and nonwords in normal subjects and in a computational model: Implications for neglect dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8, 213-248, 1991. 4. Berti, A.. Allport, A., Driver, J., Dienes, Z., Oxbury, 1084 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. E. Lfidavas et al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia J. and Oxbury, S. Levels of processing for visual stimuli in an 'extinguished' field. Neuropsychologia 30, 403-415, 1992. Berti, A., Frassinetti, F. and Umilt/t, C. Nonconscious reading? Evidence from neglect dyslexia. Cortex 30, 181-197, 1994. Caramazza, A. and Hillis, A. E. Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient. Nature 346, 267-269, 1990. Caramazza, A. and Hillis, A. E. Levels of representations, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology 7, 391-445, 1990. Coltheart, M., Cognitive neuropsychology and the study of reading. In Attention and Performance XI, ed. M. I. Posner and O. S. M. Marin. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1985, pp. 3-37. Coslett H. B. and Saffran E. M., Mechanisms of implicit reading in alexia. In The Neuropsychology of High-level Vision, ed. M. J. Farah and G. Ratcliff. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N J, 1994. Diller, L. and Weinberg, J., Hemi-inattention in rehabilitation: The evolution of a rational remediation program. In Hemi-inattention and Hemispheric Specialization, ed. E. A. Weinstein and R. P. Friedland. Raven Press, New York, 1977, pp. 63-82. Ellis, A. W., Flude, B. M. and Young, A. W. 'Neglect dyslexia' and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords. Cognitive Neuropsychology 4, 439-464, 1987. Farah, M. J., Visual perception and visual awareness after brain damage: A tutorial overview. In Attention and Performance XV, ed. C. Umilth and M. Moscovitch. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994, pp. 37-75. Farah, M. J., Wallace, M. A. and Vecera, S. P., 'What' and 'Where' in visual attention: Evidence from the neglect syndrome. In Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies, ed. I. H. Robertson and J. C. Marshall. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N J, 1993, pp. 123 136. Frederiksen, J. R. and Kroll, J. F. Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2, 361-379, 1976. Gauthier, L,, Dehaut, F. and Joannett, J. The bell test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 11, 49-54, 1989. Green, D. and Shallice, T. Direct visual access in reading for meaning. Memory and Cognition 41,753758, 1976. Hinton, G. E. and Shallice, T. Lesioning an attractor network: Investigations of acquired dyslexia. Psychological Review 98, 74-95, 1991. Humphreys, G. W. Besner, D. and Quinlan, P. T. Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology 22, 517-560, 1990. Humphreys, G. W. and Riddoch, M. J., Interactive attentional systems and unilateral visual neglect. In Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies, ed. I. H. Robertson and J. C. Marshall. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N J, 1993, pp. 123 136. Lhdavas, E., Paladini, R. and Cubelli, R. Implicit 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. associative priming in a patient with left visual neglect. Neuropsychologia 31, 1307-1320, 1993. L~idavas, E., Petronio, A. and Umilt~., C. The deployment of attention in the intact field of hemineglect patients. Cortex 26, 307 317, 1990. Lhdavas, E., Shallice, T. and Zanella T., Preserved semantic access in neglect dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 35, 257-270, 1997. Lfidavas, E., Umilth, C., Ziani, P., Brogi, A. and Minarini, M. The role of right-side objects in leftside neglect: A dissociation between perceptual and directional motor neglect. Neuropsychologia 31, 761 773, 1993. Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K. and Shapiro, K. L. Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature 383, 616-618, 1996. McClelland, J. L. and Rumelhart, D. E. An interactive model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88, 375-407, 1981. Marshall, J. C. and Halligan, P. W. Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature 336, 766 767, 1988. Milner, A. D. and Rugg, M. D., The Neuropsychology of Consciousness. Academic Press, London, 1992. Mozer, M. and Behrmann, M. Interaction of selective attention and lexical knowledge: A connectionist account of neglect dyslexia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2, 96-123, 1990. Riddoch, J. Neglect and peripheral dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 7, 369-389, 1990. Riddoch, J., Humphreys, G., Cleton, P. and Fery, P. Interaction of attentional and lexical processes in neglect dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 7, 479 517, 1990. Rugg, M. D., Conscious and unconscious processes in language and memory--Commentary. In The Neuropsychology of Consciousness, ed. A. D. Milner and M. D. Rugg. Academic Press, London, 1992, pp. 263-278. Schacter, D. L., McAndrews, M. P. and Moscovitch, M., Access to consciousness: Dissociations between implicit and explicit knowledge in neuropsychological syndromes. In Thought Without Language, ed. L. Weiskrantz. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1988, pp. 242-278. Seidenberg, M. S. and McClelland, J. L. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96, 523-568, 1989. Shallice, T. and McGill, J., The origins of mixed errors. In Attention and Performance VII, ed. J. Requin. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N J, 1978. Shallice, T. and Saffran, E. M. Lexical processing in the absence of explicit word identification. Cognitive Neuropsychology 3, 429-458, 1986. Sieroff, E., Pollatsek, A. and Posner, M. I. Recognition of visual letter strings following injury to the posterior visual spatial attention system. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5, 427-449, 1988. Sieroff, E. and Posner, M. 1. Cueing spatial attention during processing of words and letter strings in normals. Cognitive NeuropaTchology 5, 451-472, 1988. E. Lfidavaset al./Lexical and semantic processing in neglect dyslexia 38. Umiltfi, C., and Moscovitch, M., Attention and PerJormance XV. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994. 39. Volpe, B. T., LeDoux, J. E. and Gazzaniga, M. S. Information processing of visual stimuli in an 'extinguished' field. Nature 282, 722-724, 1979. 40. Warrington, E. K. Right neglect dyslexia: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology 8, 193 212, 1991. 41. Warrington, E. K. and Shallice, T. Semantic access dyslexia. Brain 102, 43 63, 1979. 1085 42. Young, A. W., Conscious and nonconscious recognition of familiar faces. In Attention and PerJormance XV, ed. C. Umiltfi and M. Moscovitch. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1994, pp. 153178. 43. Young, A. W. and De Haan, E. H. F., Face recognition and awareness after brain injury. In The Neuropsychology of Consciousness. ed. A. D. Milner and M. D. Rugg. Academic Press, London, 1992, pp. 69 90.
© Copyright 2024