Article group analysis Theoretical and Conceptual Notes Concerning Transference and Countertransference Processes in Groups and by Groups, and the Social Unconscious: Part III Earl Hopper In this article, I will define my concept of the social unconscious and specify several aspects of it, and compare this concept with the Jungian concept of the collective unconscious. I will focus on the constraints and restraints of the social unconscious on transference and countertransference processes by persons in groups (Part I, December, 2006) and by groups of persons (Part II, March, 2007). I will also discuss continuing resistance to the use of the ‘social unconscious’ in clinical work. Key words: constraints of the social unconscious, cultural unconscious, collective unconscious, transference and countertransference processes in groups and by groups, Menenius Fallacy, spirituality 1. As I (Hopper, 2003a) have discussed at length in The Social Unconscious: Selected Papers, the ‘social unconscious’ refers both to the fact that people can be and often are unconscious of social factors and forces, and to the social factors and forces of which they are unconscious, in exactly the same way that the Group Analysis. Copyright & 2007 The Group-Analytic Society (London), Vol 40(2):285–300. DOI: 10.1177/0533316407077076 http://gaq.sagepub.com Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 286 Group Analysis 40(2) notion of ‘unconscious’ generally refers both to the fact that people can be and often are unconscious of a variety of biological and psychological factors and forces, and to those biological and psychological factors and forces of which they are unconscious. Specifically, the ‘social unconscious’ refers to the constraints of social objects that have been internalized, and to the restraints of those that have not. The motivation for remaining unconscious of social objects is the regulation of the anxiety that would follow from the recognition of them, and from understanding the nature of both their constraints and restraints. Thus, a series of defensive or protective mental processes may be involved: social objects are not perceived (not ‘known’); and if perceived, not acknowledged (‘denied’); and if acknowledged, not taken as problematic (‘given’); and if taken as problematic, not considered with an optimal degree of detachment and objectivity. This series runs more or less in parallel with dimensions of the biologicallybased psychological unconscious in the more traditional sense, that is, the social unconscious involves the ‘non-conscious’, the ‘dynamic unconscious’ of the ‘repressed’ and the ‘split-off’, and the ‘pre-conscious’. ‘Constraint’ is not meant to imply only ‘inhibition’ and ‘limitation’, but also ‘facilitation’ and ‘development’. For example, processes of constraint even govern the transformation of sensations into feelings, and of concepts into thought and thinking. ‘Constraint’ is taken specifically from the work of Durkheim and other early French sociologists who used the term with respect to the unconscious constraints of ‘social facts’. Although they did not refer to the ‘unconscious’, they used the term ‘conscience collective’, which implies the ‘unconscious of the society’, but this was not really intended, at least not without qualification. The perspective in which the concept of constraint is embedded differs from the classical Freudian perspective that stressed that social phenomena are a source of ‘restraint’, as a matter of ‘society against man’. Hence, Civilisation and its Discontents, one of Freud’s last books (Freud, 1930). (It is perhaps an inconsistency, if not a contradiction, that in this book he discussed the so-called ‘death instinct’, on the one hand, and the social and cultural basis of gender identity, on the other.) Most social scientists use the term ‘social’ to include cultural, economic and political phenomena in general. However, in Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 287 recent years, some psychoanalysts and group analysts have begun to refer to the ‘cultural unconscious’ e.g. Spector-Person (1992) in the United States. French psychoanalysts and group analysts are careful to distinguish the ‘social’ from the ‘cultural’, a practice which Durkheim would have thought to be neither necessary nor desirable, a point of view that is shared by most group analysts, because this distinction between social and cultural is subsumed by ‘social’. The foundation of the ‘cultural’ is the ‘social’, which implies power relations (Dalal, 1998). This Marxist idea that the ‘economic’ determines the ‘social’, and, in turn, both the economic and the social determine the ‘cultural’, was recognized by de Mare´ (1972). However, following early traditions in sociology, social psychology and group dynamics, I tend to use the term ‘interaction system’ for social phenomena, ‘normation system’ for cultural phenomena, ‘communication system’ for communication, etc. Of course, for most purposes ‘social’ will do. Human nature is always biological, psychological and social, and such factors and forces are completely intertwined. Also, the personal is always interpersonal (and familial, group, organizational, and societal) from conception to death (Hopper, 2006a). In fact, there is no need for the concept of the social unconscious other than to denote the social nature and origins of particular objects of which people are unconscious. Actually, were it not for the fact that most psychoanalysts continue to refuse to recognize the importance of the constraints of social objects this discussion would hardly be necessary. Social objects are internalized in the same way that all external objects are internalized: on the basis of both negative processes involving identifications with aggressors of various kinds, and positive processes involving identifications with loving and nurturing objects of various kinds. They are also internalized through instinctually and physiologically governed mirroring processes (Ormay, 2001), which may be related to the constraints of mirror neurones. Nonetheless, the internalization of social objects requires processes of engagement by the other in general and the mother in particular. Of course, mutual attachment processes are central to this. Although the sociality of human nature is apparent from conception onwards, the acquisition of language and its subsequent effects on thinking and communication are especially important. Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 288 Group Analysis 40(2) Language shapes perception of all external phenomena, and the formation of concepts about it. Although the development of verbal communication is governed by a variety of innate factors, and the acquisition of language is preceded by communicational gestures, the learning of language facilitates taking a qualitatively new step in the development of intelligence and personality. From the point of view of group analysis the perception and internalization of social objects cannot be understood merely on the basis of what has been projected into them and/or the fantasies that originate within the infant-mother-dyad – if not from the work of the body and the brain on the nascent mind. Social objects can be and often are internalized in more or less their pristine forms, that is, uncontaminated by prior projections. In other words, ‘bad’ objects are not based only on the internalization of objects that have been contaminated or coloured or modified by prior projections of anxiety and fantasies associated with the so-called death instinct, and such projections are not the very first psychic actions. Thus, it is possible, at least theoretically, to have and/or to build a good enough society in general and a good enough mother and family in particular, the internalization of whom can modify the development of a primal bad object – if not to prevent it in the first place. Clearly, it is virtually impossible to disentangle clinical theory from sociopolitical orientations and values. In sum, the concepts of sociality and the social unconscious are central to the group analytic perspective that insists that persons must be understood not only in terms of the restraints of their bodies, but also in terms of their societies and emergent minds. Persons are always located within their foundation matrices, that is, the socio-cultural-communicational systems of their societies, and within their dynamic matrices as well, that is, the socio-cultural-communicational systems of their families, smaller groups and organizations. Matrices are transgenerational in their origins and development. Even insects are constrained by such factors and forces, although obviously not to the same degree as higher phyla. 2. The question must now be asked – is it helpful and valid to extend this concept of the social unconscious from the level of analysis of Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 289 persons to the level of analysis of social systems? In other words, is it helpful and valid to refer to the unconscious of groups and other kinds of social system? I personally do not use the concept of the social unconscious in order to denote the unconscious ‘life’ of social systems, although many group analysts do, e.g. Brown (2001), Weinberg (2006) and perhaps Knauss (2006). In my view, social systems do not have minds, conscious or otherwise. In order to have a so-called ‘mind’, it is necessary to have a brain. Social systems do not have brains. Similarly, I eschew phrases such as ‘collective memory’ and ‘public opinion’, except in terms of statistical data. The metaphor that social systems and groups in particular are like people is a heuristic device that directs our attention and curiosity towards certain parts, aspects and processes of social systems. However, it is misleading to refer to the ‘unconscious mind’ of a social system, e.g. of a family, group, organization, or society, because this metaphoric analogy between an organism or person and a social system becomes a homology, which implies stability rather than change and the approval of the existing power structure. According to organismic homologies, people in power are always seen as the brain and the heart of the group, and those without power, as the group’s stomach and genitals. (Of course, machines and mechanistic analogies and homologies have the opposite implications, but they are equally limiting.) The Menenius Fallacy,1 that what might be regarded metaphorically as the unconscious mind of a social system is mistakenly regarded to be the unconscious mind of it, is commonplace. In their attempts to describe groups and social systems generally, psychoanalysts especially tend to think in terms of this Fallacy.2 This is why Foulkes introduced his concept of the matrix – the dynamic matrix in the case of groups, and the foundation matrix in the case of societies. He did not refer to the social unconscious of a group or any other kind of social system, because he wished to avoid confusing the level of analysis of persons with the level of analysis of their social systems. He understood that matrices are properties of social systems, not of the individual members in them, and that social systems and their individual members were two sides of the same coin, so to speak.3 This is why group analysts prefer the concept of person to the concept of individual. Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 290 Group Analysis 40(2) Whereas some elements of the dynamic matrix of a group and the foundation matrix of a society are species-based, and, therefore, are likely to be universal, other elements of these matrices are based on their social, cultural, communicational and political structures, and, therefore, are likely to be unique to it. This is the basis for attempting to study the so-called ‘character’ of a nation or any other kind of social system. However, I try not to refer to the so-called ‘character’ of a social system, for the same reason that I try not to refer to its social unconscious, because this tends to confuse the level of analysis of persons with the level of analysis of social systems. (Of course, people in any one society are also both the same and different, partly because they have different genetic make-ups, but also because they have different locations in the foundation matrix of their society.) The one exception to this general rule that we should avoid the Menenius Fallacy concerns what might be called ‘traumatized’ social systems – or ‘wounded’ or ‘broken’ ones, because both wounded people and the systems of which they are members regress in ways that involve a sense of interpersonal merger as a defence against the fear of annihilation and group aggregation. Thus, in the same way that people regress in the face of traumatic pain, social systems also regress from complexity to simplicity. For example, societies become like organizations, organizations become like groups, and groups become like their individual members. Under these circumstances, it is reasonable to talk about social systems as though they were people. In other words, although isomorphy among organisms, persons, groups, and larger and more complex social systems, is a matter of degree, when systems are traumatized the degree of isomorphy among these systems is likely to be high. 3. Many Jungians use the ‘collective unconscious’ as a synonym for the ‘social unconscious’. The ‘collective unconscious’ emphasizes what Jungians call ‘archetypes’ and archetypical phenomena, which are innate unconscious fantasies of complex social situations and figures within them, which are said to be inherited by the species homo sapiens on the basis of acquired characteristics that originated in situations that occurred eons ago.4 The collective unconscious is rooted in the biology of the species, and the Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 291 great phenotypical diversity of cultures is underpinned by a small number of basic, elemental genotypical patterns. Although Jung was himself somewhat ambiguous about the phylogenetic basis of the collective unconscious, he assumed a very close parallel between the biological, psychological and sociological development of societies and cultures, on the one hand, and organisms and persons, on the other.5 Following the work of Henderson (1967), many Jungians use the ‘collective unconscious’ in order to acknowledge the importance of socialization processes, and the continuing constraints of society and culture in general. Some use the concept of the shared unconscious and the concept of the interpersonal unconscious in order to acknowledge the importance of co-constructed interpersonal defences within transference and countertransference processes, and to describe socio-cultural connections among the members of a particular society or segment of it. Contemporary Jungians have modified the traditional concept of the biologically-based collective unconscious by introducing the new concept of the cultural complex (Singer and Kimbles, 2004). This refers to internalized configurations of values, norms and beliefs which are transmitted through the generations. Cultural complexes may be associated with social tension and conflict, e.g. Farris (2006) has discussed racialism and racial tension in these terms, illustrating the constraints of this particular cultural complex in connection with conflict-laden interactions between different racial groups in the United States, linking these interactions with unresolved social trauma in the United States and in the slave trading world more generally. However, cultural complexes are also associated with values, norms and beliefs that are, quite simply, taken for granted. We are not aware of them until we are confronted with the realization that they are not working. In this sense cultural complexes are like the air that we breathe: we do not become aware of air until we lose our access to it. Clearly, the theory of the cultural complex has much in common with Volkan’s (Volkan et al., 2003) theory of chosen trauma, and with Haubl’s (1988) theory of model scenes. These three modifications of the classical Jungian concept of the collective unconscious, namely, the cultural complex, the shared unconscious and the interpersonal unconscious, make the contemporary Jungian concept of the collective unconscious Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 292 Group Analysis 40(2) virtually identical to the Foulkesian concept of the social unconscious. However, whether we use the Foulkesian concept of the social unconscious or the contemporary Jungian concept of the collective unconscious, it is necessary to appreciate and to understand the restraints and constraints that are structured by social and cultural objects of which people are unconscious. And whether or not we use the concept of the social unconscious in order to denote the unconscious life of social systems, there is no doubt that the members of social systems engage in collective actions which are structured by social and cultural objects. 4. I will now shift from this more theoretical discussion of the concept of the social unconscious to a brief discussion of the use of this concept in clinical work both within the clinical dyad and within the clinical group. The so-called ‘complete interpretation’ of the transference in the ‘here and now’ must include not only the ‘here and then’, but also the ‘there and then’ and the ‘there and now’, or in other words all four parts of the Johari Window.6 Whereas some psychoanalysts have begun to think in terms of a therapeutic triangle (Malan, 1979), that is, in terms of the ‘here and now’, the ‘here and then’, and the ‘there and now’, an appreciation of a sociological or group analytical perspective leads us to think in terms of a therapeutic square, the fourth corner of which is the ‘there and then’. Pre-Oedipal, Oedipal and post-Oedipal experience must all be understood in the context of social, cultural and political factors across the generations. In other words, in order to understand our relationships to our fathers and mothers, and our fantasies about these processes, we have to understand the various factors that constrain our fathers and mothers. In other words, it is necessary to work in and with the foundation matrix. A few brief examples may serve to illustrate this basic point. In order to understand what is happening between my male patient and me, it is necessary to understand what happened between my male patient and his father, and what is happening between my male patient and his boss; however, it is also necessary to understand what happened between my male patient’s father and his own father, that is, my patient’s paternal grandfather, as well as between my male patient’s boss and his own Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 293 boss within the context of the power structure of the organization in which they work, and the prevailing attitudes towards authority and power within their world of work. If my patient was a child in a small Welsh village in the early 1930s, we will need to think together about the effects of unemployment rates on the gender identity of men in that village, and, in turn, their attitude towards authority, because these factors will have constrained my patient’s relationship to his father, and how this is repeated in his relationship with me. Similarly, if my patient is a post-World War II German, we will need to think together about his/her difficulties in identifying with his/her parents, and how this is repeated in his relationship to me. And if my patient is struggling to free him/herself from the conflicting constraints of gender role definitions that have more or less been ‘deposited’ and perhaps even forced into his/her mind by traditional parents, we will need to understand the comparative importance of the intertwined influences of culture, on the one hand, and parental personalities, on the other, not to mention a variety of family dynamics and sibling relations, all of which will sooner or later be manifest in transference processes. Although Freud himself argued that a person’s super-ego is influenced not so much by his parent as by his parent’s superego, and so on, the point of view that sociality and the social unconscious are manifest in the transference and should be interpreted as such, can be traced to the work of several early German psychoanalysts, such as Bernfeld (1929), Horney (1937), Fromm (1930), Waelder (1936) and others. However, whereas Wolstein (1954) outlined the importance of social facts and forces in the determination of the transference, it was really Foulkes (1964) who insisted that working with these facts and forces should be a central feature of clinical work. The work of Fairbairn, Guntrip, Winnicott, perhaps Kernberg and in general those psychoanalysts who have contributed to the development of relational psychoanalysis should also be mentioned, as should the work of social historians and cultural critics in the French ‘mould’, such as Sartre and Foucault. Although most psychoanalysts in Britain refer to such work as ‘sociology’, several psychoanalysts in the United States have recognized that the self develops through the internalization of group and family situations and processes, and that such internal phenomena will be manifest in the transference. For example, Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 294 Group Analysis 40(2) David and Jill Scharff (1996, 1998) have formulated a contemporary object relations perspective that recognizes the power of external whole objects in structuring the internal world, and the re-creation of the internal world within many strands of the relationship between patient and analyst. Volkan and his colleagues (Volkan et al., 2003) have recognized that an analyst may be the recipient of a transference that belongs to his patient almost ‘in name only’, because his patient may be carrying elements of his own internal world on behalf of a parent or even a grandparent, and, thus, on behalf of even more distant ancestors. Perhaps special credit should be given to feminist psychoanalysts, whose work has convinced us to rethink the classical formulations concerning the constraints of the body on the mind in general and on gender roles in particular, and to contemporary self-psychologists. Although I am a psychoanalyst, I am also a group analyst and a sociologist, and, therefore, it is hardly surprising that I (Hopper, 2003a) have been particularly interested in the sociality of human nature, the social unconscious and the foundation matrix, not only theoretically but also in my clinical work, especially with respect to the constraints of the social unconscious on transference processes. Many group analysts have discussed this topic, e.g. Hearst (1993) and Brown (2001). In fact, an appreciation of the importance of the social unconscious in clinical work is now commonplace throughout Europe, but especially where Foulkesian group analysts have helped to found Institutes of Group Analysis. Some American psychoanalytical group therapists share this point of view, e.g. Scheidlinger (1964), W. Stone (2004), and H. Kibel (2005). Several psychoanalytical group therapists in France and Belgium tend to think in these terms, although not without modifications, e.g. Rouchy (1982), Kae¨s (1987) and Le Roy (1987). Although the constraints of the social unconscious on countertransference processes must also be considered, surprisingly little has been written about this, with the notable exceptions of Bion (1970), who discussed the constraints of the Establishment on psychoanalytical thinking, Racker (1968), who referred to the constraints of an analyst’s professional affiliations and institutions on his ‘indirect countertransference’, and Lacan, who discussed this topic throughout his long career. Citing the work of Dalal (1993) on the unconscious constraints of social beliefs Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 295 about race, and the work of others concerning nationality, ethnicity and class and other social, cultural and political phenomena, I (Hopper, 1991, 1995, 2003a, 2003b) have delineated and illustrated the constraints of the social unconscious on my own countertransferences to various kinds of patients, partly in terms of what Puget (1986) calls ‘overlapping worlds’. More recently, Scholte (2006) suggested that when the analyst and the patient are from different social and cultural backgrounds, cultural stereotypes are likely to characterize their experiences of one another, and, therefore, these stereotypes require special attention, primarily because they are likely to be unconscious. As discussed in Part II of this article (Hopper, 2006c) several collective phenomena can be considered as group (T)ransference and (t)ransference processes: treating the conductor of a group as though he/she were a parental figure or the parts of a parental figure, based on an analogy that the group is like a human organism and/or a person, and the homology that the group is an organism and/or a person; treating members of the group as siblings, in the same terms that the conductor of a group is treated as a parent; treating the group as a family and the conductor as a parent of it, usually regarding the group as the mother and/or the mother’s body, and the conductor as the father of the group family; the kaleidoscopic development and continuing oscillations among basic assumption processes and their personifications, based on interpersonal or shared defences against psychotic anxieties; the development and maintenance of common group tensions involving family patterns, basic assumptions and typical combinations of impulses and controls; the cocreation of microcosms within the dynamic matrix of the group that are based on situations within the transgenerational foundation matrix of the group – a process that I have called ‘equivalence’; and the development and continuation of social dreaming processes. It is self-evident that in so far as collective (T)ransference and (t)ransference processes involve the interpersonal unconscious and the shared unconscious, these processes are constrained by the social unconscious. Nonetheless, equivalence offers a perfect illustration of the constraints of the social unconscious on collective (T)ransference and (t)ransference processes, especially in the co-creation of chosen trauma and chosen glories and nemes, but also in the co-creation of other more Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 296 Group Analysis 40(2) general aspects of the foundation matrix of which the members of a group are likely to be unconscious. The effects of the social unconscious on the conductor’s countertransference to the group and to group (T)ransference and (t)ransference processes must also be considered. However, once again, virtually nothing has been written about this. Although it should not be assumed that the conductor is more conscious of the constraints of the foundation matrix than are the other members of the group, at least not when they are all members of the same society, when a group analyst conducts groups in other societies and cultures than his/her own, he/she is likely to become aware of the constraints of the social unconscious, and to become better at working in and with the foundation matrix.7 5. It is difficult to analyse the resistances to an appreciation of the constraints of the social unconscious. One reason for this is that narcissistic injuries follow from the realization that we are not fully in control of ourselves, and that we are constrained by the social world as well as by the body and the mind. Another reason is that these resistances are rooted in anxieties associated with very early experience, for example, starting with the painful awareness by the foetus that not only is it attached to a womb, but also that the womb is attached to a larger object, i.e. the mother; and, in turn, that not only is the mother ‘attached’ to the father, but also that the parental couple is ‘attached’ to a larger family; and, in turn, that there is something beyond the family. All these larger and more comprehensive ‘objects’ contribute to a sense of helplessness and powerlessness. However, such constraints can be denoted, and their dynamics discussed and used as the basis for the interpretation of transference resistance to insights into constraints by the social unconscious. Of course, other factors are also involved. Many clinicians have not been trained in the social sciences, and, therefore, are reluctant to discuss the external world. The very term ‘social’ is often taken to imply ‘socialism’, which is even more of a ‘no-no’ today than it used to be, which is an interesting example of the relevance of the sociology of knowledge to clinical practice. Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 297 I sometimes wonder if the difficulties that people have in the appreciation of the constraints of the social unconscious are similar to difficulties that they have in the appreciation of spirituality, or at least in asking questions about liminal transitions. Many people are able to perceive material facts, such as their own bodies, but neither psychic factors, such as their own minds, nor social factors, such as other persons, inter-personal relationships, groups, group processes, organizations, societies, etc. The analysis of the repetition of the concentric and spiralling nature of early life experience within transference and countertransference processes may increase the ability and willingness to be interested in what transpires between people and their groups both at any one time and across the generations. Of course, the opposite is also true: the analysis of these very early life experiences may prevent regression into those states of mind that lead to compensatory preoccupations with spirituality in the service of avoiding the demands of love and hate within the Oedipus complex. Acknowledgement A previous version of this article was presented at the 3rd Annual Conference of the Institute of Group Analysis in Warsaw, Poland, June 2006. Notes 1. Menenius is the avuncular figure in Shakespeare’s ‘Coriolanus’ who more or less starts the play by talking about Rome in terms of the ‘body politic’, using the homology that Rome was a human organism whose various parts could be compared to various classes of the population of Rome. The lower social orders were compared to the stomach and genitals of the human organism, and the higher social orders, to the brain and soul of the organism, etc. 2. I well remember when, during the late 1960s in London, Professor H. Richter presented his findings from a study of a small Swiss village, the leadership of whom had asked for help in order to manage a variety of tensions in the village, mostly stemming from higher than usual rates of immigration and changes in the class structure. Professor Richter and his team concluded that these tensions could be described on the basis of their understanding that the people who lived in the working-class housing estates of the village were the ‘id of the village’; and the doctors, lawyers, senior businessmen, and government officials who lived in an area of private detached houses and luxurious flats were the ‘super-ego of the village’. As a young sociologist in the audience I felt obliged to ask a question: who were the ‘ego of the Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 298 Group Analysis 40(2) 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. village’? Professor Richter answered that he and his research team were the ego of the village, and on this basis they would help to put it right. The audience laughed. Richter did not. In the social sciences many similar concepts have been developed which are virtually synonymous with the ‘matrix’ and its dimensions, for example: ‘social organization’, ‘social structure’, ‘cultural structure’, etc. Following the work of Cortesao (1989), group analysts in Portugal and to some extent in Brazil have developed the concept of the pattern as an alternative to that of matrix. Foulkes referred to the primordial level of communication in groups, by which he seems to have meant the recapitulation of archetypes and archetypical phenomena based in the collective unconscious, but he did not discuss this level of communication in much depth. This classical Jungian point of view is similar to the view expounded by Freud (1913) in his speculative but influential ‘Totem and Taboo’. However, although Freud certainly distinguished the primal horde from the ‘gods’, many Jungians have not distinguished more primitive species, whether living or extinct, from the inhabitants of Mount Olympus, implying that their trials and tribulations have also been encoded into the genetic structure of Homo Sapiens. Nonetheless, Jungian and Freudian perspectives about our origins were both based on the Lamarkian Fallacy that acquired characteristics can be genetically inherited. Of course, this fallacy is under constant discussion, because if true, it would have very important implications for the study of human affairs. The Johari Window is a metaphorical tool created by Harry Ingham and Joseph Luft in 1955 in the USA. It is used to help people better understand interpersonal communication and relationships. Each person selects five adjectives from a list of 55 he/she feels best describes him/her and these adjectives are mapped on to a grid defined in terms of time, space and personal perspective. I used a simplified version of the Window in order to focus attention on the constraints of social and cultural phenomena in terms of a time/space paradigm (Hopper, 2003a). Similarly, immigrants and other people who are ‘marginal’ to the mainstream culture of a particular society are also able to offer such insights. In fact, I have been struck by the over-representation of people in the field of psychoanalytically-informed psychotherapy, especially group analysis, who have been marginal and who often are still marginal. The same is true of sociology and many other pursuits in the social sciences. Of course, in order to have insights into the constraints of the foundation matrix it is not necessary to be and to remain marginal. There are ways of experimenting and even playing with multiple socio-cultural perspectives in a way that leads to taking one’s own matrices as problematic. (In this respect the work of the European Association for Transcultural Group Analysis [EGATIN] should be mentioned.) References Bernfeld, S. (1929) ‘Der soziale Ort und seine Bedeutung fu¨r Neurose, Verwahrlosung und Pa¨dagogik’, Imago XV(2): 17–24. Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 Hopper: Theoretical and Conceptual Notes 299 Bion, W.R. (1970) Attention and Interpretation. London: Tavistock. Brown, D. (2001) ‘A Contribution to the Understanding of the Social Unconscious’, Group Analysis 34(1): 29–38. Cortesao, E.L. (1989) Grupanalise – Teoria e Tecnica. Lisbon: Fundacao Calouste Gulbenkian. Dalal, F. (1993) ‘ ‘‘Race’’ and Racism – An Attempt to Organise Difference’, Group Analysis 26(3): 277–93. Dalal, F. (1998) Taking the Group Seriously. London: Jessica Kingsley. de Mare´, P. (1972) Perspectives in Group Psychotherapy. London: Allen and Unwin. Farris, S. (2006) Personal communication. Foulkes, S.H. (1964) Therapeutic Group Analysis. London: Allen and Unwin. Reprinted 1984, London: Karnac. Freud, S. (1913) ‘Totem and Taboo’, Standard Edition, Vol. 23. London: Hogarth Press, 1955. Freud, S. (1930) Civilisation and its Discontents. London: Hogarth Press. Fromm, E. (1930) The Working Class in Weimar, Germany: A Psychological and Sociological Study, trans. Barbara Weinberger. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Reprinted 1984. Haubl, R. (1988) ‘Kreativer Spiel-Raum und Gruppeninszenierung’, in J. Belgrad et al. (eds) Sprache – Szene – Unbewußtes, pp. 237–71. Frankfurt-am-Main: Nexus. Hearst, L. (1993) ‘Our Historical and Cultural Cargo and its Vicissitudes in Group Analysis’ (17th Annual S.H. Foulkes Lecture), Group Analysis 26(4): 389–404. Henderson, J.L. (1967) Thresholds of Initiation. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press. Hopper, E. (1991) ‘Encapsulation as a Defence Against the Fear of Annihilation’, The International Journal of Psychoanalysis 72(4): 607–24. Hopper, E. (1995) ‘A Psychoanalytical Theory of Drug Addiction: Unconscious Fantasies of Homosexuality, Compulsions and Masturbation Within the Context of Traumatogenic Processes’, The International Journal of Psychoanalysis 76(6): 1121–42. Hopper, E. (2003a) The Social Unconscious: Selected Papers. London: Jessica Kingsley. Hopper, E. (2003b) Traumatic Experience in the Unconscious Life of Groups. London: Jessica Kingsley. Hopper, E. (2006a) ‘The Relational Perspective in Psychoanalysis and Group Analysis: A Comment on the Exchange between Dalal and Lavie concerning the ‘‘Lost Roots of the Theory of Group Analysis’’: ‘‘Taking Interrelational Individuals Seriously’’!’ by Joshua Lavie (Group Analysis, December 2005)’, Group Analysis 39(3): 421–31. Hopper, E. (2006b) ‘Theoretical and Conceptual Notes Concerning Transference and Countertransference Processes in Groups and by Groups, and the Social Unconscious: Part I’, Group Analysis 39(4): 549–59. Hopper, E. (2006c) ‘Theoretical and Conceptual Notes Concerning Transference and Countertransference Processes in Groups and by Groups, and the Social Unconscious: Part II’, Group Analysis 40(1): 21–34. Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015 300 Group Analysis 40(2) Horney, K. (1937) The Neurotic Personality of Our Time. New York: W.W. Norton. Kae¨s, R. (1987) ‘La troisie`me diffe´rence’, Revue de Psychothe´rapie Psychoanalytique de Groupe 9–10: 5–30. Kibel, H. (2005) ‘Progress in Object Relations Theory and Group Analysis: From Classicism to the Influence of Social Forces’, Plenary Lecture sponsored by the Israeli Congress ‘The Power of the Group Space’. Tel Aviv, Israel, July 2005. Knauss, W. (2006) ‘The Group in the Unconscious: A Bridge Between the Individual and the Society’, Group Analysis 39(2): 159–70. Le Roy, J. (1987) ‘The Cultural Structuring of the Personality and Intercultural Relationships’, Group Analysis 20(2): 147–53. Malan, D. (1979) Individual Psychotherapy and the Science of Psychodynamics. London: Butterworth. Ormay, T. (2001) ‘Group in Foulkes’, Group Analysis 34(1): 169–78. Puget, J. (1986) Personal communication. Racker, H. (1968) Transference and Countertransference. London: Hogarth Press. Rouchy, J.C. (1982) ‘Identite´ Culturelle Et Groupe d’appartenance’, Revue de Psychothe´rapie Psychanalytique de Groupe 9: 13–19. Scharff. D. (1996) Object Relations Theory and Practice. Northvale, NJ: Aronson. Scharff, J. and Scharff, D.E. (1998) Object Relations Individual Therapy. Northvale, NJ: Aronson. Scheidlinger, S. (1964) ‘Identification, the Sense of Identity and of Belonging in Small Groups’, International Journal of Group Psychotherapy 14: 346–63. Scholte, R. (2006) Personal communication. Singer, T. and Kimbles, S. (2004) The Cultural Complex. Hove and New York: Brunner-Routledge. Spector-Person, E. (1992) ‘Romantic Love: At the Intersection of the Psyche and the Cultural Unconscious’, in T. Shapiro and R. Emde (eds) Affect: Psychoanalytic Perspectives. New York: International Universities Press. Stone, W. (2004) ‘Group-As-A-Whole: A Self-Psychological Perspective’, Unpublished paper. Volkan, V., Ast, G. and Greer, Jr., W.F. (eds) (2003) The Third Reich in the Unconscious: Transgenerational Transmission and its Consequences. New York and London: Brunner-Routledge. Waelder, R. (1936) ‘The Principle of Multiple Function: Observations on OverDetermination’, Psychoanalytic Quarterly 5: 45–62. Weinberg, H. (2006) Group Analysis, Large Groups, and the Internet Unconscious. Manchester Metropolitan University, submitted in partial fulfilment of PhD by publication requirements, Department of Psychology and Speech Therapy. Wolstein, B. (1954) Transference: Its Meaning and Function in Psychoanalytic Therapy. New York: Grune and Stratton. Earl Hopper is a psychoanalyst and group analyst in private practice in London. He is a training group analyst for the Institute of Group Analysis and a member of the Management Committee of the Group Analytic Society (London). Address: 11 Heath Mansions, The Mount, London NW3 6SN, UK. Email: [email protected] Downloaded from gaq.sagepub.com by Robi Friedman on April 13, 2015
© Copyright 2024