Social Conditionsas FundamentalCauses of Disease* BRUCE G. LINK Institute andNewYorkStatePsychiatric ColumbiaUniversity JO PHELAN Los Angeles University ofCalifornia, ofHealthandSocialBehavior1995,(ExtraIssue):80-94 Journal Over the last several decades, epidemiologicalstudies have been enormously riskfactorsfor major diseases. However,most of this successfulin identifying proximalcauses of on riskfactorsthatare relatively researchhasfocusedattention disease such as diet, cholesterollevel, exercise and the like. We questionthe riskfactorsand argue thatgreaterattention emphasison such individually-based mustbe paid to basic social conditionsif healthreformis to have its maximum effectin thetimeahead. Thereare tworeasonsfor thisclaim. Firstwe argue that riskfactors mustbe contextualized,by examiningwhat puts individually-based and improvethe interventions people at riskof risks,if we are to crafteffective nation'shealth.Second, we argue thatsocial factorssuchas socioeconomicstatus and social supportare likely'fundamentalcauses" of disease that,because they resources,affectmultipledisease outcomesthrough embodyaccess to important maintainan associationwithdisease even multiplemechanisms,and consequently when interveningmechanisms change. Withoutcareful attentionto these intervention strategies possibilities,we runtheriskof imposingindividually-based and of missingopportunitiesto adopt broad-based societal thatare ineffective thatcouldproduce substantialhealthbenefits interventions for our citizens. Epidemiologyhas been enormouslysuccessfulin heighteningpublic awareness of risk and prominently publicizedin the mass factorsfordisease. Researchfindingsare frequently healthnewsletters.Moreover,thereis university-based media and in rapidlyproliferating to evidencethatthe messagehas been receivedand thatmanypeople have at least attempted quit smoking,includemoreexercisein theirdailyroutine,and implementa healthierdiet. Withfewexceptions,however,thenew findingsgeneratedwithinthefieldof epidemiology have focusedon riskfactorsthatare relativelyproximate"causes" of disease, such as diet, fields,lack of exercise,and so on. Social factors, electromagnetic cholesterol,hypertension, ' This focuson whichtendto be moredistalcauses of disease, have receivedfarless attention. or bias, moreproximatelinksin thecausal chainmaybe viewedby many,notas a limitation causal to understanding of sciencefromidentifying correlations butas therightful progression relationships(e.g., Potter 1992). In fact, some in the so-called "modem" school of epidemiology(e.g., Rothman1986) have explicitlyargued that social conditionssuch as socioeconomicstatusare mere proxiesfortruecauses lyingcloser to disease in the causal chain. This focus on proximaterisk factors,potentiallycontrollableat the individuallevel, resonateswiththevalue and beliefsystemsof Westernculturethatemphasizeboththeability of the individualto controlhis or her personalfateand the importanceof doing so (Becker * We thank This comments. andSarahRosenfield forhelpful SharonSchwartz, BernicePescosolido, toBruce communications MH46101andMH13043.Address inpartbyNIMHgrants workwassupported 31D, New York,NY of MentalDisorders,100 HavenAvenue,Apartment G. Link,Epidemiology 10032. 80 This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 81 1993). This affinity betweenculturalvalues and the focus of contemporary epidemiology undoubtedlycontributesto the level of public interestin epidemiologicalfindings,and probablyinfluencesfundingpriorities as well. Thus modemepidemiologyand culturalvalues riskfactorsand away fromsocial conspireto focusattention on proximate,individually-based conditionsas causes of disease. This is notto say thattheroleof social factorsin disease causationhas been neglectedin all quarters. Medical sociologists and social epidemiologistshave kept alive classical epidemiology's(e.g., Susser,Watson,and Hopper 1985) concernwithsocial conditionsand have made major stridestowarddocumentingand understanding the connectionsbetween social factorsand disease. However,we believe thereare conceptualpitfallsthatsometimes lead medical sociologistsand social epidemiologiststhemselvesto unwittingly reinforcethe riskfactors.One ofthesepitfallsis that,in theprocess emphasison proximate, individual-level of elucidatingthemechanismsconnecting social conditionsto healthand illness-an important in and come to neglecttheimportance and desirableactivity-we may,overtime,lose interest of the social conditionwhose effecton healthwe originallysoughtto explain. Also, our tendencyto focus on the connectionof social conditionsto single diseases via single mechanismsat singlepointsin timeneglectsthe multifaceted and dynamicprocessesthrough which social factorsmay affecthealth and, consequently,may result in an incomplete and an underestimation of theinfluenceof social factorson health. understanding Our purposeshereare to highlight the accomplishments of medical sociologistsand social of social conditionsas causes of disease, to epidemiologistsin advancingour understanding underscorethe criticalimportanceof continuedwork in this direction,and to offertwo thatwe hope will facilitate and enhancethisresearch.First,we discuss conceptualframeworks of "contextualizing" theimportance riskfactors--that to understand how people is, attempting come to be exposed to individually-based riskfactorssuch as poor diet, cholesterol,lack of exercise,or highblood pressure-so thatwe can designmoreeffectiveinterventions. Second, we introducethenotionthatsome social conditionsmaybe "fundamental causes" of disease. A fundamentalcause involves access to resources,resourcesthat help individualsavoid diseases and theirnegativeconsequencesthrougha varietyof mechanisms.Thus, even if one effectivelymodifiesinterveningmechanismsor eradicatessome diseases, an association betweena fundamental cause and disease will reemerge.As such,fundamental causes can defy to eliminatetheireffectswhenattemptsto do so focussolelyon the mechanismsthat efforts happen to link themto disease in a particularsituation.We conclude by discussingthe of theseideas forresearchand social policy. implications EVIDENCE LINKING SOCIAL CONDITIONS TO DISEASE We begin witha briefreviewof the evidence concerningthe connectionbetweensocial conditionsand illness. For the purposesof thispaper,we definesocial conditionsas factors that involve a person's relationshipsto other people. These include everythingfrom withintimates to positionsoccupiedwithinthesocial and economicstructures of relationships society.Thus, in additionto factorslike race, socioeconomicstatus,and gender,we include stressful lifeeventsof a social nature(e.g., thedeathof a loved one, loss of a job, or crime as well as stress-process variablessuch as social support. victimization), Fortyyearsof medicalsociologyhave uncoverednumerousexamplesofthesocial patterning of disease. Most obvious is the ubiquitousand oftenstrongassociationbetweenhealthand socioeconomicstatus.Lower SES is associated withlower life expectancy,higheroverall ratesand higherratesof infantand perinatalmortality mortality (Buck 1981; Dutton 1986; Illsley and Mullen 1985; Adler et al. 1994; Pappas et al. 1993). Moreover,low SES is associated with each of the 14 major cause-of-deathcategories in the International Classificationof Diseases (Illsleyand Mullen 1985), as well as manyotherhealthoutcomes, includingmajor mentaldisorders(Dohrenwendet al. 1980; Kessler et al. 1994). Other of disease are plentiful.Males have highermortality ratesat examplesof thesocial patterning all ages (Walsh and Feldman 1981), as well as higherratesof coronaryheartdisease (Syme and Guralnik1987), chronicrespiratory diseases (Colley 1985) and ulcers(Gazzard and Lance This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 82 JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR in ratesof variousformsof cancer (Prout, 1982). There are pronouncedgenderdifferences Colton,and Smith1987) and mentaldisorder(Dohrenwendet al. 1980; Kessleret al. 1994). and infantmortality (Dutton 1986; AfricanAmericanshave higherratesof overallmortality Miller1987), renalfailure(Challahand Wing 1985), and stroke(Pedoe 1982a) thando Whites, but lowerratesof coronaryheartdisease (Pedoe 1982b); cancerratesalso differby race and ethnicity (Proutet al. 1987). Both physicaland mentaldisordersvarywithmaritalstatusand populationdensity(Kelsey 1993; Benenson 1987; Robins et al. 1984), and certainreligious have lowerrisksof sometypesof cancer groupssuchas Mormonsand SeventhDay Adventists (Saracci 1985). In addition, the tremendousgrowthand success of the stress paradigmhave added considerablyto the evidence for an association between social conditionsand disease (Dohrenwendand Dohrenwend1981; Pearlinet al. 1981; Turnerand Marino 1994; Turner, Wheaton,and Lloyd 1995). Stressfullifeeventshave been linkedto heartdisease, diabetes, (Miller 1987; cancers,stroke,fetaldeath,majordepression,and low birthweightin offspring Brownand Harris 1989; Shroutet al. 1989). Researchhas also extendedto the domainsof social support(Berkmanand Syme 1979; House, Landis, and Umberson1988; Thoits 1982; and Phillips 1990; Turnerand Marino 1994) and coping Turner1981; Turner,Grindstaff, (Pearlin and Schooler 1978), which have been shown to be associated with health and well-beingin theirown right. The evidencereviewedto thispointclearlyestablishesa strongand pervasiveassociation betweensocial conditionsand disease. But medical sociologistsand social epidemiologists of disease. have takenthe field considerablybeyonda descriptionof the social patterning advancesin establishinga causal role forsocial factorshave focusedon two major Important issues-the directionof causationbetweensocial conditionsand healthand the mechanisms thatexplainobservedassociations.In whatfollowswe presentprominent examplesof workon thesetwo issues. controversiessurroundsome of the Concerningthe issue of causal direction,important betweensocial conditionsand health.For example,does low SES cause poor relationships health,or does poor healthcause downwardmobility?Does social supportreducemorbidity and mortality, or does illness restrictsocial interactionand therebylead to social-support have used threegeneralstrategiesto addressthesequestions. deficits?Social epidemiologists strategieswhich involve locatingconditionsunder One approachuses quasi-experimental which alternativeexplanationsmake differentpredictionsabout observable facts. This designedto test social approachis exemplifiedby Dohrenwend's(1966) quasi-experiment selectionand social causationexplanationsfortheassociationbetweenSES and specificmental disorders.The two explanationsmake differentpredictionsabout rates of disorder in ethnicgroups,whensocioeconomicstatusis heldconstant.The advantagedand disadvantaged recent culminationof Dohrenwend's work on this problem, based on a large-scale epidemiologicalstudyin Israel(Dohrenwendet al. 1992), concludedthatsocial causationwas thansocial selectionin producingtheinverseassociationof SES to majordepression stronger in men.For schizophrenia, in women,and substanceabuse and antisocialpersonality however, theevidencewas moresupportiveof the social-selectionexplanation. medical sociologistsand social epidemiologistsidentifysocial risk In the second strategy, factorsthatcannotreasonablybe conceivedas havingbeen caused by an individual'sillness thisstrategy is a studyby Hamiltonand colleagues(1990) concerning condition.Exemplifying the effectsof plant closings on auto-workers'mentalhealth. The investigators compared workerswho werelaid offbecause of a plantclosing,workerswho anticipatedbeinglaid off, and workerswhose plantwas not closing,and foundthatthose laid offwere morelikelyto if theywere minoritiesand of consequences-particularly experiencenegativemental-health low SES. Since theillnessconditionof theworkerscannotbe thoughtof as havingcaused the betweenthegroupsstudiedare moreclearlyinterpretable as the plantclosing,thedifferences was also employedby Fenwickand Tausig effectsof social conditionson health.This strategy rateforan individual'soccupation (1994) in a studythatused theCensus-basedunemployment is higher,workers'job satisfaction, in a longitudinal designto show thatwhenunemployment decision-making latitude,and well-beingare lower.Again,since workers'healthcannotcause as demonstrating theaggregateunemployment rate,theresultsare morereadilyinterpretable This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 83 the influenceof social conditionson health-related outcomes.Finally,studiesof stressful have used thisstrategy "fateful"life eventsthatare unlikelyto circumstance by identifying havebeencausedby an individual'sbehavior(e.g., deathof spouse,plantclosing).Thuswhen Shroutet al. (1989) foundtheodds of developingmajordepressionto be morethanthreetimes a recentfateful as highamongpeopleexperiencing lifeevent,theassociationwas moreclearly interpretable as an effectof social conditions. The thirdstrategy adoptedby social epidemiologists to clarifycausal directioninvolvesthe use of longitudinal designs.Whensuchdesignscan clearlyplace theemergenceof an illnessor an illnessexacerbationbeforeor afterthe social conditionunderstudy,a greatdeal can be of cause betweenthetwo. Unfortunately, somelongitudinal learnedaboutthedirection studies do not providethe definitive do not allow clear inferencesabout time orderand therefore to them(see Linkand Shrout1992). Still, evidenceaboutcausalitythatis sometimesattributed social conditionsthatclearlypredatehealthoutcomesand somenotablestudieshave identified show thatthe social conditionspredictmorbidity and mortality even when competingrisk factorsare heldconstant.For example,Berkmanand Syme(1979) used baselinedataon social networks, collectedin 1965, to predictmortality duringthesubsequentnineyears.Theyfound indexas compared a neardoublingof riskformortality amongthoselow on a social-network formanycompeting risks(smoking, to thosehighon theindex.Althoughthisstudycontrolled obesity,physicalactivity, etc.), it did notincludemeasuresderivedfroma physicalexam. A subsequentstudyby House, Landis, and Umberson(1988) did includea baseline physical exam and controlledforblood pressure,cholesterollevels, and otherbiomedicalvariables. These investigators foundassociationsbetweensocial relationships and mortality thatwere similarto thosereportedby Berkmanand Syme. This line of workhas continuedto become moreand morerefined.For example,in a recentlongitudinal study,Berkmanand colleagues (1992) have shownthata measureof perceivedsupportcollectedbeforethe occurrenceof a heart attack predictssurvivalfollowingthe heart attack net of an impressivearray of biomedical and psychosocial control variables. Other social variables have also been effectively studiedwithlongitudinaldesigns. For example,Catalano and colleagues (1993) of alcoholabuse,andLin andEnsel (1989) relatedjob layoffsto theemergenceorreemergence circumstances and Ensel and Lin (1991) showedthatstressful predictedsubsequenthealthand mental-health outcomes. have notdeniedthepossibility Thus, whilemedicalsociologistsand social epidemiologists thatillnessaffectssocial conditions(Johnson1991), theyhave,at thesametime,demonstrated a substantial causal role forsocial conditionsas causes of illness. themechanisms Researchidentifying linkingsocial conditionsto diseasehas also donemuch of social patterns of disease. Consider,for to movesocial epidemiology beyondthedescription example, the job-stressmodel of Karasek and colleagues thatprovidesevidence for one have shown mechanismlinkingSES to coronaryheartdisease amongmen.These investigators that "job strain,"characterizedby a combinationof highjob demandsand low decision latitude,is morecommonin lower statusjobs and is associatedwithcoronaryheartdisease blood pressure (Karasek et al. 1988; Schnall et al. 1990) and elevatedlevels of ambulatory bothon and offthejob (Schnallet al. 1992). Anotherexampleis theworkof Mirowskyand thatmightaccountforsocial patterns of distress. Ross (1989), who elucidatethemechanisms They presentevidenceshowingthatalienationand perceivedcontrolover lifecircumstances underliemanysocial conditionsthatput people at riskforelevatedlevels of psychological distress.Consideras a finalexamplea studyby Rosenfield(1989) thatsoughtto understand in symptoms of depressionand anxiety.Rosenfield mechanismsproducinggenderdifferences shows thatwomenhave highersymptomsof depressionand anxiety.This workshows that womenhave highersymptomlevels thanmen whentheyare overloadedby workand family demandsor when theyexperiencelow power as a consequenceof being out of the labor low power and role overload is a market.Moreover,the commonmechanismunderlying decreasedsense of personalcontrol,whichis in turnrelatedto symptomsof anxietyand depression. Link and Dohrenwend(1989) explicitlyadvocatethe approachof elucidatingmechanisms therelativemeritofcompeting forsocial patterns becauseof itsvalue in clarifying explanations of disease. The rationaleis thatalternativeexplanationsfor these patterns,such as social This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 84 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR intervening mechanisms.Thus, causationand social selection,frequently implydifferent mechanismsaccountforthe associationcan help answer evidenceabout whichintervening causal directionand othercompetingexplanations.Moreover,if causal questionsconcerning linksbetweendistalfactors(e.g., SES) and moreproximalfactors(e.g., occupationalstress, diet)can be drawn,as Karaseket al. (1988), Mirowskyand Ross (1989), Rosenfield(1989), and others(Lennon 1987; Pearlin et al. 1981; Link, Lennon, and Dohrenwend1993; clear thatsocial Umberson,Wortman,and Kessler 1992) have done, it becomesincreasingly indirect effectson diseaseoutcomes,ratherthanmereproxiesas are causes exerting conditions Rothman(1986) and othersmightclaim. and undesirable But are thereunintended consequencesof an approachthatfocuseson interof suchan approach,it We believethereare. Despitetheobviousbenefits veningmechanisms? to thefocuson factorsthatare one mayinadvertently contribute is possiblethatin itsenactment, becomesthenew and exciting mechanism closerto diseasein thecausal chain.The intervening becometheold, passe "starting point." "nextstep,"whilethesocial conditions of The evolutionofthestressparadigmis a good exampleofsuchan inadvertent downgrading selection theissue whichprovidedthe initialimpetusforresearch.The social causation/social theassociation betweensocioeconomic statusandmentaldisorder spawned controversy concerning of the adversity in stressful lifeeventsas a directoperationalization thatmightbe an interest in lowerSES contexts(Dohrenwendand Dohrenwend1969, 1981). Whena conexperienced eventsandillnesswas identified sistent butmodestassociationbetweenstressful (see Rabkinand elaboratedthe model to considersocial supportand copingas Struening1976), investigators areinvestedin understanding themechanisms Now researchers modifiers. linkingthese potential factors withdisease.Also, researchon thebiologicalconsequencesofstress(e.g., immunestatus andelevatedcatecholamines) is seenas an excitingnew development (e.g., Cohen,Tyrrell,and has followedthe mostrecentstepin theprogression toward Smith1991). In general,interest toa pointwheresomeexpress diseaseoutcomes,whileconcernwiththeearlierfocihas dissipated in factorssuchas thecausation/selection issue and therole of stressful lifeeventsin disinterest and Klusman[1987] for causingillness(butsee Pearlin1989; Dohrenwend1990; Angermeyer and Klusman(1987) documented a sharpdeclinein the dissenting views). IndeedAngermeyer focusedon social class and mentaldisorderin theperiodfrom1966 to numberof publications disorders increasedrapidlyduringthe 1985,whilethenumberofarticleson stressandpsychiatric in mechanisms increasesat theexpenseof morefundasameperiod.To theextentthatinterest contribute to the emphasison mentalsocial conditions,medicalsociologistsmay unwittingly andplayintothehandsofthosewhoarguethatsocialfactors haveonlya modest individual factors rolein diseasecausation. ofsocialconditions as causesofdisease To thispoint,we havedescribedtwocharacterizations or inadvertently One of theseis theoutright thateitheradvertently downplaytheirimportance. declarationthatsocial factorsare onlyproxiesfortruecauses. This positionis demonstrably of medicalsociologyand social epidemiology overthepast unwarranted giventheachievements whichmaybe partially fewdecades.The othercharacterization, constructed by medicalsociolis theviewthatsocialfactors serveas starting points ogistsandsocialepidemiologists themselves, to moreproximalriskfactors.We takesharpissue is to pointthedirection whosemainfunction In the nexttwo sections,we developtwo conceptsthat withbothof thesecharacterizations. thecriticalimportance of socialfactorsin diseasecausation,provideconceptualframeillustrate thatmayensueiftheroleofsocial researchinthisarea,andpointtotheproblems worksforfuture andpolicymakers. These aretheideas of "contextualizing is neglectedbyresearchers conditions causes." riskfactors"and "fundamental CONTEXTUALIZING RISK FACTORS We suggest that medical sociologistsand social epidemiologistsneed to counterthe risk factorsthat are increasingly trajectoryof modem epidemiologytoward identifying proximateto disease-ones forwhich"biologicalplausibility"can be argued.One way they risk factors.By this we mean that can do this is by "contextualizing"individually-based framework to understand must(1) use an interpretive whypeople come to be investigators This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 85 exposed to risk or protectivefactorsand (2) determinethe social conditionsunderwhich individualriskfactorsare relatedto disease. We presentexamplesthatillustrateboththese principles. First, an importantstrategyfor reducingthe threatof AIDS is to educate the public or concerningthe steps theymust take as individualsto reduce theirrisk of contracting otherswiththe HIV virus. Clearly,however,some people are betterable to take infecting riskfactorsforAIDS, we maybe thanothers.By contextualizing advantageof thisinformation whysome people cannotavoid therisk. For example,homelessor other able to understand maynothave theoptions as a survivalstrategy poorwomenwho turnto prostitution extremely or resourcesthatwould enablethemto refuseto engagein riskysexual behaviors,no matter theymaybe abouttheriskstheyface. This examplesuggeststhatmedical how well informed riskfactorsby askingwhatit is need to contextualize sociologistsand social epidemiologists thatshapestheirexposureto suchriskfactorsas unprotected aboutpeople's lifecircumstances homelife. lifestyle,or a stressful poordiet,a sedentary sexual intercourse, Our second example concernsthe increasingattentionbeing paid to the public health of meat, poultry,and eggs withE. coli and salmonella problemposed by contamination and to bacteria.The publichas been warnedto rinseand cook meatand poultrythoroughly carefullywash hands,knives,cuttingboards,and so on. Because some followthesesafety guidelinesmoreassiduouslythanothers,one can imaginea riskprofileof individualbehaviors are onlynecessary,however,whenthe These precautions thatmightpredictbacterialinfection. actions in the 1980s that food thatreaches the marketplaceis contaminated.Government industry inspectorsand deregulatedthe meat-processing reducedthe numberof government approachto the havecreatedtheneedforvigilanceon thepartof individuals.Whilethecurrent problemfocuseson the individual,it can readilybe seen thateconomicand politicalforces shape individuals'exposureto thisrisk.This examplesuggeststhatmedicalsociologistsand needto contextualize by askingunderwhatsocial conditionsindividual social epidemiologists risk factorslead to disease and whetherthereare any social conditionsunderwhich the riskfactorswouldhave no effectat all on disease outcome. individual-level riskfactorsmay seem obvious,if we takea hard of contextualizing While theimportance areas of researchin medicalsociology,we will find look at even some of themostinfluential is needed.Consideragainthestressparadigm. thatmuchmoreof thiskindof contextualizing to health circumstances Whilethereare hundredsif notthousandsof studiesrelatingstressful of Turnerand colleagues(Turnerand Marino1994; Turner, outcomes,untiltherecentefforts data aboutthesocial origins Wheaton,and Lloyd 1995), therewas verylittleeven descriptive circumstances of stressful (butsee Smith1992; Goldbergand Comstock1980). riskfactors?One reasonis thatefforts thatwe striveto contextualize Whyis it so important to reduce risk by changingbehaviormay be hopelesslyineffectiveif thereis no clear of theprocessthatleads to exposure.For example,thereare powerfulsocial, understanding cultural,and economic factorsshaping the diet of poor people in the United States. themto abouthealthydietto poorpeople and exhorting providinginformation Consequently, of the follownutritional guidelinesis unlikelyto have muchimpact.Withoutan understanding forreducingtheriskis leftwiththeindividual,and contextthatleads to risk,theresponsibility factorsthatputpeople at riskof risks. nothingis done to alterthemorefundamental shouldturn This lineof thinking suggeststhatmedicalsociologistsand social epidemiologists to examineriskfactorsthatare evercloserto diseasein a tendency on itshead thenow-popular and search to facetheotherdirection causal chain.Rather,it suggeststhatit is justas important bothto explorethesocial forthefactorsthatputpeople at riskof risks.It exhortsresearchers in the riskfactorsare context-dependent individually-based originsof risksand to ask whether healthoutcomesonlywithinthecontextofa specificsetofsocialconditions. senseofinfluencing FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES riskfactors,medicalsociologistsand social In additionto theobviousneedto contextualize of social consideration andthorough needto takeas theirtasktheidentification epidemiologists conditionsthat are what we term "fundamentalcauses" of disease. We call them This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 86 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR "fundamental" causes because, as we shallsee, thehealtheffectsof causes of thissortcannot themechanisms thatappearto linkthemto disease. The possibility be eliminated by addressing qualitywithregardto healthwas first thatsome social conditionshave this fundamental presentedby House and colleagues in a discussionof potentialreasons for the persistent associationbetweenSES and disease (House et al. 1990, 1994). We elaborateupontheseideas social causes of disease. to buildourconceptof fundamental The Case of SES and Disease. The idea thatsocial conditionsmightinfluencehealthwas assertedby nineteenth-century physicianswho foundedthefieldof social medicine. forcefully Virchow(1848), forexample,declaredthat"medicineis a social science." And, of course,it was in partthestrongassociationbetweenindicatorsof povertyand healththatsupportedthis to be apparent,residingin claim. The reasonsforthepowerfulassociationwerealso thought thedire housing,sanitation,and workconditionsof poor people at the time(Rosen 1979). Withtremendous medicaladvances and extensivepublic healthinitiatives,the incidenceof such diseases as diphtheria,measles, typhoidfever,tuberculosis,and syphilisdeclined In addition,in modernwelfarestates,poor people's access to care increased dramatically. as linkingSES to substantially. By the 1960s, manyof the factorsthathad been identified diseasehad been addressed,and one mighthave expectedtheassociationto wane and perhaps Indeed,thisis exactlytheconclusionthatCharlesKadushinreachedin a disappearaltogether. 1964 articlein Sociological Inquiry(Kadushin 1964). Startledthatsocial scientistshad not recognizedthedemiseof theSES gradientin health,Kadushinremindedhis readersthatmost in healthin theUnitedStateshad been to produceSES differences of themechanismsthought addressedand that "as countriesadvance in theirstandardof living, as public sanitation proceeds,and as Dr. Spock becomesevenmorewidelyread, improves,as mass immunization the grossfactorswhichintervenebetweensocial class and exposureto disease will become more and more equal for all social classes" (1964:75). As a result,Kadushindeclared, Americansfromthe lowerclasses are no morelikelyto developdisease thanthosefromthe middleor upperclasses. incorrectas indicatedby Of course, Kadushin'spredictionturnedout to be dramatically an enduringor even an increasing(Pappas et al. 1993) studies(cited above) documenting associationbetweenSES and manydisease outcomes.But whatwas wrongwithKadushin's reasoning?Hadn't he engagedin logic thatmostof us notonlyacceptbut takeforgranted? drawnthepathmodelwithSES as thedistalfactorthatis linkedto disease Havingimplicitly by moreproximalriskfactors,and havingobservedthattheproximalriskfactorsin themodel had been largelyeliminatedas causal agents,he concludedthatthe SES-disease association shouldhave disappeared.But it didn't. turnedout to be wrongis readily On theface of it, thereasonKadushin's 1964 prediction risk riskfactorshe consideredto theintervening apparentwhenone comparestheintervening factorsidentified by Adlerand colleaguesin their1994 reviewof socioeconomicstatusand thatKadushinmentionedare and immunization health.The "gross" riskfactorsof sanitation replacedin theAdlerandcolleagues'reviewbyriskfactorsthatincludesmoking,exercise,and theevidencesuggeststhatseveralof theriskfactorsmentioned by diet,amongothers.Further, mechanismswhen Kadushin wrote. intervening Adler and colleagues were not important Beforethe1960s,forexample,therewas no evidencethatratesof smokingwerehigheramong lowerSES individuals.Rather,the associationemergedduringthe 1960s because people of highersocioeconomicstatuswerelikelyto startsmokingand morelikelyto quit if theyhad started(Ernster1988; Novotnyet al. 1988). Similarchangeshave occurredin otherrisk-related behaviors.For example,in consideringthe strongevidencethatdeclinesin coronaryheart disease have been greatestamongpeople of highersocioeconomicstatus,Beaglehole(1990) aboutand moreable pointedto thefactthathigherSES individualshave been betterinformed to implement changesin healthbehaviorslike smoking,exercise,and diet.The resulthas been a wideningof the gap in ratesof heartdisease betweenthe rich and the poor (Beaglehole 1990). Thus studiesof theassociationbetweenSES and disease overthepast severaldecades revealan important fact-the riskfactorsmediatingthe associationhave changed.As some riskfactorswere eradicated,othersemergedor were newlydiscovered.As new riskfactors becameapparent,people of higherSES weremorefavorablysituatedto knowabouttherisks to avoid them. efforts and to have theresourcesthatallowedthemto engagein protective This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 87 Fromone vantagepoint,thisaccountof theassociationbetweenSES and disease mightbe seen as a curiousstoryin the historyof social epidemiology-an instancein whichunique putforward historicaleventspulledtherugoutfromunderan otherwisereasonablehypothesis by Kadushinin 1964. Far morelikely,however,is the possibilitythattheeffectof SES on riskfactors-because a deeper disease has endured-despiteradical changesin intervening sociologicalprocess is at work. If so, what happenedover the past severaldecades will of pointin time,we presumethatan understanding continueto happenand if,at thisparticular thatcurrently appearto linkthetwo, theSES-disease associationlies intracingthemechanisms timewill proveto be as wrongas Kadushinwas. This will occur,we argue,because SES is a cause of disease. fundamental FundamentalSocial Causes ofDisease. Our discussionof SES to thispointhas focusedon mechanisms.However, associationwithdisease despitechangesin intervening its persistent cause, mightmaintainthis we have notyetexplicitlyindicatedwhySES, or anyfundamental withdisease. relationship kindof enduring social associations,and theessentialfeatureof fundamental The reasonforsuchpersistent causes, is thattheyinvolveaccess to resourcesthatcan be used to avoid risksor to minimize the consequencesof disease once it occurs. We defineresourcesbroadlyto includemoney, resourcesembodiedin theconcepts knowledge,power,prestige,andthekindsof interpersonal of social supportand social network.Variableslike SES, social networks,and stigmatization to directlyassess theseresources2 are used by medicalsociologistsand social epidemiologists causes. However,othervariables and are therefore especiallyobviousas potentialfundamental and examinedby medical sociologistsand social epidemiologists,such as race/ethnicity gender, are so closely tied to resources like money, power, prestige,and/orsocial causes of disease as thattheyshouldbe consideredas potentialfundamental connectedness well. causes to emergeis changeover An additionalconditionthatmustobtainforfundamental humans,therisksforthosediseases,knowledgeaboutrisks,or timein thediseases afflicting fordiseases. If no new diseases emerged(such as AIDS), no of treatments theeffectiveness new risksdeveloped(such as pollutants),no new knowledgeaboutrisksemerged(as about were developed(such as cigarettesmokingin the 1950s and 1960s), and no new treatments social causes would notapply.In such a static theconceptof fundamental hearttransplants), betweena social cause and disease are blocked,the system,as riskfactorsknownto intervene associationbetweenthesocial cause and diseasewoulddeclinein lockstep.But,ofcourse,this withregardto health.In thecontext is nothinglike thesituationhumanshave everconfronted of a dynamicsystemwithchangesin diseases, risks,knowledgeof risks,and treatments, causes are likelyto emerge.The reasonis thatresourceslikeknowledge,money, fundamental fromone situationto another,and are transportable power,prestige,and social connectedness situationschange,thosewho commandthe mostresourcesare best able to as health-related whatthecurrent profile avoidrisks,diseases,andtheconsequencesofdisease. Thus,no matter of diseases and knownriskshappensto be, those who are best positionedwithregardto by disease. social and economicresourceswill be less afflicted important causes. Because a of fundamental attributes The foregoingreasoningsuggeststwo further cause involvesaccess to broadlyserviceableresources,it influences(1) multiple fundamental observation,because it riskfactorsand (2) multipledisease outcomes.This is an important cause and disease can be thattheassociationbetweena fundamental alertsus to thepossibility or in theoutcomes.The idea thatmultiple changeseitherin themechanisms preservedthrough to a persistent associationbetweena cause and an effectcomes mechanismsmay contribute fromsociologistStanleyLieberson.Lieberson(1985) proposedthatsome causes, whichhe effectson a dependentvariablebecause, whentheeffect called "basic causes," have enduring We of one mechanismdeclines,the effectof anotheremergesor becomes moreprominent. have already describedthe example of the changingrole of mechanismslike smoking, exercise,and diet in relationto the associationbetweensocioeconomicstatusand disease. While these variables were no doubt always linked to disease, their connectionto socioeconomicstatuschanged when knowledgeabout theirimportancein healthbecame available. We take the idea thata cause can affectmultiplehealthoutcomesfromsocial JohnCassel. Cassel (1976) pointsoutthatsomesocial factorsmakeindividuals epidemiologist This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 88 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR vulnerable,notto a specificdisease,butto a wide arrayof diseases. As a result,investigations of such social factorsto specificmanifestations of therelationship of disease are of limited utility.Since onlyone manifestation of the social cause is measuredin such studies,thefull impactofthesocial cause goes unrecorded (also see Aneshensel1992; Aneshensal,Rutter,and Lachenbruch1991; Cullen 1984). However,in additionto underestimating thefullimpactof social causes at anygiventime,a narrowfocuson one disease at a timemissesthepossibility that changes in particulardisease outcomes can lead to enduringassociationsbetween fundamental causes anddiseaseoverall.Whenhealthsurveillance or immunization systemsfail and old diseases begin to reemerge(TB, measles) or whennew diseases entera population (AIDS), theydo so in thecontextof existingsocial conditionsthatare ripeenvironments for social causes to new or reemerging producingmechanismsthatlink fundamental diseases. use of drugs,whichin turn Thus,forexample,before1980,SES was linkedto theintravenous had negativehealthconsequences.But withthe emergenceof AIDS, this SES-linkedrisk factorcame to have an even morepotenteffecton health.Indeed,AIDS will likelybecomea in the time ahead due to the rapid in mortality contributor to SES differentials significant in low-incomeareas(Brunswicket al. 1993). Similarly, thereemergence of spreadof infection is striking drug-resistant tuberculosis poor inner-city populationsto a fargreaterextentthanit is higher-status suburbanareas. In sum,a fundamental social cause of diseaseinvolvesresourcesthatdetermine theextentto and mortality.3 which people are able to avoid risks for morbidity Because resourcesare important determinants of risk factors,fundamental causes are linkedto multipledisease mechanisms.Moreover,because social and economic outcomesthroughmultiplerisk-factor fundamental social causes have resourcescan be used in different ways in different situations, effectson disease even whentheprofileof riskfactorschangesradically.It followsthatthe effectof a fundamental cause cannotbe explainedby theriskfactorsthathappento linkit to disease at anygiventime. even those of us who believe that social Research Implications.All too frequently, forhealthare lulledintothinking thatthebestway to understand and conditionsare important theintervening addresstheeffectsof social conditionsis to identify links.Indeed,it ultimately is preciselythisreasoningthatAdlerand colleaguesuse to assertthatpsychologists have an importantrole to play in addressingthe SES-disease association-the risk factorsthey were individually-based behaviorsthatpsychologists are well-equippedto address. identified But theconceptof a fundamental cause sensitizesus to thepossibility thatfundamental social causes cannotbe fullyunderstoodby tracingthe mechanismsthatappear to link themto disease. To be sure, a focus on mechanismscan help identifyvariablesmore proximalto health,and if suchrisksare addressed,thehealthof thepubliccan be improved.However,in thecontextof a dynamicsystemin whichriskfactors,knowledgeof riskfactors,treatments, and patterns of disease are changing,theassociationbetweena fundamental social cause and to new situations.If one disease will endurebecause theresourcesit entailsare transportable genuinelywantsto altertheeffectsof a fundamental cause, one mustaddressthefundamental cause itself. There are two implicationsof this reasoning.First, medical sociologistsand social and communicating themeaningof research need to be carefulin interpreting epidemiologists and disease. Specifically,ifthesocial factor involvingsocial factors,intervening mechanisms, is a fundamental cause, one cannot claim to have accountedfor its effectsby having variablesin a pathor regression "explained"its associationwiththeinclusionof intervening model. Second, to understand associationsbetweenfundamental causes and disease, medical of theresourcesthatfundamental causes sociologistsneed to examinethebroaderdeterminants entail. This distinctlysociological enterprisewill link medical sociologiststo the broader how generalresourceslikeknowledge, disciplinein a productive way as we seekto understand into the health-related money,power, prestige,and social connectionsare transformed of morbidity and mortality. resourcesthatgeneratepatterns This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 89 POLICY IMPLICATIONS Mechanicand Aiken (1986) arguethat,ratherthanleadingto specificpolicies, the main of social science researchto social changeis throughits influenceon the way contribution and thegeneralpublicthinkaboutsocial and healthproblems.This paperaimsto policymakers to theprogressmade by medicalsociologists by drawingattention make such a contribution risk theconceptsof "contextualizing in recentyearsand by offering and social epidemiologists causes." factors"and "fundamental risk factorsthat has dominatedmuch On its own, the focus on individually-based medicalresearchin recentyearsis inadequate.4To be sure,thisfocusis a population-based since the findingsit generatesare highlyrelevantto any given compellingattention-getter, in fatintake,a little individual.Theycan lead to "personalpolicy"changessuchas a reduction exercise,or an aspirina day-actions thatindividualpeople can controlpersonally.But those limitedto a focus whocraftpolicyforpopulationscan be led astrayiftheirpurviewis narrowly riskfactors.This paperrevealstworeasonswhythisis so. First,without on individually-based risk factors, the social conditionsthatexpose people to individually-based understanding will will failmoreoftenthantheyshould.This will occurbecause interventions interventions reasons.The consequence to changeforunrecognized to behaviorsthatareresistant be targeted will be thatlives and moneyare wasted,and theAmericanpublicwill lose confidencein our changesthatreallyimprovehealth.Second, some social conditionsare abilityto implement the addressedby readjusting causes of disease and as such cannotbe effectively fundamental mechanismsthatappearto linkthemto disease in a givencontext.If we individually-based of disease, we mustdo so by directly wish to alterthe effectsof thesepotentdeterminants in waysthatchangethesocial conditionsthemselves. intervening shoulduse The issuesaddressedin thispapersuggestthreegeneralcriteriathatpolicymakers in evaluatingwhetherto commitfundsto a proposedhealthintervention: riskfactors,policymakers shouldrequirethat withtheidea of contextualizing (1) Consistent all interventions seekingto changeindividualriskprofilescontainan analysisof factorsthat of interventions aimed at changing putpeople at riskof risks.This will avoid the enactment If the influencedby factorsleftuntouchedby the intervention. behaviorsthatare powerfully of a riskfactor, manipulation evidenceis to come fromstudiesthatinvolvetheexperimental works outside of the policymakersshould require confirmationthat the intervention theriskfactor manipulating context.The reasonforthisis that,byexperimentally experimental thesocial factorsthat have removedfromconsideration (e.g., dietor exercise),theresearchers Outsidethe experimental determineexposureto the risk factorin the naturalenvironment. in theintervention's context,thefactorsthatputpeopleat riskofrisksmaydominate,resulting ultimatefailure. shouldconsider withtheconceptof fundamental causes, healthpolicymakers (2) Consistent becauseof willhave an impactonjust one diseaseor whether, whether a proposedintervention thathas cause, it will affectmanydiseases. An intervention its influenceon a fundamental than one thathas a even a modestimpacton manydiseases may be far more important relativelystrongimpacton just one. concernedwithbroadsocial conditionsas causes of disease should (3) Healthpolicymakers variablesbut claim to thatfocus only on intervening regardwith skepticisminterventions that addressesthe addressthe broadersocial condition.Even an "effective"intervention social riskfactorwill, in thelong run,failto eliminatetheeffectof a fundamental identified theresourcesthataccrueto themoreadvantagedallow condition.In a changingstateofaffairs, bytheintervention. themto regainthehealthadvantagethatmayhavebeendentedtemporarily mustaddressinequality social causes, theintervention If one wishesto addressfundamental causes entail.Many people and some medicalsociologists in theresourcesthatfundamental believethatthisis impractical-evento thinkabout-because, forthem,inequalityis so firmly thatnothingmuchcan be done aboutit. Believingthis,theonlyreasonablethingto entrenched riskfactors,even if doing so has little do is to focus on moreproximalindividually-based social causes and disease. long-runbearingon theassociationbetweenfundamental Thereare manypoliciesthathave a directbearingon the But thisreasoningis shortsighted. social extentof inequalityin oursocietyand thuson theextentto whichpeople fromdifferent This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 90 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR have access to health-related circumstances resources.To be sure, thesepolicies are rarely discussedwithreferenceto theirhealthimplications.Still, policies relevantto fundamental causes ofdiseaseforma majorpartof thenationalagenda,whether thisinvolvestheminimum wage, housingforhomelesspeople, capital-gains taxes,parenting leave, head-start programs, or otherinitiatives of thistype.Such policyinitiatives oftenlie outsidetherealmof influence and expertiseof healthpolicyexperts.Yet if fundamental causes are potentdeterminants of disease, the potentialhealth impact of these broad policies needs to be thoroughly understood-ataskthatmedicalsociologistsand social epidemiologists shouldtakeup more than they have. Ideally, a research-based"health impact statement"should thoroughly accompanysuch plans, and healthexpertsshouldbe trainedin the skillsneededto produce sucha statement. CONCLUSION The dominantfocus in epidemiologyand perhapsthe Americanculturein generalis on riskfactorsthatlie relativelyclose to disease in a causal chain. But this individually-based focusoverlooksimportant sociologicalprocessesand, as a result,could lead us to actionsthat limit our abilityto improvethe nation's health. We have focused on two conceptsriskfactorsand fundamental causes-that directourattention contextualizing topreciselythose factors thatareleftunexaminedin thecurrently dominant orientation to researchon riskfactors fordisease. If futureresearchby medicalsociologistsand social epidemiologists increasesour of the processesimpliedby theseconcepts,we will be betterpositionedas a understanding societyto further improvethenation'shealth. NOTES 1. Using the American Journal of Epidemiologyas an indicationof the currentemphasis of epidemiologicalresearch,we. reviewedthe 240 articlespublishedbetweenNovemberof 1992 and 1993. Excludingmethodologicalreports(N=44) and studies focused exclusivelyon descriptive epidemiology (N = 15), we foundthatonly13.3 percent(24/181)of thearticlesfocusedon riskfactors thatcould be construedas social in nature.Moreover,because manyof thesearticlesexaminedrace, or gender,withoutexplicitreference to thesocial aspectsof thesecharacteristics, ethnicity our figure estimateof thejournal'sfocuson social factors. of 13.3% shouldbe consideredan upper-bound because it is so closelytiedto theprestigesystem(Goode 1978). Prestige, 2. We includestigmatization or thegeneralstandingthatan individualholds in theeyes of others,is an important resourcethatis likelyto havemanyimplications forhealth-eitherindirectly through resourceslikemoney,power,or whata personand/orthosearoundhim/her social connections, or moredirectlythrough believehe/she is important because itinvolvesthedenialof the deservesfromthesocial environment. Stigmatization benefitsof prestige. 3. We focus here on fundamental social causes of disease. It is possible to conceive of fundamental psychologicalor biological causes as well. For example, at the psychologicallevel, one might considera masteryorientation to be a resourcethatwouldbe linkedto manymechanismsand thusto manydiseases. Similarly,at the biologicallevel, the immunesystemmightbe conceptualizedas a resourcethatwouldinfluencemanyspecificmechanismsand thusmanydiseaseoutcomes.In eitherof cause (masteryor immunesystem)and disease thesecases, theassociationbetweenthefundamental outcomeswould likely endureeven if the specificmechanismswere to change. Our main point causes shouldbe takenseriously social factorsas fundamental causes is notthatfundamental regarding becausetheyare oftensocial, butratherthatsocial conditionsneed to be takenseriouslybecause they are oftenfundamental causes. to recall thatthisindividually-based risk 4. In additionto thefactorswe considerhere,it is important an excessiveportionof theblame to the factorapproachcan also have deleteriouseffectsby shifting on individually-based individual.Whenresearchfocusesattention causes of disease, theonus is often taken off broader-basedconditions.Morbidityand mortalitydue to tobacco is attributed to an bad habit ratherthan to a heavily advertised,government-subsidized, individually-based highly killerindustry. profitable This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 91 REFERENCES Adler, Nancy E., Thomas Boyce, MargaretA. Chesney,Sheldon Cohen, Susan Folkman,RobertL. Kahn, and S. Leonard Syme. 1994. "Socioeconomic Status and Health: The Challenge of the Gradient."AmericanPsychologist49:15-24. Aneshensel,Carol S. 1992. "Social Stress: Theory and Research." Annual Review of Sociology 18:15-38. Stressand Mental Aneshensel,Carol, CarolynRutter,and PeterLachenbruch.1991. "Social Structure, Health." AmericanSociologicalReview56:166-78. Angermeyer,Matthias and Dietrich Klusman. 1987. "From Social Class to Social Stress: New Developmentsin PsychiatricEpidemiology."Pp. 2-13 in From Social Class to Social Stress:New New York: Springer-Verlag. Developmentsin PsychiatricEpidemiologyeditedby M. Angermeyer. Morbidity,and Trendsin CoronaryHeartDisease Mortality, Beaglehole, Robert. 1990. "International Risk Factors."EpidemiologicReviews12:1-16. Becker,MarshallH. 1993. "A Medical SociologistLooks at HealthPromotion."JournalofHealthand Social Behavior34:1-6. Benenson, AbramS. 1987. "InfectiousDiseases." Pp. 207-26 in Epidemiologyand Health Policy, editedby S. Levine and A. Lilienfeld.New York: Tavistock. Berkman,Lisa and Leonard Syme. 1979. "Social Networks,Host Resistance, and Mortality:A Nine-Year Follow-Up Study of Alameda CountyResidents." AmericanJournalof Epidemiology 109:186-204. Berkman,Lisa, Linda Leo-Summers,and Ralph Horwitz.1992. "EmotionalSupportand Survivalafter A Prospective,Population-BasedStudyof the Elderly." Annals of Internal MyocardialInfarction: Medicine117:1003-9. Brunswick,Ann, Angela Aidala, JayDobkin, JoyceHoward, StephenTitus, and JaneBanaszak-Hall. Cohort." and RiskBehaviorsin an UrbanAfricanAmericanCommunity 1993. "HIV-1 Seroprevalence AmericanJournalof Public Health 83:1390-94. Buck, Carol W. 1981. "Prenataland PerinatalCauses of Early Death and Defect." Pp. 149-66 in Medicine,2d ed., editedby D.W. Clarkand B. MacMahon. Boston,MA: Preventive and Community Little,Brown,and Company. to HostResistance."AmericanJournal oftheSocial Environment Cassel, John.1976. "The Contribution of Epidemiology104:107-23. Catalano,Ralph, David Dooley, GeorjeanaWilson, and RichardHough. 1993. "JobLoss and Alcohol Abuse: A Test Using Data fromtheEpidemiologicCatchmentArea Project." Journalof Health and Social Behavior34:215-25. Disease. " Pp. Challah, Sabri and AnthonyJ. Wing. 1985. "The Epidemiologyof Genito-Urinary 181-202 in OxfordTextbookof Public Health, editedby W.W. Holland, R. Detels, and G. Knox. Press. Oxford,England:OxfordUniversity to the Cohen,Sheldon,David Tyrrell,and AndrewSmith.1991. "PsychologicalStressand Susceptibility CommonCold." The New EnglandJournalof Medicine325:606-12. System."Pp. 145-66 in OxfordTextbookofPublic Health,editedby Colley,J.R.T. 1985. "Respiratory Press. W.W. Holland,R. Detels, and G. Knox. Oxford,England:OxfordUniversity Crime and Deviance Theory:The Emergenceof a Structuring Cullen, FrancisT. 1984. Rethinking Tradition.Totowa, NJ:Rowan and Allenheld. Dohrenwend,Bruce P. 1966. "Social Statusand PsychologicalDisorder:An Issue of Substanceand an Issue of Method." AmericanSociologicalReview31:14-34. . 1990. "Socioeconomic Status and PsychiatricDisorders:Are the Issues Still Compelling?" and PsychiatricEpidemiology25:41-47. Social Psychiatry DohrenwendBruce P. and BarbaraS. Dohrenwend.1969. Social Statusand PsychologicalDisorder. New York: Wiley. . 1981. "Part 2. HypothesesAbout StressProcessesLinkingSocial Class to VariousTypes of Psychology9:146-59. AmericanJournalof Community Psychopathology." Dohrenwend,Bruce P., BarabaraS. Dohrenwend,MadelynGould, Bruce Link, RichardNeugebauer, and RobinWunsch-Hitzig.1980. MentalIllnessin theUnitedStates:EpidemiologicalEstimates.New York: Praeger. Dohrenwend,Bruce P., ItzhakLevav, PatrickShrout,SharonSchwartz,Guedalia Naveh, Bruce Link, Andrew Skodal, and Ann Stueve. 1992. "Socioeconomic Status and PsychiatricDisorders:The CausationSelectionIssue." Science 255:946-51. Dutton,Diana B. 1986. "Social Class, Health,and Illness." Pp. 31-62 in ApplicationsofSocial Science to Clinical Medicineand Health Policy,editedby L. Aiken and D. Mechanic.New Brunswick,NJ: Press. RutgersUniversity This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 92 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Ensel, Walterand Nan Lin. 1991. "The StressParadigmand PsychologicalDistress." The Journalof Healthand Social Behavior32:321-41. Ernster,VirginiaL. 1988. "Trends in Smoking,Cancer Risk, and CigarettePromotion."Cancer 62:1702-12. Fenwick,Rudyand MarkTausig. 1994. "The MacroeconomicContextof JobStress." JournalofHealth and Social Behavior35:266-82. Gazzard,B.G. and P. Lance. 1982. "PepticUlceration."Pp. 211-19 inEpidemiology ofDiseases, edited by D.L. Millerand R.D.T. Farmer.Oxford,England:BlackwellScientificPublications. Goldberg,Evelyn L. and George W. Comstock. 1980. "Epidemiologyof Life Events: Frequencyin GeneralPopulations."AmericanJournalof Epidemiology111:736-52. Goode, William. 1978. The Celebrationof Heroes: Prestige as a ControlSystem.Berkeley,CA: of CaliforniaPress. University Hamilton,V. Lee, CliffordBroman,WilliamHoffman,and Deborah Renner.1990. "Hard Times and VulnerablePeople: InitialEffectsof Plant Closings On Autoworkers'Mental Health." Journalof Healthand Social Behavior31:123-40. House, JamesS., Karl R. Landis, and Debra Umberson.1988. "Social Relationshipsand Health." Science 241:540-45. House, JamesS., JamesM. Lepkowski,Ann M. Kinney,RichardP. Mero, Ronald C. Kessler,and A. of Agingand Health." Journalof Healthand Social Regula Herzog. 1994. "The Social Stratification Behavior35:213-34. House, JamesS., RonaldC. Kessler,A. Regula Herzog,RichardP. Mero, AnnM. Kinney,and Martha J. Breslow. 1990. "Age, Socioeconoimic Status, and Health." The Milbank Memorial Fund 68:383-411. Illsley,Raymond.and Ken Mullen. 1985. "The HealthNeeds of DisadvantagedClientGroups." Pp. 389-402 in OxfordTextbookof Public Health, editedby W.W. Holland, R. Detels, and G. Knox. Press. Oxford,England:OxfordUniversity Johnson,Timothy.1991. "Mental Health, Social Relations, and Social Selection: A Longitudinal Analysis."Journalof Health and Social Behavior32:408-23. Kadushin, Charles. 1964. "Social Class and the Experience of Ill Health." Sociological Inquiry 35:67-80. Karasek,Robert,Tores Theorell,JosephSchwartz,PeterSchnall,Carl Pieper,and JohnMichela. 1988. in Relation to the Prevalenceof Myocardial Infarctionin the U.S. Health "Job Characteristics ExaminationSurvey(HES) and theHealthand Nutrition ExaminationSurvey(HANES)." American Journalof Public Health 78:910-18. Kelsey, JenniferL. 1993. "Breast Cancer Epidemiology: Summary and Future Directions." EpidemiologicalReviews15:256-63. Kessler,Ronald, KatherineMcGonagle, ShanyangZhao, Christopher Nelson, Michael Hughes, Susan Eshleman,Hans-UlrichWittchen,and KennethKendler.1994. "Lifetimeand 12-MonthPrevalenceof DSM-III-R Psychiatric Disordersin theUnitedStates:ResultsfromtheNationalComorbidity Survey." 51:8-19. Archivesof GeneralPsychiatry Lennon, Mary Clare. 1987. "Sex Differencesin Distress:The Impactof Genderand Work Roles." Journalof Healthand Social Behavior28:290-305. Lieberson,Stanley.1985. Makingit Count:The Improvement ofSocial Researchand Theory.Berkeley, CA: University of CaliforniaPress. Lin, Nan and Walter Ensel. 1989. "Life Stress and Health: Stressorsand Resources." American Sociological Review54:382-99. Link, Bruce, and Bruce Dohrenwend.1989. "The Epidemiologyof MentalDisorders."Pp. 102-27 in TheHandbookofMedical Sociology,4thed., editedby H. Freemanand S. Levine. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ:PrenticeHall. Link, Bruce and PatrickShrout.1992. "SpuriousAssociationsin LongitudinalResearch." Researchin and MentalHealth 7:301-21. Community Link, Bruce, Mary Clare Lennon, and Bruce Dohrenwend. 1993. "Socioeconomic Status and Depression:The Role of OccupationsInvolvingDirection,Control,and Planning."AmericanJournal of Sociology98:1351-87. Mechanic,David and Linda H. Aiken. 1986. "Social Science,Medicine,and HealthPolicy." Pp. 1-9 in Applicationsof Social Science to Clinical Medicineand Health Policy,editedby L. Aiken and D. Mechanic.New Brunswick,NJ: RutgersUniversity Press. Miller,C. Arden. 1987. "Child Health." Pp. 15-54 in Epidemiologyand Health Policy, editedby S. Levine and A. Lilienfeld.New York: Tavistock. Mirowsky,Johnand CatherineE. Ross. 1989. The Social Causes of PsychologicalDistress.New York: Aldinede Gruyter. This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FUNDAMENTAL CAUSES OF DISEASE 93 S. Kendrick,and PatrickRemington.1988. "Smoking Novotny,ThomasE., KennethE. Warner,Juliette by Blacks and Whites:Socioeconomicand DemographicDifferences."AmericanJournalof Public Health 78:1187-89. Pappas, Gregory,Susan Queen, WilburHadden, and Gail Fisher. 1993. "The IncreasingDisparityin MortalityBetween Socioeconomic Groups in the United States." The New England Journalof Medicine329:103-109. Pearlin,Leonard. 1989. "The Sociological Studyof Stress." Journalof Health and Social Behavior 30:241-56. of Coping." Journalof Health and Social Pearlin,Leonardand CarmiSchooler. 1978. "The Structure Behavior19:2-21. Pearlin,Leonard,MortonLieberman,ElizabethMenaghan,and JosephT. Mullen. 1981. "The Stress Process." Journalof Healthand Social Behavior22:337-56. Pedoe, H. Tunstall.1982a. "Stroke." Pp. 136-45 in Epidemiologyof Diseases, editedby D.L. Miller and R.D.T. Farmer.Oxford,England:BlackwellScientificPublications. . 1982b. "CoronaryHeartDisease." Pp. 103-21 in Epidemiologyof Diseases, editedby D.L. Millerand R.D.T. Farmer.Oxford,England:BlackwellScientificPublications. Potter,JohnD. 1992. "Reconcilingthe Epidemiology,Physiology,and Molecular Biology of Colon Cancer." Journalof theAmericanMedical Association268:1573-77. Prout,MarianneN., TheodoreColton,and RobertA. Smith. 1987. "Cancer Epidemiologyand Health Policy." Pp. 117-56 in Epidemiologyand HealthPolicy,editedby S. Levine and A. Lilienfeld.New York: Tavistock. Rabkin,Judithand ElmerStruening.1976. "Life Events,Stress,and Illness." Science 194:1013-20. JackD. Burke, Robins,Lee, JohnE. Helzer,MyrnaM. Weissman,Helen Orvaschel,ErnestGruenberg, Disordersin ThreeSites." and DarrelA. Regier. 1984. "LifetimePrevalenceof SpecificPsychiatric 41:949-58. Archivesof GeneralPsychiatry Rosen, George. 1979. "The Evolutionof Social Medicine." Pp. 23-50 in The Handbookof Medical Sociology,3d ed., editedby H. Freeman,S. Levine, and L. Reeder. EnglewoodCliffs,NJ: Prentice Hall. PersonalControland Sex Differences Rosenfield,Sarah. 1989. "The Effectsof Women's Employment: in MentalHealth." Journalof Health and Social Behavior30:77-91. Rothman,Kenneth.1986. ModernEpidemiology.Boston,MA: Little,Brown,and Company. Saracci, Rodolfo. 1985. "Neoplasms." Pp. 112-29 in OxfordTextbookof Public Health, edited by Press. W.W. Holland,R. Detels, and G. Knox. Oxford,England:OxfordUniversity Schnall,Peter,Carl Pieper,JosephSchwartz,RobertKarasek,Y. Schlussel,R. Devereux,M. Alderman, KatherineWarren,and Thomas Pikering.1990. "The RelationshipBetween'Job Strain,'Workplace Diastolic Blood Pressure,and Left VentricularMass Index." Journal of the AmericanMedical Association263:1929-35. Schnall, Peter,JosephSchwartz,Paul Landsbergis,KatherineWarren,and Thomas Pickering.1992. 19:488-94. Blood Pressure."Hypertension "RelationBetweenJobStrain,Alcohol, and Ambulatory Shrout,Patrick,Bruce Link, Bruce Dohrenwend,AndrewSkodal, Ann Stueve, and JeroldMirotznik. Life Eventsas Risk FactorsforDepression."JournalofAbnormalPsychology 1989. "Characterizing 98:460-67. Smith,Tom W. 1992. "A Life EventsApproachto Developingan Indexof SocietalWell-Being."Social Science Research21:353-79. Susser, Mervyn,WilliamWatson,and Kim Hopper. 1985. Sociologyin Medicine. New York: Oxford Press. University Syme, S. Leonard and JackM. Guralnik.1987. "Epidemiologyand Health Policy: CoronaryHeart and HealthPolicy,editedby S. Levine and A. Lilienfeld.New Disease." Pp. 85-116 in Epidemiology York: Tavistock. Turner,R. Jay and Franco Marino. 1994. "Social Support and Social Structure:A Descriptive Epidemiology."Journalof Health and Social Behavior35:193-212. Turner,R. Jay, Blair Wheaton, and Donald Lloyd. 1995. "The Epidemiologyof Social Stress." AmericanSociological Review60:104-25. Umberson,Debra, Camille Wortman,and Ronald Kessler. 1992. "Widowhood and Depression: Journalof Health and Social Behavior ExplainingLong-TermGenderDifferencesin Vulnerability." 33:10-24. Virchow,Rudolf. 1848. "The Public HealthService" (in German).MedizinischeReform5:21-22. Walsh, JoyceK. and JosephG. Feldman. 1981. "Health of the U.S. Population." Pp. 583-602 in Preventiveand Community Medicine,2d ed., editedby D.W. Clarkand B. MacMahon. Boston,MA: Little,Brown,and Company. This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 94 JOURNALOF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR and researchscientistat Bruce G. Link is associateprofessorof publichealthat ColumbiaUniversity typesof thesourcesof particular lie in understanding Institute. His interests New York StatePsychiatric of healthand and its consequencesas these bear on the social patterning inequality,its legitimation, is reflectedin his workon theassociationbetweensocioeconomicstatusand major illness.This interest mentaldisordersand thepossiblerolethatoccupationalconditionsmayplay in thisassociation,research on the healthand well-beingof homelesspeople, and researchon the social and economicadversities engenderedby the stigmaof mentalillness. of California,Los Angeles. Her research Jo Phelan is assistantprofessorof sociologyat theUniversity interests includehomelessness,social stigma,the impactof social conditionson healthand illness,and concerninginequalityand itslegitimacy. attitudes This content downloaded from 129.2.61.92 on Fri, 17 Apr 2015 14:07:21 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
© Copyright 2024