An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande March 29, 2015 1 Introduction Goals: 1. Describe the typologically remarkable phonologically determined agreement system of Gu´ebie, an undescribed Kru language (Niger-Congo) spoken in Cˆote d’Ivoire. 2. Determine an ideal model to account for this and other cases of phonologically determined agreement. Phonologically determined agreement systems challenge our assumption that syntax cannot access phonological features (cf. Pullum and Zwicky 1986, 1988). Syntax Phonological Form Logical Form The big question: Can we model these systems without requiring phonological features to be present in the syntax? The data: • The data here comes from original work with Gu´ebie speakers in Berkeley and Gnagbodougnoa, Cˆote d’Ivoire from September 2013 through July 2014. – Six speakers, ages 19-76 – One woman, five men – Combination of text and elicitation 2 Phonological agreement in Gu´ ebie 2.1 Phonological agreement between pronouns and antecedent nominals • Human third person pronouns take the form /O3 /, singular, and /wa3 /, plural. • Non-human third person pronouns agree with their nominal antecedent not in semantic class, but in phonological features. An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 – The final vowel of the noun stem determines the vowel of the pronoun. (1) Mapping of Final vowel i, I, e, E @, a u, U, o, O Gu´ ebie root-final vowels to pronoun vowels 3.sg pronoun vowel Plural suffix 3.pl pronoun vowel → e -i → i → @ -a → wa → u • The complete personal pronoun chart is given in (2). (2) Human and non-human subject pronouns Human Non-human Singular Plural Singular Plural 4 3 1st e a 1st – – 2nd e2 a2 2nd – – 3 3 3 3 3 3rd O wa 3rd e ,@ ,u i3 ,wa3 • Non-human pronouns are always phonologically determined by their antecedents. (3) Phonological agreement of pronouns with antecedents Noun a. éie2.2 b. kw ala4.2 c. to3 Gloss ‘a prison’ ‘a farm’ ‘battle’ Object e-4 ni-4 e2 ji3 e-4 ni-4 @2 ji3 e-4 ni-4 u2 ji3 Gloss ‘I see it (prison)’ ‘I saw it (farm) ‘I saw it (battle)’ Subject e-3 kadE3.2 @-3 kadE3.2 u-3 kadE3.2 Gloss ‘It (prison) is big.’ ‘It (farm) is big.’ ‘It (battle) is big.’ • This process is completely productive and without exception. – It holds in loan words and nonce words. (4) Phonological agreement in loan words from English/French ji3 a. sukulu1.1.3 kOda.3.21 e-4 ni-4 u2 school exist. I see it(school).acc see ‘There is a school. I saw it(the school).’ ji3 b. baraZE2.3.2 kOda.3.21 e-4 ni-4 e2 I see it(dam).acc see exist. dam ‘There is a dam. I saw it(the dam)’ (5) Phonological agreement in nonce words a. fo2 kOda.3.21 e-4 ni-4 u2 ji3 Nonce-word exist. I see it(nonce).acc see ‘There is a nonceword. I saw it(the nonceword).’ b. gbele4.3 kOda.3.21 e-4 ni-4 e2 ji3 Nonce-word exist. I see it(nonce).acc see ‘There is a nonceword. I saw it(the nonceword).’ • The antecedent does not have to be in the same utterance, nor nearby in the discourse for this agreement to hold. 2 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley 2.2 WCCFL March 2015 Phonological agreement between nouns and modifiers • The same kind of agreement pattern holds between nouns and the final vowel of adjectives that directly modify them (6). (6) Noun-modifier phonological agreement1 a. [email protected] [email protected] éEla1.1 b. fu3 lelo1.2 éElO1.1 house new red sponge new red ‘A new red house’ ‘A new red sponge’ • I return to noun-modifier agreement in Section 5.2 3 Unsatisfying possible analyses • A purely phonological analysis would involve stipulating that the pronoun and its nominal antecedent are in correspondence, and ensure identity between the two corresponding items (Sande, 2014). – A phonological account alone won’t work because the nominal antecedent does not have to appear within the same utterance, nor nearby in the discourse for the phonological agreement to hold. • A purely syntactic account could take one of two forms: 1. Phonological features are present in the syntax and available for copying during morphosyntactic agreement processes. 2. Final vowels on nouns, and their agreeing pronoun vowels, are simply arbitrary noun classes that coincidentally surface as entirely phonologically predictable. • Option one defies accepted theoretical claims that syntax does not have access to phonological information (Pullum and Zwicky, 1986, 1988). • Option two predicts exceptions to the phonological predictability, and might predict a default noun class for loan words or certain semantic categories. – Since we find no exceptions and all pronoun agreement is predictable based on the phonological features of the noun, this analysis fails to capture the generalization that all noun-pronoun agreement is phonologically predictable. 4 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement • I propose a new approach to phonologically determined agreement that relies on specific interactions between morphology and its interfaces. • To account for the data, I propose a novel model of how ellipsis occurs at PF. 1 Word-internal ATR harmony influences the quality of the final vowel of the adjectives; however the backness and rounding values of the final vowel are determined by the final vowel of the noun. 3 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 • The analysis in summary: 1. The agreement-controlling noun is present in the syntax, even when it does not surface. ∗ Pronouns in Gu´ebie are in complementary distribution with the definite marker, /-wa/, (7e). ∗ Like the definite marker, pronouns can occur with an overt noun, like the ‘we linguists’ construction in English, (7c). ∗ They can also license ellipsis of their nominal complement, (7d). · The noun-pronoun construction differs from topic, focus, and definiteness in Gu´ebie. (7) Distribution of definite markers and pronouns2 a. b. c. sukulu1.1.3 sukulu-wa1.1.3.3 sukulu1.1.3 u3 ‘school’ ‘the school’ ‘it school’ d. e. f. u3 *sukulu-wa1.1.3.3 u3 *wa ‘it’ (the school) ‘it the school’ ‘the (one/school)’ 2. The morphology inserts an Agr(eement) node on the pronoun, and features of the noun are copied to it. 3. The phonology, which applies at phase boundaries, can see the morphosyntactic features of the heads within that phase. 4. Phonological identity ensures that agreeing heads are phonologically similar. 5. Ellipsis of the noun occurs at PF and is licensed by overt phonological agreement between the noun and the pronoun. 4.1 The analysis: Syntactic structure to morphology • The syntactic structure: The nominal antecedent of a pronoun is always present in the syntax as complement to the pronoun D (cf. Elbourne 2001). (8) Syntax of pronoun DPs in Gu´ ebie DP NP D {sukulu:N,E} • The structre in (8) is identitcal to the sturucture of a definite DP. This follows Postal (1966); Elbourne (2005); and Arkoh and Matthewson (2013) in unifying determiners with pronouns. • Morphology and phonology apply cyclically by phase (Marvin, 2002), and DP is a phase (Svenonius, 2004). • The morphological structure: An AGR-node is inserted on D, and the N feature is copied to it from the noun (cf. Halle and Marantz 1994). 2 There are four contrastive tone heights in Gu´ebie, marked here with numbers 1-4 where 4 is high. 4 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 (9) Morphological agreement DP NP {sukulu:N,E} 4.2 D D {AGR:N} The analysis: From morphology to phonology • The phonological application: The morphological structure in (9) is linearized via Distributed Morphology Linearization mechanisms (as laid out in Embick 2010). • The morphological features associated with terminal nodes are preserved through morphology and are available to phonology. • I adapt a constraint-based approach, combining Agreement-by-Correspondence (Rose and Walker 2004) with paradigm output-output faithfulness (Burzio 1994, Benua 1997, Kager 1999). • The following widely used constraints accurately select the correct noun-pronoun structures in Gu´ebie: [sukulu u], not *[sukulu e], *[sukule e]. – Ident-OO(N) (Benua, 1997): Heads that Agree in N must be phonologically identical. ∗ This constraint is a stand-in for the combination of a Correspondence constraint (Hansson, 2001; Rose and Walker, 2004) and an Ident constraint. – Anchor-R (McCarthy and Prince, 1993): Segments at the right edge of agreeing heads correspond. – RealizeMorph(eme) (Kurisu, 2001): Each morpheme has segmental content. – *Struc(ture) (Prince and Smolensky, 1993): Assign one violation for each output segment. – Ident-IO (Prince and Smolensky, 1993): Assign one violation for each output segment whose features differ from the corresponding input segment. – Additional constraints (PeriphVowel, *i) account for the reduced number of pronoun vowels compared to the full Gu´ebie vowel inventory. (10) Ranking: Ident-OO(N), Anchor-R, Ident-IO, RealizeMorph *Struc • For cases where a pronoun surface without a nominal complement, I posit that the noun is present in the syntax but is elided at PF, [sukulu u] (cf. Merchant 2001; Lasnik 2007). • Constituents that can optionally be elided are marked with a feature E in the syntax (Merchant, 2001). • I propose a new model of ellipsis where the phonology has access to the E feature of the noun, and the option of eliding the noun is determined via constraints. 5 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 – Faith-NoElide: For each form in an ellipsis paradigm, assign one violation for each output segment whose features differ from corresponding output segments across the paradigm. ∗ This output-output paradigm correspondence constraint ensures that the phrase containing the elided element be as similar to the non-elided output as possible. ∗ For example, the elided form [sukulu u] must be faithful to the non-elided [sukulu u]. (11) A constraint-based approach to ellipsis {sukulu:N,E} {D:N} a. sukulu u, u Faith-NoE b. sukulu u, Ø c. sukulu s, s d. sukulu e, e e. sukule e, e f. sukulu u, e Id-IO Id-OO(N) Anchor Realize Max *Struc * **! * * * * 6 6 6 6 6 6 8 7 8 8 8 8 *! *! *! *! • This analysis forces phonological agreement and provides the option of ellipsis at PF simultaneously via constraints. (12) An interface model of Gu´ ebie pronoun DP agreement Syntax Morphology Phonology DP {sukulu:N,E} {AGR:N} DP NP {sukulu:N,E} 5 D NP D {sukulu:N,E} {sukulu/sukulu} [u] D {AGR:N} Extending the model to account for the entire Gu´ ebie agreement system 5.1 Extending the model to human pronouns in Gu´ ebie • Human pronouns in Gu´ebie do not follow the phonological agreement pattern that all other nouns follow. • They predictably take the forms /O/, singular, and /wa/, plural. • I repeat the pronoun chart for Gu´ebie from (2) in (13) below. (13) Human and non-human subject pronouns Human Non-human Singular Plural Singular Plural 1st e4 a3 1st – – 2 2 2nd e a 2nd – – 3 3 3 3 3 3rd O wa 3rd e ,@ ,u i3 ,wa3 6 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 • The model described in Section (4) need be modified only slightly to account for human pronouns in Gu´ebie. • We saw that nouns are present in the syntax as complement to the pronoun, and their features are copied to the pronoun via a morphological Agr node. • Human nouns not only have a Noun feature which is copied to the pronoun, they also have a [Person] feature (Richards, 2008; Van der Wal, 2015), as in Nudi ‘man,’ (14). (14) Syntactic representation of human pronouns DP NP D {Nudi:N;Person:3;Sg;E} • When features are copied from a human noun to the Agr node on the pronoun D, Person, Number features are copied along with the Noun feature. (15) Morphological agreement between human nouns and pronouns DP NP {Nudi:N;Person:3;Sg;E} D D {AGR:N;Person:3;Sg} • During Vocabulary Insertion, this bundle of features is spelled out as [O], as in (13). (16) Phonological representation of human pronouns {Nudi:N;Hum:3,Sg;E} {AGR:N;Hum:3,Sg} {Nudi/Nudi} [O] • We see that if certain semantic features of the noun (human) are copied to the pronoun D via morphological agreement mechanisms, the pronoun is not subject to phonological identity. • Phonological identity is a last resort agreement strategy. • This follows from Corbett (1991)’s generalization that when semantic and phonological criteria for determining noun class are at odds, the semantics takes precedence. 5.2 Extending the model to Gu´ ebie adjectives • We saw that adjectives agree in final vowel with the nouns that they modify. (17) Noun-modifier phonological agreement a. [email protected] [email protected] éEla1.1 b. fu3 lelo1.2 éElO1.1 house new red sponge new red ‘A new red house’ ‘A new red sponge’ 7 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 • We can derive this agreement in the same way as noun-pronoun agreement. • Syntactically, nouns and the adjectives that directly modify them are present in the same syntactic phase (DP). • An AGR node is inserted on the adjective by the morphology. • Features of the noun are copied to the adjective so that the adjective and noun are in morphosyntactic agreement. • The phonology ensures that agreeing heads (the noun and its adjectival modifiers) are phonologically similar via the constraints discussed in Section 4. – In the same way that pronouns license ellipsis of their nominal complement (7d), adjectives agree with the head noun and license ellipsis of that noun. (18) Overt agreement on adjectives b. a. [email protected] éEla1.1 new red ‘A new red one’ (house) licenses ellipsis of the noun lelo1.2 éElO1.1 new red ‘A new red one’ (sponge) • Just like optional nominal ellipsis in noun-pronoun constructions, candidates are evaluated in paradigms, with two forms in each paradigm: one where the noun is elided and one where it is overt. • A Faith-NoElide constraint ensures output-output paradigm faithfulness so that the adjective agrees phonologically with the noun even when the noun is elided. 6 Extending the model to other phonologically determined agreement systems 6.1 Typological predictions • The constraints presented in Section 4 account for the Gu´ebie data. • They also predict the existence of phonologically determined agreement systems slightly distinct from the one found in Gu´ebie. • Predictions: A. Any two elements within the same syntactic phase could surface in phonological agreement. B. Phonologically corresponding segments will be edge-based or surface within some prominent position in a word. C. Any edge-aligned or prominent segment or suprasegmental can control agreement. • Very few languages outside of Kru have described phonologically determined agreement systems; however, we see the above predictions born out in phonological agreement systems crosslinguistically. 8 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley 6.2 WCCFL March 2015 Other Kru languages • A similar phonologically determined agreement system is present in related Kru languages: – Krahn, a Western Kru language (Bing, 1987) – Godi´e, an Eastern Kru language (Marchese, 1986, 1988) – Vata, an Eastern Kru language (Kaye, 1981; Marchese, 1979; Corbett, 1991) (19) Phonological agreement across Kru Gu´ ebie Number of Agreeing Vowels 3 (Non-human) Pronoun-Noun Yes Adjective-Noun Yes Demonstrative-Noun N/A Definite-Noun No Relative Pronoun-Noun No Krahn 3 Yes No No No No Godi´ e 3 Yes Yes Yes Yes No Vata 5 Yes No No No Yes • The phonologically determined agreement systems in Krahn, Godi´e, and Vata all closely resemble the Gu´ebie system except that a different set of elements agrees with the noun in each language. • Because all of the agreeing elements occur within the DP phase, each system above is predicted by the proposed analysis (cf. Prediction A) 6.3 Bainuk • Bainuk is a Western Atlantic language spoken in Senegal and Guinea (Sauvageot, 1967). • Most nouns in Bainuk take one of 18 fixed noun class prefixes; however, there is a class of prefixless nouns that triggers phonologically determined agreement. • The first syllable of a prefixless noun, no matter its shape, surfaces as the agreement marker on demonstratives, numerals, Wh-words, adjectives, and pronouns. (20) Bainuk prefixless noun agreement a. kata:ma-˜a ka-nak-˜a b. dapOn river two grass ‘two rivers’ ‘long grass’ da-wuri long • Left-anchored correspondence as in Bainuk is predicted by the proposed analysis (cf. Prediction B). 6.4 Abuq • Abuq is a dialect of Arapesh spoken in Papua New Guinea (Nekitel, 1986). • The final consonant of a noun triggers phonological agreement on demonstratives, adjectives, and verbs in Abuq (Dobrin, 1995). 9 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 (21) Abuq phonological agreement (Nekitel 1986 cited in Dobrin 1995) a. aleman afu-neri n-ahe’ man good went ‘a good man went’ b. almil aful-li l-ahe’ bird good went ‘a good bird went. c. ihiaburuh afu-hi h-ahe’ butterfly good went ‘a good butterfly went. • Traditionally there are 13 possible final consonants. Since contact with Tok Pisin and other languages, words have been borrowed with other final consonants. • Even in borrowed words with non-native segments, the final consonant of the noun triggers agreement. This is clearly a phonologically-determined system. (22) Borrowed words undergo phonological agreement a. pater ara priest this ‘This priest’ b. paip apa pipe this ‘This pipe’ • The analysis proposed for Gu´ebie predicts a system like Abuq where edge-aligned consonants (as opposed to vowels) control agreement (cf. Prediction C). 7 Conclusion • Here I provide an initial description of the phonologically determined agreement system of Gu´ebie (Kru, Niger-Congo). • I provide an interface analysis where agreement arises through phonological identity to output forms via morphological agreement mechanisms. • The analysis includes a formal account of ellipsis via constraints at PF. • I have shown that the proposed analysis predicts the attested cross-linguistic phonologically determined agreement systems (Corbett, 1991; Dobrin, 1995). – I leave as a question for further research whether the proposed analysis could serve as a model of gender and noun class systems more generally. • I demonstrate that phonologically determined agreement systems can be modeled without requiring phonological features to be present in syntax. Thus, I conclude we can maintain that syntax is not sensitive to phonological features. 10 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 References Arkoh, Ruby, and Lisa Matthewson. 2013. A familiar definite article in akan. Lingua 123:1–30. Benua, Laura. 1997. Transderivational identity. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Bing, Janet. 1987. Phonologically conditioned agreement: Evidence from krahn. Current approaches to African linguistics 4. Corbett, Greville. 1991. Gender. cambridge textbooks in linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge . Dobrin, Lise M. 1995. Theoretical consequences of literal alliterative concord. In The Proceedings of the 31st Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, volume 1, 127–142. Elbourne, Paul. 2001. E-type anaphora as np-deletion. Natural Language Semantics 9:241–288. Elbourne, Paul D. 2005. Situations and individuals, volume 90. MIT Press Cambridge. Embick, David. 2010. Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology, volume 60. MIT Press. Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1994. Some key features of distributed morphology. MIT working papers in linguistics 21:88. ´ Hansson, Gunnar Olafur. 2001. Theoretical and typological issues in consonant harmony. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. Kaye, Jonathan D. 1981. La s´election des formes pronominales en vata. Revue qu´ebecoise de linguistique 11:117–135. Kurisu, Kazutaka. 2001. The phonology of morpheme realization. Doctoral Dissertation, University of California Santa Cruz. Lasnik, Howard. 2007. On ellipsis: The pf approach to missing constituents. University of Maryland working papers in linguistics 15:143–153. Marchese, Lynell. 1979. Atlas linguistique kru. Abidjan: ILA. Marchese, Lynell. 1986. Tense/aspect and the development of auxiliaries in kru languages. Summer Institute of Linguistics. Marchese, Lynell. 1988. 16 noun classes and agreement systems in kru: a historical approach. Agreement in natural language: Approaches, theories, descriptions 323. Marvin, Tatjana. 2002. Topics in the stress and syntax of words. Doctoral Dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. McCarthy, John J, and Alan Prince. 1993. Generalized alignment. Springer. 11 An interface model of phonologically determined agreement Hannah Sande, UC Berkeley WCCFL March 2015 Merchant, Jason. 2001. The syntax of silence: Sluicing. Islands, and the Theory of . Nekitel, Otto. 1986. A sketch of nominal concord in abu’(an arapesh language). Papers in New Guinea Linguistics No. 24, Pacific Linguistics A70 . Postal, Paul. 1966. On so-called pronouns in english. Monograph series on language and linguistics 19:177–206. Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993. 2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar . Pullum, Geoffrey, and Arnold Zwicky. 1988. The syntax-phonology interface. In Linguistics: The Cambridge survey, ed. Newmeyer, 255–280. New York: The Cambridge University Press. Pullum, Geoffrey K, and Arnold M Zwicky. 1986. Phonological resolution of syntactic feature conflict. Language 751–773. Richards, Marc. 2008. Defective agree, case alternations, and the prominence of person. In Scales, 137–161. Linguistische Arbeitsberichte. Rose, Sharon, and Rachel Walker. 2004. A typology of consonant agreement as correspondence. Language 80:475–31. Sande, Hannah. 2014. Pronoun-antecedent phonological agreement in Gu´ebie. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report . Sauvageot, Serge. 1967. Note sur la classification nominale en bainouk . Ed. du Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique. Svenonius, Peter. 2004. On the edge. In Peripheries, 259–287. Springer. Van der Wal, Jenneke. 2015. Object clitics in comparative Bantu syntax. BLS 41. 12
© Copyright 2024