INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN ATTITUDES TOWARD LOVE ____________ A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Chico ____________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Psychology Option in Psychological Science ____________ by Alan T. Cook Summer 2012 INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN ATTITUDES TOWARD LOVE A Thesis by Alan T. Cook Summer 2012 APPROVED BY THE DEAN OF THE SCHOOL OF GRADUATE, INTERNATIONAL, AND INTERDISCIPLINARY STUDIES: ____________________________________ E.K. Park, Ph.D. APPROVED BY THE GRADUATE ADVISORY COMMITTEE: _____________________________________ Lawrence G. Herringer, Ph.D. _____________________________________ Margaret M. Bierly, Ph.D. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to acknowledge Dr. Lawrence Herringer for his invaluable guidance as a mentor for this thesis and graduate school in general. I am also grateful to Dr. Margaret Bierly (thesis committee) for her kindness and insight into the realm of developmental psychology. I give thanks to Dr. Sandra Machida and Dr. Paul Romanowich for their collegial attitudes and suggestions throughout the course of my graduate career, my sister Laura Dixon for introducing me to Chico and supporting my educational endeavors, my girlfriend Katy Sylvia for putting up with this process, and last but not least, my fellow Psychological Science peers who have been with me through the times, whether good or bad. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................ iii List of Tables ................................................................................................................. vi Abstract .......................................................................................................................... vii CHAPTER I. Introduction .................................................................................................................... 1 II. Literature Review ........................................................................................................... 9 Love as Destiny or Growth Belief ................................................................................. 9 N Love Rationale and Hypotheses Styles..................................................................................................................... 13 e Personality Traits and the Experience of Love .............................................................. 16 ur Attachment Orientation and Romantic Bonds ............................................................... 20 o Hypotheses ..................................................................................................................... 24 a III. dMethodology .................................................................................................................. 26 a IV. pt Findings and Results ...................................................................................................... 30 at io Descriptive Statistics ...................................................................................................... 30 n Bivariate Correlations .................................................................................................... 31 aM Simultaneous Multiple Regression Analyses................................................................. 31 ne m V. dDiscussion ...................................................................................................................... 34 T or yol ...................................................................................................................... 44 References er R aet nri ce e N v eal iv ur o d e g e n er at CHAPTER PAGE Appendices A. Beliefs About Relationships Scale and Demographics……………………… v 53 LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1. Means and Standard Deviations for each Research Variable……………….. vi 30 ABSTRACT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN ATTITUDES TOWARD LOVE by Alan T. Cook Master of Arts in Psychology: Psychological Science Option California State University, Chico Summer 2012 Are you destined to find your soul-mate? According to Knee, Patrick, & Lonsbary (2003), those with a destiny belief place a high value on whether a relationship is fated, while those with a growth belief believe that relationships develop gradually. I examined whether attachment, love styles, and personality factors predicted belief in destiny or growth. I hypothesized that individuals who scored higher on anxious attachment and the Chance component of locus of control would score higher on belief in destiny. Undergraduate psychology students (n = 117) completed measures of attachment and love styles, belief in destiny versus growth, perceived control over falling in love, and locus of control. Simultaneous multiple regression found that belief in destiny could be significantly predicted from high Eros, Mania, Pragma, and Avoidance. A separate analysis found high conscientiousness and high internal and high chance orientations of locus of control also predicted belief in destiny. None of the variables significantly predicted belief in growth. vii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION “My soul spills into yours and is blended. Because my soul has absorbed your fragrance, I cherish it. Every drop of blood I spill Informs the earth, ‘I merge with my Beloved when I participate in love’” Rumi - The Ruins of the Heart Love has given humankind meaning and pleasure (as well as pain) for millennia. The Sufi mystic Rumi seems to capture the essence of falling in love: its beauty and its pain in his poem “the Ruins of the Heart.” It is interesting that such a powerful force as love can be the vehicle for both incredible elation and equal misery. What is it that causes some people to revel in joy for the majority of their lives because of love, while others slip in and out of love, or attempt to shut it out? It has long been found that social relationships help determine well-being, and furthermore that close and supportive relationships are more important to well-being than basic social interactions (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). The absence of a close companion often results in loneliness and sadness. Human beings have taken different individual approaches to finding companionate love throughout time. For some, it is a constant painful process (although great songs are written because of this), while others see their lover as their best friend and companion for life. 1 2 How much control does a person have when “falling in love?” Does love “happen” and a person is helplessly swept up in it, or does love “grow” and develop as people consciously choose to deepen their relationship? Do perceptions of the process of falling in love depend on a person’s previous experiences with love and the thoughts that an individual has formed over time concerning love? In a romantic relationship, do peoples’ expectations about their partner remain stable, or change over time? Is love fixed or evolving? Of course, the issue of personal control when falling in love is actually one of perceived control, since actual control in human relationships is almost impossible to establish. Psychologists have attempted to answer these very questions by classifying and examining individual differences in perceptions and expressions of love, though simple classification schemes are often inadequate to explain “love.” One major psychological theory that can explain differences in the course of romantic relationships is attachment theory. Attachment theory, with its origins in the work of John Bowlby (1969), uses a biological approach to the idea of falling in love and mating. During infancy, human beings establish a behavioral system that regulates interactions between offspring and parents and is activated in times of stress or anxiety. In addition, a cognitive schema, or “internal working model” develops to represent the expected course of interactions concerning attachment. The attachment system is highly evolutionarily adaptive in that it helps ensure survival of offspring. Bowlby’s colleague Mary Ainsworth identified three attachment orientations through empirical research that differentiate peoples’ responses 3 to their caregivers: secure, anxious/ambivalent, and avoidant (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). A secure orientation is one in which the caregiver has been responsive to the child’s needs and bids for attention. In these cases, after the attachment system has been activated, it soon becomes deactivated by the parents’ attentions. Children with secure orientations see their parents as both a safe haven in times of need and as a secure base from which to explore the environment. It is thus associated with the best developmental outcomes because children are free to develop through exploration and have no fear that their caregivers will abandon them. In contrast, an anxious attachment is characterized by a caregiver responding inconsistently to a child’s bids for affection, so that the individual’s attachment system is persistently hyper-activated. Someone with an anxious attachment may vacillate between the need for attention and responses such as pushing the caregiver away. Individuals with an anxious attachment are more likely to be governed by a fear of abandonment, and are less likely to use their caregiver as a secure base from which to explore their environment. Finally, avoidant attachment is characterized by a caregiver neglecting or ignoring the child’s bids, and leads to a persistent de-activation of the attachment system. Avoidantly attached people attempt to maintain autonomy and independence from their caregivers to avoid the negative feelings of neglect or abandonment. People characterized by an avoidant attachment are less likely to be concerned with closeness and prefer to maintain distance from caregivers and relationships. While anxious and avoidant 4 attachments may be seen as negative outcomes, they are also in fact adaptive survival responses to a neglectful or inconsistent environment. According to more recent attachment theorists, current romantic relationships are a reflection of early attachment bonds to caregivers (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). People will have similar ways of interacting with their romantic partners as with their early caregivers, as their attachment orientations are for the most part very stable traits. In romantic relationships, an anxiously attached partner may be characterized by dependency and jealousy. Anxiously attached individuals may also be governed by a fear of abandonment in relationships and will be more likely to exhibit “clinging” behaviors. They may feel a mix of tumultuous emotions when in a relationship ranging from elation to despair. An anxious person may disclose highly personal information inappropriately at an early stage in a relationship, causing a potential partner to withdraw (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). An avoidantly attached partner may be characterized by emotional apathy and unavailability in a relationship. People with an avoidant attachment orientation may struggle the most on initial dates due to their lack of self-disclosure and their discomfort with personal disclosures. People who exhibit high avoidant behaviors also feel uncomfortable with intimacy in relationships and sometimes believe that true love is something that only exists in stories and movies (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). A secure orientation is associated with the best outcomes romantically, as securely attached individuals are more comfortable with intimacy and generally have higher self-esteem. A secure person is comfortable with autonomy but also with 5 emotional intimacy, and does not consider these to be mutually exclusive. A securely attached person’s relationships are more satisfactory because a securely attached person is able to continue to develop as an individual while at the same time growing within the relationship, and is responsive to his or her partner in times of need or stress. Separate, yet closely related to attachment orientation in adult romantic relationships are the so-called “love styles.” Hendrick and Hendrick (1986) identified six “love-styles,” which describe individuals’ general beliefs and thought processes concerning love. A Ludus love-style (game-playing and uncommitted behaviors in relationships) is similar to avoidant attachment in that it is reflective of an avoidance of intimacy and a tendency to view sex in a non-emotional, self-gratifying way. The Manic love-style (obsessive, intense and uncontrollable feelings in relationships) may be associated with anxious-attachment, as it is characterized by excessive and intrusive thoughts about the partner that may become obsessive. In past research, Mania has been positively associated with anxious-attachment while Ludus has been associated with avoidant attachment (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). An additional concept that may be relevant to the idea of perceived control when falling in love may be the issue of whether people believe their relationships are dependent upon fate or upon individual thought and effort. Knee, Patrick and Lonsbary (2003) proposed that individuals fall along a continuum concerning both a belief in destiny and a belief in growth regarding falling in love. They conceptualize these as two independent dimensions: high belief in destiny is not necessarily associated with low belief in growth. Those people strongly believing that love is destiny were shown to be 6 more judgmental and evaluative towards relationships. Those strongly believing that love is growth were more patient and willing to work through challenges in relationships. Also relevant to perceptions about love relationships is the theory of “locus of control,” with its origins in the work of Julian Rotter (1966). This “generalized expectancy” concerns whether people believe that they control outcomes in their lives or whether external influences have more control. People who believe that external influences (e.g., fate, luck or other people) control their life outcomes are said to have an external locus of control, while people who believe that their efforts and abilities are responsible for their life circumstances are said to have an internal locus of control. Locus of control may in fact provide a more general explanation for showing differences between belief in destiny (external) and belief in growth (internal). So, specific differences in belief in destiny or growth may not be necessary to explain differences in love relationships. General personality traits may also explain more specific “love styles” or other aspects of romantic relationships. The Five Factor Model (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is the most widely used and accepted theoretical model used to explain personality traits, which includes extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, and neuroticism. It provides a parsimonious and comprehensive system of the major dimensions of personality. Extraversion (sociability and outgoingness) may be related to the frequency or “chances” of falling in love, as extraverts may put themselves in more social situations than people with low extroversion. Since neuroticism is associated with emotional instability, it may explain some dysfunctional relationship attitudes that an 7 individual may have, such as avoidant attachment and the Ludic love style. Thus, individual differences in relationships may be predicted by personality traits more than either love styles or attachment styles. I would expect these various theories to be related to the idea of perceived control when falling in love. Is perceived control when falling in love related to attachment orientation, love-style, and destiny and growth belief? Or is it actually more a function of general aspects of perceived control, for example locus of control or personality traits? These are the questions that I attempted to answer in my study. To examine these questions, a short 5-item survey (the Beliefs About Relationships Scale) was constructed to measure perceived control when falling in love. Standardized measures were used to assess relationship-specific attitudes and behaviors: the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) to examine attachment orientation, the Love Attitudes Scale: Short-Form (Hendrick, Hendrick, & Dicke, 1998) to examine the six love-syles, and the Implicit Theories of Relationships Scale (Knee, Patrick & Lonsbary, 2003) to measure belief in growth and belief in destiny. To examine more general personality, the Big-Five-Inventory (BenetMartinez & John, 1998) was used to measure the Five Factor Model and the Internality, Powerful Others, and Chance Scales (Levenson, 1981) were used to measure three components of locus of control. California State University, Chico undergraduate students completed these six scales in random order and the results and bivariate correlations were examined between perceived control when falling in love, attachment orientations, love styles, and belief in 8 growth and belief in destiny. Simultaneous multiple regression analysis was used to determine whether perceived control when falling in love can be predicted from the lovestyles, attachment orientations, and belief in destiny and growth. Simultaneous multiple regression analysis was also used to determine whether perceived control when falling in love could be predicted simply by the Five Factor Model personality measures or locus of control (including internality, powerful others, and chance). In general, I expected that securely attached individuals (i.e., low avoidance and low anxiety) would have stronger belief in growth and would report higher perceived control when falling in love. Anxiously attached individuals would have stronger belief in destiny and would report lower perceived control when falling in love. Finally, I expected those with a generally external locus of control to have stronger belief in destiny, and those with a generally internal locus of control to have stronger belief in growth. CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW Love as Destiny or Growth Belief The question of how an individual falls in love is one that human beings have pondered since the dawn of time. Love is the subject of countless poems, rock ballads, and heartbreak. Influential thinker Erich Fromm (1956) believed that Westerners had negative conceptions of love such as a belief in falling in love spontaneously and with little effort. He also distinguished between different types of love: love for children, brotherly, self-love, romantic and erotic, perhaps setting the pace for future classifications of love. Other philosophers and thinkers such as Carl Jung (1953) believed that love in its purest form is a positive experience that leads to personal transcendence and integration of the masculine and feminine aspects of self. It is interesting how human beings become so attached to one special individual, and how even others with similar characteristics are never as attractive or appealing. In fact, sometimes love is so overwhelming that it can blind one to the potential of other people and even become a painful obsession. Some psychologists believe that a new relationship offers the opportunity of self-expansion and incorporating a partner’s beliefs and resources into oneself (Aron & Aron, 1986). Of special importance in the experience of love is whether people believe that they are destined to fall in love with one particular individual or if they have more of a personal choice in the matter. In other words, is falling in love a matter of destiny, or a 9 10 matter of growing feelings of compatibility through time? Knee (1998) first introduced the idea of viewing relationships as “destiny” or a “growth” process. He elaborated on this distinction in later research, culminating in the creation of a self-report measure (Knee, Patrick & Lonsbary, 2003). Knee et al. (2003) defined destiny belief as the belief that potential romantic partners are either compatible or they are not. Those with a predominant destiny belief place a high value on whether a relationship is fated, or “meant to be.” In other words, a person believing primarily in destiny has an internal thought process that explains relationship events as “fixed.” Such a person tends to set an early emphasis on whether a romantic partner will be compatible, and uses negative cues as explanations that a relationship will not be successful (Knee & Canavello, 2006). Those placed high on the destiny belief spectrum also are more judgmental and evaluative towards relationships. In contrast, those believing strongly in growth assume that relationships develop gradually over time, and that relationship challenges can be overcome. A person endorsing a belief in growth sees conflict and setbacks as a learning experience, rather than as a sign that a relationship will not work out. Furthermore, someone endorsing belief in growth does not see a potential partner as incompatible forever just because they are incompatible at the initial meeting (Knee & Canavello, 2006). Orientations towards belief in destiny or belief in growth can be seen as “implicit theories” an individual has regarding whether relationship events are fixed and stable, or flexible and amendable. Knee et al. (2003) conceptualize belief in destiny and belief in growth as two separate dimensions, reporting a non-significant negative correlation between the two. 11 While it may seem that people high on destiny belief are low on growth belief and viceversa, individuals might have a strong belief in destiny (such that they believe relationship events to be fated) but also have a strong growth belief (which would enable them to work through relationship problems). In theory, someone who has a low growth belief and high destiny belief will experience the most troubles with relationships, while someone with high growth belief and low destiny belief would be flexible in working through problems and would not place blame on a relationship partner for problems. Research on implicit theories of relationships may be similar to the theory of “approach and avoidance” goals in that those with a destiny belief are more motivated by avoiding negative events in relationships rather than seeking out positive experiences and growth (Impett et al., 2010). Those focused primarily on avoiding negative events are less satisfied with relationships because they are less responsive to working out conflicts and less communicative in relationships. Such implicit theories of relationships may also be important as factors that lead to a happy or unhappy marriage. For example, if individuals have a destiny belief that leads them to believe that they will never experience relationship difficulties with their spouses, then they may be in for a rude surprise. In their study comparing young adults and professional educators, Wright, Simmons and Campbell (2007) found that married individuals believed that a successful marriage depended partly on whether love was viewed as a deep friendship and whether problems were viewed as solvable. Furthermore, they found that young adults endorsed more of a destiny belief, believing that in healthy relationships, partners will not disagree on relational issues. In contrast, 12 educational professionals surveyed in the study were more likely to endorse a growth belief, responding that healthy relationships are characterized by good communication, effort and maintenance. Interestingly, Finkel, Scissors and Burnette (2007) found that when attachment anxiety was primed in an individual, those with strong destiny beliefs were less forgiving towards their partner (whether their partner was real or hypothetical), compared to those who were primed for attachment security. Considering that forgiveness and communication seem to enhance relationship functioning, this is an important finding. It will be important to find the extent that attachment security is related to orientation towards growth or destiny, because attachment security could moderate the effects that beliefs in growth or destiny have on an individual. In a further study concerning implicit theories of relationships, Canevello and Crocker (2011) found that in college freshmen, “compassionate goals” predicted an increased belief in growth (perhaps because both partners’ needs were being met). This is evidence that under certain circumstances, an orientation towards belief in growth or belief in destiny is flexible and can change. It also encourages people to take steps towards changing their own possibly negative approaches towards relationships. More research needs to be completed to determine whether implicit theories of relationships have both trait-based and state-based attributes. Researchers curious about individual differences people take in the experience of love have examined love as destiny/growth belief and how it relates to love-styles and attachment orientation. 13 Love Styles It has been seen by both the general population and love researchers that individuals take different attitudes and approaches to the subject of love. For example, some people have been known to persistently pursue multiple partners, while others fall intensely in love with only a single individual. What is it that drives these individual differences and perceptions? Robert Sternberg (1986, 1998) proposed a triangular theory of love composed of three components: intimacy, passion and decision/commitment. Sternberg believes that the intimacy and passion components are emotional aspects of the experience of love, while the decision/commitment component is a cognitive factor. Intimacy involves “desiring to promote the welfare of the loved one,” and counting on a loved one in times of need. Passion involves the longing for a state of union with the desired love-object. Finally, decision/commitment involves a short-term decision to love a partner and a longterm decision to commit to that relationship. Sternberg proposed that the three components of the triangle could blend to create several forms of love, for example “empty love” (passion without intimacy or commitment), “companionate love” (intimacy and decision/commitment but no passion) and “consummate love” (the closest to “true love,” including intimacy, passion and decision/commitment). Similar to Sternberg’s taxonomy of loving are the “love-styles.” Hendrick and Hendrick (1986) developed the Love Attitudes Scale based on the sociologist John Alan Lee’s (1976) love taxonomy. Using the analogy of a color wheel in which there are solids and mixtures, Lee conceptualized three primary love 14 styles: Eros, Ludus, and Storge. Based on the primary love styles, Lee believes that compounds (two primary ingredients forming to create a completely new style) and mixtures (a blend of two primary colors such that the result is a reflection of the primaries “but in a diluted form”) exist. The compounds and mixtures comprise the secondary styles. Secondary styles include Pragma, Mania, and Agape, out of which several mixtures are possible. While Lee (1976) originally declared twelve love styles, he later gave eight priority. Hendrick and Hendrick (1986) devised the Love Attitudes Scale after revising and performing factorial analysis on Lee’s original styles. Their results indicated six, rather than eight love-styles. The love-styles may be considered general attitudes toward love or thought-complexes towards love. Eros involves passionate/erotic love, while Ludus depicts game-playing/uncommitted love. Storge is the name given for companionate/friendship love, Pragma concerns practical love (analogous to having a “shopping list” of qualities looked for in a mate), Agape is unselfish, altruistic love, and Mania regards obsessional love. High Agape is rare in the general population. In addition, cross-cultural evidence shows that Americans are more likely to endorse both Storgic and Manic love attitudes and that passionate love was among the best indicators for relationship satisfaction (Contreras, Hendrick & Hendrick, 1996). In 1998, a new 24-item version of the Love Attitudes Scale was created, which demonstrated equal and sometimes better reliability than the original scale (Hendrick, Hendrick & Dicke, 1998). According to Hendrick (2006) love-styles can be subject to change due to life circumstances and particularly the age of the individual. 15 Interestingly, destiny belief has been found to be correlated with Pragma, while growth belief correlated positively with Storge, or the friendship based approach to love (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982). Perhaps these findings reflect that a person endorsing the Pragma love-style would require certain traits in a partner and would consider the relationship to be doomed if these traits did not exist. Storge may correlate with growth belief because rather than taking the extreme view of fate as the reason a relationship succeeds or fails, a person endorsing Storgic attitudes sees love as a friendship in which arguments and conflict may be overcome. In regard to personality, destiny belief has been found to be associated with less openness to experience, and higher extraversion and neuroticism. In contrast, growth belief was associated with higher conscientiousness and agreeableness (Knee, 1998). Relationships to attachment and personality. Research has found that anxious attachment may be positively predictive of the love-styles Mania and Ludus (Heaven, Da Silva, Cary, & Holen, 2004). Considering that Mania is associated with emotional dependency and Ludus is associated with an unwillingness to depend on others, they may both reflect different sides of the anxious-attachment orientation (ambivalence). Regarding personality and the love-styles, neuroticism was found to be positively linked to both Ludus and Mania. This shows that people with an emotionally unstable personality trait may engage either in possessive, jealous attitudes or in game-playing, non-committed behaviors. In their review of attachment and love-styles, Mikulincer and Shaver (2007) report that secure attachment is related to Eros and Agape, which are both love styles that promote intimacy and commitment. Avoidance was associated with low 16 Eros and high Ludus. Collins and Read (1990) found that secure orientations were associated with high Eros and Agape and were less likely to be associated with Ludus and Mania. In addition, they found that avoidant attachment was related to Pragma. Thus, avoidant attachment favors both Ludic game-playing and practicality and inhibits lasting romance. Personality traits and the experience of love It is important not to discount the power of general personality variables when examining falling in love. It may be important to consider common personality traits to account for the fact that findings regarding falling in love may really be a reflection of a global personality trait rather than a specific love-style or attachment orientation. The most widely accepted personality trait framework today is the Five Factor Model which comprises the personality traits of openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism, each on a continuum (Goldberg, 1993). Extraversion may be an important trait to examine when researching falling in love. For instance, people scoring higher on the sociability dimension of extraversion report being in love more often than those who are shy (Asendorpf & Wilpers, 1998). This is a reflection of the fact that people high in sociability are more likely to put themselves in contact with other people and have more dating options. In Asendorpf and Wilpers’ study, students low in shyness had fallen in love “twice as often as the shy participants (pg 1541).” Interestingly, in the same study relationship changes had no effect on personality, but personality had an effect on the incidence of peer interactions 17 and falling in love. Past research has shown that relationship satisfaction is important to well-being. For example, increased relationship satisfaction can be predicted from decreases in neuroticism and increases in extraversion longitudinally (Scollon & Diener, 2006). Personality dimensions have been found to be related to attachment. For example, Heaven et al. (2004) found extraversion and agreeableness to be positively related to an intimacy and trust scale (aspects of the secure attachment orientation), while neuroticism negatively predicted intimacy and trust. They also found that neuroticism correlated with Ludus and Mania, and negatively with Eros. The fact that neuroticism negatively predicted the intimacy and trust scale explains that it is related to avoidant attachment, as avoidance is characterized by an inability to depend on or be close to others. Using the Views of Love Scale, Fehr and Broughton (2001) found positive correlations between neuroticism and “sexual, infatuation, and puppy-love” and a negative correlation between neuroticism and “friendship love.” Although they did not include love-styles, sexual and infatuation love are consistent with Ludus and Mania conceptually and friendship-love is similar to Storge. Consistent with these findings, White, Hendrick, and Hendrick (2004) found that neuroticism correlated with Ludus for males, and with Mania for females. They further found that neuroticism was negatively correlated with Eros for males. The findings with extraversion and agreeableness are consistent with the idea that securely attached individuals are able to have a healthy amount of dependency (for example, trusting others) in relationships with others. By contrast, high neuroticism may 18 at times reflect an emotional instability that does not allow for feelings of security or closeness with others. However, one study that considered personality traits as an explanation for insecure attachment found that insecure orientations were not explained by the Big Five or other personality measures such as depression and self-esteem (Noftle & Shaver, 2006). White et al. (2004) found that both extraversion and agreeableness were positively related to relationship satisfaction and intimacy. In addition to the intimacy variable, White et al. also included a love-styles scale, and found that neuroticism was a significant predictor of Mania. Interestingly, they found that for women, Mania mediated the relationship between neuroticism and relationship satisfaction. This may show that possessive or dependent relationship attitudes indicate particularly strong neurotic tendencies. They further found the unexpected result that agape was correlated with neuroticism for women and speculated that while Agape in moderation may reflect a healthy concern for a love-partner, excessive Agape may reflect an all-consuming concern with the partner that is detrimental to the self. In a study examining the relationship of “fundamental motives,” the fivefactor personality traits, and Sternberg’s (1986) tripartite model of intimacy, passion and commitment, Engel, Olson, and Patrick (2002) found that for males conscientiousness significantly predicted all three of Sternberg’s variables and was correlated with relationship satisfaction. Conscientiousness involves a sense of self-control and duty; therefore it could be related to intimacy because conscientious people may apply their dutifulness to relationships. It is also related to achievement motivation, and this need for 19 achievement may be generalized to their love relationships. Commitment was predicted by the “assertiveness” facet of extraversion, and was negatively predicted by the “vulnerability” facet of neuroticism. For females, relationship satisfaction was correlated with extraversion and conscientiousness. The deliberation facet of conscientiousness predicted passion for females (deliberation reflects a tendency to think things through before acting or to engage in cautious planning before acting). Interestingly, Nemechek and Olson (1999) found that spouses who had a similar level of the conscientiousness trait reported greater marital satisfaction. Another relevant consideration regarding personality and falling in love is locus of control. Locus of control refers to a person’s internal states and how someone deals with circumstances. Locus of control may be an important variable regarding belief in destiny when falling in love because an individual with an external locus of control trait may be more likely to endorse a belief in destiny orientation, while an individual with a high internal locus of control may be more likely to possess the belief in growth orientation. Early research on locus of control by Julian Rotter (1966) focused on a typology of internal states. For example, individuals were considered to have either an internal or an external locus of control, believing that they had control over outcomes in their life or conversely that life events were the cause of external influences. Of note, people may differ individually on their desirability of control over events in their lives (Burger & Cooper, 1979). For example, people high in the desire for control would be characterized by taking leadership roles and would manipulate situations to best match their most desired outcomes. People who are low in the desire for 20 control are passive and show a preference for being led by others. People both low and high in desirability of control are more likely to be active when manipulation of a situation is obviously advantageous and has a high pay-off. Furthermore, the strength of individuals’ desire for control interacts with situational variables to determine their perceptions of control over a given domain (be it concerning relationships, work, or an academic setting). More recent research has focused on separate components of locus of control and specific areas in which individuals vary in locus of control (for example, marital, work-related, and relationships). In 1981, Levenson created the Internality, Powerful Others, and Chance subscales. These scales more accurately acknowledge that individuals can believe in their own efficacy while at the same time believing that powerful others and chance may have some control over their lives. Would these components of locus of control be related to belief in destiny and belief in growth? Attachment orientation and romantic bonds The theoretical framework of attachment helps to explain bonds between an infant and their caregivers. The work of John Bowlby (1969) suggests that all human beings are born with an innate attachment system which regulates behavior in times of stress and anxiety. Specifically, a child must see his/her caregiver as a “safe haven” in times of need and a “secure base” from which to explore the environment in order to develop optimally in terms of healthy relationships. Bowlby’s ideas were empirically tested by Mary Ainsworth et al. (1978), who identified three attachment orientations that 21 differentiate children’s responses to their caregivers in times of stress and anxiety: secure, anxious/ambivalent, and avoidant. Individuals with secure orientations perceive their caregivers as responsive to their needs and also regard them as a secure base. This means that they feel free to explore their environments but also return to their caregivers as needed, or if they feel unsafe. Those with secure orientations are confident that their caregivers will not abandon them. Securely attached individuals are associated with the best relationship outcomes, as they are free to develop individual initiative as they explore their environment without the fear of abandonment. The two other orientations are considered “insecure,” and they are a child’s adaptive responses to perceived inadequate care. An anxious attachment is characterized by the fear of abandonment and the belief that a parent or caregiver will not be responsive to needs. Anxious attachment is caused by an inconsistent parenting style in which sometimes the parent is responsive to the child, but at other times is not. This encourages anxiously attached children to “hype up” attempts to elicit care from their parents, since they have been successful in some scenarios. This can cause anxiously attached individuals to appear “clingy,” or needy. The other insecure attachment orientation, avoidant, is caused by neglectful parenting in which a caregiver does not acknowledge the child’s bids for attention. Avoidantly attached children adaptively respond by withdrawing into themselves, since continuing to attempt to elicit attention would be ineffective. Children with an avoidant 22 attachment are characterized by an exaggerated independence and self-sufficiency that often continues later into their lives. Attachment orientations can also be described as enduring mental models, or “representations” in the form of schemas based on early relationship experiences (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994). For example, a secure orientation is one in which a person has a positive view of self and other. This tends to generalize to the individual’s romantic relationships. An anxious orientation is one in which a person has a negative view of self and a positive view of other, which explains why that person might exhibit “clingy” behaviors in order to become closer to a partner, but is not confident that he or she will receive the attention needed. Finally, an avoidant orientation is characterized by a positive view of self and negative view of other, such that someone with an avoidant orientation does not perceive “the need for relationships,” and is convinced that all personal problems can be solved independently. In 1987, Hazan and Shaver conceptualized that early attachment bonds and styles may be predictive of future romantic bonds. The attachment styles include anxiousattachment, involving a hyper-activation of the attachment system (an innate behavioral system regulating actions in times of stress), avoidant attachment (which involves attachment-system deactivation), and secure attachment. Secure attachment is associated with the best developmental outcomes out of the three styles, including better coping strategies and emotional regulation. Early research involved placement in one of the three attachment categories. More recent approaches view the three attachment styles as continuous dimensions, and obtain a score on each. Further, although retrospective 23 reports of childhood experiences are still sometimes used, many measures emphasize self-report of current attitudes and behaviors (Ravitz, Maunder, Hunter, Sthankiya, Lancee, 2010). Recent research shows that attachment style can change over time due to life experiences rather than remaining static. For example, changes in relationship status such as becoming newly single, divorce of parents, and traumatic experiences such as death can temporarily shift attachment orientations. In general, the longer a person has been in a relationship, the more likely that person is to shift from an insecure toward a secure attachment style (Fraley & Brumbaugh, 2004). Hazan and Shaver (1988) believed that love styles could actually be subsumed under attachment orientations: Eros under secure, Mania under anxious/ambivalent, and Ludus under avoidant. Thus, love styles are more specific, and limited, expressions of various aspects of attachment orientations. Feeney and Noller (1990) tested the contentions of Hazan and Shaver (1987), using attachment measures and love style measures. Replicating Hazan and Shaver, Feeney and Noller found that securely attached people were more likely to endorse positive early family bonds and anxiously attached people were more likely to endorse a lack of parental support. Avoidantly-attached individuals avoided intimacy in relationships, and endorsed more Ludic love style behaviors, which includes gameplaying aspects of love, and having multiple partners. However, there is more to avoidant attachment than just game playing in love (Ludus). Avoidant attachment comprises many behaviors including absorbing oneself in professional or work-related activities at the expense of relationships at all (Collins, 24 Guichard, Ford, & Feeney, 2004). In addition, Ludus is associated with narcissism, reflected in a need for power and lack of commitment in relationships (Campbell, Finkel, & Foster, 2002). This is seen in narcissists’ self-focus and resulting unconcern for others, and the tendency for narcissists to disconnect sex from intimacy. Hypotheses I am interested in the idea of perceived control when in falling in love and what personality types and attachment styles would be more likely to report having perceived control when falling in love. Belief in less control when falling in love seems to be related to Knee’s “destiny belief.” Conversely, the belief of having control when falling in love is most likely related to “growth belief.” These constructs furthermore are likely to be related to locus of control, as destiny belief reflects extrinsic motivations while growth belief reflects intrinsic motivations. Hypothesis 1 (H1): having perceived control when falling in love is correlated positively to both growth belief and the secure attachment style. In accordance with previous attachment research (Hazan & Shaver, 1987), H2: anxiously attached individuals will report less perceived control when falling in love (more intense feelings). For the “control in falling in love” concept, I created a short measure to assess this. It is important to include a general locus of control measure in this research to control for the fact that those reporting less perceived control when falling in love might actually just have a stronger external locus of control in all areas of their lives. Using Levenson’s Internality, Powerful Others and Chance Scales (1981), I hypothesize (H3) that the participants who score higher on the chance component of locus of control will 25 also score higher on the belief in destiny variable. Furthermore, I hypothesize that those who score higher on the internality component should also score higher on belief in growth (H4). Regarding personality dimensions, I hypothesize (H5) that conscientiousness will predict growth belief, given its focus on duty and attentiveness. Finally, I hypothesize that avoidant-attachment will be correlated with the love style Ludus (H6), as it has in past research. I believe this study is important because much research has examined changes in relationships that already exist or relationship beliefs, but have not examined ideas about falling in love and specifically the idea of perceived control or destiny regarding love. Furthermore, I believe that locus of control is important in this idea of having perceived control. People who generally score higher on an intrinsic trait motivation may be more likely to report feeling control concerning falling in love. Additionally, lovestyles may be important in examining whether practical loving styles or passionate styles are related to both beliefs in perceived control and attachment style. This research is also important in helping to understand which relationship attitudes and beliefs lead to better developmental outcomes. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY Participants Participants were 117 CSU, Chico undergraduate psychology majors (96 female, 21 male) with a mean age of 22.77 who participated for course extra credit. Of the participants, 46.2% identified themselves as in a relationship, 34.2% identified themselves as single, 10.3% identified themselves as “dating,” 4.3% identified themselves as married, 3.4% as engaged, and 1.7% as separated/divorced. Materials Attachment orientation was measured using the Experiences in Close Relationships questionnaire (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). This comprises two 18item scales (anxiety and avoidance), using a 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly) response format. Brennan et al. (1998) report the test-retest reliability for the anxiety scale as .93 and the test-retest reliability for the avoidance subscale as .95. These two scales, respectively, indicate greater or lesser anxious or avoidant attachment characteristics. Secure attachment is represented by low scores on both of these scales. Avoidant attachment is represented by high scores on avoidant and low scores on anxious attachment, and anxious attachment is represented by high scores on the anxiety dimension with low scores on the avoidance dimension. 26 27 Love-styles were measured using the Love Attitudes Scale: Short Form (Hendrick, Hendrick & Dicke, 1998). Each of the items was scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). There were 24 items, with 4 items representing each lovestyle (Agape, Eros, Mania, Pragma, Storge and Ludus). Hendrick et al. (1998) report the test-retest reliability as ranging from .75 for Mania to .88 for Agape, and .63 for Pragma to .76 for Storge. Orientation towards belief in destiny and belief in growth was measured using the Implicit Theories of Relationships Scale (Knee, Patrick & Lonsbary, 2003). Each of the items were scored from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). There were 22 items on the scale (11 representing belief in destiny and 11 representing belief in growth). Separate, independent scores for destiny and growth are computed from these items. Knee et al. report internal reliabilities of .82 for destiny and .74 for growth. Personality traits were measured using the Big Five Personality Inventory (Benet-Martinez & John, 1998). This 44-item inventory measures Extraversion, Agreeableness, Openness, Conscientiousness and Neuroticism each on a 5-point scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). Benet-Martinez et al. (1998) report internal reliabilities of .88 for Extraversion, .79 for Agreeableness, .82 for Conscientiousness, .84 for Neuroticism, and .81 for Openness. Locus of Control was measured using the Internality, Powerful Others, and Chance Scales (Levenson, 1981). Internality, Powerful Others, and Chance (IPC) were each measured using eight items on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The 24-item IPC Scale reports internal reliabilities of .64 for Internality, 28 .77 for Powerful Others, and .78 for Chance based on a student sample of 152 (Levenson, 1981). A short measure of perceived control when “falling in love” was constructed for use in this study (Beliefs About Relationships; Appendix A). Five items were written to reflect aspects of “falling in love” which would allow planning and intentional decisions (3 items) versus being swept up in an uncontrollable, emotional process (2 items). Each of these statements were rated on a scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). Prior to their use in this study, these items were informally pilot tested and revised to promote consistency. Results from the 117 participants of this study indicated a poor internal consistency reliability for the five-item scale of α = .36. Dropping items yielded only a modest improvement in reliability (.49 with two items). Thus, the best version of this scale summed only two items: “There is one person I am destined to fall in love with.” and “Being in a romantic relationship is more a matter of personal choice, than of fate (reverse-scored).” Higher scores on this scale would indicate less perceived control when falling in love. This new measure of perceived lack of control when falling in love had a potential range of 2 to 10, and responses for it ranged from 2 to 10 (M = 5.62, SD = 1.80). Procedure All participants read and signed a consent form upon their arrival to the research room. The six surveys were given to the participants after explaining to them the terms of the voluntary policy and that they had the right to discontinue participation. The surveys were administered in a randomized order. The packets were collected at the end 29 of the sessions in a randomized order to ensure anonymity. Each participant received sufficient amount of time to complete the surveys (approximately 30 minutes or longer). The setting of the testing area was in a classroom at California State University, Chico. A debriefing form was handed out to each participant after the completion of the surveys. CHAPTER IV RESULTS Descriptive Statistics Descriptive statistics for each of the research variables are shown in Table 1. Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for each Research Variable Research Variable Eros Storge Mania Ludus Pragma Agape Destiny Belief Growth Belief Avoidance Anxiety Internality Powerful Others Chance Extraversion Agreeableness Conscientiousness Neuroticism Openness Beliefs About Relationships Valid N (listwise) N 117 117 117 115 116 115 117 116 116 115 117 117 117 116 116 116 116 116 117 Min. 6.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 6.00 21.00 34.00 18.00 18.00 29.00 8.00 8.00 10.00 18.00 21.00 12.00 19.00 2.00 111 30 Max. 20.00 20.00 19.00 20.00 20.00 20.00 67.00 77.00 121.00 111.00 50.00 41.00 41.00 40.00 45.00 45.00 38.00 49.00 10.00 Mean 15.91 13.10 11.28 8.32 11.21 13.46 43.99 58.46 46.94 66.34 40.56 24.11 24.69 28.25 36.41 34.97 24.43 36.01 5.62 Std. Deviation 2.98 3.79 3.54 3.19 3.46 2.90 9.94 8.48 19.67 19.76 4.81 6.82 6.57 6.30 5.16 5.02 5.75 6.15 1.80 31 Bivariate correlations Bivariate correlations were used to analyze the relationships among 11 measures: two attachment styles (anxiety, avoidance), belief in destiny, belief in growth, six love-styles (Agape, Mania, Ludus, Storge, Pragma, Eros), and the (2-item) Beliefs About Relationships scale. Using a two-tailed test, a strict .05 alpha level, and a listwiseN for all correlations of 112, 16 out of the total of 55 correlations were significant. Attachment avoidance was correlated with Eros (r = -.44, p < .001) and Ludus (r = .32, p = .001), while attachment anxiety was correlated with Mania (r = .63, p < .001) and Agape (r = .19, p = .046). Next, Destiny belief was correlated with Mania (r = .26, p = .006), Pragma (r = .25, p = .007), and Lack of Control (r =.24, p = .01). Growth belief was not significantly correlated with any of the variables. Of the love-styles, Eros was correlated with Storge (r = .42, p <.001), Ludus (r = -.35, p <.001), Agape (r = .27, p =.003), and Lack of Control (r = .24, p = .01). Storge was correlated with Pragma (r = .22, p =.022) and Agape (r = .27, p = .004). Mania was correlated with Ludus (r = .22, p = .02) and Agape (r = .25, p = .008). Finally, Pragma was correlated with Lack of Control (r = .19, p = .044). Simultaneous multiple regression analyses Although the Beliefs About Relationships scale did correlate with belief in destiny and three love styles, its poor reliability cast serious doubt on its usefulness as a primary dependent variable for further analyses, as well as what, conceptually, it measures. For those reasons, it was dropped from further analyses and belief in destiny and belief in growth were used instead as the focal dependent variables. 32 Next, the combined effects of the predictors were examined using multiple regression. Since there was no a priori reason to test predictors in a specific order, simultaneous regression was chosen as the appropriate technique. However, with an N of 111, the N/K ratio would be inadequate if all 16 predictors were included. Stepwise regression was rejected as a solution because of its atheoretical nature and emphasis on prediction rather than variable testing. So, the predictors were divided into two sets of eight predictors each: relationship-relevant predictors (six love styles and two attachment dimensions) and general personality variables (five personality traits and three locus of control dimensions). Separate simultaneous regressions were performed for each criterion variable (destiny belief, growth belief), using each set of predictors (relationship-relevant, general personality), resulting in four regression analyses. A simultaneous multiple regression analysis was used to predict belief in destiny from eight predictors: the six love-styles, attachment-anxiety and attachmentavoidance. A significant multiple correlation was found, R = .432, F (8, 111) = 2.958, p = .005, accounting for 18.7 percent of the variance. Four predictors were significant in the model: Eros (β = .238, p < .05), Mania (β = .268, p < .05), Pragma (β = .205, p < .05) and Avoidance (β = .214, p < .05). An identical regression analysis using belief in growth as the criterion failed to obtain significance, both in the model R and individual predictors. A second simultaneous multiple regression predicted belief in destiny from eight different predictors: Internality, Chance, Powerful Others, Extraversion, Openness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Agreeableness. This model was significant, R = .442, F (8, 115) = 3.255, p < .005, accounting for 19.5 percent of the variance. Internality 33 (β = .202, p < .05), Chance (β = .279, p < .05), and Conscientiousness (β = -.230, p < .05) were significant predictors. A simultaneous regression model predicting growth belief from these eight variables was not significant (R =.362; p = .053). Although two predictors (Internality and Openness) were significant, interpreting these is problematic, involving a suppression effect (Internality) and a counterintuitive relationship (Openness). CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Beliefs About Relationships Measure and Correlations (H1 and H2) Due to the fact that the measure created for perceived control when falling in love (Beliefs About Relationships scale) was not statistically reliable, orientation towards growth or destiny became the criterion variables. The measure created only had five items, one of which had been replaced after the pilot study was done. The measure would have better reliability if it had more items that measured more accurately a belief in control or not having control when falling in love. The scale included items that may have measured a different construct, such as item 5, “Romantic love is something that takes time and patience to grow with each individual.” The idea of perceived control when falling in love may actually be too complex or layered of a concept to be examined with the five items that I used. Furthermore, I underestimated the difficulty in constructing an entirely new measure, a task that may have been beyond the scope of this study and would have taken possibly multiple studies to refine items and finally complete. Although belief in destiny and belief in growth are not synonymous with belief in control, they were the closest variables I had to substitute as criterion variables. Because the Beliefs About Relationships Scale was unreliable, H1 and H2 were not testable. 34 35 In my bivariate correlation analysis, the strongest correlation was between Mania and Anxiety (r = .63). This reflects the similarities between the anxious clinging intense feelings of Mania and the insecurity of the anxious attachment variable. The association between Mania and anxious attachment may reflect an emotional lack of control but the Beliefs About Relationships scale failed to adequately measure this. There was also a negative correlation between Eros and Ludus (r = -.35), which is expected because in previous research Eros has been linked to positive relationship outcomes while Ludus has been linked to negative outcomes (Hendrick & Hendrick, 2006). Attachment Orientation and Destiny Belief I found a significant correlation between avoidance-attachment and destiny belief, but not a significant correlation between anxious-attachment and destiny belief (H2), contrary to my expectations. This is interesting because in the past, growth belief has been found to be “modestly correlated” to attachment security, but no other associations with attachment orientation were found (Knee et al., 2003). This could be because an avoidant person is more likely to evaluate a relationship instantly rather than taking the time to carefully assess a relationship. It is consistent with the finding that destiny belief is characterized by neglecting a relationship. An avoidant person also would be more likely to see the first moments of conflict as a sign that the relationship was not worth the time and effort (according to Knee et al., 2003, people who score higher on the destiny-orientation scale are more evaluative towards relationships). This 36 reflects the fact that people with an insecure-avoidant attachment orientation come to view new relationships with already existing negative expectancies. An anxiously attached person, while still insecure, may not be judgmental of relationships in the same way an avoidant person is (thus reflecting the higher belief in destiny scores that avoidant people had). This is consistent with the findings of Orina et al. (2011), who found that “weak-link” partners in a relationship have greater power over the outcome of a relationship because they are less committed and emotionally invested in the other person. The “self-protective” motives of avoidantly attached individuals are a way of avoiding negative outcomes on interpersonal investments. Personality and Locus of Control Findings (H3, H4 and H5) As expected, the Chance component of locus of control was significantly related to belief in destiny (H3). However, the Internality component of locus of control was significantly related to belief in destiny rather than belief in growth, therefore H4 was not supported. This is confusing at first glance since the two variables seem to contradict one another. This finding may not be as contradictory as it seems, however, due to the fact that it is possible for a person to have both a high score on orientation towards growth and also orientation towards destiny (they are not mutually exclusive). It could also be that the internality component of Levenson’s locus of control measure (1981) does not tap adequately into the relationship constructs that I examined. A person might score on the internal locus of control spectrum while still holding extrinsic beliefs about love. Although H5 was not supported as conscientiousness did not predict belief in 37 growth, conscientiousness was found to negatively predict belief in destiny. The reason for this could be that conscientiousness is related to having patience with other people and working through problems, rather than immediately drawing conclusions as an individual scoring low in conscientiousness might do. Love Styles, Attachment Avoidance and Belief in Destiny (H6) Avoidant-attachment was found to be positively correlated with Ludus (r = .32), therefore H6 was supported. Ludus is associated with non-committed behaviors in love relationships and with a disconnection of sex with intimacy that is very similar to avoidant-attachment. Avoidance was also correlated negatively to Eros (r = -.44), the romantic passionate love-style. This is expected considering avoidant individuals’ lack of intimacy and passion in relationships (as well as lack of marital satisfaction). Other correlations revealed that Eros, Mania, and Pragma were significantly positively correlated with belief in destiny. This is compatible with Hendrick and Hendrick’s conception of Eros (2006) as being emotionally intense and having a “sense of inevitability.” Mania relates to belief in destiny because of its association with having intense feelings about a partner that often alternate between agony and ecstasy. People who score high on Mania do not feel as if they are in control of their feelings. Because both Eros and Mania are associated with intense emotions, they are more related to the experience of “falling in love” than some of the less emotional styles (such as Storge or Pragma). I speculate that Pragma is related to belief in destiny because individuals scoring high in Pragma would look for certain attributes in their partners, and if these 38 attributes were missing, their relationships would be considered “not meant to be.” In contrast, belief in growth would be related to working through differences rather than looking at missing attributes or finding fault with one’s partner. Limitations and Future Research It is interesting that there were no significant findings in the simultaneous regression analysis of belief in growth with the sixteen predictor variables. It could be that in general, young college aged individuals are more oriented towards a belief in destiny, partially due to their exposure to popular culture and myths. One potential confound of this study is that because my sample was so young, it does not adequately represent people who have had a lot of relationship experience. It is possible that people who are older have more experience working through problems in relationships, and might possibly be more naturally oriented towards growth. In other words, they may have had more salient relationship experiences than younger people (Knee et al., 2003). Also, I had few married individuals in my sample, and college students may be going through relationships and experimenting romantically at a higher rate than older individuals. Those who had recently ended a relationship may have scored higher than they usually would on anxious attachment and possibly other variables (such as the Ludic love style). Furthermore, these data represent primarily white females and cannot be generalized to both males and females. The fact that my sample was primarily white could also predict cultural differences in the results. It could be beneficial to run a longitudinal study with the same participants to see how they change from one relationship to another over time. The majority of the sample was also heterosexual, and 39 it would be interesting to find how a homosexual population might differ (or be similar) to heterosexuals in terms of orientation towards belief in destiny and how that is related to attachment style. It may have been advantageous to include the length of relationship as a variable. Susan Sprecher (1999) found that couples assessed over a 4 year period reported an increase in positive feelings towards their partner over time. She also found that participants who experienced a breakup during her study reported decreases in positive feelings (satisfaction and commitment) prior to the breakup. For this reason, the state of the relationship (length over time) may have given insight as to whether there was growth or change within the relationship that affected responses. It would be erroneous to think of participants’ scores as stable personality traits without considering situational factors such as current relationship development and change. According to Sternberg (1986), individuals have a high degree of control over the decision/commitment aspect of love, but not over the motivational or arousal aspect. It may be that individuals involved in shorter-term relationships would score higher on passionate variables such as Eros or Mania (as reflected in the correlation between Eros and lack of control), while those invested in a long-term relationship or marriage would score higher in Storge. The passionate component of love has been found to decline over time (Sternberg, 1986). It is also difficult to say whether the physiological arousal state of passion is considered “falling in love” by most people or whether the longer-term companionate commitment is considered love. It may be that this passionate aspect of love is more difficult to control due to the chemical/hormonal changes in the body (for 40 example, the release of dopamine) during these moments (Fisher, 2004), but that after some time passes in a relationship, intimacy and commitment become more important. For this reason, it would also have been advantageous to include some free-response items that would tap into participants’ thoughts in an unforced manner. It would be interesting to see how participants’ romantic partners scored on traits, to examine how much of a match between love-styles and attachment orientations there was. A future study could take into account both partners’ perceptions of falling in love, and both partners’ attachment styles. Unfortunately, the secure attachment dimension was not tested in my statistical analyses. A more complete study would take into consideration the level of secure attachment in participants rather than only addressing the level of anxious and avoidant attachment in participants. The secure dimension was difficult to include in my analyses because of the way the scale (Experiences in Close Relationships) in my study scored each dimension. However, only one of my hypotheses addressed secure attachment (H1), while the other hypotheses addressed the insecure orientations, personality or love styles. A possible confound of this study is that there may be a certain type of individual wishing to take part in studies on love. It could be that people who score higher in romantic qualities and aspirations may be more likely to participate. This would mean that my sample may lack a number of people who would score in the avoidant spectrum. Also, it could be that one gender is more drawn to this type of research than another (although the prevalence of females in my sample is more likely a matter of 41 student population demographics). One of the strengths of this study is the randomization of the order of surveys given, eliminating possible order-effects. One of the difficulties in doing research on love is defining love. For instance, do Storge and Agape truly represent romantic love? Hazan and Shaver (1988) argue that they do not, as Storge seems to represent a friendship based approach that has little to do with passion, and Agape is more spiritual in origin. However, Storge and Agape may coincide with Sternberg’s idea of “consummate love,” the mixture of passion, intimacy and commitment (Sternberg, 1986). Love without passion seems to exist in some relationships of those who have been together for many years into old age, and there does seem to be an important friendship aspect to relationships that last. In Aron and Westbay’s study of what people viewed as essential to love (1996), people showed “reliable individual differences,” consistent with the way that individuals endorse different love-styles. In their analysis of the prototype of love, they found that at the core of most participants’ ratings of essential aspects of love were selfdisclosure and “interconnectedness of selves.” These are both features of the concept of intimacy. Future research should determine how important the aspect of sexuality is in regards to intimacy, as some researchers of love have emphasized its importance more than others who emphasize the commitment aspects (Aron & Westbay, 1996). Another concern regarding the love-styles approach is that perhaps Pragma and Ludus are not truly aspects of love because they are not consistent with what lay-people identify as parts of love (they do not have associations with intimacy). They may in fact be indicators of dysfunctional relationship attitudes rather than love-styles. Furthermore, 42 cross-cultural studies of falling in love are important to see the differences in what is considered loving behaviors and attitudes in different contexts. Conclusions Although a connection was found between avoidant-attachment and belief in destiny, no causal relationship has been determined. It is important for future research to consider ways in which to take preventative measures against individuals becoming avoidantly-attached or having the negative “belief in destiny” orientation which may be too rigid a way of approaching relationships. In addition, continued research into what causes an individual to shift from an insecure attachment style to a secure one is invaluable in promoting mental health. For instance, there is evidence that romantic partners who are able to regulate their emotions well can serve as a protective buffer for individuals with insecure attachment histories whom they are involved with (Simpson, Collins & Salvatore, 2011). This may have to do with existing mental representations or internal working models being revised due to new, more positive relationship experiences. Continued research into the determinants of healthy relationships is invaluable, considering the impact loving relationships give to an individual’s sense of well-being. There are documented relationships between insecure attachments (both the anxious and avoidant attachment styles) and the experience of loneliness (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Of note, avoidant individuals are even more likely than anxious individuals to believe that they will be persistently lonely. Although anxiously attached people tend to decline in anxiety over time, avoidantly attached individuals have 43 loneliness scores that remain steady consistently across time (Mickelson, Kessler, & Shaver, 1997). This is likely a reflection of an avoidant person’s exaggerated need for interpersonal space and independence in relationships. By having attachment needs met, one greatly reduces the potential for the experience of loneliness. Furthermore, those with secure orientations are more likely to see challenges in a positive light and resolve conflicts more easily. Falling in love may give people the chance to act out parts of themselves that they have suppressed in other social situations and may be a positive period of selfdiscovery (Aron, Aron & Paris, 1995). Having an intimate partner may also lead to increases in self-esteem (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1988). Finally, it would be interesting to examine the mechanisms that lead securely attached individuals to be more resilient against negative relationship outcomes, and to find ways to promote shifts towards secure orientations. REFERENCES REFERENCES Ainsworth, M. D. S, Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Aron, A. & Aron, E. (1986). Love and the Expansion of Self. Washington, New York, London: Hemisphere Publishing Corporation. Aron, A., Aron, E., & Paris, M. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective studies of selfconcept change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 1102-1112. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1102 Aron, A., & Westbay, L. (1996). Dimensions of the prototype of love. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 535-551. doi: 10.1037/00223514.70.3.535 Asendorpf, J.B. & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on social relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1531-1544. doi: 10.1037/00223514.74.6.1531 Baumeister, R.F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497-529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497 Benet-Martinez, V. & John, O. (1998). Los Cinco Grandes across cultures and ethnic groups: Multitrait multimethod analyses of the Big Five in Spanish and English. 45 46 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 729-750. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.729 Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Brennan, K.A., Clark, C.L, & Shaver, P.R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J.A. Simpson & W.S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment Theory and Close Relationships (pp 46-76). New York: Guilford Press. Burger, J.M., & Cooper, H.M. (1979). The desirability of control. Motivation and Emotion, 3(4), 381-393. doi: 0146-7239/79/1200-0381$03.00/0 Canevello, A., & Crocker, J. (2011). Changing relationship growth belief: Intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of compassionate goals. Personal Relationships, 18, 370–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2010.01296.x Collins, N.L, Guichard, A.C., Ford, M.B., & Feeney, B.C. (2004). Working models of attachment: New developments and emerging themes. In J.A. Simpson & W.S. Rholes (Eds.) Adult Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Implications (pp 196-239). New York: Guilford Press. Collins, N.L., & Read, S.J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644663. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.4.644 Campbell, W., Finkel, E., & Foster, C. (2002). Does self-love lead to love for others? A story of narcissistic game playing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 340-354. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.340 47 Contreras, R., Hendrick, S.S., & Hendrick, C. (1996). Perspectives on marital love and satisfaction in Mexican American and Anglo-American couples. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74, 408-415. doi: 10.1002/j.15566676.1996.tb01887.x Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO FiveFactor Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Eidelson, R.J., & Epstein, N. (1982). Cognition and relationship maladjustment: Development of a measure of dysfunctional relationship beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 715-720. doi: 10.1037/0022006X.50.5.715 Engel, G., Olson, K., & Patrick, C. (2002). The personality of love: Fundamental motives and traits related to components of love. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 839-853. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00090-3 Feeney, J. & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 281-291. doi: 10.1037/0022- 3514.58.2.281 Fehr, B. & Broughton, R. (2001). Gender and personality differences in conceptions of love: an interpersonal theory analysis. Personal Relationships, 8, 115-136. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00031.x 48 Finkel, E.J, Scissors, L.E. & Burnette, J.L. (2007). Vengefully ever after: Destiny beliefs, state attachment anxiety, and forgiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 871-886. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.871 Fisher, H. (2004). Why We Love: The Nature and Chemistry of Romantic Love. New York, New York: Henry Holt and Company. Fraley, R. & Brumbaugh, C. (2004). A dynamical systems approach to conceptualizing and studying stability and change in attachment security. In J.A Simpson & W.S. Rholes (Eds.) Adult Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Implications (pp 86-132). Fromm, E. (1956). The Art of Loving. Harper Collins Publishers, New York, NY. Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48(1), 26-34. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26 Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the Self and Other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 430-445. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.430 Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511-552. doi: 10.1037/00223514.52.3.511 Heaven, P.C., Da Silva, T., Carey, C., & Holen, J. (2003). Loving styles: Relationships with personality and attachment styles. European Journal of Personality, 18, 103113. doi: 10.1002/per.498 49 Hendrick, C. (2006). Styles of Romantic Love. In R.J. Sternberg and K. Weis (Eds.) The New Psychology of Love (pp. 149-170). New Haven: CT. Yale University Press. Hendrick C. & Hendrick S. (1986). A theory and method of love. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 392-402. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.392 Hendrick, C., Hendrick, S.S., & Dicke, A. (1998). The Love Attitudes Scale: Short form. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 137-142. doi: 10.1177/0265407598152001 Hendrick, S. S., Hendrick, C., & Adler, N. L. (1988). Romantic relationships: Love, satisfaction, and staying together. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 54(6), 980-988. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.980 Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., Gable, S. L., & Keltner, D. (2010). Moving toward more perfect unions: Daily and long-term consequences of approach and avoidance goals in romantic relationships. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 99(6), 948-963. doi:10.1037/a0020271 Jung, C.G. (1953). Two Essays on Analytical Psychology. Bollingen Foundation: New York, NY. Knee, R.C. (1998). Implicit theories of relationships: Assessment and prediction of romantic relationship initiation, coping, and longevity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 360-370. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.360 Knee, R.C., & Canevello, A. (2006). Implicit theories of relationships and coping in romantic relationships. In K.D. Vohs & E.J. Finkel (Eds.), Self and Relationships (pp 160-176). New York: Guilford Press. 50 Knee, R., Patrick, H., & Lonsbary C. (2003). Implicit theories of relationships: Orientations toward evaluation and cultivation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(1), 41- 55. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0701_3 Lee, J.A. (1976). The Colors of Love. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and chance. In H.M. Lefcourt (Ed.) Research with the Locus of Control Construct (Vol. 1, pp. 15-63). New York: Academic Press. Mickelson, K., Kessler, R., & Shaver, P. (1997). Adult attachment in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 1092-1106. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1092 Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. (2007). Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics and Change. New York: The Guilford Press. Nemechek, S., & Olson, K.R. (1999). Five-factor personality similarity and marital adjustment. Social Behavior and Personality, 27, 309-318. doi: 10.2224/sbp.1999.27.3.309 Noftle, E.E. & Shaver, P.R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the Big Five personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict relationship quality. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 179-208. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.11.003 Orina, M., Collins, W., Simpson, J., Salvatore, J., Haydon, K., & Kim, J. (2011). Developmental and dyadic perspectives on commitment in adult romantic relationships. Psychological Science, 22(7), 908-915. doi: 10.1177/0956797611410573 51 Ravitz, P., Maunder R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., & Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment measures: A 25-year review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 69, 419-432. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.08.006 Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General & Applied, 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976 Scollon, C. & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1152-1165. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1152 Shaver, P.R. & Hazan, C.H. (1988). A biased overview of the study of love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 473-501. doi: 10.1177/0265407588054005 Simpson, J., Collins, A., & Salvatore, J. (2011). The impact of early interpersonal experience on adult romantic relationship functioning: Recent findings from the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(6), 355-359. doi: 10.1177/0963721411418468 Sprecher, S. (1999). “I love you more today than yesterday:” Romantic partners’ perceptions of changes in love and related affect over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(1), 46-53. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.46 Sternberg, R.J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93, 119-135. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.119 Sternberg, R.J. (1998). Cupid’s Arrow: The Course of Love through Time. London: Cambridge University Press. 52 White, J.K., Hendrick, S.S., & Hendrick, C. (2004). Big five personality variables and relationship constructs. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1519-1530. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.019 Wright, D., Simmons, L., & Campbell, K. (2007). Does a marriage ideal exist? Using Qsort methodology to compare young adults’ and professional educators’ views on healthy marriages. Contemporary Family Therapy, 29, 223-236. doi: 10.1007/s10591-007-9044-0 APPENDIX A Beliefs about Relationships Please indicate the degree to which you agree with the following statements. While answering the questions, please think of the way that you approach relationships (regardless of whether you are currently in a relationship or single). Disagree strongly Disagree somewhat 1 Neither agree nor disagree 2 Agree somewhat 3 4 Agree strongly 5 1. _____ There is “one person” who I am/was destined to fall in love with. 2. _____ Successful romantic relationships begin with a careful consideration of the person’s attributes and suitability as a romantic partner. 3. _____ Being in a romantic relationship is more a matter of personal choice, than of fate. 4. _____ Romantic love is an intense experience that I cannot control. 5. _____ Romantic love is something that takes time and patience to grow with each individual. Demographics Age: ________ Your Gender (please circle): Male/Female How would you identify your ethnicity? __________________________________________ College Grade Level: Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Graduate Current Semester Units: ______ Relationship Status: (married, single, in a relationship, engaged, dating, other – please specify) _____________________________________________________________________________ Describe your sexual orientation: Heterosexual Other 54 Bisexual Gay/Lesbian
© Copyright 2024