The Acquisition of a Morphosyntactic Variable in Spanish: The Analytic and Synthetic Forms of Present Progressive Aspect* Stephen Fafulas Indiana University Abstract The current study adds to the growing body of literature on the acquisition of variable structures in Spanish as a second language (Geeslin, forthcoming 2010). In doing so, it sheds light on a generally unexplored area of research in the field: the acquisition of the Spanish present progressive (Bardovi-Harlig, 2000). To achieve this goal, data on both native and non-native use of the simple present (synthetic) and present progressive (analytic) forms of present progressive aspect in Spanish were collected. A total of 24 participants (6 native Spanish speakers, 6 near-native, 6 advanced level, and 6 intermediate level L1 English learners of Spanish) completed a written preference task in the target language. The instrument was previously coded for the linguistic factors of lexical aspect (stative, activity, accomplishment, achievement) and semantic value of the adverb (habitual, immediate), which have been found to influence native speaker selection of these forms (Fafulas, 2009). The results obtained from the multivariate analysis of the current study show that lexical aspect, semantics of the adverb, and participant group are all significant factors in predicting selection of the synthetic, analytic, or both verb forms, by learners of Spanish as a second language. 0. Introduction A number of previous studies have investigated the second language (L2) development of tense and aspect following the suppositions of what is commonly referred to as the aspect hypothesis 1 (Andersen & Shirai, 1995). The premise of this hypothesis, that the inherent semantic aspect of verbs and predicates influences learner development of tense-aspect morphology, has been well documented (Bardovi-Harlig, 2000) and is fundamental to the current investigation. The majority of studies employing this framework have focused on learner morphological development (imperfective vs. perfective) of ‘pastness’, and the evolution of this aspect over time (Andersen, 1986; Bardovi-Harlig, 1998; see section 2.4 for a more detailed review). These studies have led to a richer understanding of how learners form their initial form-meaning associations and of subsequent development of these connections over time (i.e. the one-to-one *I am indebted to Kimberly Geeslin, Manuel Díaz-Campos, César Félix-Brasdefer, and Kathleen BardoviHarlig for their encouragement, inspiration, and continued assistance on this project. Additionally, the current version of the paper has benefited tremendously from the commentary provided by 2 reviewers, the editor, Scott Lamana, Greg Newall, and many colleagues from the IU Department of Spanish and Portuguese. Of course all errors remain my own. 1 See Bardovi-Harlig 2000 for a comprehensive review of tense and aspect studies from a functionalist perspective. 1 principle and multi-functionality; Andersen, 1984; Bardovi-Harlig, 2000). Not only has this line of research deepened our understanding of second language learners’ acquisitional processes but it has additionally provided insight into language universals and the way in which native speakers may process linguistic data. Another particularly fruitful area of investigation that has shed light on both L1 and L2 acquisition has been the study of those areas where native speakers’ (NS) use of a structure varies in the native language and, how in turn, learners incorporate this information into their evolving interlanguage grammar (Geeslin, 2003, forthcoming 2010; Gudmestad, 2006). As far as L2 Spanish morphosyntactic variation is concerned, studies on copula contrast (Geeslin, 2000, 2003), mood distinction (Gudmestad, 2006), future time expression (Geeslin & Gudmestad, forthcoming), and leísmo (Geeslin et.al, 2008) have been carried out. The present study addresses another variable morphosyntactic structure in Spanish: alternation of the present progressive (analytic) and simple present (synthetic) verb forms. Empirical studies on learner production of present progressive aspect are relatively scarce. As far as Spanish L2 acquisition is concerned, to my knowledge, no previous study has specifically sought to address learner usage of the simple present and present progressive forms in light of L1 variability. Previous findings on native speaker variability of present progressive aspect in Spanish (Fafulas, 2009; Fafulas & Killam, 2010; Torres Cacoullos, 2000) indicate that an analysis based on errors will be insufficient in accounting for learner use of these forms. In order to specifically address this issue, the present study employs a written contextualized questionnaire and establishes a native speaker baseline by which to evaluate learner usage of these forms in similar contexts. 1. Background 1.1 Lexical and Grammatical Aspect The current study examines the role of lexical aspect, among other factors, in the acquisition of progressive aspect by learners of Spanish as a second language. Evidence that lexical aspect is one of the key factors that influence native speaker selection of the analytic and synthetic verb forms (discussed below) in present progressive contexts comes from Fafulas (2009), Fafulas & Killam (2010), Klein (1980), Marquez-Martínez (2010), and Torres Cacoullos (2009). Given the present study’s focus on both tense and aspect, a basic review of these concepts follows. Tense, which locates an event on a timeline, such as past or present, is a deictic category anchored in the implied reference point at the time of speech (Comrie, 1985). Aspect, on the other hand, refers to the inherent temporal makeup of a verb or predicate (Comrie, 1976). This inherent temporal makeup, such as whether a verb depicts an action with inherent duration (talk or sleep), is punctual (recognize or notice), or has elements of both leading up to a point of culmination (paint a painting), has been documented to play a fundamental role in the acquisition process (Bardovi-Harlig & Reynolds, 1995). For example, (1a) below, John runs (present) vs. John ran (past), illustrates a contrast along the tense dimension. On the other hand, (1b) John runs 2 (simple present) and John is running (present progressive), displays a difference of grammatical aspect (note that both are in the present tense). (1) a. Tense John runs (present) John ran (past) b. Grammatical Aspect John runs (simple present) John is running (present progressive) Importantly, although a single verb such as run in example (1b) may show a contrast in grammatical aspect, its inherent lexical aspect remains the same. A challenge then, at least in the case of Spanish, is that an L2 learner must come to understand the interaction of both morphologically marked (grammatical) aspect and the inherent (lexical) aspect of the verb or predicate. Vendler (1967) was one of the first to capture these notions in his categorization of four aspectual classes: states, activities, accomplishments, and achievements. Three further semantic features (punctual, telic, and dynamic) can be used to distinguish these aspectual classes from one another. Punctuality denotes something which happens instantaneously and lacks duration. Telicity is identifiable as an action with a specific endpoint. Dynamicity indicates change as well as a lack of stativity. Under this framework, stative verbs (STA) such as querer (to want), differ from the other three categories in that they lack dynamicity [- dynamic] and persist over time without being affected by mental or physical action. On the other hand, activities (ACT), accomplishments (ACC), and achievements (ACH), are all dynamic [+ dynamic]. These last three aspectual classes may be distinguished from one another using the features of duration and telicity. Achievements and accomplishments are both [+ telic], while activities and states are both [- telic] and have no specific endpoint. Activities and accomplishments are both [-punctual] whereas achievements are [+ punctual] and as such capture the beginning or end of an event. For example, the difference between caminar ‘walk’ (activity) and caminar 5km ‘walk 5 km’ (accomplishment) is that the former lacks a specifiable endpoint whereas the latter denotes completion in that once one ‘walks 5km’, the event will be over. Achievement verbs on the other hand, such as llegar ‘arrive’ in llegar a la fiesta ‘arrive at the party’, mark something that happens in an instant [+ punctual]. Tables 1 and 2 below capture these notions of verbal aspectual class and provide some examples from both English and Spanish. Table 1: Semantic features of verbal aspectual classes Features States Activities Accomplishments Punctual Telic + Dynamic + + 3 Achievements + + + Table 2: Examples of verbal aspectual classes English STA know, believe, want ACT walk, sing, play ACC sing a song, walk 5 km ACH wake up, arrive Spanish saber, creer, querer caminar, cantar, jugar cantar una canción, caminar 5 km despertarse, llegar 1.2 Differences and Similarities: Spanish and English Progressive Aspect English and Spanish differ in their expression of present progressive aspect. In Spanish, two verb forms, the synthetic (simple present) and analytic (present progressive) are both capable of encoding the meaning of ‘action in progress’ while in English one verb form, the analytic (be + V-ing), predominates. This is illustrated in example (2) below. (2) a. Synthetic verb form: b. Analytic verb form: Mira, sale ahora el sol. Look, comes-out now the sun. (*‘Look, the sun comes out now.’) Mira, está saliendo ahora el sol. Look, is coming-out now the sun. ‘Look, the sun is coming out now.’ In English, the predominant way of encoding progressive aspect in the present tense is with the construction auxiliary + progressive participle (be + V-ing). That is, while English obligatorily uses the analytic verb form for present progressive aspect, in Spanish a speaker has the option to use an analytic and synthetic form. Importantly, while both English and Spanish prefer use of the synthetic verb form for habitual meaning, in Spanish the synthetic verb form may also encode progressive meaning. In a study that looked at past tense progressive contexts, Westfall (1995) affirms that in Spanish these two verb forms are compatible, that the duration of one is not less than the other, and that semantically they are the same. Bardovi-Harlig (2000: 212) states that the Spanish progressive (Juan está cantando ‘Juan is singing’) can be expressed without loss of progressive meaning with the non-progressive (Juan canta), whereas English obligatorily uses one form. Comrie (1976) asserts that the synthetic form can replace the analytic form without excluding progressive meaning in Spanish. Koontz-Garboden (2004: 1294) states that while the two Spanish verb forms overlap in meaning, in that both can be used with progressive aspect, they only overlap in truth-conditional meaning partially: ‘The progressive interpretation tends to be the only interpretation available for the analytic form, but the synthetic form also has a habitual interpretation that tends to be the only way of expressing habitual meaning’. Finally, Butt & Benjamin (2000) declare that while the analytic adds a nuance to present progressive contexts it does not substantially differ in meaning from use of the synthetic form, resulting in a virtual interchangeability of these constructions 2. At the same time the authors note a common, 2 But see Lamana (2008) for a review of authors who argue for a distinction in meaning of these two forms. 4 but not empirically documented assumption: that English-speaking learners of Spanish constantly over-use the present progressive form. 1.3 Theoretical Framework for the Study of Tense and Aspect Previous research has shown an associative bias in learner distribution of verbal morphology and lexical type, resulting in the following recognizable patterns: preterit morphology most often with telic verbs, imperfect morphology most often with states, and progressive morphology predominantly with activities (Bardovi-Harlig, 2000). This notion was first implemented in second language acquisition research under what is commonly referred to as the aspect hypothesis (Andersen & Shirai, 1995). This hypothesis predicts that both first and second language learners will initially be guided by the inherent lexical aspect of verbs and predicates in the acquisition of tense and grammatical aspect. Using the aspectual verbal classes mentioned above, the aspect hypothesis gives rise to four testable claims (Bardovi-Harlig, 2000): (3) a Learners first use perfective past markings on achievements and accomplishments and only later spread this use to activities and states. b Perfective appears before imperfective, in languages that distinguish between these two, and imperfective use begins with statives and passes to activities, accomplishments, and finally achievements in that order. c In languages that have this aspect, progressive marking begins with activities, extending next to accomplishments and finally achievements. d Progressive marking is not incorrectly overextended to statives. This hypothesis predicts that the lexical aspect of verbs and predicates will guide learners in the acquisition of grammatical aspect. Relevant to the current study, it provides testable claims in that (present) progressive aspect should initially be marked with activity verbs, only later expanding to accomplishments and then achievements. Further, present progressive usage should not be overextended to state verbs. An implication of this hypothesis is that as learners approach a more native-like grammar, they will begin to consider both grammatical and inherent lexical aspect. As for native speakers, the distributional bias (Andersen, 1993; Andersen & Shirai, 1995) posits similar tendencies in that preterit morphology occurs more often with telic verbs, imperfective morphology more often with atelic verbs, and progressive morphology more often with durative verbs. At the same time the distributional bias allows for variation in native speaker pairing of verbal morphology with inherent lexical aspect, such that speakers may manipulate these in order to highlight their perspective on an event. A final principle of the aspect hypothesis, the congruence principle, may help explain why activity verbs are marked most frequently with progressive morphology, at least in English 3 . The congruence principle states that the more similar the meaning of a morpheme is to the lexical item it attaches to, the stronger the form-meaning connection will be. So in English, the morpheme -ing has a strong form-meaning connection with activity verbs given that these contain the semantic features (+ dynamic, - punctual), 3 See Torres Cacoullos (2009) for a discussion of the evolution of the Spanish progressive as an obligatory marker of action in progress. 5 evidenced by the fact that this is the primary way to mark action in progress in English (Bybee et. al 1994). The current study focuses on these claims as they relate to the acquisition and variation of present progressive aspect in Spanish (as both an L1 and L2). In her review of previous studies on tense and aspect, Hasbún (1995; 4) concludes that ‘… further study is needed to determine how similar the biases observed for learners and native speakers of a given language are and to speculate why these biases occur’. Although outside the scope of the current paper, this line of research could have bigger picture implications for linguistic theory, such as whether learners’ behavior of these forms as compared to native speakers is best explained as part of linguistic universals or as inputdriven phenomena. 1.4 Previous Research on Acquisition of Tense and Aspect The aspect hypothesis has been tested on a variety of languages, mostly to account for the acquisition of ‘pastness’ in learner interlanguage grammar. More specifically, these studies focused on the aspectual distinction between the preterit and imperfect forms (see Bardovi-Harlig, 2000 for a comprehensive overview). Again, this preterit-imperfective bias is not completely surprising given that Andersen’s original studies (1986, 1991), leading to the formulation of the aspect hypothesis, were designed to account for the order of emergence of learner past-tense morphology. In contrast only minimal attention has been paid to the acquisition of present progressive aspect or what can be thought of as a learner’s means of expressing action in progress at the present moment. For instance, in the romance languages, to my knowledge only Giacalone Ramat (1995) has studied the (Italian) progressive under the aspect hypothesis. Furthermore, in the concluding sections of her comprehensive monograph on Tense and Aspect in SLA, Bardovi-Harlig (2000) reports that studies focusing exclusively on the progressive are still rare. Bardovi-Harlig reports that before the year 2000, the progressive had only been studied extensively under the aspect hypothesis in the second language acquisition of Italian (Giacalone Ramat, 1995), English (Bardovi-Harlig, 1998), and Japanese (Shirai, 1998). The findings of the studies she reviews are as follows: in Italian, Giacalone Ramat (1995) observed that 63% of all progressive tokens occurred with activity verbs, 22% with mental states, and that only in later stages were accomplishments and achievements used with the progressive (8% and 4% respectively). In Japanese L2 acquisition, Shirai (1998) documented that the progressive was used in over half of all contexts with activities. Similarly, in English a strong correlation was found between activities and the progressive (Bardovi-Harlig, 1998). In conclusion, Bardovi-Harlig (2000; 234) states: ‘the review of the literature shows that several studies of a range of target languages provide evidence in support of one or more claims concerning the effects of lexical aspect proposed by the aspect hypothesis’. 1.5 Relevant Tense and Aspect Studies on Spanish As previously mentioned, the vast majority of Spanish L2 tense and aspect studies have focused on the emergence and subsequent development of the preterit and imperfect in learner language. Although none of these studies specifically focused on present 6 progressive aspect, they add to our understanding of the L2 acquisition of tense-aspect in Spanish. The first of these which deserves mention comes from Andersen (1986, 1991) who was able to identify eight stages of acquisition for the preterit and imperfect, in the interlanguages of two English-speaking children learning Spanish. This is the work that led Andersen to the formulation of the aspect hypothesis. Ramsay (1990) tested the aspect hypothesis on adult learners of Spanish. The findings were similar to those predicted by Andersen in that achievements and accomplishments tended to appear more frequently with the preterit whereas states and activities generally emerged with the imperfect. Subsequent studies such as those carried out by Hasbún (1995) and Salaberry (1999) provided somewhat mixed results regarding the aspect hypothesis 4 but nevertheless documented some stages of development in which learners were guided by the inherent lexical aspect of verbs. Cadierno (2000) and Liskin-Gasparro (2000) furthered the previous research by incorporating more proficient learners into their studies. In these studies the advanced learners were more native-like in that they used grammatical forms, both preterit and imperfect, with different verbal predicates based on their lexical aspect. It is important to note that the advanced learners in both these studies continued to show an influence of verbal lexical semantics, as natives do, but they were able to move from a one-to-one mapping to multi-functionality, furthering Andersen’s original hypothesis. Although they did not specifically seek to account for the acquisition of progressive aspect, the following studies report data relevant to the current investigation given that they test factors which predict native speaker selection of the synthetic and analytic forms, as well as influence from English. One study of particular interest is that of Klein (1980) who compared 8 Spanish monolinguals from Puerto Rico with 10 Spanish-English bilinguals living in New York City. Her methodology consisted of a semi-structured oral interview and a picture description task, which she designed to test for convergence 5. Given the aforementioned differences between the progressive aspect in English and Spanish, Klein hypothesized that these groups might differ regarding their use of the analytic and synthetic verb forms in their expression of present progressive aspect in Spanish. Her results show a statistically significant difference in that the Spanish monolinguals used the synthetic (simple present) form for progressive aspect (26%, n=63) more than the bilinguals (6%, n=18). Klein concludes that the data are in support of her hypothesis for convergence in that the bilinguals are subconsciously aligning their L1 with the L2. In other words, the bilinguals favor the analytic form more than the monolinguals given that the analytic form aligns the Spanish system more closely with the English system without affecting grammaticality from the perspective of monolingual Spanish norms. Klein posits that social factors are largely at work in this shift; i.e. the bilinguals are exposed more frequently to the L2 English data which obligatorily marks progressive aspect with the analytic form, and furthermore, English is the prestigious language of the NYC speech community. While the study shows an increase in use of the present progressive it does not account for the factors which underlie native speaker 4 See Montrul & Salaberry (2003) for a critical review of studies carried out in Spanish under the Aspect Hypothesis. 5 This term was named ‘Indirect Transfer’ by Silva- Corvalán (1994) which relates to convergence in Klein (1980). Normally in the SLA literature transfer refers to the carry over of an L1 feature to the L2 system, but in this case it is the inverse effect: the L2 influences/changes the L1 system. 7 selection of these forms to begin with. At the same time, although Klein reports that the monolinguals residing in New York could not speak English, it is nevertheless conceivable that they had knowledge of the language. Finally, it is not clear whether the speakers were actually evaluated in similar contexts given the author transcribed the data into English and then made intuition judgments on her own. A study which specifically sets out to account for the linguistic factors which govern native speaker selection of these forms comes from Fafulas (2009). In this study the author established a different way of analyzing the usage/variation of the simple present and present progressive forms, reporting on data from 10 monolingual Spanish and 10 bilingual Spanish-English speakers. The methodology of the study, a 20 item contextualized questionnaire, was designed as a way to present participants with identical contexts by which to evaluate their use of these forms (see Geeslin 2003 and methodology of current study for a detailed explanation). All sentences were previously coded for the linguistic variables of verbal aspectual category (stative, activity, accomplishment, achievement) and semantic value of the adverb (habitual or immediate). The data, including the extralinguistic variables of bilingualism and gender, were analyzed with the statistical program GoldVarb X (Sankoff, Tagliamonte & Smith, 2005). The results of the study showed that acceptability of both forms (variation) was produced in a considerable number of contexts. As far as the monolinguals are concerned, they accepted use of the present progressive form in 40.6% of the contexts provided, a higher than expected number given the results of previous studies such as Cortés-Torres (2005), which reported a 22% usage rate for the progressive form. This finding led the author to conclude that the use of the analytic (present progressive) form is more wide-spread in Spanish monolingual communities than previously believed (similar to the proposals of Quesada 1995 and Torres Cacoullos 2000), while at the same time its usage may be increased by contact with English. A further finding is that other factors aside from bilingualism (Klein, 1980) were found to be more significant predictors of the use of the forms under investigation. That is, stative verbs almost categorically disfavored variation, as they strongly favored the simple present form, while dynamic verbs (activities, achievements, and accomplishments) all favored variation. Again, of importance were the following findings: that variation of the simple present and present progressive was found in a large number of contexts, that semantics of the adverb and lexical-aspectual class of the verb are significant factors in predicting the use of both forms in identical contexts, and lastly that monolinguals accepted the use of the present progressive in more contexts than prescriptively prescribed. The current study seeks to build on this previous literature in a number of ways. First, it gathers empirical data regarding learner usage of the analytic and synthetic verb forms of the Spanish present progressive. Second, it uses native speaker data of these forms in similar contexts (Geeslin, 2003) in order to establish a baseline by which learners can be measured, thus uncovering the linguistic/contextual factors which influence both native and non-native use of these forms. Lastly, it tests the influence of lexical aspect on English learners of Spanish as a second language and relates these findings to the Aspect Hypothesis. 8 2. Identifying the target: acquisition of the synthetic and analytic forms of present progressive aspect in Spanish The overarching goal of the present investigation is to begin a line of research which can measure the influence of the linguistic and extralinguistic factors in the L2 acquisition of Spanish progressive aspect in the present tense. An account of the variation between the synthetic and analytic verb forms in native Spanish is essential to achieving this goal. Previous work on the (L2) acquisition of linguistic structures which vary in native speaker speech, has called for a radical departure in the way learner data is investigated (see Geeslin, 2000; 2003). Specifically, research on the acquisition of the Spanish copula and subjunctive has shown that an analysis based on learners’ errors is insufficient in accounting for the development of these structures in learner language (Geeslin, 2003; Gudmestad, 2006). Alternatively, these studies have employed a more productive means of comparing native and non-native data (discussed below) by establishing a native-speaker baseline by which to judge how learners align with native speaker norms (Geeslin 2003). Research Questions (1) Do the factors of lexical aspect and semantics of the adverb predict selection of the simple present and present progressive for learners of Spanish? Do these factors change or remain the same based on proficiency level? (2) In which specific contexts do native speakers allow variation of these forms and in turn how does learner usage model this? (3) What can the learners of this study inform us about predictions (3c) and (3d) of the Aspect Hypothesis outlined above? 2.1 Methodology A total of 24 people from the same mid-western university participated in the study. These participants were divided into the following groups: The first is comprised of 6 learners from an intermediate level Spanish class (S250), the last level needed to fulfill the undergraduate foreign language requirement for this language. The second group is comprised of 6 learners at the advanced level (S326), an introductory course in Spanish Linguistics which requires an advanced level of competency in Spanish. The third group consists of 6 near-native Spanish speakers, all graduate students and instructors of Spanish, with a first language of English. The final group is made up of 6 native Spanish speakers, all of whom were born in either Latin America or Spain and who had immigrated to the USA after the age of 18. 2.2 Procedure and Instruments All participants completed a background questionnaire eliciting place of origin, years of study and familiarity with Spanish and English, travel and residence in Spanish- and English-speaking countries, and other information such as age, sex, and level of education. In addition, all participants completed a written preference task based on 9 previous research which employed a similar instrument (Fafulas, 2009; Geeslin, 2003; Gudmestad, 2006). The results gathered from the instrument used in these previous studies proved its effectiveness in controlling the linguistic variables and providing similar contexts by which to evaluate learners and native speakers. The written preference task contained a series of 12 contextualized items written in the target language (appendix A). The instructions required participants to mark one of the three options following each context in order to indicate what they would say in the given situation. As shown in example (3) 6, all sentences were designed to include the factors of verbal aspectual category (states, activities, achievements, accomplishments) and semantic value of the adverb (giving the situation a habitual or immediate reading). (3) Rebecca le cuenta a su amiga sobre su rutina diaria. Ella le dice: (‘Rebecca tells her friend about her daily routine. She says:’) A. “Estoy corriendo en el parque todos los días después de la escuela.” (‘I am running in the park every day after school’.) B. “Corro en el parque todos los días después de la escuela.” (‘I run in the park every day after school.’) ___ Prefiero A. ___ Prefiero B. ___ Ambos. (I prefer A.) (I prefer B.) (Both.) Coding: correr ‘to run’ [+ activity] todos los días ‘every day’ [+ habitual] The reading of the adverbs as either habitual or immediate was defined as follows: for habitual adverbs the action or event is repeated over a span of time, as in the adverb todos los días (every day); regarding adverbs leading to an immediate reading, such as ahora (now), the criterion was defined as an event occurring at the moment of speech and not passing into the future. The coded data were then entered into the statistical program GoldVarb X (Sankoff, Tagliamonte & Smith, 2005), which evaluates how the independent (linguistic/extralinguistic) variables interact with the dependent variable. The regression analysis used in this program measures the probabilistic weight of each independent variable in relation to the application value (synthetic/analytic/both). A weight above .5 indicates that the particular factor in question favors the selected application value, while a weight below .5 indicates a disfavoring effect. 3. Results and Discussion 3.1 Acquisition of a Variable Structure A total of 288 tokens were coded and analyzed for the current discussion. The results of the statistical analysis presented in Table 4 include all significant factor groups selected by the program for the application value both 7. 6 English gloss is provided here, and in the appendix, for the reader. The English text did not appear for any of the participants; only the text in the target language (i.e. Spanish) was presented to them. 7 For purposes of the discussion I consider the selection of both by participants as their indication that both forms are equally possible in that given context. It should be recalled that participants were indicated to choose the option they would say in a given context. Most important is the fact that both learners and natives were very selective in employing the both option. 10 Table 4: Factor groups selected as significant by GoldVarb X when the dependent variable was both. Input 0.126 Log likelihood -112.512 Significance 0.026 Factor Groups Factors Weight % (N) Achievements 0.718 26.3% Lexical Aspect of the Verb Accomplishments 0.494 18.4% (9/49) Range 0.458 Activities 0.373 9.7% (7/72) States 0.260 8.3% (6/72) Native 0.682 26.4% (19/72) Proficiency Level Near Native 0.558 18.1% (13/72) Range 0.391 Advanced 0.474 13.9% (10/72) Intermediate 0.291 6.9% (5/72) Immediate 0.596 19.8% (19/96) Semantic Value of Adverb Habitual 0.585 18.8% (18/96) Range 0.271 None 0.325 10.4% (10/96) (25/95) First, consider the factor group of lexical aspect of the verb. As indicated by the range 8 (0.458), this factor group is the most important of the three in determining selection of both forms by participants. The factor favoring both selected by the model for verbal aspectual class is achievements (0.718). In other words the lexical aspectual class of achievements favors acceptability of both the present progressive and present simple forms. Given that achievements are both [+] punctual and [+] dynamic the acceptance of both forms may indicate that speakers recognize a selection of one or the other form (analytic/synthetic) would strengthen either of these semantic properties; thus depending on speaker viewpoint both forms are possible. The aspectual lexical class of accomplishments is practically split down the middle (0.494) and could be interpreted as neither favoring nor disfavoring usage of both forms. The other two aspectual verbal classes, activities (0.373) and states (0.260), disfavor permissibility of both forms under investigation. The strongest argument as to why states and activities should disfavor a selection of both is that, because of their semantic features, they are strongly associated with the simple present and present progressive forms (respectively) in Spanish. 8 The range is found by subtracting the lowest probabilistic weight of the factor group from the highest. The closer the value is to 1, the stronger its effect for the model. This is common practice in variationist studies in sociolinguistics. 11 Activities are both [+] dynamic and [-] telic, this links them to the present progressive, while states lack the semantic features of punctuality, telicity, and dynamicity so that they are almost categorically paired with the simple present. These results are largely in line with the findings of the previous studies mentioned above as well as the predictions of the Aspect Hypothesis (Andersen & Shirai, 1995). That is, states and activities strongly favor one form over the other, not both, and most importantly learners show sensitivity to both grammatical and inherent lexical aspect, as expected in attainment of a mature grammar (Cadierno, 2000; Liskin-Gasparro, 2000). The next most significant factor group is proficiency level. The favoring groups selected for proficiency level were native (0.682) and near-native (0.558). In other words the native and near-native groups significantly favor the selection of both forms over the other two groups. The advanced group almost reached favoring status (0.474) while the intermediate group (0.291) clearly disfavored such usage. What is most striking about these results is the steady increase from intermediate to advanced to near-native, and finally to native, for the selection of both forms. This means that as learners gain proficiency they increase their acceptance of both forms and move toward a L2 grammar which permits variation similar to native speaker usage. These results could be interpreted as evidence that learners are moving from a one-to-one mapping of verbal aspectual class with each form toward a system incorporating multi-functionality, as predicted in Andersen’s 1:1 and multi-functionality principles (Andersen & Shirai, 1995). Although a longitudinal study is needed to test these claims in greater detail, my prediction is that the results shown here would hold. The final factor group selected, semantic value of the adverb, reveals that immediate (0.596) and habitual (0.585) adverbs slightly favor a selection of both, while when no adverb is present a disfavoring effect occurs (0.325). In other words, when additional contextual cues are present (adverbs), the overall interpretation of the predicate is substantially different than when no adverb is provided. This indicates that a lack of additional contextual cues increases the importance of lexically and morphologically marked aspect, as these may be the only way to distinguish durative from punctual situations, for example. In turn then, absence of an adverb strongly reduces the permissibility of both forms given that speakers rely on the inherent aspect of the verb and its interaction with morphological marking. 3.2 Predictors of use for the Spanish Present Progressive form We now turn to Table 5 to consider those linguistic features which predict selection of the present progressive form in Spanish. For this part of the analysis the application value was selection of the analytic form by participants. 12 Table 5: Factor groups selected as significant by GoldVarb X when the dependent variable was the present progressive form. Input 0.133 Log likelihood -106.187 Significance 0.000 Factor Groups Factors Weight Semantic Value of Adverb Immediate 0.835 47.9 (46/96) No adverb 0.318 7.3 (7/96) Range: 0.538 Habitual 0.297 9.4 (9/96) Activities 0.765 36.1 (26/72) Accomplishments 0.717 40.8 (20/49) Achievements 0.368 10.5 (10/95) States 0.249 8.3 (6/72) Lexical Aspect of the Verb Range: 0.516 % (N) First, consider the factor group semantic value of the adverb. Within this factor group, adverbs of immediacy (ahora ‘right now’) strongly favor selection of the present progressive form (0.835). This can be explained by the fact that the semantic value of these adverbs adds to the already inherent aspect of the present progressive of ongoing action at the time of speech (Bardovi-Harlig, 2000). The fact that adverbs with a semantic value of habitualness do not favor the present progressive form (0.297) can best be interpreted as a clear indication that these adverbs favor the simple present form. This is logical given that continuous and repetitive activity has generally been expressed by way of the simple present form in Spanish (Quesada, 1995). Regarding the factor group lexical aspect of the verb, activities (0.765) and accomplishments (0.717) significantly favor selection of the present progressive. This is in line with the majority of studies mentioned above and the congruence principle of the Aspect Hypothesis (Andersen & Shirai, 1995). Activities and accomplishments share the semantic features of [-] punctual and [+] dynamic. Arguably this is evidence that there exists a similar form-meaning connection for the Spanish morpheme –ndo as for the -ing morpheme in English. These results seem to provide preliminary support for prediction (3c) of the Aspect Hypothesis although a longitudinal study incorporating beginning level learners is necessary before drawing any definitive conclusions. What about prediction (3d) of the Aspect Hypothesis, that progressive marking should not be overextended to statives? Observe the data presented in Table 6. Table 6: Percentage of present progressive with stative verbs Participant Group % Intermediate Advanced Near-Native Native 28% 6% 0% 0% 13 Here, we have preliminary findings that at earlier levels learners may incorrectly overextend progressive marking to stative verbs but, once proficiency in the target language increases, they pattern similar to native speakers. Overall these findings suggest that learners, even at the intermediary stages, are guided by the inherent lexical aspect of the verb and contextual cues of predicates, much like native speakers. At the same time some of the data suggest L1 transfer in overreliance on the progressive form, at least with statives. Although this may be true there does not appear to be as much overuse of the present progressive as claimed by grammarians such as Butt and Benjamin (2000), given that the statistic analysis did not include participant group as significant for selection of the analytic form. This could in part be interpreted as evidence that the learners at this level already possessed a proficiency level which had moved past such transfer. Again, this will need to be specifically addressed in a future study. 4. Conclusions and areas of future research Although the results from this study are preliminary in nature they point toward a number of suggestions and fruitful areas for future investigation. One of the major findings of the study is that more advanced learners behave more similarly to native speakers in accepting both forms, the synthetic and the analytic, which unlike in English are both capable of expressing present progressive aspect in Spanish. Equally as important is the finding that learners are sensitive to lexical aspect and contextual cues much like native speakers, even at the intermediate levels. A further goal of the study was to test the claims of the Aspect Hypothesis and the applicability of this framework for the investigation of the Spanish present progressive. Although future research is necessary, the results presented in the current study seem to confirm many of the results of previous authors employing the Aspect Hypothesis for a diverse range of languages. Finally, the study has served to illustrate the importance of employing a native speaker baseline by which to compare learner performance with a structure, when the phenomenon under investigation is variable in the target language (Geeslin, forthcoming 2010). At the same time it was noted that future investigation observing a range of learners from the beginning stages of acquisition is necessary in order to get a fuller picture of how present progressive aspect develops over time. In addition future study should compare these results with those obtained from the oral data of a similar group of participants. REFERENCES Andersen, R.W. (1984). The one to one principle of interlanguage construction. Language Learning, 34, 77–95. Andersen, R. W. (1986). El desarrollo de la morfología verbal en el español como segundo idioma. In J. M. Meisel (Ed.), Adquisición de lenguaje/Aquisição da linguagem (pp. 115138). Frankfurt, Germany: Vervuert. 14 Andersen, R. W. (1991). Developmental sequences: The emergence of aspect marking in second language acquisition. In C. A. Ferguson & T. Huebner (Eds.), Second language acquisition and linguistic theories (pp. 305-324). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Andersen, R.W. (1993). Four operating principles and input distribution as explanations for underdeveloped and mature morphological systems. In Progression and Regression in Language, K. Hyltenstam and A. Viborg (eds.), 309–339. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Andersen, R. W. & Shirai, Y. (1995). The Acquisition of Tense-Aspect Morphology: A Prototype Account. Language, Vol. 71, No. 4, pp. 743-762. Bardovi-Harlig, K. & Reynolds, D. (1995). The Role of Lexical Aspect in the Acquisition of Tense and Aspect. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 107-131. Bardovi-Harlig, K. (1998). Narrative structure and lexical aspect: Conspiring factors in second language acquisition of tense-aspect morphology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20, 471-508. Bardovi-Harlig, K. (2000). Tense and aspect in second language acquisition: Form, meaning, and use. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Butt, John and Benjamin, Carmen. (2000). A New Reference Grammar of Modern Spanish. London: Arnold. Bybee, Joan, Revere Perkins & William Pagliuca. (1994). The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Cadierno, T. (2000). The acquisition of Spanish grammatical aspect by Danish advanced language learners. Spanish Applied Linguistics, 4, 1-53. Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect. New York: Cambridge University Press. Comrie, B. (1985). Tense. New York: Cambridge University Press. Cortés-Torres, M.E. (2005). ¿Qué estás haciendo?: La variación de la perífrasis estar + ndo en el español puertorriqueño. Selected Proceedings of the 7th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. 42-55. Fafulas, Stephen. (2009). Sobre la variación de las formas sintéticas y analíticas en el español; Paper presented at the 62nd Annual Kentucky Foreign Language Conference; University of Kentucky; Lexington, KY; April 17, 2009. Fafulas, Stephen & Killam, Jason. (2010). Sociolinguistic Predictors of Use of the Present Simple and Present Progressive Forms in the Spanish of Caracas, Venezuela. 7th Annual Graduate Student Conference on Luso-Brazilian and Hispanic Literature, Linguistics, and Culture. Indiana University. March 25, 2010. Geeslin, Kimberly L. (2000). A new approach to the second language acquisition of copula choice in Spanish. In R. Leow & C. Sanz (Eds.), Spanish applied linguistics at the turn of the millennium: Papers from the 1999 CLASP (pp. 50-66). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Geeslin, Kimberly L. (2003). A comparison of copula choice in advanced and NS Spanish. Language Learning, 53, 703-764. Geeslin, Kimberly L. (2006). Linguistic contextual features and variation in L2 data elicitation. In C. Klee & T. Face (eds.), Selected proceedings from the 7th Conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as a first and second language, pp. 74-85. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. 15 Geeslin, Kimberly L. (forthcoming, 2010). The acquisition of variation in second language Spanish: How to identify and catch a moving target. The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics, Manuel Díaz-Campos (ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Geeslin, Kimberly, Lorenzo Garcia-Amaya, Maria Hasler Barker, Nicholas Henriksen & Jason Killam. (2008). The SLA of direct object pronouns in an immersion environment where use is variable. Paper presented at the 12th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (HLS), Université Laval, Quebec City, Quebec. Geeslin, Kimberly & Aarnes Gudmestad. (forthcoming). An exploration of the range and frequency of occurrence of forms in potentially-variable structures in second language Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 32(3). Giacalone-Ramat, A. (1995). Tense and aspect in learner Italian. In P. Bertinetto, V. Bianchi, O. Dahl, & M. Squartini (Eds.), Temporal reference, aspect, and actionality, Vol. 2. Typological pespectives. Torino: Rosenberg & Sellier. Gudmestad, Aarnes. (2006). L2 Variation and the Spanish subjunctive: Linguistic features predicting mood selection. In C. Klee & T. Face (Eds.), Selected proceedings from the 7th CLASP (pp. 170-184). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. Hasbún, L. (1995). The role of lexical aspect in the acquisition of the tense/aspect system in L2 Spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington. Klein, F. (1980). A quantitative study of syntactic and pragmatic indications of change in the Spanish of bilinguals in the U.S. In Labov, W. (Ed.), Locating Language in Time and Space. Academic Press, New York, pp. 69–82. Koontz-Garboden, A. (2004). Language contact and Spanish aspectual expression: a formal analysis. Lingua, 114, 1291–1330. Lamanna, Scott. (2008). Usage of Imperfect and Imperfect Progressive Verb Forms in Spanish as a Majority and Minority Language: Is There an Effect for Language Contact? In Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, ed. Joyce Bruhn de Garavito and Elena Valenzuela, 251-264. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Liskin-Gasparro, J. (2000). The use of tense-aspect morphology in Spanish oral narratives: Exploring the perceptions of advanced learners. Hispania, 83, 830–844. Márquez-Martínez, Miguel. (2010). La perífrasis estar + ndo en puertorriqueños bilingües con residencia en Estados Unidos. To appear in: Boletín de Filología de la Universidad de Chile. Montrul, S., & Salaberry, R. (2003). The development of tense/aspect morphology in Spanish as a second language. In B. A. Lafford & R. Salaberry (Eds.), Spanish second language acquisition (pp. 47-73). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Quesada, J. Diego. (1995). Estar + -ndo y el aspecto progresivo en español, Iberoromania 42, 8-29. Ramsay, V. (1990). Developmental stages in the acquisition of the perfective and the imperfective aspects by classroom L2 learners of Spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene. Salaberry, R. (1999). The development of past tense verbal morphology in classroom L2 Spanish. Applied Linguistics, 20, 151–178. Sankoff, David, Sali Tagliamonte & Eric Smith. (2005). GoldVarb X: a variable rule application for Macintosh and Windows. Department of Mathematics, University of Ottawa, and Department of Linguistics, University of Toronto. 16 Shirai, Y. (1998). The emergence of tense–aspect morphology in Japanese: universal disposition. First Language 18, 281–310. Torres Cacoullos, Rena. (2000). Grammaticization, Synchronic Variation, and Language Contact: A study of Spanish progressive -ndo constructions. Amsterdan/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Torres Cacoullos, Rena. (2009). Variation and grammaticisation: the emergence of an aspectual opposition. In Stavroula Tsiplakou, Marilena Karyolemou & Pavlos Pavlou (eds.), Studies in Language Variation: European Perspectives II, 215-224. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Vendler, Z. (1967). Linguistics in Philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Westfall, R.E., (1995). Simple and Progressive Forms of the Spanish Past Tense System: A Semantic and Pragmatic Study in Viewpoint Contrast. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Texas, Austin, Austin, TX. Appendix A Instrucciones: Marque con una X una de las tres opciones que siguen a cada par de oraciones para señalar qué diría usted en las siguientes situaciones. Instructions: Mark with an X one of the three options that follow each sentence in order to signal what you would say in the following situations. 9 1. Juan le cuenta a su amigo sobre su rutina diaria. Él le dice: Juan tells his friend about his daily routine. He says to him: A. “Estoy corriendo en el parque todos los días después del trabajo.” “I am running in the park every day after work.” B. “Corro en el parque todos los días después del trabajo.” “I run in the park every day after work.” ___ Prefiero A. ___ I prefer A. ___ Prefiero B. ___ I prefer B. ___ Ambos. ____ Both. 2. Diana quiere saber cuándo saldrá la próxima película de Batman. Ella le pregunta a su novio, el cual debe saber porque es fanático de Batman. El novio le dice: Diane wants to know when the next Batman movie will be out. She asks her boyfriend, who should know because he is a fanatic of Batman. The boyfriend says to her: A. “Sale el próximo mes.” “It comes out next month.” B. “Está saliendo el próximo mes.” “It is coming out next month.” 9 The instrument is provided here with English gloss for the reader only. This English text was not present for any of the participants. 17 3. Diego le pregunta a Niki si su hermano Ernesto va a jugar basquetbol para la escuela. Niki responde: Diego asks Niki if her brother Ernesto is going to play basketball for the school. Niki responds: A. “Es que, Víctor cree que es demasiado bajo para poder jugar basquetbol.” “It’s that, Victor believes that he is too short to play basketball.” B. “Es que, Víctor está creyendo que es demasiado bajo para poder jugar basquetbol.” “It’s that, Victor is believing that he is too short to play basketball.” 4. Tatiana le da instrucciones a la mucama para la semana que viene porque necesita irse de viaje. Ella le dice a la mucama: Tatiana gives instructions to the housekeeper for the following week because she needs to go on a trip. She says to the maid: A. “El bebé necesita tomar una siesta todos los días al mediodía.” . “The baby needs to take a nap everyday at midday.” B. “El bebé está necesitando tomar una siesta todos los días al mediodía.” “The baby is needing to take a nap everyday at midday.” 5. Jorge le pregunta a su amigo sobre los pasatiempos de su padre. El responde: George asks his friend about the hobbies of his father. He responds: A. “Entre muchas otras cosas, él juega futbol.” “Among many other things, he plays football.” B. “Entre muchas otras cosas, él está jugando futbol.” “Among many other things, he is playing football.” 6. Miguel y Antonio están ansiosos por el comienzo del partido. Miguel ve que los jugadores empiezan a entrar en la cancha. Él le dice a Antonio: Miguel and Antonio are anxious for the start of the game. Miguel sees the players start to enter the stadium. He says to Antonio: A. “Está empezando el partido ahora.” “Is starting the game now”. B. “Empieza el partido ahora.” “Starts the game now”. 7. Juan le pregunta a Víctor dónde se encuentra su hermano en este momento. Víctor le responde: Juan asks Victor where his brother is at this momento. Victor responds: A. “Camina en el parque ahora.” “He walks in the park now”. B. “Está caminando en el parque ahora.” “He is walking in the park right now.” 18 8. La doctora le pregunta a Emilia sobre sus hábitos de comer. Emilia dice: The doctor asks Emilia about her eating habits. Emilia says: A. “Estoy comiendo dos manzanas todos los días.” “I am eating dos apples everyday”. B. “Como dos manzanas todos los días.” “I eat dos apples everyday”. 9. Tomás y Lucila están en el supermercado. Tomas escucha el grito del bebé y pregunta: Thomas and Lucile are in the supermarket. Thomas hears the scream of the baby and asks: A. “¿Qué quiere el bebé ahora?” “What does the baby want now?” B. “¿Que está queriendo el bebé ahora?” “What is the baby wanting now?” 10. Susana le lee un cuento a su hija. Se escucha lo siguiente: Susana reads a story to her child. The following is Heard: A. “La princesa se despierta después de dormir por mucho tiempo.” “ the Princess wakes up after sleeping for a long time.” B. “La princesa se está despertando después de dormir por mucho tiempo.” “ the Princess is waking up after sleeping for a long time.” 11. El jefe de Héctor lo regaña después de llegar tarde a la oficina. Su jefe le dice: Hector’s boss yells at him after arriving late at the office. His boss says to him: A. “¿Qué te pasa? estás llegando tarde a la oficina todos los días.” “What’s going on with you? You are arriving late at the office every day.” B. “¿Qué te pasa? llegas tarde a la oficina todos los días.” “What’s going on with you? You arrive late at the office every day.” 12. La cena casi está lista en la casa de los Chávez. La madre le pregunta a su hija donde está su hermano en este momento. Ella le responde: Dinner is almost ready at the Chavez house. Mom asks her daughter where her brother is at this time. She responds: A. “Como se muere de hambre, julio está comiendo unas galletas para aguantar hasta la cena”. “Given he is dying of hunger he is eating some crackers to hold off until dinner”. B. “Como se muere de hambre, julio come unas galletas para aguantar hasta la cena”. “Given he is dying of hunger he eats some crackers to hold off until dinner”. 19
© Copyright 2024