Literacy, Dyslexia and How to Teach Children to Read by David Morgan Why bright children so often struggle to read and how to make it easy for them with Guided Phonetic Reading Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 1 DRAFT MANUSCRIPT This manuscript is shared with you for feedback and editing purposes only. It is not for sale and should not be printed beyond the requirements for this purpose. We are looking for feedback on what is good, what is bad, what does not make sense, any errors you have spotted and what you would like us to add for the final version. The manuscript is not currently complete, with certain markers for images still in place. Contents: Main Section: The Story So Far Introduction Understand the 7 Main Causes of Reading Difficulty Know How to Solve the Different Forms of Reading Difficulty What People Mean By Dyslexia Find Out More About the Neurology of the Reading Process A Summary of the Symptoms, Causes and Solutions Background Section: The History of Writing The History of Literacy Teaching The Future: Easyread and Guided Phonetic Reading The Shannon Trust Story Bibliography Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 2 The Story So Far “Life is what happens while you make plans” When Jack was introduced to me, by his parents, it was obvious that he was a bright, energetic boy with great potential. His parents had been very involved in his education, he was at a good school and he was evidently popular. But Jack was 8 and could not read. Despite everyone’s efforts his reading was on a plateau and going nowhere. Both his parents and his teachers were baffled because he had seemed to be progressing well through Reception and during Year 1. But then he steadily slipped behind through Years 2 and 3. Then he began to lose confidence. He was intensely aware of his friends reading more advanced books and even his younger sister was now overtaking him. His parents and teacher tried various approaches in order to help him out of this situation. He really worked hard at them, but nothing seemed to make any difference. It was now beginning to infect his whole view of school and his attitude towards more reading practice was becoming very negative. In desperation his parents started hunting on the Internet. 6 months later Jack’s life had changed. He was in the top half of his class and being awarded a commendation in school assembly for his reading. Looking back he could no longer even remember why he had found reading hard. It Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 3 seemed like second nature to him now. And his life was back on track. I have changed Jack’s name, but in any case he is entirely typical of what we see again and again. I could interchange him with hundreds of boys and girls, even from just the last few months. Bright children come to us apparently able to read a bit, but actually they are just memorising words and guessing the ones they don’t know. For a bright child that strategy works in the early stages of reading, with simple first reading books. But it leads to real difficulties, as the text gets more complex. Their spelling is usually disastrously bad too. So what had changed for Jack over those 6 months? That is what this book is about. We will talk through the very common cases we see like his, plus all the other less common problems that lead to reading difficulty. We see 7 main causes of reading difficulty and I will describe what you can do about all of them. But first, we need to rewind further. To set the scene, I had better tell you how the Easyread story developed. In the Beginning… My first contact with literacy, like all of us, was learning to read. I can remember learning to read… and you will know that isn’t a good sign. I was bad at it. I was slow to talk as well. Even now I am not a natural communicator. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 4 I can remember the enervating sensation of just not getting it. I can even remember the sight of my poor mother’s exasperated face one afternoon in our sitting room in 1968. I feel a bit guilty about it actually, because I now know how tough it must have been for her. As far as the family is concerned, I am still definitely the digital member of it. I studied engineering at university while they all did English at least to A-Level standard. And while I love novels, theatre and music, I have to confess that most poetry is as puzzling to me as a cryptic crossword. In 1974 I went through the whole thing again when I had to do a year of Greek lessons. Those long minutes of being expected to read that stuff out in class are well etched in my memory. I can remember using many of the strategies that we see with children learning to read English. Then, 30 years after stressing my mother, it was my own turn to be in that seat. I was amazed by how hard my two boys found learning to read and how difficult it was trying to teach them. As an untrained parent I didn’t have the first clue how I should be setting about it. We have a policy in the Morgan household that the parent who is worst at something is the one that should be helping a child who is struggling with it too. The reason is that it is much easier to empathise with their difficulty if you find the subject hard yourself. Somebody with an innate talent for a subject often finds it harder to explain. So teaching the children to read was clearly my task since my wife is a writer! My only use for maths has been to solve the riddles that their teachers send them back with. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 5 However, in this case, my own frustration used to boil within me, and out of me sometimes too! To my shame I was much less good at controlling my emotions than my mother. I remember thinking with my first son “I am sure he will eventually learn to read… but he will probably never want to look at a book again in his life after this experience!” I am certainly not alone as a parent in finding it hard. Teaching a child to read can be a very tough, even traumatic, experience for both parties. As parents we usually have no knowledge at all of how to teach reading and yet it is a highly skilled process, when done properly. There seemed to be no structured reading instruction happening in our schools at the time. So I just muddled through, virtually working it out from first principles. In 1996 I can remember driving down the road with someone talking on the radio about the “phonemes” in words. It was a term I had never heard before. It turned out to be the technical term for the individual sounds in words. So I went to the dictionary later that day to look them all up. At about the same time in the late 1990’s, my father became aware of the fact that 67% of our prison inmates were effectively illiterate. There was no question in his mind that it had almost certainly contributed to many of them being there. It has a huge effect on your self-esteem and earning ability. And frankly I am in awe of the people who can go through 10 years of schooling unable to read the whiteboard without becoming delinquent. Humans do not like to fail at things. Rather than fail, most of us will avoid them. We all know we tend to do that with things we are bad at; we just say we don’t like them! OK, yes, it is possible to Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 6 be good at something and not like it too. But that is less common, I am sure you will agree. As a schoolchild you don’t have the opt-out option and so some children take the next logical step and choose to “succeed” at rejecting the system, rather than being seen to fail within it. We will talk more about that, but we all know people who have bragged about their latest punishment. Anyhow, during a long, hot soak in the bath it occurred to my father that there was still a third of the population in each prison who could read. They were effectively a free resource that could be used to teach the others. They all had time on their hands after all! In that moment The Shannon Trust was born, as a plan. Now, twelve years later, it teaches thousands of people to read every year, at almost negligible cost to us taxpayers. The Shannon Trust story is an interesting one. It was also crucial for the development of my thinking. So I have included it in the background section of the book. And you can see more details at www.shannontrust.org.uk. It is a great organisation doing an amazing job. Anyhow, my father had a spell in hospital, during which I ran the activities of the Trust for him. Before then I had been interested, but not heavily engaged in it. That now changed. When I started visiting prisons I was particularly struck by two things. First, for the prisoners the project was about much more than learning to read. It was changing their lives. Learning to read rewinds them back to when they were 6 or 7 and things started to go wrong. Without getting too fluffy about it, I think it is fair to say that hope is often reborn inside them. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 7 Some people take the view “you guys are just teaching them to be better criminals”. If you want proof of the reverse, consider this; the Prison Officers’ Association was our first and easily our most important supporter. The POA is the union for the prison officers in the UK and has not been famed for its “progressive” attitudes. Well, it is true that they are pretty single-minded in fulfilling their role as the champion for their members’ interests. That is their job. However, the POA management unlocked the doors to the system for us (sorry…). Many prison officers would never have considered working with us without their recommendation. The management of the association had not suddenly come over all gooey about a new mission to help prisoners. They are not against helping prisoners, but that is not task and they are clear about that. Nonetheless, it was an enlightened but simple decision for them. They knew from their officers that the inmates became more rational and positive to work with, after doing our reading course. That was clearly good for their officers. So, if the aim of the association was to make life better for their officers, it was their duty to help us. We do not yet have figures for it, but I will eat my library of literacy books if it is not shown, in due course, that learning to read reduces the rate of recidivism amongst the released prisoners. Since the average reconviction rate is currently 80% within two years of release (in the UK), it won’t be a hard measure to beat. As I have said, children often become delinquent because it feels better to succeed at disrupting than it does to fail at conforming. Each new punishment becomes a new qualification as a successful disrupter, to be valued just as much as the stars on the classroom Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 8 chart. Who hasn’t heard someone bragging about the punishments they have received? But, as new readers, the prison inmates were developing a new belief in the future within the systems of society. The prison officers became our supporters because they saw the psychological impact it had on the inmates. So that was all very inspiring. But there was something else. My second realisation was that these people were often learning to read in 4-5 months. That is as fast as you or I. Much faster than me in fact! It meant that they had had no intrinsic reason for not learning to read in the first place. It just hadn’t worked out for them. So I got into literacy knowing that there had to be at least a partial solution lurking in the dark. A positive attitude can be a real help sometimes! The Millennium Arrives In early 2000 I was looking for new a new direction in life. Designing and marketing commercial furniture, rather moderately successfully, had run its course for me. I was looking to break out of the pattern of business management 8-6 combined with full-on parenting 6-8. So I decided to start wasting a bit of time, studying things that interested me again and letting the world get back into my life. I have a very poor memory. No, I really mean a very poor memory. I really can’t remember much at all! Not names, faces, stories, facts, figures or almost anything. I can read a book twice and only realise a hundred pages in that it is familiar. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 9 It is quite hazardous really, especially when sent on a mission to the shops on the way home. I fill my life with systems to help me manage the things I do. The computer diary is a godsend for me because you have to remember to look at a paper diary. So I did some research on how the memory should work and how to help it. It was extraordinary to me that after a little bit of study, I could read through a list of 50 random words and recite them verbatim. I couldn’t see a use for it… but it was amazing nonetheless! To achieve that all I had to do was engage my visual memory in the process. My visual memory is poor in comparison to most people, but still immensely powerful in absolute terms. How many objects, places and people can you recognise? It is almost limitless. Every one of those has to be matched to a stored memory in your head, for the recognition to take place. In order to remember the 50 words I was just using the ancient Greek technique. You create a visual image of the word and drop it into an easily remembered location. I had 50 “slots” around the house and would put one image in each. Then, to recite the list, I would just go to each slot (in my imagination) and see what image I had stored there. To make it easy you need to make the image very vivid. For instance, my first slot was the arm of our green sofa. If the first word of the list was banana I would imagine a mass of soft, rotting banana flesh being squidged into the fabric of the sofa. That pops back into the memory rather better than a neat little yellow crescent because the emotions are involved. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 10 Anyhow, it really works as a technique. It still didn’t help me remember the shopping, but I felt it must have a practical use somewhere. The more I talked to people about this discovery the more I found that people with a good memory were naturally visualising things. So, since literacy was on my mind, I started thinking about the alphabet. It is very abstract, which makes it hard to remember. My inclination was that if we could turn the letters into locations where some bizarre animal or person was seen, it would make them easier to remember. I wanted the animal to be interacting with the shape of the letter, but to be a separate entity. So I teamed up with a brilliant graphic artist, called Robin Evans, to develop a set of images we could use in that way. The characters that we created look pretty simple, because I wanted them to have simple block colours. As adults our visual processing is fully developed and is far more sophisticated in its interpretation of the world than a child’s. That is why they respond more to a less rendered picture. Picture books for young children are usually very simple for a reason. Here is a selection of some of our images. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 11 These are the ants in pink pants, the bear with long hair, the cat having a chat, the duck covered in muck and the eggs with little legs. Each one has interacted with the shape of the letter in a very specific way. I took our work into a local school to test it on the children there. The results were startling. We were teaching children the alphabet in just days. The alphabet is timetabled over 6 months using conventional approaches. Most experts will flatly deny that it is possible to teach a child the alphabet in a week, because they have never seen it happen. But they are wrong. So, I thought I had a rather good alphabet book to publish. I put the artwork together and called it the Ants in Pink Pants, because the first animal was a group of ants in pink shorts clambering over the letter a. The final element of the book to be designed was the front cover. I decided to use the letters and images from within the book to spell out the title. Here is the original artwork for that: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 12 XXXXXXXXImage Ants in Pink pants cover artwork As I looked at the cover it occurred to me that it had the letters and then a parallel code in the characters, showing what sound each letter represented. I thought the images could potentially be a real aid to an early reader. The characters would help the child decode the text. Reading is really a skill, like riding a bike. To be good at a skill you need to know good technique, but you also need to practise. The problem is that it is difficult to practise reading as a learner, because you just get stuck. This could be a system to help a child practise reading without needing constant intervention from an adult. So I generated a few words with the letters and images. They were words that I knew the children would really struggle to read normally. Again, the effect was amazing. The children found reading the words easy once they had the assistance of these phonetic images. I quickly found that this approach has lots of advantages: • The child has the tools to work out an unfamiliar word unaided. • The adult is no longer a crutch to lean on. • The process is private rather than public • Each new word decoded becomes a moment of achievement rather than failure We called the new system TrainerText. It has developed a bit from the early days. In particular, the images are now floating above the text rather than within it: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 13 TrainerText is the fundamental tool we have used to help every child learn to read. It is the keystone to Easyread, the system that we have developed over the last 9 years. We guide the children along the right track, but essentially we just give them the tools they need to teach themselves how to read. Using TrainerText we can build a child’s confidence step by step through short bursts of successful reading practice each day. This is the first time that children have had a practical tool to develop their phonetic knowledge of words by reading. That is what we mean by Guided Phonetic Reading. So we definitely had a useful tool, but now Lady Luck had her moment. It turned out that the very visual nature of our tool fitted exactly with the very visual nature of the majority of children having reading difficulty. Roughly 80% of children struggling to read have a very strong visual style of learning. From this base we have developed a sequential process for the children to follow. During that process we are hunting for the symptoms of the different causes of reading difficulty. As soon as something is spotted, we then deliver additional support to deal with that. So the course becomes an individual experience for each individual child. That is not the easiest way to go about things, but I do not know of an alternative that will get equivalent results. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 14 Introduction Learning to read is quite clearly the most important thing that we all learn at school. Modern society is largely built on literacy and a weakness in reading and writing puts an individual at a great disadvantage. On the other hand, strong literacy frees people to teach themselves whatever they want to know. Yes, there are famously successful dyslexics. But consider this; roughly one in five children reaches 11 unable to read, across the main industrialised English-speaking countries. Are one in five successful people very poor readers? No, perhaps one in a hundred or less. Indeed 43% of poor readers in the USA live in poverty. As you become aware of these things, the inevitable question, “how can this be the situation we live with?” lodges in your head. Puzzling Patterns But literacy education is full of conundrums, which can seem quite perplexing even to experts in the field. The neural complexity of reading has led to many confusing patterns. For instance: • Bright children often find learning to read particularly hard. • Many children will seem to progress well between the ages of 4 and 6, but will then find their progress reaches a low plateau between 6 and 9. • Balance, co-ordination and exercise can have a big impact on a child’s reading development in some cases. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 15 • Some children can read single words fine, but not lines of text. For others it is the reverse • Some children are able to read text on a beige background, but not on a white background. • Some children make more mistakes with short words than long words. For others it is the reverse. • Some children seem able to read out loud fluently, but have no understanding of what they have read, however simple it is. If you then read back to them the text they have just read out to you, they will understand it instantly. So the whole subject can seem confusing and even daunting. Many sources of information on literacy talk a lot, without saying much about what is really happening and why. That is something this book will change for you. With so much dispute washing around the issue, it can also seem hard to know whom to believe on the right way forward. Debates rage over the right approach to literacy, the causes of reading difficulty, what dyslexia is, whether dyslexia can be helped and even whether it exists. You can hear an expert put forward very cogent arguments, which seem quite convincing… until you hear some other expert exactly countermands those arguments, with equally impressive reasoning. And here is the funny thing; both of them can be right in certain circumstances. It is only by developing an understanding of the underlying mechanisms taking place in the head of the reader that you can start to know what is right and what is misconceived. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 16 I am afraid that it is only by developing your own knowledge of the subject, and particularly the neural issues involved, that you can have confidence in what you know. The aim of this book is to give you that knowledge: • It will explain the reasons for each of the above conundrums and many others. • It will explain the 7 main causes of reading difficulty. • And it will give simple methods for overcoming each cause of difficulty. Then, in the second section, you will also find a lot of background material on the whole subject, which you can explore to whatever degree suits you. It is not meant necessarily as a sequential read. The heart of the book is in the explanation of the different types of reading difficulty and the specific solutions to each specific difficulty. With the knowledge presented here and by using a multi-stranded approach that delivers the solution each child needs for his or her own mix of issues, we have found that easily nine out of ten children struggling with reading can come to be proficient in around 6-9 months and sometimes far quicker. Can This Be Real? There is an obvious question that is probably running around your head as you read this. It is probably something vaguely along the lines of “Yes David, what you are saying sounds great... but… how is it that… the combined resources of the entire English-speaking world’s education systems have battled with this for 200+ years apparently without success and you are saying you have a simple solution to Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 17 almost all of it? Without wanting to be rude… that just does not sound likely!” It’s a good question and requires an answer. I agree that it does seem surprising and I have pondered the question myself. I am always concerned not to overstate what we can do.. Furthermore I am only too aware that I am no Einstein. So it cannot be put down to some stroke of genius. On the contrary, the reason we have had success is, I think, due to an unusual combination of circumstances, the rather patchy nature of what intelligence I do have and how it contrasts with that of most people in the field. And probably the key to it has been that I came to the subject knowing nothing. That may sound like a disadvantage, but it can be very useful. It does not take long to read into any subject. Knowledge is quick and easy to pick up. But not knowing what others think you should know is very liberating. I guess that is maybe one thing I do have in common with Einstein! By contrast to us, most people have followed a relatively narrow path of formal training, developed for them by a school of thinking. They are emotionally attached and partly bound to their particular speciality. As a result, they are often closed to other possibilities and tend to want their particular knowledge to be “the answer”. The truth is that reading is a complex neurological process and there are many ways it can go wrong. Any attempt to resolve all reading difficulty down to one cause is sure to be wrong. Many of the arguments that rage about it are based on this error. Both participants can be right for different individual cases. For instance, I can show you situations where a particular solution is critical for perhaps 5% of the population but irrelevant to 95%. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 18 As a unitary solution it looks like rubbish (5% success rate) and easily discounted. But for some individuals it can be their only possible salvation. For those individuals it is a 100% success. In addition, while initially knowing nothing, I have now worked with thousands of children, helping them to overcome their individual challenges. That is more than many specialists do in a lifetime. This has almost always been done remotely. So I have taken a detailed description of their situation, tried to match it to one of the causes of difficulty described by other people and then we have delivered a solution suitable for that problem. If we did not have the training resources needed for that child, we would generate them there and then, and get immediate feedback on the results. As you do that again and again, you start to see patterns and the process gets easier and easier. In contrast to the academics involved, you will find most hands-on dyslexia specialists will have a toolbox of systems to draw from, because they know that different children need different styles of support. We too have found that there is no single solution to every child. We are constantly open to what might be at the heart of the individual’s difficulty and adjust our response as necessary. Each child who struggles will have an individual mix of problems leading to their individual situation. If you propose an inappropriate intervention you are actually doing more harm than not getting involved, since you will be reinforcing their experience of failure. We are very aware of that and are always very concerned to get it right for each individual. It is only by applying the appropriate intervention for each individual child that you can get routine success. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 19 Collected Strands of Thought So all I have done, in essence, is to collate all the excellent work done by others and to present it as a unified process for helping children with their reading. Newton’s quote of standing on the shoulders of giants was perhaps never more appropriate. Our training system is now a combination of coaching and diagnostics, allowing us to add specific elements of support where necessary for an individual. Along the way we have inevitably made our own discoveries and developed some new innovations. Those have combined to create our new approach, which we call Guided Phonetic Reading. And to deliver Guided Phonetic Reading, we have our online system that we call Easyread. But the truth is that none of this is rocket science and anybody can do it, as you will see as you read on. My hope is that, in time, Guided Phonetic Reading will become a standard approach to literacy and other publishers will produce materials that compete with Easyread. That competition will be good for us and good for the children. The Results that are Possible Do we always get it right? No, sadly not. Or at least not yet, because we always view the system as being a work-in-progress. Seldom a week goes by, even now, without us making some sort of slight adjustment. However, we give a 100% guarantee on our training and we deliver around just 1-2% in refunds. We never, ever dispute a refund. By the way, people often think we must be mad because anybody could rip us off. You will be delighted to hear that it is very Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 20 unusual. I guess if we were selling swag bags the situation might be different! Most of the refunds are for children that we have genuinely failed to engage in our process over the first 2-3 weeks. Good psychology and motivation is very important to its success. Without it we don’t stand much of a hope of success because the children often come to us with extremely negative emotions towards reading. We aim to generate that motivation with a combination of entertainment, praise, reward, challenge and a sense of developing progress. It is a very fragile thing and that is where we still fail most often. With the nature of human variation, it is possible that we will always fail with some children. Anyhow I take a lot of pride in that hit rate. We would love to get a 100% score and I hate ever failing a child, but 98% isn’t bad in a group probably around half of whom would never have learnt to read otherwise. In schools we achieve a 2.3 gain on average and our record is 4 years in reading age gain over six months of training. That means that the children doing Easyread are progressing over twice as fast as an average child (ie. one not having any difficulty). Given that most of the children on Easyread were not progressing at all when they started and had developed a very negative psychology to the whole task, we believe that is also a great figure. With the pressures of the school timetable, it is inevitable that many children are not that regular with their lessons. If you look at the progress per lesson done, it equates to around a 7-fold gain (ie the child gains 7 days in reading age for each lesson completed). So I am confident that what I am presenting here is new and effective. Some people will always dispute whatever you suggest Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 21 (at least in this arena!), but I am happy for anyone to look at our data. Author’s Warning It will be natural for you to discuss all of this with an independent literacy or dyslexia specialist. I must warn you that you may easily get a very sceptical response from some literacy academics. Indeed they may even suggest it cannot be true. However, I think you will be able to see that that response is only natural and might be how any of us would react. First, they have never seen results like the ones I have just quoted. So it is quite reasonable to doubt them. And then all academics feel their reputation is based at least partially on their knowledge. So it is a very unusual and very special academic who easily accepts new information that is at odds with their current knowledge. I can give you numerous examples of entire academic disciplines being held back by this mechanism. Perhaps the most extraordinary is the field of geology, much of which did not accept the theory of continental drift and plate tectonics for the best part of a century, despite literally massive, indeed mountainous evidence! If it is important to you, I can introduce you to highly trained and experienced specialist teachers who now view our approach as their core response to a child needing support. And I can introduce you to any number of parents who have seen Guided Phonetic Reading do its magic. Wherever you are in the world, there will be one not far away. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 22 The Seven Main Causes Of Reading Difficulty “Pareto’s Principle at work: 80% of problems come from 20% of the causes” Introduction The key to helping a child, who is struggling to read, is to understand why it is going wrong for that individual. This is the aetiological approach. To deliver the solution, you need to know the underlying problem in detail. As Sun Tzu said, you must fully evaluate and know your enemy before launching an attack. We have found there are seven main reasons, accounting for at least 95% of reading difficulty. We have practical solutions for all of them. These seven main reasons are: • Auditory deficit • Dyspraxia & Neuro-developmental delay • Irlen syndrome • Low declarative memory capacity • ADHD • Meaning Blindness • Stress Spirals Of these seven key problems, one is by far the most important. Around 80% of poor readers display an auditory deficit. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 23 Let’s just stop and consider that figure. It is very interesting. It means that a solution to the auditory deficit alone could be a solution to 80% of reading difficulty. And we have found that to be roughly the case. That would instantly yield 96% literacy instead of the current 80%. Furthermore, once you have cut down most of the forest, the remaining trees are so much easier to spot and deal with. At the moment, children struggling to read swamp the special needs teachers in schools. Just by dealing with this one issue, auditory deficit, we could change that situation substantially, allowing them to concentrate all their resources on the remaining children. The next most important cause of difficulty is Dyspraxia. We see elements of dyspraxia in around 15% of the children we help. It is definitely possible to alleviate dyspraxia in the majority of cases, with the right exercise regime. The other five causes of reading difficulty are each found in far fewer cases as the main cause of difficulty, but may all contribute quite frequently. They have simple solutions too. So now we are talking about roughly 98% or 99% literacy. You will see that my aim is not an incremental change. It is a revolution in expectation. Auditory Deficit Let’s have a look at each of these all-important underlying causes in turn. For me they are the heart of this whole subject. So what is this “auditory deficit”, which we can now see is the key to the citadel? Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 24 Well, there is a sequence that your brain goes through as you read this text, if you are an average reader. We will explore this in much more detail in the chapter on the neurology of reading, but here is an overview. • First your eyes perceive a small group of 2-3 words. • The images pass down your optic nerve, switches left to right and gets processed in the visual cortex in each hemisphere at the very back of your brain. • The processed signal is then passed to the auditory cortex in your left hemisphere, in from your left ear. Here the letters and letter groups are mapped to the sounds represented and those sounds are blended into words. • Next the words are passed to Wernicke’s area in the linguistic processing cortex, just forwards of the auditory cortex, in from your left temple. • The stream of words is then passed forward to the prefrontal cortex where you actually “think” about the meaning of them. • If you are reading out loud, the information will now go to Broca’s area in the linguistic cortex, in from the back of your left eyebrow, which generates the spoken words that your motor cortex makes your mouth voice. • Finally the eyes jump to the right to take in the next group of words. That movement is known as a saccade and is controlled by the flocculus in the cerebellum. • The process repeats. To get us orientated, here is a picture of the brain sat in the skull: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 25 Cross Section of the Human Head Now let’s see where the key areas for reading are situated: Left view of the brain Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 26 Some people will say, “No, I don’t process the text in that way”. That is potentially true. First, you may be using a visual reading strategy, in which you recognise some words, work out the others from the context and skip the remainder. Unfortunately that means you have not be taught to read well. Even if you were unaware of it, you are constantly working at a disadvantage to someone who can decode every word. And new words and place names are a constant hazard for you. Also, very highly trained readers, like lawyers and academics for instance, can move to a purely visual approach, to increase their speed. They scan the page for elements of meaning. That is how they can read at such a phenomenal rate (around a page a second). Once they see something interesting that they need to read in detail, they will switch to the auditory process. However, you may also be reading in the way I have described, but feel as if you are “recognising” words visually. I can show you that while it might seem that you are recognising the words in this line of text, the process is actually much more subtle than that. You are actually mapping sounds from the letter patterns and blending them. For example, as an exeprinceed rdeaer you can pobbraly raed this snentece qiute ealsiy, diespte the fcat that I hvae meovd all the lertets aonrud, expcet the frist and last. Weird, isn’t it? If we were just “recognising” words visually, then that sentence would be impossible to read fluently. By the way, if you did find it hard to read, then you are reading visually and reading is much harder for you than it needs to be. And the following nonsense words will be very difficult for you: Trubnot Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 27 Sninghack Flissplum A normal reader will be able to read those words out without any difficulty. A purely visual reader will find them very hard. In the History of Literacy Teaching chapter I will describe how some academics have taken the high-speed visual scanning technique I have mentioned and base their literacy instruction on it. The truth is that it is possible to use it for collecting a rough idea of the meaning in a page very quickly, but is not an appropriate strategy for a learner. It is like a rally car driver teaching a new learner-driver to power slide around the corners, as the quickest way to get across town! In any case, the process described contrasts with the processing pattern of children with an auditory deficit, who we already know have a de facto reading difficulty. We know that they are not using their auditory cortex and when we move them over to the more conventional approach to reading, their problems go away. So the empirical evidence is that visual reading strategies clearly cause difficulties. Anyhow, in a person with an auditory deficit the visual signal passes down the optic nerve and is analysed in the visual cortex. It then passes symmetrically forward through the two hemispheres directly to the pre-frontal cortex, bypassing the auditory cortex, which remains dormant. The frontal lobe is where you do most of your higher thinking. Effectively the word cow is processed in virtually the same way as a picture of a cow. Effectively the word is treated as a logogram (a meaning picture). Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 28 Later in the book, in the History of Writing chapter, I will show how that approach is completely at odds with the aim of any text that is constructed with an alphabet. The very basis of our text is that it is a code representing the sounds of the spoken word on paper. So, for the auditory cortex to be bypassed is a negation of the fundamental design of an alphabet script. Letter and Word Flipping It is well known that many people viewed as dyslexic have a tendency to flip letters and words. b switches with d and was switches with saw. The pattern I have described above is the explanation of that. Left and right are surprisingly hard concepts for the brain. We all know people who always confuse them, even as adults. It is something worth knowing about if you are taking directions from them. It seems that you need asymmetry within the brain in order to give some differentiation to left and right. The very asymmetric processing pattern, with the auditory cortex purely in the left hemisphere of the conventional reader, helps to develop the directional stability of the reading process. By contrast, the child with an auditory deficit does not have that. They process each image of the text symmetrically across the two hemispheres. The empirical proof of this theory is that, as we develop the auditory engagement of the child, we usually see this flipping syndrome disappear. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 29 How the Auditory Deficit is Misunderstood Nothing I have said so far is news. The literacy world has long been aware of this situation. But here I am going to diverge sharply from conventional wisdom. Auditory deficit has always been viewed as a major cause of reading difficulty. There is a belief that it is a neurological problem with the auditory cortex. It is a natural conclusion to make, given the evidence. However, in most cases we have found that to be not true. We have found that the auditory deficit is usually a symptom of the reading difficulty, not the root cause. That is a fundamental and crucial difference. It is that misunderstanding that has held back reading intervention. These children have nothing substantially wrong with their auditory ability if they can hear and speak normally. They are just not using their auditory cortex when reading. There is usually no corroborating evidence to make one think that they have a problem with their auditory cortex. And that is because they don’t! Once you understand the real cause, I think you will see that the solution becomes obvious. Step Forward The Real Villain… So here we have our Poirot moment… Everyone is gathered in the salon to hear the answer to the riddle. If the obvious suspect is innocent, who is looking shifty? It is always the most unlikely person, of course. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 30 Poirot turns slowly to point his highly manicured finger at the child’s High Visual Intelligence, stood quietly in the corner of the room. You may be familiar with visual, auditory, kinaesthetic and digital learning styles. If so, skip this next paragraph. The brain has different functions within it and we each vary in our strengths and weaknesses relating to those functions. If your hearing perception and processing is acute, you will tend to use it more and further strengthen it through increased use. Of course you can still see and feel and calculate. But, generally you will use your auditory processing by choice. The same goes with someone who is visual. Kinaesthetic means that you process things more physically through touch and manipulation. And finally, there is digital, for those of us who construct things in a mathematical way. Actually you could go on designating different intelligences all day, but these four are the important ones for this debate. The key point is that a small initial difference in your natural abilities will become more and more pronounced as you repeatedly use your strengths rather than your weaknesses. You can see that mechanism in operation around you all the time. Visual Learner Syndrome We have discovered that visual learners are most at risk of displaying an auditory deficit when reading. They seem to do OK at first, but slowly struggle more and more as the text gets more complicated. They often reach a reading plateau between 6 and 9. Here’s why. In a child’s early years we need to hope that the parents and nursery teacher will be playing with sounds through rhymes and alliteration. This is actually the beginning of reading. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 31 Text is a code to record the sequence of sounds we make when we are speaking. It is an innovation that was developed about 4000 years ago, although some languages like Chinese do not use an Alphabet. So playing with the sounds in words is good preparation for reading. It is by far the most important activity you can engage in with a preschool child. We recommend a game we call Easyread I Spy. It is exactly the same as conventional I Spy except you use the initial sound of the word instead of the initial letter. So you look around the room and see a rug and say, “I spy with my little eye something beginning with rrrr…” It is amazing how much children like playing it. Anyhow, whether or not that sort of thing is happening, children start formal literacy with the alphabet. We use letters derived from an alphabet developed by the Phoenicians. The Greeks adopted the Phoenician alphabet because the letters are so elegant and sparse. However, they were also very abstract. The human brain always struggles to remember abstract shapes. It remembers things by association with existing memories. Something abstract is difficult to make connections to. So, conventionally, you are forced to just memorise the shape. We have changed that now in Easyread, as I will explain, but in a conventional school curriculum the alphabet is normally introduced over 6 months. Even after all that time, many children will struggle to remember all the letters. However, visual learners do well at this task. They are good at visual memorisation. So all will seem to be OK. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 32 Meanwhile, the visual child’s understanding of the related sounds will be much less secure. It is almost impossible to say the actual sound of many letters in isolation. Try saying the letter l sound without saying /luh/. And the hard g sound is even worse. So it is hard to create strong links between the letters and the exact sound of the letter. The sound chanting sessions of a conventional phonics class are very dangerous because they can actually connect a series of incorrect sounds to the letters, which then have to be forgotten again for successful blending. For instance, the chanted sounds for d and a can lead to dad being pronounced like duradder The visual child with a relatively weak auditory facility will also tend to find these little sounds very hard to hear in speech. And so none of it will make much sense. I was having dinner with some friends the other day and a pianist amongst the group was listening to the music playing. He picked out the notes being used in a rather unusual chord and could instantly play the tune on the piano behind him. For most of us it was astonishing (and yes, vaguely annoying!) because we were completely unable to differentiate the notes in the chords. Our ears had not been trained to do that. In the same way, the visual child will not be naturally hearing the sounds within a word. So letter-sound relationships will be a baffling concept and the letter names will be much more comfortable for the child. During this early reading instruction simple Consonant-VowelConsonant (CVC) words are introduced. In the early stages a small vocabulary will be used, drawn from the words with just short vowel sounds. So bed, bad, hid, him and rod are all fine. And ball, same, line and rule will be avoided as being later concepts. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 33 You will be amazed how limiting that is for your vocabulary. In this whole paragraph only 5 words use these initially taught sounds for every letter. So the same words will appear again and again. With this small number of words, the highly visual child will just memorise them all visually, like pictures. In the “real books” and “whole word” reading systems, this is actually taught as the fundamental reading process. So, for a bright visual learner it is definitely possible to pick up a little vocabulary of simple words quite quickly. Then they can apparently “read” them rather proficiently. They will even be a bit quicker than a child who is struggling to decode each word. So all will still seem well at this stage. In fact the child will be praised. Next, he or she will start taking home “early reader books”. It is natural and apparently logical for these books to start with simple text and a clear picture relating to the text. Logically…what else would you do? More on that in a while, but the vocabulary will often be recycled through each page as well, because that makes it “easier” for the child to seem to read. But now you will be able to see what I am about to say. This is feeding into the same strategy for the children. They can quickly memorise these words and, for bright children particularly, it is easy to guess the ones they don’t know. The context of the sentence and the picture make it obvious most of the time. They will often use the first letter of the word as a cue as well. So, all will seem well. However it is usually now apparent that a new word is always a challenge for the child. Do you see what is happening? Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 34 The child has set off down an alternative path, that seems easier than all the phonics and decoding that is being explained at school. That is laborious and hard work in comparison to recalling a word from memory or guessing. Those are both instantaneous. Decoding a word can take several seconds when you are learning. If you go to help with reading practice in a primary school, you will often find the same book being presented again and again by a child. I can remember reading with a very bright boy when my oldest son was in reception class. He was sent over to read to me and set off through the book, making the odd mistake but generally doing well. However, on about the fifth page he made no sense at all. It took a few moments to work out what was going on. He was reciting the sixth page not the fifth... He knew the entire book by heart! Anyhow, in less extreme cases, the child is just reading in what seems the easiest way. The child is unaware that this nifty shortcut usually leads to a dead end. During phonics teaching sessions the child will happily ignore what is going on, as incomprehensible and unnecessary, when there is an easier approach. It will be very hard for the teacher to pick up on this because the child will often be able to answer a question when asked. In a class of 30 you can go through a lesson without being asked anything at all. And the average teacher manages only around 10 minutes one-on-one with each child in the class each week. And that is for everything, not just reading. It may not sound much, but it adds up to 5 hours straight, without a coffee break. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 35 So the teacher and parents will seem fairly at ease with things when the child is 4 and 5 years old. The first serious indications of problems will appear somewhere between 6 and 7. This is when the text starts to get a bit more complex. The relationship between the picture and the text gets less clear. And the extent of the vocabulary starts to increase substantially. Now the child will be forced to guess more and more. Because it is getting harder to guess accurately, there will be more mistakes. Often the guess will be quite wild, with only the first letter in common with the word on the page. You may have seen that happening. The child is baffled by how much harder it is becoming, rather than easier. After two or three years of effort a negative psychology begins to creep in, aided by the evident frustration of parents and teachers. It is only natural. Having worked hard it seems unfair and bewildering to find reading just getting more difficult rather than easier. The child has also now become aware that many friends have moved onto higher-level books. Children are intensely aware of these things, as you know. So the child heads onto a plateau of glacial progress with increasing stress levels and no clear path to follow out of this predicament. Sometimes they will actually go into reverse as their confidence collapses. So, it’s ironic isn’t it? A sharp mind and a high intelligence in one area can lead to such pain, frustration and low self-esteem. Once you see it, the evidence is all around you. Go into an art school and you will find huge numbers of dyslexics who never read. Go into the music school down the road and you Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 36 will all find them reading books over a cup of tea. In fact, savvy book marketers have always used mailing lists of music buyers! Proof of the Visual Learner Syndrome Process I am going to try to show you the process I have described, by teaching you to read again. I hope that I will be able to entice you to use the same strategies that the visual learner uses. Of course you can fight it if you wish and buck the experiment. But if you let yourself flow with the process, I think you will see what I mean. Here is the Greek alphabet: αβχδεφγηιϕκλµνοπθρστυϖ ωξψζ Some of the letters are familiar and others are less easy. So it is close to the situation of a 5-year-old who has been recently introduced to the alphabet in English. Let’s now do some reading in Greek symbols: Τηε δογ ηασ α βαλλ (the dog has a ball) Ηε ηασ α βαλλ (he has a ball) Τηε χατ ισ ον τηε µατ (the cat is on the mat) Τηε χατ ισ σοφτ (the cat is soft) Τηε βαλλ ισ σοφτ (the ball is soft) Τηε δογ κιχκσ τηε βαλλ (the dog kicks the ball) Τηε χατ κιχκσ τηε σοφτ βαλλ (the cat kicks the soft ball) Good. Now have a quick glance at this sentence, without working it all out: Τηε δογ σιτσ ον τηε µατ ανδ χατχηεσ τηε σοφτ, ψελλοω βαλλ Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 37 Could you feel yourself visually drawn to the words you knew? They stand out to you as a pattern, even through the rest of the sentence is using letters that you also know. Pattern recognition is a very strongly developed visual strength of the brain. That is exactly what these children are doing too. They are naturally good at spotting familiar patterns. So the words δογ, σοφτ, βαλλ and µατ were probably quite clear. OK. Now have a look at this picture: Now read the sentence again looking at all the familiar words and then focus on the first letters of the unfamiliar words (χα=ca, ψε=ye and σι=si): Τηε δογ σιτσ ον τηε µατ ανδ χατχηεσ τηε σοφτ, ψελλοω βαλλ Could you feel the next process starting to operate? You knew the context and you knew your initial letters and so it was easy to guess the words you didn’t know. You had: The dog si__ on the mat and ca______ the soft, ye___ ball Amazing! You just read the tricky word catches, without seeing it before... Or maybe you said “caught”, which is the sort of guess you will often hear from a child with visual learner syndrome. Before we move on I want to give you a feeling of the emotional impact of text on the struggling learner. This is what it looks like: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 38 Τηε δογσ ωερε ρυσηινγ αφτερ τηε σοφτ βαλλ λικε µαδ τηινγσ, βαρκινγ αλλ τηε ωαψ. Τηε βαλλ γοτ χοϖερεδ ιν σλιµψ γοο αν δ εϖεντυαλλψ τηεψ πυνχτυρεδ ιτ. Σο ωε γοτ α λογ ανδ υσεδ τ ηατ ινστεαδ. Τηερε ωασ α χατ ωατχηινγ φροµ ηισ µατ. Ηε ε ϖιδεντλψ τηουγητ τηεψ ωερε χραζψ. What is your emotion as you look at that? For me it is like a drowning man looking for any strategy that will make it easier to survive! Can you feel the urge to look for familiar words and doesn’t guessing seem like a good option? It is possible for children to have actual auditory difficulties. However you will be aware of those because their speech and hearing will be affected. Learning to read is very hard for deaf children. It is far harder for them than it is for blind children, for instance. However, if the child has no known auditory difficulties and yet displays an auditory deficit when reading, it is almost certainly due to visual learner syndrome and can be fixed easily. Dyspraxia & Neuro-Developmental Delay We think of the main part of the brain as being the grey matter in our cerebrum. And that is where we do all our thinking. However, low down at the back of the brain is the cerebellum. Even though it is only the size of two small squashed tangerines, and you can function without it, the cerebellum has half the neurones of the entire brain. It accepts all the sensory input (except the olfactory nerves of the nose), it decides what is important and filters out what can be ignored. It also channels all the motor neurones to our muscles. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 39 So virtually nothing gets in or out, without being processed to a degree in the cerebellum. Dyspraxia is caused by the poor functioning of the cerebellum. You will see a child with severe dyspraxia very easily distracted by noises, movement and itches. The child will also seem poorly coordinated, consistently dropping plates and knocking things over. Please note that all children do go through periods of poor coordination anyhow as they grow. This can also be linked to neuro-developmental delay. As you develop from a newborn baby to an adult various programmed responses change. As a baby for instance you will grip anything put into your hand. As an adult, that should stop. Equally, as a baby you have no ability to stand or sit unsupported. As an adult you have automatic responses, which allow you to hold your posture without thinking about it. There are various factors that can affect this development of neuro-responses during the growth of the child. However you may be completely unaware of any of this and yet see it affecting a child’s reading. Even mild dyspraxia can disrupt your reading ability. The reason is that the eyes do a series of little jumps along the line of text, called saccades. So you need good control of your eyeballs to do this. We find around 15% of the children we work with display some dyspraxia. Astronauts are often unable to read on their return from space as well, for this reason. The pull of gravity messes up their balance and eye coordination. They have to go through a period of daily exercises to fix the problem. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 40 The DORE Story There is an interesting story related to dyspraxia. A man called Winfred Dore had a child who was apparently so frustrated by her dyslexia that she kept trying to kill herself. He was a successful businessman who had made a fortune selling industrial coatings (paint). So he decided to make it his life mission to find a solution to his child’s reading difficulty. Eventually he found someone with an explanation and a series of exercises that he was told would help. And sure enough they did. His daughter learnt to read. So, with the best intentions I am sure, Dore decided to bring this “miracle dyslexia cure” to the world. He developed a diagnostic process and series of exercises to cure people and was extremely good at creating PR to support his project under the name DORE. His initial consultation cost £500 (for later comparison the current price of a Mars Bar roughly 50 pence and so he was selling his consultation for 1000 Mars Bars). And a year of exercises cost £2000 (ie 4000 Mars Bars). Tens of thousands of people took his course and some had great results. They were the ones who had dyspraxia. Unfortunately the exercises were irrelevant for anybody without dyspraxia. Of course the rest did the exercises and presumably had no improvement at all. We don’t know because DORE always refused to show their statistics. Literacy academics began to question Dore’s claims and ask to see his figures. They were never published and the negative stories began to build and build. People have said that it was difficult to claim on his guarantee because you had to prove that you had Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 41 been doing the exercises prescribed every day – an impossible request. In the end the DORE reading organisation went bust in 2008. The organisation has been refinanced by one of their happy customers. And they now have much more limited claims on their website about what they can achieve for people and no suggestion of a guarantee. Irlen syndrome Helen Irlen noticed in the 1980s that some children are particularly sensitive to the contrast of black text on a white background. You know how difficult it is to read red text on a blue background? For children with Irlen syndrome black on white is like that. The text shimmers. You will hear the child say that the text is “moving around”. It makes it very difficult to focus on and read. The visual cortex is programmed to emphasise changes of contrast, to pick out patterns. The tiger’s face in the long grass was very useful to be able to spot! Irlen syndrome seems to be an overapplication of this facility. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 42 Low Short-Term Memory Capacity Reading makes tremendous requirements on our working memory, especially when you are learning to read. If your working memory capacity is small, reading will seem much harder. Your short-term memory has very defined limits. For instance, I can remember 6 digits in a sequence pretty much every time, but not 7. With 7 I just get confused. I don’t remember the first 6 and not the last one, I get lost after 2 or 3. You can have a go yourself. Cover up the column of numbers below and then reveal the top one for long enough to read it. Then cover it again and write down what the number was. Reveal it again and see if you got it right. Then reveal the next number and do the same thing: 42760 94815 80321 356297 843069 982541 6378405 7951428 9387540 89475261 96740325 60591273 How did you get on? Could you feel your limit arrive with quite sharp definition? One moment you have it under control, the next you have lost it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 43 Well, the same can effectively happen with the reading process. If there is too much trying to go on in the child’s working memory it easily turns to chaos. What you will observe is the child struggling with decoding each word and then being unable to remember anything of the sentence, once it has been read. That inevitably makes the whole procedure pretty boring. Once the child’s reading is more advanced, you will continue to see them struggle with longer words and the more complex sentences. This all combines to make reading a drag for them. So they do as little as possible and with a heavy heart. ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder is an extraordinary phenomenon that I would love to understand better. When I was at school there was nobody that I knew of who suffered from ADHD. Now some schools have 10% of their children on medication and it is growing at 12% per annum! Those are huge figures. In another ten years they indicate one in five children being on ADHD medication. That means that there must be some sort of environmental factor leading to this growth. The obvious places to look are nutrition, nurture patterns during early life, sleep patterns, exercise patterns and major activities like television. The nature of the drugs used is very interesting. I always imagined that they were suppressants. In reality they are stimulants, very similar in formulation to cocaine. The reason is that the frontal lobe of a child with ADHD is not applying sufficient control over the rest of the cerebrum to Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 44 maintain focus. Once the medication is absorbed, the activity in the frontal lobe increases which then brings order to the rest of the cerebral cortex. ADHD clearly makes learning to read difficult because reading requires a steady focus over a period of minutes. It is virtually impossible to learn to read successfully without that. And learning to read is a particularly hard activity for children with ADHD because the gratification (from progress) is so long delayed. Word Meaning Blindness You will find some children are able to read text out quite proficiently, but are unable to understand what they have read out. By that I mean that they understand nothing at all, even at the simplest level. So this is different from short-term memory issues. The child can read out “point to the red box” and be unable to follow the instruction. The reason for this is that the language creation cortex (Broca’s area) is being engaged, but not the language comprehension cortex (Wernicke’s area). So it is normally an added symptom of visual reading strategies. There are cases of adults who have had a stroke and sustained damage to Wernicke’s area. This has left them able to speak and write, but quite unable to understand the spoken word or read. So they can write something down and be unable to read it back to you. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 45 Stress Spirals The brain has very fixed responses to stress. In an evolutionary sense, stress was usually related to fear, which was related to danger from large animals and people. The best responses to danger are fight, flight or immobility and the higher thinking areas of the frontal lobe tend to get shut down during moments of stress so that the brain stem can control these three possible responses. For reading, that is bad news. The stress that children feel when getting something wrong, can then reduce their ability to read the next word. This is often increased by the reaction of the person they are reading with. Eventually their stress spirals out of control and you end up with a complete shutdown of higher mental processes and a purely emotional response. You may have seen that in action. And I can prove the process to you right here and now. I would like you to add up the following four numbers: 4+3+3+7. How hard was that? How many seconds did it take you? Around 4-5 seconds is comfortable. OK, well done. Now I want you to imagine yourself cooped up in a windowless basement with a deranged mathematics teacher who believes that mental arithmetic has been woefully sidelined since the advent of the calculator. For your own benefit he has abducted you and he is now going to make you learn to add, whatever it takes. He is sweating slightly and you are now strapped to a chair with electrodes connected to the mains electricity supply. He is a DIY interrogation specialist as well. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 46 Are you there? Can you feel the sweat on your palms? Now he is shouting at you “You have FIVE seconds! WHAT IS 3 PLUS 5 PLUS 4 PLUS 2?” His spit is all over your face. Quick, quick, what is the answer… He bangs the table shouting “FOUR… THREE… TWO… “ Can you feel your brain scrambling? Anyhow, stress can be a very significant factor in the ability to read in the early stages. And the more a child feels inadequate to the task, the higher the level of stress developed and the harder the task becomes. I know that as a parent it can be VERY hard not to contribute to this. I definitely did with my two boys in the 1990’s. So it may seem exaggerated but I would place the role and influence of psychology within Easyread at 50% of the course. If we have not got an enthusiastic and relaxed child then our task becomes almost impossible. I have another interesting personal example of this. My first son was finding maths very hard when he was about 9. He was doing badly and his maths teacher had taken to shrieking at the bottom quarter of her class that they were lazy no-goods who were going to ruin her career. As you can tell, stress was taking its toll on her performance too! And she was right about her career. Anyhow, he did badly and hated every minute of it. The following year he had a different teacher and at tea towards the end of the first week he said to us (and I quote virtually verbatim) “I don’t know why I found maths so hard… it’s really pretty easy”. We had to pick our jaws up off the floor. Now this new teacher (who was an ace teacher all-round) had not really done any teaching that week. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 47 All he had done was make them feel good about how much they knew and how well they were doing. For him, that was obviously the basis to work from. And he got great results with all of them. The previous year still had its costs. My son never really understood fractions and algebra until taking his physics exams at 17. It looked as if this missing part of his knowledge might wreck his whole result. So he finally got to grips with what the scary mad woman had failed to elucidate for him. Your mental state is critical for all learning and especially with a highly demanding task like early reading. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 48 Cures and Solutions for the 7 Main Causes of Reading Difficulty Now that we know the enemy, let’s have a look at the ways we can defeat each of these causes of dismay and failure. How to Cure Visual Learner Syndrome OK, we can now see the most common cause of the auditory deficit. The solution becomes quite obvious. We need to reengineer the reading process for the child so that it follows the conventional path through the auditory cortex. The difficulty is that the visual to auditory link is normally quite weak and the visual to frontal link is always very strong. In addition, by the time people become aware of a problem, the child has been working on reading for several years and has wellestablished habits that do get some results with minimum effort. So existing habits have to be made unattractive and the new approach has to be made easy. A half-hearted attempt will not deliver the result needed. I believe the solution to Visual Learner Syndrome has three main elements: • Use the visual memory properly • Give the right tools for success during short, daily lessons • Force the new neuron patterns Let’s look at each of those in more detail. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 49 How to Use The Visual Memory It is one of our greatest facilities; the human brain can remember an almost limitless amount of visual data. Almost every child has a good visual memory and the children of our main target group all have excellent visual memories. So this is a strength that we can leverage. We can play right into the visual child’s comfort zone. We have always had imagery used in phonics teaching, but you have to do it the right way. Apple, banana, carrot… just doesn’t work. It is too boring. To remember an image easily it needs to be bright, active, unusual and engaging. A picture of an apple beside an “a” isn’t any of those things. A picture of the Ants in Pink Pants clambering over the “a”, roped together as mountaineers, with one falling off is bright, active, unusual and engaging. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 50 The ants in pink pants on the letter a What happens is that the letter now becomes a location where this strange event or scene took place. It becomes the memory hook to it. As soon as the child sees the letter there is an instant recall of those crazy little ants. And crucially, that instantly gives the child the phoneme for the letter effortlessly. In a moment we will also come onto what we do when the letter actually represents a quite different phoneme, in words like waste, was, said and saw. One of the processes that the brain is terribly good at is recognising landmarks on a route somewhere. If you have driven a road to a Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 51 friend’s house, a series of landmarks will get lodged in your mind (eg, the big white house where you turn left) so that you can retrace the route a second time. We are using the same facility here. With this approach we can teach the average child most of the alphabet in around 6 sessions. Conventionally it takes 6 months. So there is a quantum leap in performance (2,500%). Isn’t that exiting? I love quantum leaps! Imagine what school would be like if you could make all of it 25 times easier. That is what is sometimes possible when you understand and harness the child’s internal strengths and work with them. We also get around the unpronounceable nature of the individual phonemes. We almost never try to voice them. You will hear classes of children chanting the sounds of letters in conventional phonics classes. It is actually very hard to get right. As a result, you get a word like lad pronounced luh-a-duh, which is closer to luradder than lad. With Easyread the child has an image and the name of the image to work with. So that problem goes away. This means that highly visual children can quickly become more comfortable with using phonics and start to develop an understanding of the structure of words. Spelling This engagement with the internal structure of the words has a secondary benefit and that is it helps a child to become a reasonable speller. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 52 We have quite a few parents come to us saying, “my child can read OK, but her spelling is atrocious. She can do a spelling test fine, but a week later it has all gone.” The problem here is with the girl’s reading. She is a visual reader, memorising words. What is fairly easy for her to recognise is far harder for her to reproduce. There are all sorts of things we can recognise with ease but would be hard pushed to reproduce on paper from memory with much accuracy in the detail. The reason she can pass a spelling test is that she has a semiphotographic memory for the words overnight, but that then fades over the following days. Once we get her reading going the right way, spelling becomes much easier. The child becomes engaged and familiar with the internal structure of the words and is then able to rebuild them. 2 – The Right Tools How should a child learn to read? By reading? No, we have found that to be counterproductive initially, although we do switch to daily reading practice after our foundation is built. Getting a learner to read before building the foundation tends to over face the child and leads to serial failure. Each time a child gets stuck on a word while reading with you, it is a moment of failure in their eyes. That is why they are not usually keen on reading practice. Their pride and self-esteem takes a constant bashing. However, if you give them a series of tasks that they can do, they love it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 53 So the key is to break the reading process down, so that you can exercise the components of it. Then give the child the tools needed to be able to succeed. Easyread TrainerText is the key for us in this process. TrainerText shows the text with the phonetic images floating above the words. Here is an example: Easyread TrainerText The word gas has a regular pronunciation. So we have the goat in a boat, the ant in pink pants and the snake with a shake above the three letters. You would expect has to rhyme with gas. But it doesn’t. So when the child reads it and it doesn’t make sense, the Zuto from Pluto is floating above the “s” to indicate what the right pronunciation is. And was is even stranger. Now you have the octopus who knocked a puss above the “a”. This simple device revolutionises the reading experience for the children. It empowers them to be able to read without help. It allows them to practise decoding even unfamiliar words. Subdivide The Process We get the child to practise initially with exercises that are less demanding than straight reading. It is a bit like doing your scales on a piano. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 54 For instance, in our Easyread Mushroom Picker game we read a word out and ask the child to pick the right one when it appears on the screen (that is the mushroom and the rest are toadstools). They have to read the words, but it is not as taxing as having to read them from scratch. There is something that always amazes me when I am climbing a mountain, or even a hill. You keep taking these little steps. And after an hour you look back down this big drop to the valley floor as if you have leapt up there like a giant. Learning stuff should be the same. If each step is small and easy, then you can learn almost anything. I believe great teachers have two key abilities: • To be able to inspire • To be able to break difficult concepts into easily understood elements. One of the conundrums of teaching is that the more easily you understand a subject yourself, the harder the second part is. We should all teach the subjects we find hardest really. The Right Learning Rhythm It is crucial to keep any literacy lessons short and regular. We recommend a daily lesson of around 5-15 minutes. If you try to teach a child reading for too long, the child’s concentration will drop. And if you don’t do a lesson every day, it is very difficult to build momentum. Repetition with sleep between each repetition is one of the pillars of good learning practice. People are often doubtful that we can achieve much with such a short lesson. Many of us have a puritan feeling that we must do Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 55 more work in order to progress faster. With early reading practice that is definitely counterproductive. 3 - Force the New Synaptic Connections When we are helping children with their reading we are usually rewiring the reading process in their heads. The “Evil Twins” (Memorisation and Guessing) tempt children off into the wild woods of reading. They make the process seem easier and so divert bright kids with masses of potential down a path that slowly gets harder and harder to follow. Eventually they leave the children tired, lost and sad in a dark place. All we are doing is leading them back onto the main track, around the outside of the wood. So we need to put some fencing in place to keep the children out of the woods. This is the third element of a successful system. In Easyread we do that through the structure of our games. For instance, in the mushroom picking game that I mentioned, we present toadstools that look very similar to the mushrooms (a bit like real mushroom picking!). So, if the child snaps to a guess because the word looks roughly right, they will certainly lose eventually. And we are harsh on mistakes. They have to go back to the beginning of the game. Somehow you have to make decoding words the easiest and most immediately successful approach. Eventually, we present text to the child and this sort of fencing becomes impossible. So, our final strategy is to set it up as a challenge “Can you read this without guessing?” It is amazing how energised most children are by a challenge. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 56 With these policies in place, we find that very nearly every child has the capability of developing phonetic decoding. They then drop their guessing and start building confidence. How long that takes to establish varies, but is generally between 1 and 6 months. Helping Dyspraxia Dyspraxia is caused by the weak processing of the motor control of muscles by the cerebellum. So we are not re-engineering the process, but we are exercising neural connections in order to lift the performance of the particular circuits we need for reading. There are plenty of more global treatments for dyspraxia, but we confine ourselves to just getting the eyeball control heightened sufficiently to let reading progress smoothly. Dore was right that the cerebellum does improve with exercise, like almost any part of the brain. We use a simple exercise to improve a child’s eye tracking control: • Sit with a pen held vertically at arm’s length. • Move the pen back and forth from left to right while tracking it with your eyes. • Keep your head still. • Once this becomes easy, do the same thing standing on one foot. This will exercise the neurone pathways to the muscles controlling the movement of the eyes. That will then help them navigate the lines of text as well. It is simple but effective. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 57 In order to get the best results, you should aim to do this 10-15 times a day, for around 60 seconds each time. It does not have to be anything formal. Being sat on the lavatory is an obvious opportunity! That is one of the advantages of keeping it so simple. Many of the apparently more technical treatments for dyspraxia and neurodevelopmental delay are ten-minute sessions that it is only practical to do once or twice a day. I find that our very simple routine gets far quicker results because it can be done once or twice an hour rather than daily. We also have software built into Easyread that exercises the child’s eye tracking skills in a more entertaining way. Working with Irlen Syndrome Irlen syndrome is caused by sensitivity to the contrast between black text and a white background. The normal solution to Irlen Syndrome is to change the contrast of the black on white in some way. That can be achieved with coloured paper or coloured transparent film placed over the paper or coloured reading glasses. If you think that your child is suffering from Irlen syndrome get some sheets of coloured acetate and lay them over the page. Different colours will have varying impacts on it. If you find any of them help, then you can go to the next stage and find an optician who specialises in Irlen syndrome. They can test different colours more scientifically and potentially make reading glasses with the optimum colour for your child. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 58 Dealing with a Limited Short-Term Memory Capacity I have a poor short-term memory and after years of working it, it is still poor! So there is no magic bullet to this, but I do have two bits of good news. First, your brain is very plastic. If you use a facility in it, it will usually strengthen at least a bit. If I got you to do that number sequence exercise every day, you would get better at it, up to a point. Reading itself is a very good exercise for developing your working memory. So perseverance will lead to some improvement. And there is a second, rather brighter light at the end of the tunnel. The decoding process will become automatic with practice. There are two types of memory; declarative and procedural. Declarative is the “thinking” memory that you have voluntary control over and procedural is your automatic memory for running routine responses. Most everyday tasks are run in your procedural memory. That is why you can constantly do things without thinking about them. As you read this you are not thinking about how to read. It is like riding a bicycle or driving a car. The reason sports players and musicians practise so much is that they are pushing all of the procedures of the activity into their procedural memory. It is sometimes referred to in this context as “muscle memory”, although the muscles are actually totally memory-free. All of these automated responses are run from the central nervous system, which is made up of the brain, the spinal chord and the eyes. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 59 The spinal chord actually does quite a lot of clever stuff. For instance, when you trip on something with your right leg it automatically starts to lift, the left leg starts to press down and your arms start to stretch out to save you, before the brain is even aware of the problem. If you are a cat lover, skip this paragraph… but you can put a cat with no brain at all on a treadmill and it will match its walking or running to the pace of the mill, entirely as an automatic response to the sensations in its paws and muscles linking to circuits in the spinal chord. This is also the explanation of why the English football team players often perform so badly, even though we know they are all talented. Playing for their clubs they are relaxed and are depending on their procedural memory for each pass and shot at goal. However, as they run out onto the field in an England shirt their whole psychology changes and they start to try too hard. They are worried about the panning they are going to get in the press for being overpaid and lazy. So they start to think about their play. That makes them end up looking like a bunch of schoolboys. Their errors may look like sloppy, lazy play, but it is actually quite the reverse. In fact the key to almost all ball sports at the top levels is being able to remain in the procedural memory zone, whatever the external tensions of the moment. Managing that is one of the most important roles of a good coach or team captain. It is easily measured in tennis. Often the weaker player will have equally good strokes, but will tend to lose more of the break points. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 60 Anyhow, our aim is for reading to move from a declarative memory task to a procedural memory task. That then frees the declarative memory for concentration on the semantic meaning of the text. It is good daily practice that will achieve this. In Easyread we give the child the tools to be able to practice reading successfully and therefore start pushing it into procedural memory. In addition, our Guided Phonetic Reading process places a lower demand on the child’s memory than reading conventional text. So it frees up declarative memory during the early stages too. However, it is important to know that improvement is very incremental. Each day there will be some small progress. And the final level achieved, in terms of fluency, will vary from child to child and by how much the child keeps up a daily programme of reading practice. Solutions to ADHD With ADHD we know that the lack of activity in the frontal lobe is the root of the problem. Medication is one option, of course. But I am always against medication unless it is absolutely necessary. Where possible I think it is always better to get the body to deal with its own difficulties. The real key to ADHD is to train up the frontal lobe to do its job. That can be done by engaging it and exercising it. Many of the activities of the modern world, and television in particular do the reverse. They deliver stimulation to all of the brain except the frontal lobe. Did you know that you expend more energy in your brain staring at a blank wall than you do watching low quality television? Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 61 We have found ADHD is not a serious problem when we are working with children, because we are engaging their focus with entertainment. That achieves the same aim as the drugs. Good motivation and the stimulation of the prefrontal cortex will get it to apply sufficient control over the rest of the brain for a period for daily reading practice to take place successfully. Once a child can read it becomes good brain exercise and I would imagine is an aid to reducing ADHD. The Solution to Word Meaning Blindness We know that word meaning blindness is caused by the reader not engaging Wernicke’s area, of the linguistic cortex, when reading. The solution to this situation is to engage the auditory cortex in the process, which then connects naturally to Wernicke’s area. In Easyread we do that by delivering exercises that force the visual cortex to link through to the primary auditory cortex, through the Angular Gyrus. This does effectively require a re-engineering of the reading process in the same was as with the auditory deficit. That can seem hard when the child is apparently fluent with their current reading strategy. But if a positive result is the aim, I do not think that there is a real alternative. Solution to Stress Spirals To stop stress getting out of control there are two options. You can either train the child to deal with stress or remove the source of the stress. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 62 We aim for a combination of the two. We make the process easier, we explain that it is normal for there to be moments when it is still hard and we switch the focus from reading failure to game failure, which is a more familiar scenario for the child. We also aim to switch the relationship from child-textparent/teacher, to child-TrainerText with the parent and teacher as an assistant on the side. The TrainerText acts as a private coach and delivers solutions for the child without the child having to announce a difficulty. With the help of the TrainerText each decoding of a new or tricky word becomes a moment of achievement, rather than failure. So we aim to build a success spiral leading to ever increasing confidence, rather than a stress spiral into failure and unhappiness. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 63 Dyslexia Introduction There is probably no single term that is more contentious in education than dyslexia. I am not going to try to resolve that here. I will only give you the situation and my personal view of the use of the term, for what it is worth. It first came into general use in the 1970s and quickly became a battleground in the public sphere, with some people taking the view that it did not exist. Professor Meredith of the University of Leeds referred to it as “The unidentified flying object of psychology”. Many have been very publicly on his side across the spectrum of society. Meanwhile, others have made a series of detailed studies of the variation in reading ability of different children and reported unexplained differences. These are normally put down to the often equally unexplained phenomenon of dyslexia. The more common technical term is “specific learning difficulties”(SPLD). And predating that was the term “word blindness”, which was first reported by James Kerr in 1896. Many of the telltale patterns relating to reading difficulties were soon logged by researchers, such as Hinshelwood (1900, 1902, 1904 and 1907), Rutherford (1909), Orton (1925, 1937), Hallgren (1950), Miles (1961), Bakker and Satz (1970), Naidoo (1972). In the UK local dyslexia associations were set up in the late 60s and the British Dyslexia Association was formed in 1972 and held their first conference in 1989. Despite all of this, there is still little agreement on even what the term dyslexia means. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 64 The Compact Oxford English Dictionary states it as meaning “A disorder involving difficulty in learning to read or interpret words, letters, and other symbols”. At the other end of the scale, the Dyslexia Handbook 2008/9, published by the British Dyslexia Association defines dyslexia as: “Dyslexia is a specific learning difficulty that mainly affects the development of literacy and language related skills. It is likely to be present at birth and to be life-long in its effects. It is characterised by difficulties with phonological processing, rapid naming, working memory, processing speed, and the automatic development of skills that may not match up to an individual’s other cognitive abilities. It tends to be resistant to conventional teaching methods, but its effects can be mitigated by appropriately specific intervention, including the application of information technology and supportive counselling.” In their view this is a “scientific” definition rather than a dictionary definition as used by the layman. You will know by now that I cannot agree with the nature of this use of the word. You will know that I view reading difficulty as being caused by a number of specific problems with the reading process. These problems are quite distinct and unrelated. If we are to have an umbrella term (and that is evidently useful as shorthand), then it must remain relevant to what is under the umbrella. The specific patterns of the different forms of reading difficulty that we see are exactly that; specific to the individual cause of reading difficulty. To draw them into the definition of the umbrella term can be deeply misleading and confusing. I am also very unhappy with the rather defeatist nature of the BDA’s definition. Right there in the definition they are talking about “mitigation” with IT and counselling. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 65 What that means is that you give the child a computer that reads text out loud and you tell the child it is OK not to be able to read and that people lead very happy lives in that position. That is a sticking-plaster solution to a gunshot wound. So I personally prefer the definition “Specific difficulty with reading relative to the person’s broader cognitive abilities”. The term has use in distinguishing specific reading difficulty from general learning difficulties. It also distinguishes it from straight illiteracy, which can be caused by environmental factors such as a lack of education. Here is the definition used by the American Dyslexia Association: “A dyslexic person of good or average intelligence perceives his environment in a different way, and his attention diminishes when confronted by letters or numbers. Due to a deficiency in his partial performances, his perception of these symbols differs from that by non-dyslexic people. This results in difficulties when learning to read, write and do arithmetic.“ That means that we are in a situation where a child can learn to read well and yet still be defined as dyslexic by some people. On that score, I might easily be assessed as dyslexic. That is fine, but personally I think we need a different word for that because it is such a qualitative assessment, whereas reader/nonreader is binary and is of massively greater significance to the individual. It is a bit like having the same word for a boat that does not sail that well as one that sinks when placed in the water. Taking the sailing simile further, many of the design details of a poor sailing boat are taken as symptoms of the problem. For instance, artistic ability is often linked to dyslexia. I think you can now see that artistic ability is linked to strong visual skills, Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 66 which can lead to what we call visual learner syndrome. If that is fixed, there is no intrinsic reason why an artist should not be as literate as Shakespeare. In the same way, tub-like sailing boats tend to sail badly. So you could say a large living area is a sign of poor sailing ability. But that is not true. Nelson’s flagship the Victory has a huge salon, but sailed very well. In the UK the government commissioned a report by Jim Rose on the whole subject. This is his definition of dyslexia, which I like: “Dyslexia is a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling.” He also notes: “ Characteristic features of dyslexia are difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory and verbal processing speed. Dyslexia occurs across the range of intellectual abilities. It is best thought of as a continuum, not a distinct category, and there are no clear cut-off points. Co-occurring difficulties may be seen in aspects of language, motor co-ordination, mental calculation, concentration and personal organisation, but these are not, by themselves, markers of dyslexia. A good indication of the severity and persistence of dyslexic difficulties can be gained by examining how the individual responds or has responded to well-founded intervention.” The Symptoms of Dyslexia The patterns that I have described relating to the causes of reading difficulty tend to be quoted as the symptoms of dyslexia. For instance, an auditory deficit will coexist with phonological difficulties. It will also often be seen in the very visual “rightPre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 67 brained”, left-handed, artistic and creative people that are so famously associated with dyslexia. Low short-term declarative memory capacity will be linked to other difficulties with working memory and organisational ability. But they will not necessarily show any of the symptoms listed in the two paragraphs above. This is why hunting for any general set of symptoms of “dyslexia” is very dangerous and confusing, to my mind. All you can do is hunt for the patterns of specific causes of reading difficulty. I believe that this has led to a lot of confusion with parents and teachers. Even some literacy specialists do not seem to be clear on it. They want dyslexia to be just one thing when there are really several quite distinct forms of dyslexia. Spelling We help a surprising number of children who are felt to be reading OK, but have great difficulty spelling. But poor spelling is often a clear indication of poor reading technique and dyslexia. Usually they can do well in a spelling test, but are unable to spell the same words a week later. This is almost always a reflection of an auditory deficit. If they are visual readers, they have the ability to remember the rough layout of a word, but they are not truly engaged at all with the detailed interior structure of the word. So, for a spelling test they will use a virtually photographic memory to exactly reproduce the words. However, with time that breaks down. In an attempt to recreate the word they will often build it using the most basic phonetic use of the letters. For instance a word like aunt might be written rnt. And they will write words that Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 68 are almost right, but not quite, like corect or carrect for the word correct. However, once we get them decoding words, analysing the structure to the words and activating their auditory cortex, this begins to change. There is a lag between the two, but there is a clear link. The brain maps letter patterns to sounds in a surprisingly sophisticated way. I can give you a couple of examples of this. First, if I mix up the leterts in thsee wrods you wlil still be able to cmpohreend waht I am witring qiute elsiay. The reason is that you are mapping individual letter patterns to sounds and assembling the word from them. Your brain is not looking to “recognise” the words. My second example of this effect is the following paragraph. I would like you to read it through once and count how many times you see the letter “f”: “France is a delightful country full of beautiful countryside, great wine and delicious food. You will find it full of around 60 million people with a wide variety of origins. They are united in their great love of the country and are full of passion for all things French”. How many did you count? Ten or eleven perhaps? Well there are in fact fifteen, but don’t feel bad… we all do the same thing! If you got the right number you are quite unusual. Have another go and see if you can spot them. It’s still hard, isn’t it? The task I set was essentially an auditory one, even though you were looking at visual symbols. You were looking for fs which your brain mapped to the sound /f/ as in fish. When you saw the word of you mapped the sound /v/ which is a more unusual form of the letter and did not count it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 69 The only way to learn to spell English successfully is to combine a mapping of different letter patterns to sounds and then overlay a visual memory of different words. One or other on its own is insufficient for full accuracy. For me the mass of homophonic words are the hardest to deal with. You can write out their and there switched around with no internal mechanism to spot the mistake. Of course you know how to spell both words, but as you pile through the text it is easy to make the switch and a spell checker does not even help! The Dyslexia Diagnosis If the underlying cause of reading difficulty is not dealt with, most parents will be getting worried by the age of 7 or 8. And they are right to, given that one in five children fails to learn to read. The most worried are the ones who have seen another child learn to read without mishap. So they know that the new pattern is not usual. Almost no parents have the technical knowledge needed to intervene. So they find themselves in a bind. The teacher of a 7-year-old child is often fairly unconcerned on the basis that “we all learn at different rates”. These teachers have often been given surprisingly little technical training in literacy themselves. They will usually just advise the parent to “do more reading”. But that is just more of what has already failed. So, at this moment there is an emotional volcano building up pressure within the parent. This often leads to a need for the child to be diagnosed “dyslexic”. That will then satisfy the parent that the child is bright, but has a “reason” for not being able to read that cannot be dealt with. It Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 70 frees them from this impasse of feeling they should be doing something with no knowledge of what to do. In this way, we end up with another child heading for life with a massive disadvantage. Yes, there are successful dyslexic people. That is because they are often highly intelligent and gifted, as I have described. But just try to find me one of them that says “Oh yes, I am so glad reading was hard for me and I have been far better off without it!” And how many frustrated, angry, underpaid and undervalued dyslexics are there for each success? It will certainly be hundreds and probably tens of thousands. And for me, the dyslexia diagnosis is very worrying. It is like going to your garage with your shiny car that isn’t working to be told, “Yes sir, your car is dysmotive. It is a beautiful car, the leather trim is excellent, I love the stereo and you have evidently paid quite a bit for it. It just isn’t running. Here is a bus timetable”. Would you walk away satisfied with that? No way! You would say “But why isn’t it running? And how we are going to sort it out!?” When I speak to parents of children who have been given a diagnosis of dyslexia, almost none have been given any serious information on why the child is struggling. And what happens then towards fixing the problem…? Usually virtually nothing. The focus moves to gaining work-around support like text-reading software, voice-input typing software, extra time in exams and having questions read out to them. There is a belief in much of the literacy world that dyslexia is incurable and that you can only work around it. Friends have said to me “Don’t talk about a cure for dyslexia. People won’t take you seriously.” Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 71 So I hate it when a parent talks about getting a diagnosis of dyslexia for their child. It is often followed by an acceptance of “the inevitable”. The End of This Road – The Life of a Dyslexic What happens next? They will struggle through, with the risk of humiliation sat hovering around them every day of their school career. Some will get the support of workaround solutions like laptops with voice-input systems and text readers. All of that has to be fought for or paid for by the parents, since councils are not keen to deliver. Many will start focusing on the sciences or the visual arts, presuming they can read enough to understand the questions. Others will just fail completely and join the group who get “no useful qualification” from their entire school career. Of that group, my estimate is that around a third will do time in prison, which isn’t good for them or the people they have committed offences against. Myelination – Time is Important There is an important process called myelination that is relevant to mention at this moment. Myelin is a fatty covering of the neurone that improves its speed and strength of signal. A newborn baby has a brain with no myelin. This slows the performance but makes it much easier for new branches and connections to be made. So your baby’s brain is very “plastic” and able to adapt to stimuli. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 72 The myelination process begins at the back of the brain with the vocal cortex and sweeps forward from the age of around 2 to the mid twenties. As this happens the brain matures but becomes less plastic. That is why it is much harder, for instance, to generate new speech sounds after the age of 2, but your peak brain speed and creativity is in your early adulthood. The relevance of this is that a child at 7, who has Visual Learner Syndrome affecting their reading, has been using their habit of memorisation and guessing for 2-3 years and the myelination of their brain has only recently begun. Children at 11 have been using their memorisation and guessing habit for 6 years and the myelination has been progressing for an extra 4 years. With each year that goes by, it is noticeably harder to help them change direction with their reading. It isn’t impossible in any way... far from it. But it does take more work and dedication from the child. Summary So there you have it. In our experience around 98% of dyslexic children can learn to read. They may not become the next Tolstoy, but they can read confidently and that is the key. Anybody who suggests workaround solutions like read-aloud software on computers and extra time in exams are an equal solution is failing the child. We find that 75% of the children on our Easyread home user course are in the middle or top of their class after 5 months. And almost all of the remaining 25% are still behind but catching up steadily. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 73 In school we see an average of over 7 days of reading age progress for each Easyread lesson that a child does. That leads to children advancing over twice as fast as the rate of an average child with conventional tuition, even though they were going nowhere before starting. On occasion we fail to see that sort of progress and I always know that it is us who have failed, not the child. I don’t know of a single child who I have felt was beyond help. If it goes badly it is because we got it wrong for that child and we need to address why that was. So, while some people may doubt what I am saying, the results we see every day suggest that their theorising does not equate with practical reality. And I am happy to show the data to prove that to anyone. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 74 The Neurology Of Reading “Some things just ain’t as simple as they seem” Introduction Reading is an activity that involves almost every major area of your brain. It is a wonder of integrated processing. As a result, a weakness in any of the processing areas involved can cause reading difficulty. This can be very specific, as we have already seen. For instance, a person with damage to Wernicke’s area in their parietal lobe will be unable to read, while potentially quite able to write. They will be able to write something down and then be unable to read it. I know that sounds bizarre, but it is quite possible. With a suitable cranial drill and scalpel a good brain surgeon could mess up any one of the many processes involved, while leaving most of your brain function intact. So this chapter is a detailed audit of this process and the ways it can go wrong, including the actual neurology of the main causes of difficulty that we have already discussed. There are two things to note: First, problems other than the ones I have already described are very unusual. Second, any book you read about the brain involves simplification, generalisation and many unknowns. While we know a lot about how the individual neurons of the brain work, we know a lot about how they are connected and we know a Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 75 lot about how damage to any specific area will affect you, we know almost nothing about how it really all works together! Neuroscience is an empirical body of knowledge, developed through observation. Nobody can tell you what a thought or a memory really is. Books on neuroscience are very strong on the wiring of the brain and naming all the bits. But you will often get towards the end of a long chapter on some area of cortex to be told, “We have no clear idea as to what the function of this area of the brain is”. To give you an idea of the complexity of the brain, have a look at the two images below: Schematic diagram of a neuron Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 76 Image of an actual neuron One you will recognise as a diagram of a neuron, with the dendrites receiving synapses and the axon travelling to other neurons or a muscle to deliver a signal. The image on the right is reality. That is a typical neuron with hundreds of dendrites and an axon that has split into hundreds of synaptic endings. Of course this is just one example. Neurons come in an utterly amazing range of sizes and shapes. Examples of different neuron types Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 77 So the brain is a wondrous enigma, processing countless forms of sensory input from every tissue in your body, controlling the thousands of glands, muscles and functions around your body and, in its spare time, letting you feel alive with conscious thought. We do not, and probably never will, fully understand it. But that does not mean we cannot get some understanding, particularly with the new scanning technologies. At last we can actually see a brain in action, or at least the movement of blood or some other indicator of which areas are active. Glossary Once you start reading neuroscience you quickly get engulfed in terminology. I have tried to simplify it, but there are still certain terms that are unavoidable. Here is a collection of the most essential. Don’t try to understand them all now. Just read through them. They are more here for reference. Action Potential A large signal that travels down the full length of the axon. It contrasts with a low energy spreading potential that decays as it travels. Amygdala Part of the brain stem involved in procedural memory and emotion Angular Gyrus A gyrus is one of the folds in the surface of the brain cortex. A sulcus is the fissure between two gyri. The Angular Gyrus is a key part of the Parietal Lobe on the left side of the brain that controls language comprehension. The lobes of the brain are the main visible lumps (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital). Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 78 Axon The part of the neuron that carries signals to other neurons and muscles. Basal Ganglia Neurons tend to collect in concentrations known as nuclei. A ganglia is a group of nuclei and the Basal Ganglia is a key area of the brain stem that coordinates and modulates the cerebral cortex. Broca’s Area A part of the parietal lobe controlling the production of language Cerebellum A dense area of cortex at the base of the brain behind the brain stem that has a modulating effect on the sensory perception and muscular control. It is also intimately involved in memory development. Cerebral Cortex The top and outer surfaces of the main lobes of the brain. Different areas of the cerebral cortex are involved in different types of processing of the senses, motor control of muscles and thoughts. Corpus Callosum The connection between the two cerebral cortex hemispheres Declarative Memory The memory that you have voluntary control over Dendrites The extensions of the neuron cell body that receive most of the synaptic inputs from axons. Episodic Memory Memory of sequential events Equilibrium Potential The electric charge across the membrane of a neuron that is at rest. Extra ocular Muscles The six muscles that control the position of each eye Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 79 Flocculus The part of the cerebellum that controls eye movement. Fovea The point of the eye with the greatest concentration of retinal cones and the lowest degree of grouping of cones by ganglion cells. This gives the most accurate part of the vision of each eye. Frontal Eye Field Part of the cortex controlling the tracking and saccade movement of the eyes Ganglion Cells The cells that collect inputs from a group of retinal rods and cones. Inferior Frontal Gyrus Contains Broca’s area Inferior Parietal Lobe Contains Wernicke’s area and the angular gyrus Labyrinth The structures of the inner ear that measure head movement and help control balance Long-term Potentiation The increased synaptic efficacy and sensitivity that seems to be involved in memory formation Magnocellular A ganglion cell that receives input from a large number of axons from a wide spatial area. As opposed to Parvocellular which relates to a ganglion cell that receives inputs from just a few axons. Some of the ganglion cells of the retina are magnocellular and some are parvocellular. Motor Cortex Part of the cerebral cortex involved in the initiation of muscle contraction Occipital Lobe The rear part of the cerebral hemispheres, containing much of the visual cortex Optic Chiasm Where the optic nerves meet and split again, with the neurons being regrouped as Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 80 the left and right field from each eye and then travelling to the contra-lateral cerebral hemisphere. Parvocellular Ganglion cell receiving just a few axon inputs from a limited spatial area. Prefrontal Cortex Part of the frontal lobe involved in the processing of memory, planning and responses to live situations. Semantic Memory The memory of facts and information The lobes of the brain The Reading Process Step 1 - Sitting still and maintaining attention It almost goes without saying that to read you have to be stationary and you need to focus your mind on the task in hand, in order to learn to read. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 81 So Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder is clearly a cause of reading difficulty. There are millions and millions of neurons pouring information into the brain with signals relating to touch, vision, hearing, smell, muscle tension, muscle position, balance, movement and an array of chemical states of different organs. It is no mean feat to bring order to this cacophony of information. It is like every blog in the world arriving on your screen at the same time. And with any number of conscious areas of cortex wanting to have their say, the ability to shut out all the options except one takes effort and practice. Focusing on the text The next issue is equally obvious. Text is a visual medium and you need to be able to see it clearly to use it. At the most elementary level, every child should visit the optician on a regular basis, to check that there are no basic problems. As you grow your body is changing size and shape. It is quite possible for eye problems to develop without being noticed. However, it goes a bit further than that. Most of us find reading red print on a blue background very hard. It tends to shimmer. Some people see the same effect with black text on white paper. And it is possible to have complete blind spots in your vision, without you being aware of it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 82 How The Eyes Work These are all neurological issues and so we need to delve into neurology in order to begin to understand them. We all know that there are rods and cones in your retina. The cones can detect colour in strong light and the rods can work at very low levels of illumination, but see no colour differentiation. However, did you know that there are around 100 million rods and cones in each eye and just 1 million neurons coming out of the eye down the optic nerve? So our eyes are not working like a digital camera, which has a fixed number of pixels and an equal number of spots of colour on the picture created. The eyes are a lot subtler than that. I say “just 1 million neurons” in the optic nerve, but that is pretty amazing isn’t it? Our neurons are perhaps the greatest evolutionary development of the vertebrate evolutionary tree. Perhaps they are more important than bones, for me. You see the very best invertebrate neurons are 10,000 times bigger than your optic neurons and a quarter as fast. If you were an invertebrate, the nerve coming out of your eye would be the size of your neck. To achieve this our great invention was myelin. There are cells in your brain and all through your nervous system that wrap themselves around each neuron to insulate it. That increases the speed of transmission of the signal and reduces the diameter needed. Myelin is a white fatty substance. The “white matter” in your brain is made up of myelin-covered axons, whereas the “grey matter” is unmyelinated cortex. So all the connections along the spinal chord and between areas of cortex are white matter because they need to transmit fast. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 83 The processing cortex is grey matter because the neurons are all far shorter and so maximum speed of transmission is not so critical. The ability to branch and form new synapses is the key to your grey matter. So the neurons there do not want the hindrance of a myelin covering. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 84 So those omega oils genuinely are critical to your brain function. You do need to make sure you have a decent supply of oils in your diet. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 85 There is a theory, which I like, but has been rejected by most anthropologists, but only because there is no archaeological evidence for it. Anyhow, it is based on the idea that we had an aquatic phase and built our oversized brain capacity on a high fish diet. Our brain needs lots of energy (provided by the protein) and oils (provided by the fish oils) in order to work. There are also other indicators of a significant aquatic phase (such as the shape of our nose, subcutaneous fat and our hair patterns). It would also be an explanation of our linguistic skill, because to tell someone about what you have seen under water, you have to describe it. You can’t just point and grunt! It is true that there is no archaeological evidence to support any of this. But then the sea level is very high at the moment (because we are in an interglacial age) and so any evidence that there is will be under 20m of seawater off the African coastline. If we go into another ice age, perhaps we will find all the villages of our forebears. Or maybe I will spend my retirement scuba diving for them. Getting back to our visionary system, since there are 100 million rods and cones and just 1 million neurons in the optic nerve from each eye, something very interesting is going on actually in the eye. The processing of the image is beginning right inside it. In fact the eyes are part of what is called the Central Nervous System (CNS), as opposed to the Peripheral Nervous System that makes up all of the other nerves outside of your skull and backbone. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 86 Our Neural Structure Understanding a little bit of the development of the nervous system is helpful here. When you are developing during your first month in the womb, a tube of cells form, which will become your CNS. Even now your brain shows the vestiges of that tube and all of your cerebral cortex is effectively a thickened tube, which has been folded in order to increase the surface area to around 2 square feet, if flattened out. That is about four times the area of a chimpanzee’s cerebral cortex. Your cerebellum is even more amazing. The cerebellar cortex would cover about 6 square feet, despite only being the size of two small tangerines. The thickness of the cortex varies considerably, but probably the thinnest areas are the retinas of your eyes. Each eye is effectively part of your brain extruded out through the skull. The walls of your eyeballs are contiguous with the outer wall of the rest of your brain. A bit freaky! Anyhow, the retina has four layers of neurons: • Rods and cones • Horizontal cells • Bipolar cells • Ganglion Cells Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 87 Schematic View of the Retina Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 88 Views of an actual retina So the rods and cones each connect to several horizontal cells, which spread laterally and to several bipolar cells, which connect through to the ganglion cells. The bipolar cells have two possible reactions to a signal; there are ones that depolarize and others that hyperpolarize. They do not Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 89 produce an action potential (ie a strong unitary signal) but rather a low intensity post-synaptic potential. Each ganglion cell collects information from an area of rods and cones. The information collected is from a network of bipolar cells, which are also connected in a mesh of horizontal cells. And every area of each eye is covered by more than one ganglion cell. This is important because your eyes are doing much more than just detecting the light in the way a camera records a map of the image. They are processing that signal as well and looking to react to patterns within it. The ganglion cells take a census of the rods and cones and look for differences between the signals coming from them. In particular, they look for the difference between the peripheral rods and cones and the central rods and cones of their area. How they respond to a difference between the centre and the periphery will depend on what types of bipolar cells they are connected to. The eyes are looking to detect variation in intensity, rather than just the overall brightness. If you have ever been in an eclipse of the sun, you will have experienced this. The moon can cover 95% of the sun before you begin to notice any difference in light intensity. Even then, everything looks fairly normal, but just feels a bit weird. Your eyesight adjusts to the dropping level of illumination so well that th you do not even notice that it is getting darker (to a 1/20 of the original daylight). Your eyes can see effectively in anything from good starlight through to midday sunlight, which is roughly a millionfold increase in intensity. It is an astonishing feat. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 90 Indeed, your rods can detect a single photon of light, although the aggregation that I have described above means that you will not actually “see” that single photon. And your visual system is constantly adjusting what you see to present what it thinks is there, rather than the raw information received. Here are a couple of illustrations of that. Colour Cube Effects Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 91 How would you describe the centre cube of each face of the top right image? Well, you may be surprised to read that they are the same colour. I doubt you will believe me. In order to confirm it you will have to get some paper, make a couple of holes and lay it over the page. They are both a mid-brown colour. The reason you are fooled by the picture is the your eyes are constantly calculating what you are looking at and adjusting your perceived vision as a result. For instance, if someone is wearing a red dress, it looks more or less equally red whether you are outside on a bright day or inside in artificial light. In reality, the actual colour you are seeing will be quite different in the two situations. This process begins right in the eye because the ganglion cells are looking for differentiation of colour and brightness. Some ganglion cells look for increased brightness in the centre of their area of collection and others look for a decreased brightness. In either case, they send no signal if they detect no difference. What this does is help the visual cortex pick up patterns. It is one of the features of our vision. We are good at picking up pattern, even if it is not there. Here is an example of that. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 92 Kanizsa’s triangle Patterns in nature have always been important to us. They make sense of our surroundings. For instance, look at this next image: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 93 Downlit Circles Why is it that some of the circles pop out at you like 3-dimensional balls and the others look flat? The images are identical but flipped over. The reason is that the circles with the darker lower halves match the reality of a ball in sunlight. So the visual cortex presumes they are balls in sunlight. If you turn the book upside down, it will be the other circles that pop out at you. When the book is on its side, they all look flat. For this reason most wildlife has a dark top and a light lower side to break this effect. Impala for instance, have a pale belly and a very dark back in order to break up this effect in the lion’s vision. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 94 And thinking of lions and tigers, how important was it for us to spot the telltale patterns of a lion’s face in the long grass? Yes, very! So our cortex was madly trying to spot the patterns and tiger facial patterns are trying to break them up. So we see patterns well. And letters are patterns. Linked to this is one of the causes of dyslexia that we have already discussed; Irlen Syndrome. If the break between light and dark is over-emphasised, it leads to funny effects and makes the patterns hard to focus on. That is what happens when people with Irlen Syndrome look at black lettering on white paper. The text appears to shimmer and move around. If you reduce the contrast with a coloured film, the effect can be reduced. This probably links into another clever process that is going on as you focus on a spot on the page. Your eyes are not completely steady. There is movement of the image on your retina and your cortex has to compensate for this, to give you the impression of a static image. One other possible problem with your ability to focus on the text is actual damage to the retina. You may be quite unaware of there being any damage, because the brain is exceptional at filling in gaps in your vision. I can demonstrate this to you because we all have a blind spot, where the axons from the ganglion cells collect and pass through the retina into the optic nerve. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 95 Blind Spot Demonstration Cover your left eye and stare at the spot above A. Now move the book to around 12 inches from your face, you will find the head of the renaisance fellow disappears. With the book the same distance from your eyes move your focus to the point above the letter A. Now you will find that the gap in the grid gets filled in. The brain has just guessed what is there and hidden the fact that you cannot actually see that point. In the same way, you can have substantial damage to your retina without being aware of it Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 96 Our retinas have a small area of very high acuity vision, called the fovea. The area of collection of each ganglion cell is very small and the fovea is entirely filled with cone receptors. If you have damage to your fovea, where you would normally be focusing on the text, it will make it very hard to see it clearly. It is critical that your child knows not to watch very bright lights, and particularly the arc of a welder and the sun. Once a blind spot is burnt in the fovea the brain will adjust the positioning of the eye to bring the bright spot back into vision (knowing that it is there and is perhaps important) and so the area of damage will steadily spread until the whole fovea is destroyed. A few minutes watching a welder can do that. That would be a pity since your foveas are probably the two most critical square millimetres of tissue in your body. We will come back to the ganglion cells in due course, but let’s follow the signals coming from them back through the optic nerves. The two nerves pass through the skull wall and then cross. It is one of those things we have no explanation for, but generally the left hemisphere of the brain deals with the right hand side of the body and world. This is an example of the latter, because the two optic nerves actually split themselves and recombine left with left and right with right at the optic chiasm (the crossing point). What happens is that the neurons from the left vision of both eyes combine and travel to the visual cortex of the right hemisphere. And the neurons from the right vision of both eyes travel together to the left cortex. They pass through the thalamus, because everything seems to go through the thalamus, and then to the primary visual cortex right at the back of your head, in the occipital lobe. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 97 This has six layers and different types of neuron from around the eyes travels to each of the six layers. If you drill down through them, the same bit of vision (ie a particular bit of the outside world) is being processed through each layer in a column. So any part of the “view” created is a collation of these six layers. Three layers take neurons from the left eye and three layers take neurons from the right eye. The biggest difference between the three layers of each eye is the size of ganglion cell generating the axon. We will come back to that too. One handy effect of this Scoobie-Snack sandwich style visual cortex is that differences between the view from the left and right eyes can be easily measured. This is what delivers part of that 3dimensional feel we get to the image we see. Of course some of it is also computed, as we saw with the shaded images earlier. The world still seems 3-dimentional, even if you close one eye. Anyhow, this is your primary visual cortex, but around a third of your brain is involved in visual processing. Much of the occipital lobe and lower parietal lobe is taken up. We have the best colour vision of all mammals. So, as you scan a word your visual system is looking for patterns, particularly patterns it recognises. And this leads to probably the most common form of dyslexia, which we have already discussed as the auditory deficit and the visual learner syndrome. It is possible for the reader to recognise the shape of a word and match it against one in memory, just as you match the characteristics of a rhino against the shape characteristics stored in memory. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 98 When you recognise the shape of a rhino in a picture, the image match is made in your visual memory cortex and that then links with your semantic memory of the fact that the image is a rhino. During this process the centre of processing moves from your visual cortex in the occipital lobe to your pre-frontal cortex in your frontal lobe, where the semantic knowledge of the rhino is stored. As we each move around our personal space in the world, those two bits of cortex are constantly engaged with one another, observing and processing the visual information around us. As you walk towards a door, your hand moves towards the handle, your muscles pull, the door opens, you step forward through it, you pull the handle behind you, you step on into the room… There is a constant flow of incoming information being received, processed and reacted to. These visual-to-frontal links are very strong through constant exercise. As you do all of that, you are not processing it linguistically (unless you are showing signs of dementia!). The linguistic cortex is unengaged. In fact, you can be simultaneously processing auditory thoughts while doing all of this spacial management. That will be happening between your auditory cortex and your pre-frontal cortex. By contrast, our direct visual to audio/linguistic links are naturally much less strong (at least until you learn to read or play music). It is not a direction of linkage that we naturally have great need of. So it is quite normal for a new reader to learn to sight-memorise early words and process them in the pre-frontal cortex directly from the visual cortex. It gets quick and gratifying results. I have already written about it in length and so we won’t go over it again here. But of all reading difficulty, this accounts for around 80% of it. Most of what we do Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 99 in Easyread is to break this link and develop the visual-to-auditory cortex links. I have mentioned that the audio-linguistic processing cortex has two distinct areas. Broca’s area deals with the production of language and Wernicke’s area deals with comprehension of language. So it is quite possible for someone with damage to Wernicke’s area to be able to write and not read. They cannot even read what they have just written. This leads to a possible explanation of another, very unusual, form of dyslexia. There is a small number of children who develop considerable fluency in reading out loud, but are unable to comprehend what they have read. If you repeat back to them what they have said, they will understand it perfectly. This is different from children who are so busy working on the decoding that they find it hard to follow the meaning. These children are fluent readers but cannot follow the meaning at all. For instance, you could hold up a card saying “red” and ask them to read it, which they will do with ease. You can then say “point to the correct colour” and they will be unable to choose between a blue box and red box, even though they have just read it out. If you say “point to the red box” they will do so without hesitation. It is bizarre to behold, but you can see how this might arise, by using Broca’s area to read, while bypassing Wernicke’s area. Our approach to this in Easyread is to build the links to the auditory cortex and also to encourage very explicit comprehension of words from very early in the course. For instance, we present words in the shooting gallery, which need to be comprehended for the right target to be shot. By adding time pressure (the gallery doors start to shut) we are trying to force Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 100 the most direct link between the visual processing and Wernicke’s area. As we will see, almost every link in the brain can be made if there is sufficient stimulus to make it happen. It is what Pavlov found with his dogs. The brain is very adabtable. So I almost never feel that despair is an appropriate reaction to a problem. Indeed the brain is often able to wire up completely new areas to deal with a process, if the original cortex for that process has been damaged. Auditory Processing Begins Anyhow, a conventional reader will process the image of the letters in the visual cortex and then link through to the auditory cortex to convert the visual images into sounds. The very inconsistent nature of English spelling and pronunciation makes this mapping process harder than it might be. But the process can be seen in brain scans nonetheless. The visual data travels from the visual cortex in both hemispheres to the auditory cortex, which is usually in the left hemisphere. The data from the visual cortex in the right hemisphere has to pass through the Corpus Callosum, the neuron bridge between the two cerebral hemispheres. This leads to an interesting situation if the corpus callosum is severed, as is sometimes done surgically for people with severe epilepsy. The patient retains full vision in both eyes, but if they look at text in the left half of their field of vision, it gets processed in their right visual cortex and cannot then connect with the auditory cortex in the left hemisphere. So they will be unable to read it. If they move the same text to their right field of vision they can read it without a problem. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 101 Anyhow, the letters and graphemes (the letter patterns relating to a single phoneme) have to be mapped in some way to the related phoneme. In order for that to happen, the reader needs to have an awareness of the sounds that build our words. This is usually in our procedural (ie non-voluntary) memory. Most of us are unable to list the sounds in words without training or a considerable amount of time to think about it. So, through childhood we build up a subconscious awareness of them. This is heightened by the interactions we have with our parents and nursery staff. All those nursery rhymes and word games are actually critical preparation for reading. The mother who over-emphasises the sounds in words as she speaks to even a baby in the cot is performing a critical role. Anyhow, if there is a lack of phonemic awareness (the awareness of the phonemes in the language), then it is essential to build this before reading is attempted. Without it the child will always struggle to develop good decoding skills because there is no parallel auditory map with which to map the visual images to. Sometimes a lack of phonemic awareness is viewed as untreatable. I think this is extremely unlikely (although I would rate nothing relating to the brain as impossible). The brain, even in adulthood, is astonishingly good at adapting to stimuli. Good phonetic training will lead to at least a basic phonemic awareness, unless there is a physiological problem with the hearing. Hearing difficulty is a regular problem that we see with children doing Easyread. They have often gone through a period of poor hearing at some stage (usually through glue ear) and that has led to a reduced phonemic awareness. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 102 However, if you actually destroy an area of the brain you will still find it deploying other areas to try and recover the function lost. I have personal experience of this since my aunt Pauline was struck down with polio as a teenager, which destroyed her motor neurons and paralysed her. She slowly regained her limbs as the nervous system began to deploy some of her sensory nerves as motor neurons instead. There are lots of examples of this sort of plasticity, particularly after strokes. The damaged brain tissue never recovers after a stroke. All stroke recovery is due to redeployment of surrounding cortex. So, you can see why I am not in favour of any fatalistic “dyslexia is untreatable” views. They are mostly wrong. Anyhow, the raw auditory data, that has been mapped from the visual data, is held briefly in the auditory sensory memory while being processed in Wernicke’s area to make sense of it in combination with your frontal lobe. The meaning derived for the word is then stored in your working memory. Several parts of the brain seem to be involved in delivering the facility we regard as our working memory. The memory is one of the most amazing parts of our brains. What is a memory? To even begin to understand that, we need to look at how a neuron works and how they interconnect. How Neurons Work and Form Memory We know that the neuron receives signals on its dendrites and passes them down the axon towards other neurons. Each neuron may have hundreds or thousands of axons forming synapses on its dendrites all delivering signals to it. So what is a neuron signal? We had better have a look at neuron function. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 103 The membrane of the neuron forms a barrier to potassium and sodium ions (amongst others, but we will keep this simple). The neuron has a high concentration of potassium and low concentration of sodium within it. Potassium gates in the membrane wall allow some potassium ions to leak out. Osmosis (diffusion from a high concentration to a low concentration) encourages them to leak out, which creates a negative charge within the neuron. This process reaches equilibrium when the voltage created is pulling the potassium ions back through gates in the membrane as much as the osmosis is encouraging them out. This small negative voltage (65mV) across the neuron membrane encourages the sodium ions in the fluid surrounding the neuron to be drawn to the neuron wall. The concentration of sodium ions in the neuron is very low because it gets pumped out of the neuron through the membrane. When an axon forming a synapse on a dendrite of the neuron discharges, chemicals from the axon float across the synaptic gap and cause sodium gates to open in the dendrite membrane. As a result sodium will flow across the membrane by osmosis. This will shift the voltage across that part of the neuron membrane from negative towards positive. This triggers the opening of more sodium gates in the neighbouring membrane and so you will get a gentle wave of positive charge running along the dendrite of the neuron. This spreading wave will decay with time and distance travelled, unless it is strong enough to reach what is called the action potential of the neuron. The action potential is the level of positive charge within the dendrite or neuron cell body, which will trigger the firing of the neuron. When this voltage is reached all the sodium gates open in that part of the neuron membrane and a Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 104 flood of sodium momentarily rushes into the neuron creating a spike of positive voltage. The spike will now travel down the axon of the neuron with virtually no decay and at speeds of around 100m/sec. So a signal will travel the length of one of your 1m neurons in around 10 milliseconds. While we are mentioning that, a 1 metre long cell is quite an achiement. It is a logistical nightmare to maintain a cell that long from a neucleus at one end. As a result all sorts of clever mechanisms are built into your long neurons to rush chemicals around them in special high-speed tubules. When I say “high speed”, I mean centimetres an hour rather than millimetres a day, as would be the case without this help. This spike of voltage across the membrane also closes the sodium gates again and locks them shut for a short period. This means that a single high-voltage wave rolls down the neuron. So, each neuron is working like a transistor or semi-conductor in a radio. What this means is that the output of the neuron is not linear with the input. It can receive thousands of inputs without responding at all. Then, when the aggregate level of incoming synapse action reaches a critical threshold level, the neuron fires. So it is acting as a filter. Low inputs get no reaction and then when it reaches a certain level it heavily over reacts in a non-linear way. It also leads to huge subtlety in how or why it fires. There are dozens of different synaptic chemicals which increase or decrease the response of the dendrite. The sensitivity of the dendrite is affected by these and also the history of previous firing. The process also takes energy (provided by mitochondria in the neuron) and eventually a neuron that is over exercised will begin to Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 105 lose its ability to fire, until it has replenished itself. Your nerves deaden for a while after excess stimulation. So the whole system can adapt to usage and changes in its responsiveness over time. To try to illustrate this, imagine someone sat in a room surrounded by little speakers on desks all around him. Our Buzzerman is instructed to press his buzzer when he feels he is getting an important message from the array of incoming speakers. So he sits there, listening to little buzzes in the different speakers. If some speakers seem to be buzzing more frequently than others, he is instructed to pull those a bit nearer to him, to listen to those ones more carefully. Now if one of the speakers were to start shrieking, then the Buzzerman would instantly hit his buzzer (to send his own signal out to other similar rooms that have his speakers sat in them). However that is unlikely. What happens more is a group of speakers all buzz together and that triggers the Buzzerman to press his buzzer. He can only press his buzzer once a second. And after about 10 buzzes his buzzer battery begins to get low and he has to rest it. This is somewhat similar to what is going on as you read this. Millions and millions of neurons are connected in this way, trying to determine whether signals are relevant or not. In parallel to that, there are all sorts of other factors affecting the attendant. How much he is paid to buzz, how long it is since he has buzzed, how encouraging his buzz system manager is, what the quality of the air in his relay room is like, how long he has been at work, what brand of batteries are in his buzzer… All of this will affect his buzz responsiveness. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 106 Regular signals get stronger and stronger because the buzzermen pull those speakers towards themselves. That seems to be the nature of a memory. It is a signal that transmits more easily than expected because it has been processed several times before. Your neuron synapses are rather more elegant, but essentially similar. How all of that ends up in the sense of reality we all know, nobody has a clue. But the state of your neurons, how well connected they are, the chemicals within them, the supply of nutrients to them, how long they have been operating without a rest… all of these things will affect how they operate. In particular, if they are not used, they will be weakly connected, have poor blood supply, poor nutrient levels and be ineffective. That spirals into reduced usage. Giving them a good exercise can reverse that and reading is one of the most neuron exercising activities we have. So, all of this leads us to another of the common causes of dyslexia that we have already discussed; poor working memory capacity. If you don’t have access to a good capacity to hold information in working memory, reading is hard since it is very demanding of working memory. There are several broad types of memory that can be seen operating separately. The shortest form of memory is the sensory memory. In the milliseconds after you have perceived something, a map of it exists in your neurons. But this fades very quickly since you generally have a flow of new incoming sensory information. This raw sensory information is generally processed and passed into what we term as working memory. That includes short-term Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 107 memory. Short-term memory is mainly visually based but can relate to any of the senses and semantic short-term memory, holding a store of meaning. This working memory can usually hold between 5 and 9 individual and random pieces of information. I have a slightly below average working memory and so I find I can remember a sequence of 6 digits without difficulty, but if you give me 7 I am lost. It is not that I remember the first six and forget the last one. I just get scrambled. Someone with a good working memory can hold nine digits in memory. Of course with specialist techniques it is possible to cheat this by quickly passing the information into other forms of memory such as episodic, or by chunking it into groups. I can remember my entire credit card details for instance. A bad sign I know, but quite different to being given a new sequence to remember. So, you can see that someone with only five slots has a problem on their hands when processing a flow of letters, graphemes, phonemes, words and meanings. I have already discussed this as one of the seven main causes of reading difficulty. Strictly speaking it is not specific to reading, since it will affect any form of learning, and therefore is not dyslexia. But who really cares? What we have found is that incremental improvement can be achieved in three ways. • First, if the learner is relaxed and happy, access to the working memory will be easier. • Second, working at reading will in itself develop the working memory to its maximum potential. • And third, with regular daily practice, the reading process moves steadily into procedural memory, which reduces Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 108 the load on the declarative elements of the working memory. While we are talking about memory we had better look at the other forms that we have at our disposal. Your working memory is a bit like a buffer. As new stuff pours into it, the old will get shoved out. That is why, when you have just been told a telephone number you don’t want someone to start talking to you before you write it down. From the working memory information moves into a variety of longer term memories. The strongest of these is your visual memory, which has a startling capacity for remembering at least sufficient data to recall an image when seen again. It is a miraculous ability. There is an almost infinite number of locations and faces that I could show you, each of which can be recalled from a variety of different viewpoints, which you may not have even had as a view before. So the brain is able to map the salient features and also a model of them in 3 dimensions so as to be able to recognise them from different angles. Even the strongest computer is still unable to compete with us in that facility. This feat occupies at least a third of your cerebral cortex and involves billions of neurons networking and resonating when a familiar pattern is detected. Many of these visual patterns are linked to semantic memories located more in the frontal lobe, which give meaning to them. And you have a far smaller auditory equivalent doing the same thing. This is all in your declarative memory, which is in your conscious awareness. You can actively hunt for a declarative memory until you get that moment of resonance when you “find” it. You can Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 109 imagine all the buzzer operators suddenly buzzing away in harmony when you get the pattern right for the memory you are searching for. Working in parallel with this is your procedural memory, which you have no conscious control over. All of your skills lie in this form of memory. Most activities are run in your declarative memory initially, but migrate to procedural memory as they become ingrained. Once that process has happened, it becomes very dangerous to try to run the operation in declarative memory. Your performance will almost certainly decline. As you read this, your reading process is happening entirely in procedural memory. The whole process flows from shapes on page to meaning understood, without conscious thought on your part. A big element of the later phases of Easyread is designed to move the process from declarative to procedural memory. Long-term memory is something else again. A long-term memory goes through various phases of development. It initially gets layed down during your experience of the day, as a copy of each fleeting short-term memory. It is sometimes said that everything you have experienced exists as a memory for you somewhere in your brain. But if we remembered everything we heard and saw throughout our lives, it would be very debilitating. There are people who more or less have that ability and it is generally a curse. A good memory is a great asset, a faultless memory is a disaster. So an important part of our memory processes is to forget stuff. That means that we have a filter at work. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 110 The emotional areas of the brain stem have a critical role to play in operating the filter. One of the key roles of emotion is to reinforce memory. Emotions trigger the release of chemicals around the brain, which then affect how the synapses work. So high emotion can help lock a memory in place by enhancing the permanence of those synapse connections. For instance, we have all had that experience of seeing the events of an accident play back to us like a video recording. As soon as it has become apparent that something serious is happening, an alarm runs through our cortex and our visual memory is switched into full record mode. A period of just a couple of seconds can be recalled in great detail, frame by frame. Very happy moments and very sad moments are also imprinted in far higher clarity than normal. The classic for this used to be “where were you when you heard President Kennedy had been shot?” The modern equivalent is now the 9/11 Twin Towers attack. I can tell you the nature of the light that day (a soft, warm Indian Summer afternoon in Oxford), exactly who told me to turn on the television (my old next door neighbour), my children arriving back into the house (it was happening mid afternoon UK time and they arrived back from school at about the time the towers actually collapsed), the voice of the commentary and, of course, the images being portrayed. We had the bizarre experience of seeing the towers apparently standing with the commentary saying they had collapsed. I presume heavy copyright negotiations were taking place. th That is not normal! I can tell you nothing about the 11 of September last year, even though I experienced it in essentially the same way. My senses were just as active, but my emotions were unengaged and the memory is lost entirely as far as I know. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 111 So, if you are unengaged emotionally during an experience, the memories of it will almost certainly not be retained. Boredom is death to learning. How important a statement is that to education? To my mind it is fundamental. In past centuries the solution was often to instill fear as the predominant emotion. Fear is certainly a powerful force in memory retention. Now that has been removed as a favoured option, there is an absolute requirement for educators to engage, inspire and entertain in other ways. My own belief is that there is absolutely no reason to bore, if you can entertain. In Easyread we try whatever we can do to entertain, encourage and inspire. Psychology is half what we do and once positive emotions are in play, the job gets a lot easier. One of the huge problems with conventional phonics teaching is that it is fundamentally very boring. Have a look through a phonics textbook to see what I mean. That is partly why such a significant number of children are unengaged by them and do not learn to read in a phonetic way. Anyhow, as I was saying, we happily forget most of what we see and hear. It is only when the brain stem is activated with emotion that we begin to take a serious note of things and log them in memory. At this stage the memory laid down is very raw and will not last long without reinforcement. So there are three mechanisms that are critical to long-term memory retention. First is the emotional state of the brain at the time of perception of the sensory input, as discussed. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 112 Second is repeated exposure to the sensory input. This will reinforce the strenthening of the synapses involved. And the more actively that is done with all the senses the better. Hearing something is good, but seeing something as well is better. And hearing, seeing and actively repeating something is best. This is the basis to revision before an exam. The last element of memory fixing is your sleep. While we are asleep we work through much of what we have experienced and this seems to have a big effect on our understanding of it and memory of it. It has been shown in studies that if a sound is linked to an experience during the day and that same sound is played to you when you are asleep, it will trigger processing of the memory while you sleep and improved retention the next day. When you have a big knock to the head your most recent memories are often lost. With a little bit of concussion that might be just a few minutes. But with more serious amnesia everything from that day will be gone. However, there will usually be a time in the fairly recent past from which everything backwards can be remembered. What this indicates is that your memories are going through a process over 24-48 hours to convert them from something that can be disabled in this way, to something more permanent. Sleep is clearly a critical element of this process. That can be measured in the laboratory. Do not revise on the day of an exam. The night before is far more valuable. Anyhow, with Easyread we build sleep into the system by limiting the amount of new material the children can be exposed to in a single day. After doing a lesson the next one only becomes Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 113 available 12 hours later. That means that they are forced to sleep between lessons. Children have a tendency to gorge themselves on a new experience. That means that much of it is not retained well and that they get satiated very quickly. So we constantly hold stuff back to reverse both effects. Eye Control At this stage you need to look at the next word. This is a critical moment in the reading process because your eyes need to do a little jump to the right. The eyes are usually highly trained at this because they are always doing it. If you ask someone to look around the room and watch their eyes carefully, you will see that they do not sweep their vision about at all. Their head turns in a steady sweep but their eyes fixate on a spot and then jump to the next. These little jumps are called saccades. We cannot process a moving image. When your eyes are in motion you are blind, although your brain gives you the impression that you aren’t. It fills in an impression of the image, even though it is quite incapable of interpreting the mass of data coming from the actual moving image. The whole system of control of your eye position is a beautiful thing. You have a map of your vision within your head and matching that map are the motor controls needed to move the focus of your eyes to a particular point in your current vision. So, say you are staring at a rabbit in the long grass and there is a movement in the hedge to your left, your visual cortex will log the point in your vision where the movement occurred (the peripheral vision is particularly good at seeing movement as we will see), map Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 114 that to the parallel motor cortex map for the control of your extraoccular muscles to achieve the right movement to carry the fovea to that point and apply the movement. It is entirely automatic. If your head needs to swivel the brain can deal with that too, because the channels in the labyrinth of your inner ear are also linked directly to your eye control. I can demonstrate that to you. Hold your finger in front of your face, about 12 inches from your nose. Now move it from side to side in front of you as fast as is comfortable for your eyes to track the finger and keep focussed on the first joint in the finger. As you do that count the passages back and forth up to six. Now hold the finger stationary and turn your head left and right instead, to get the same relative movement and count how quickly you can turn your head while keeping focus on the stationary finger. Unless something strange is going on, or I have not explained myself properly, you will have been able to do the second exercise about twice as fast as the first. Why is that? The eyes have to do the same amount of movement. The reason is that the two mechanisms are entirely different. In the first, the visual cortex is tracking the finger, calculating the movement and instructing the extra-occular muscles appropriately. In the second the inner ear is calculating the rotation of the head and moving the eyes to counter the movement. Is it not beautiful…? Perhaps I am turning into a neurology geek. Anyhow, this saccade movement control is critical for reading and can cause problems if it is not running smoothly. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 115 Around 15% of the struggling readers that we help are unable to control their eyes sufficiently well to navigate each line of text in this way. There are different possible reasons for this. You will remember that the ganglion cells in the eyes are a mix of magnocellular and parvocellular designs. The parvocellular neurons are gathering information from a fairly small group of rods and cones and are used for detailed analysis around the fovea. The magnocellular neurones tend to be more towards the periphery of the vision and gather information from a much larger area of rods and cones. This means that they are more sensitive to shape and movement. This also helps with the integration of the vision from the two eyes and the positional stability of the eyes. The two different types of cell get processed in different layers of the primary visual cortex. Here is a pair of pictures that demonstrate the two types of neural structure at work: High Pass vs Low Pass images Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 116 Look at the two pictures and then start to move away from the page. Somewhere between 6 and 12 feet from the pictures you will see them change. How has that happened? The creators of the pictures took three shots; smiling, neutral and frowning. They then passed them through a filter to split them into either fine detail or coarse detail. And then then combined neutral fine detail with smiling coarse detail into the left picture, and frowning fine detail with neutral coarse detail in the right picture. When you are close to the pictures your parvocellular pathsways can pick up the fine detail and that is the picture you see. When you move away you lose the ability to see the fine detail and only the coarse detail is picked up, this time by your magnocellular pathways. That is then the picture you see. The two systems normally work in harmony, helping you pick up both the larger shapes and patterns and the fine detail in parallel. There is evidence that some dyslexic readers struggle because the magnocellular cortex is less strong. This can have a variety of effects. First it may contribute to Irlen syndrome, where the learner finds the text appearing to move around on the page. It certainly means that the reader will find locking onto a word in a line of words harder. The magnocellular element is heavily involved in the positional stability of the eye and adjusting for the small movements that are always happening. And it may make phonemic analysis harder in the auditory system. However, it may not be the magnocellular structures themselves that are at fault but where their signals get processed. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 117 The cerebellum seems to do a lot of processing of magnocellular neuron activity and integrates that with the activity in the cerebral cortex areas as a form of moderation and refinement of the motor neuron cortex. There is an area of the cerebellum called the flocculus that is particularly involved in the motor control of the extraocular muscles. Most often the difficulty with the control of the eyes seems to be due to a weakness in the control of the eye motor neurone cortex by the flocculus. We find that this condition often responds well to exercise of the circuitry. So we include a simple exercise for children to do while developing their reading that improves their eye navigation control. If there is particular difficulty due to this issue it is very obvious when the children move from decoding single words to decoding sentences. We then push the child to use this exercise for 10-15 short sessions through the day. Generally that seems to get good results. You will find a whole range of “dyslexia cures” focusing on this issue. Not to put too fine a point on it, we have found most of them to be over-complex, over-priced and over-sold, when the solution seems to be really quite simple. However, I am sure that they do get a good result if this is the learner’s difficulty. It is just that you don’t need to spend a lot of money on it and most reading difficulty does not originate in the flocculus. So for most children coordination exercises will have absolutely no impact on their reading. OK, so the eyes have now moved to the next word and the process repeats itself. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 118 Comprehension from Perception As the words in a sentence are decoded they get processed into working memory, the meaning of the phrases are then analysed in Wernicke’s area and the prefrontal cortex to build a comprehension of it. I have already mentioned that this can be an issue. It is possible for text to be read and not understood at all. However, in all the children going through Easyread we do not know of a single example of this phenomenon continuing once the reading process has been successfully re-engineered through the auditory cortex. What we do encounter is learners struggling with the decoding so much that they do not keep track of the words as they decode them. However, as the process moves into procedural memory, this slowly goes away. But that can take months. So, there you have it: the reading process, step by step. I hope you will now agree that any search for “the single cause” of reading difficulty and dyslexia is absurd. People have this great urge to fine The Answer, but it is the equivalent of trying to find the single cause of car breakdowns. There isn’t one! There are many possible causes of reading difficulty and we have to be alert to the patterns of symptoms of those causes, to know the best possible solution for each individual learner. The efficacy of a solution is only relevant in relation to the individual. For instance, coloured film is probably useful in around 5% of cases (sufferers of Irlen syndrome). That does not mean it is 5% effective or fails 95% of the time. It is perhaps 80% effective for that 5% of individuals and so a very important resource for them. But you have to know who needs it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 119 Summary of Reading Difficulty Symptoms, Causes and Solutions “Know your enemy” So here is a table of symptoms, the underlying problem causing each symptom and the best solution: Symptom Cause Solution Lack of concentration, fidgeting, impulsive behaviour, difficulty sitting still ADHD, which is a lack of focus and control eminating from the frontal cortex. Good sleep patterns, low sugar and caffeine consumption, high omega oil consumption, stimulating and entertaining reading materials Unable to focus on the text Eye focus problems Visit the optician for glasses Finds the text appearing to “move around” on the page Irlen syndrome Visit a specialist optician for coloured films or lenses Tends to guess words, progressed initially but now on a plateau of little or no progress and collapsing selfconfidence, finds long words easier than short words, good with familiar books but Text being processed visually without using the auditory cortex Use exercises designed to engage the auditory cortex and prevent guessing Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 120 cannot read words in isolation Has difficulty blending the individual sounds into words Lack of familiarity with the phonemic structure of words and difficulty with the blending process Exercise the separation and recombination of the phonemes in words Can blend short words but not longer words Either due to brain immaturity due to age (3-5 years old) Leave reading until the child has matured Or due to limited working memory capacity Work through exercises that move the process from declarative memory to procedural memory Tends to switch letters and words laterally (eg b/d and was/saw) Text being processed visually without using the auditory cortex Use exercises designed to engage the auditory cortex and prevent guessing Decodes words very slowly and struggles to follow the meaning of a sentence Limited working memory capacity Work through exercises that move the process from declarative memory to procedural memory Can read a single word competently but not sentences or paragraphs Poor eye control due to weakness in the flocculus. Exercises to improve eye tracking and movement control Can read fluently but cannot follow the meaning of the text at all Engagement of Broca’s area but not Wernicke’s area, probably as a result of visual reading Engage the auditory cortex and Wernicke’s area more actively in the reading process through suitable Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 121 strategies exercises Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 122 The History of Writing “The Foundation of Modern Civilisation” Introduction Isaac Newton said that he stood on the shoulders of giants to be able to see further, in the development of his theories. What he was really stood on was their books. Text has been the critical tool for the development of human society and technology over the past 5000 years. Without it, such progress would have been impossible. Socrates was a vehement opponent to the development of literacy, believing that it impoverished the mind and limited people’s depth of understanding. What he could not imagine was the globalisation and accumulation of knowledge through text. He lived in a city-state where every intellectual knew every other and where progress was glacially slow. Since then the specialisation of expertise has become extraordinary. I was talking to a Nobel Laureate the other day (as you do). He is a specialist in genetics and the mechanisms of cell division. It is an area of interest for me because I have a doubt about the molecular structure of DNA as presented by Crick and Watson. Strange as that may sound, I had gone along to his talk partly to get the answer to my unusual question. The conundrum is this. In a length of DNA there are thousands of turns of the helix. And the molecular structure presented by Crick Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 123 and Watson does not offer the possibility of reverses of direction. During cell division the nucleus opens up the DNA strands, to split and recombine them to form a new duplicate set. It all happens in a matter of minutes. If you have ever split a twisted-pair rope, you will know this suggests a considerable physical problem. As depicted in textbooks, it is impossible. The end of the DNA would be spinning so fast that the fluid in the cell would boil. There are theories about the DNA strand being cut into thousands of bits and recombined. But it is difficult to imagine a system working so accurately with such a messy process. One would imagine this was of concern to anyone involved in cell division mechanics and DNA. Here was a world expert... but I am still none the wiser. Despite a lifetime in the field, his specialisation was such that he was unable to answer me. In his words, it was just “not exactly his patch”. How could Socrates have imagined that? What would society look like now if we took literacy away from the human story? Vastly different, it is certain. Some may feel it would be better. But I dare say they have never attempted survival in a feudal, agrarian society. Perhaps they can only imagine themselves in the lord’s manor, rather than struggling for survival in the lowly masses! But very few of the lords died peacefully in their beds either. Those that live by the sword, tend to die by it too. Anyhow, apart from being a rather good story and totally central to our civilisation as we know it, the story of the development of writing is critical to understanding the reading process and how it should be learnt. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 124 The Relevance to How Children Read Now What I will show you is how the memorisation approach of the visual learner, that I have already described, is the way many people have learnt to read in other languages. Many scripts have been designed to be memorised. And a scribe with a good visual memory can recall around 10,000 words in these languages. The problem is that English script, using our 26-letter alphabet has not been designed to be read in that way. You are using the wrong tool for the job. And, as any craftsman will tell you, that leads to heartache. It is impossible to recall our 1 million plus words from sight memory. Here is the story. Early Writing – Pictograms and Logograms They say that sex and money drive the development of the world. In this case it is at least half true. Back in the early stages of civilisation people started to trade goods, sometimes over great distances. This meant that they needed to record their transactions. To do that they took little shaped stones and made marks on them. These were used as tokens to record stocks. The first accountants had arrived in 8000BC! Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 125 Clay Tokens The tokens would be collected in a clay envelope, which would be sealed shut. You would mark the envelope in a recognisable way so that the recipient knew it was from you. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 126 Clay envelopes You could then send someone off with a wagon full of goods and the recipient would be able to tell what should have arrived by breaking open the envelope. The same technique could be used when depositing goods into a communal or royal storehouse. The manager of the store could effectively give you a receipt for your goods. To avoid having to break open the envelope, they started to mark the contents of the envelope on the outside. In around 3300BC the Sumerians of Mesopotamia, who lived in what is now southern Iraq, realised that the tokens were no longer needed, because everything was recorded on the surface of the envelope. Why bother with the tokens inside? So they moved to a simple tablet of clay. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 127 Early clay tablet The marks were basic pictures of the goods being traded with simple annotations for quantity. In the early stages the designs were easily recognised as representations of the goods. We call them pictograms. Of course you can present far more than just farms goods in this way. They developed pictures for almost anything that could be drawn. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 128 They used a sharp stick to make the impressions on the soft clay. Over the next thousand years they switched to using a wedge to indent the clay and so this form of writing is known as cuneiform (from the Latin cuneus meaning wedge). Cuneiform Tablet So they could now write on a flat piece of clay and “sign it” with their personal design. To achieve a signature the sender would roll a personal design across the tablet using a carved cylinder. It was an early form of the signet ring that people used for centuries to seal envelopes, using wax. In some countries it is still the way people sign a document. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 129 In time the designs on these cylinder seals became quite stunningly beautiful. They usually combined some exquisite relief artwork with the name of the owner in cuneiform. They formed a very effective security device. XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX cylinder seal and design XXXXXXXXXXXXXXX This type of writing is known as a logographic. That is derived from the Greek Logo=meaning and Graph=picture. Almost every time a writing system has been independently developed (Sumerian, Egyptian, Mayan, Chinese, Indus Valley) this has been the starting point. The main exception is the Inca Quipu system that used knots in strings. Apparently they could write whole histories in this way. The missionaries burnt every Quipu they could find, in order to suppress the ancient history of the Incas and convert them to Catholicism. So now we don’t know how it worked. The Early Logosyllabary Logograms are quite handy for representing physical things and quantities. But one naturally develops a desire to represent other words too. So the next step was the Logosyllabary. To create a logosyllabary you use the rebus principle. That means that any logogram can also represent the first syllable of the word it represents. For instance, you might want to create a representation of the word treaty. One way to do that would be to use your logograms for tree and tea and combine them. You now have a new image representing tree-tea. That could either be a tree of tea or a treaty. And hey presto! You have created a new word in your writing system. This allowed for a huge growth in what could be written. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 130 Indeed, it became limitless. In time they began to write stories, how-to manuals and legal systems in this way. The Developed Logosyllabary Fairly quickly the early Sumerian logograms began to morph into symbols that were quicker and easier to create. This was a slow evolution over the centuries XXXXXXXimage of stages of cuneiform The result of all this was huge complexity. When you are reading a logosyllabary you have to switch back and forth between actual logograms and syllabic representations using the rebus principle, all the time. And the number of logograms increased steadily until there were thousands. Of course, it is a workable system of writing. The Chinese and Japanese use logosyllabaries and they manage large and complex societies with them. But a logosyllabary contains an intrinsic problem. In order to be a proficient reader, you have to memorise thousands of logograms. Whatever way you look at that, it involves a lot of time-consuming and tedious rote learning. For me that is quite close to Hell on Earth, without the heat and physical pain, presuming your schoolmaster has had his cane confiscated. Sumerian masters running the schools for scribes seem to have been particularly keen on the benefits of a good beating. The Sumerians would spend 7 years learning to read and write in “tablet houses”. The students would copy and copy and copy, until it was drummed into them, or rather caned into them. The Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 131 students used to write subversive comic tablets that made regular use of the phrase “…and then he caned me”. Sometimes people view time spent studying as intrinsically good. More study is therefore “better”. I see it differently. Time spent studying one thing is time not spent studying another. There is always an opportunity cost. Efficient use of time, combined with a balanced curriculum, is the key in my world. That is why I now object to the thousands of hours I had to spend on Latin (and a horrific bit of Greek). I am not disputing its benefits. I would just rather have been studying, with those same hours, French, German, Spanish, Portuguese, Russian, Mandarin, Arabic… or any of the languages that I could now be using. So a child who spends thousands of hours perfecting Chinese literacy is limited in other directions. There is another issue that has never been solved with the logosyllabary; how do you organise a dictionary? An English dictionary is ordered by our abecedary, the order we all agree on for our letters. There is no equivalent in Chinese. They usually try to gather words semantically, in groups of related types of meaning. But that is inevitably very loose. The grouping is very subjective and at least 10% end up in the miscellaneous group. Anyhow, we know that there is an alternative, with which you are very familiar. We will get to that soon... Sumerian Cuneiform appeared in roughly 3300BC and was last written in around 100AD. The Sumerian civilisation had long before fallen, replaced by the Akkadians. Sumerian has not been spoken since around 1600BC. The Akkadians maintained the “ancient script” for long after that, to maintain a link to the past. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 132 We do the same when we plaster lintels with Latin script. There is no reason to do that except to give the writing some form of added gravitas, aesthetic value or poetry. Or perhaps it is just for intellectual snob value. In every other sense it is a negative because 99% of the people looking at your words are untouched by their meaning. The Akkadians had developed their own script, which was also written in cuneiform, but was a straight syllabary and so easier to read. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 133 Akkadian cuneiform syllabary They kept a few of the old logograms, for old time’s sake, but they were now writing works on tablets and papyrus, designed for general consumption. The Epic of Gilgamesh, Advice of a Father to his Son and Dialogue of a Man with his God were a long way from the early accounting of 2000 years before: He who has seen everything, I will make known (?) to the lands. I will teach (?) about him who experienced all things, ... alike, Anu granted him the totality of knowledge of all. He saw the Secret, discovered the Hidden, he brought information of (the time) before the Flood. He went on a distant journey, pushing himself to exhaustion, but then was brought to peace. He carved on a stone stela all of his toils, and built the wall of Uruk-Haven, the wall of the sacred Eanna Temple, the holy sanctuary. Look at its wall which gleams like copper(?), inspect its inner wall, the likes of which no one can equal! Take hold of the threshold stone--it dates from ancient times! Go close to the Eanna Temple, the residence of Ishtar, such as no later king or man ever equaled! Extract from the Epic of Gilgamesh Writing had become very human and cultural. Complex information was being shared, laws were being written and explained and stories were being told. The sum of human knowledge was beginning to accelerate through each generation. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 134 The green shoots of what would become modern civilisation were growing. As you read the words of some author who lived 3000 years ago, you can actually feel that you know them as a person. Their personality shines through. We can see so clearly how similar they were in their worries, delights, fears and joys. By contrast, we can stare at the detritus pit of some Neanderthal village and know a lot about their lives, but we will never know their thoughts. We will never know in any detail how they felt about life and its challenges. That is forever a frustration with all of archaeology. Egyptian Hieroglyphs At almost the same moment as the development of early pictographic tablets in Sumeria, the Egyptians began to develop their system of hieroglyphs. We know that there was extensive trading going on and so it is inconceivable to me that the one was not inspired by the other. There is some debate as to who the first writers were, but generally it is thought to be the Sumerians by a couple of hundred years. Egyptian society was quite different to the Mesopotamian civilisation. The latter was based on city-states ruled by kings. Egypt was a single entity, ruled by a pharaoh. This structural difference was reflected in their writing as well. Cuneiform developed as a practical mechanism for running a system of trade within and between cities. From that base it spread into religious, legal and ceremonial roles as well. Hieroglyphs were foremost a royal and religious tool. There is much less evidence of it being used through the rest of society. Having said that, the Egyptians wrote on papyrus rather than clay Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 135 and so perhaps that is just a reflection of the different media, rather than the different use of the text. While cuneiform became very abstract, hieroglyphs remained gloriously pictorial. The rebus principle was still used and they could create words from combinations of pictograms. The story of the decoding of hieroglyphs is interesting. It shows how difficult it is for people to throw off the yoke of conventional wisdom in their thinking. The Renaissance was a time of renewed interest in all things classical. And Rome had more Egyptian obelisks than Egypt, thanks to the Roman taste for them in classical times. In 1419 a th manuscript from the 4 century AD was discovered in Greece, giving translations of hieroglyphs. It was by an Egyptian, called Horapollo, and a copy was put into print in 1505. It was very popular and went through 30 editions, spawning an entire academic discipline of hieroglyph decipherment. Here is an example of translation by Athanasius Kircher, one of the leading academics of the field in 1666: “The protection of Osiris against the violence of the Typho must be elicited according to the proper rites and ceremonies by sacrifices and by appeal to the tutelary Genii of the triple world order to ensure the enjoyment of the prosperity customarily given the Nile against the violence of the enemy Typho.” This was his learned view of the meaning of a cartouche on an obelisk just being erected at the time in Rome. A cartouche is a set of hieroglyphs placed in an oval within the line of the text. It was called a cartouche because it resembled the cartridges used in the rifles of French soldiers of the time. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 136 Example Cartouche of Ramesses II Current knowledge of the same cartouche would give the th translation as “Hahibre of the 26 Dynasty”. It was just the name of a pharaoh. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 137 Haropollo had written pure fantasy and an entire academic discipline had followed him down the path he created for 300 years. It can happen! The key to the accurate decipherment of hieroglyphs was the Rosetta Stone. It was dug up in 1799 by a French team and is now in the British Museum. Like the Mona Lisa, it is surprisingly small. It is about 3ft high and 2ft wide and has text in hieroglyphs, Greek and a previously unseen cursive form of hieroglyphs, now known as demotic. Here was a word for word translation in the three scripts. You would imagine it was child’s play to now break the code properly. In fact it took 24 years. There was this strong academic train of thought running, with power and prestige. It required a maverick to jump free of that and the world had to wait 24 years for it to happen. Interestingly that is also the same average time that it takes a new invention to become accepted as a mainstream product. A man demonstrated a steam engine, giving rides around Berkeley Square in London, two decades before Stephenson’s Rocket. And some poor chap was wandering around trying to get investment in his functioning MP3 player in the 1980s, over two decades before the ipod. They were absolutely typical inventors. Anyhow, it was quickly evident that some of the demotic script was phonetic. The immediate suppositions were that the demotic script was based on an alphabet and the hieroglyphs were entirely non-phonetic, as conventional wisdom stated. Both were wrong. The first person to start to break free of this thinking was an Englishman called Thomas Young. He had realised that the conventional thinking must be wrong and published his thinking. However it was Jean-Francois Champollion who finally cracked the code in 1823 and is given the credit for it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 138 Hieroglyphs are a mix of logograms, representing whole words, and phonetic signs using the rebus principle... just like cuneiform. The Arrival of the Alphabet In around 1500BC there was one of those huge moments in history that arrive with a whisper rather than a thunderclap. The very first evidence of a possible alphabet was found in a remote mine in Sinai. A Semitic group, working for the Egyptians seem to have taken a set of 30 hieroglyphs and created the first consonantal alphabet script. It is referred to as Proto-Sinaitic. There is only a tiny amount of it, but it seems that they had analysed the sounds in their language and had taken one hieroglyph for each consonant. They were not the first to have this idea. The Egyptians had also effectively developed an alphabet around a 1000 years before. In order to write a foreign name, for instance, they would spell it out. However, rather than use this innovation, they had gone the other way and developed more and more logograms with fewer and fewer scribes fully literate in them. It would be interesting to see how that happened. The scribes evidently did all the hard work of analysing their language and producing an alphabet, but then chose not to use it. I have a suspicion that they soon noticed how quickly a new apprentice could pick up the alphabet system, realised anyone would be able to learn to read fairly easily and began to shelve the project quicker than you can say “jobs for the boys”! The scribes were in a very privileged position in Egyptian society, standing just behind royalty and the priests. For them it made a lot of sense to stick with the old ways. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 139 Alphabet Power The alphabet soon proved to be an extraordinary invention. It had three massive advantages over the earlier systems: • It was possible to become literate in months rather than years. • The brain found it easier to process • It was very adaptable and plastic These are all very critical issues. The first meant that literacy became a mass activity, rather than the preserve of a few. The second meant that there was more time for the reader to actively consider what was being read, rather than just decoding it. The third meant that any new word could be encoded and decoded in any language with the same set of characters. This was a genuine revolution. There was soon a general spread of the use of alphabets all around the region, with experimentation in all sorts of directions. There was inevitably a lot of borrowing of designs between them. By 1400 a Kingdom called Ugarit had developed an alphabet using cuneiform characters Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 140 Ugaritic alphabet They were based in what is now northern Syria. We have thousands of Ugarit tablets, covering all the functions of a trading society, including religious texts with similarities to some of the Old Testament of the Bible. This is the earliest body of tablets that we have written with an alphabet script. The people from the area around modern Tunis were called the Phoenicians. They were famous traders. The name Phoenician means “dealer in purple” in Greek. One of their products was a purple dye. They had developed a particularly attractive and sparse alphabet in around 1100BC, which was copied by the Greeks in around 900BC. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 141 Phoenician and Greek Alphabets The Greeks now made the final innovation in the development of the full alphabet. Semitic languages can easily be written without vowels. Mny Nglsh wrds r rdbl wtht vwls s wll, but it isn’t easy! The Greeks added a Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 142 set of characters representing vowels to their alphabet, because it suits the Greek language better. This was both the first complete alphabet and also the entry of the alphabet into the European continent. It is a hard life as an innovator, as I have mentioned, and the scribes that developed the Ancient Greek alphabet must have died a little disappointed. It was 400 years before there was a mass switch to their new innovation. The Greeks had an oral culture and there was great resistance to changing that. Oratory was viewed as the highest intellectual endeavour by all ancient Greeks. Homer and Socrates probably never wrote a word. All we have is the transfer of their words from oral history to written history, by later writers. There is a new view that literacy actually took hold amongst much of the population much earlier. Apparently you can find rocks in the interior covered in graffiti by bored shepherds. However, I have to confess that was told to me by a local expert, late in the evening here in Oxford. I haven’t a clue whether it is true or not. Anyhow, literacy eventually became a Greek obsession. They wrote and wrote. We only have a fraction of what they generated. Study of the “Classics” and theology were for many centuries viewed as the only serious forms of academic endeavour. It is only in the last 100 years that the study of English writing has begun to be taken seriously. Most English universities had no department of English literature until around 1900. The arriviste Romans always viewed the Greeks as their more highly cultured neighbours. Initially the Etruscans and then the Romans adopted and adapted the Greek script to their own language. Of course there were many languages and dialects Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 143 across Italy and then throughout the Roman Empire, but it was administrated in Latin, using the Roman script. This touches on a very interesting area of language development. Until recently most people were partly bilingual. You always had a ruling elite speaking and writing one language and a population almost always speaking another language. Generally only the former is recorded, but both have an influence on the other. That has had a huge impact on English, as we will see. The Romans ran their western empire for around 600 years. So their influence on almost all the European languages since has been massive. Even after the fall of the empire every intellectual could read, write and speak Latin for another 1200 years. The Western Roman Empire collapsed around 400AD and Europe went through a thousand years of turbulence. Through the second millennium there was a gradual development of the nation state and a coalescing of national languages. The idea of a whole nation speaking the same language is a relatively new one. For instance, the Languedoc is the area where people spoke Occitan in southern France. Basques is still spoken in southwest France and northwest Spain. Catalan is still spoken around Barcelona. Three languages are spoken in Switzerland. Localised language or dialect has generally been the norm in most populations throughout history. The Formation of Tricky Old English Some languages such as Spanish have a considerable uniformity to their structure and pronunciation. Others, such as English, do not. English is a summer pudding of influences and origins. It is stamped with our history. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 144 In pre-Roman times England was largely inhabited by native tribes and ruled by Celts. The Celts were a majority in the population on the west coast of England, Scotland, Wales and Ireland and by the last century BC they were the overlords of all of the British Isles. I know that we are generally taught that the Celts have been “driven off” to the western extremes of the British Isles, but the evidence is the reverse. They seem to have settled originally in the west, overrun the country as the ruling elite and then got pushed back out of that role by the Romans. The evidence for that is the English language itself. It has Celtic, Roman, Danish, Anglo-Saxon and French influences. But at the core of all of that is a language, sometimes referred to as Anglian. Where does it come from, if not the native population of the island? Being an overlord of a native population is always a tricky situation when another invader appears. For instance, Cortes was able to take control of the whole of the Aztec empire with just a handful of Spaniards because everyone hated the Aztecs. So the local population backed the new invaders and helped overthrow their former masters. The Celts were fighting the invading Romans mainly with the local population. The Romans were evidently pretty good at what they did and were successfully administering most of the continent. The native Brits would have been well aware of that and I think that probably explains why Caesar was able to take over so easily. He actually seemed a better bet than those wild, woad-covered, ginger-haired Celts! After all, they generally ran a pretty good show, the Romans. Everything in life is relative. So the Romans now slid into position as the ruling class. The Brits still spoke mainly their own language, with Celtic influences and now Latin influences working their way in. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 145 This is again a disputed subject, but in my view, there were a number of local dialects spoken in ancient Britain. They are sometimes referred to as Anglian. That is a little confusing because it is so similar to Anglo-Saxon, which was the language of the Saxon overlords after the departure of the Romans. Anyhow, neither Celtic nor Anglian were written languages at this time. th The western Roman Empire fell in the 5 Century AD and the Roman legions were withdrawn in the preceding years. Any native with a tendency to military life was a Roman legionary by this time. So the islands were left virtually defenceless, both in personnel and military knowledge. Nature abhors a vacuum and so very warlike Scandinavians began arriving on our shores to fill the gap. Some of that was just your average daytrip plunder-and-move-on style tourism. Others settled in quite large numbers down the east coast. So now a whole new set of vocabulary began to work its way into the local dialects. To counter the threat from the Vikings, the leaders of southern Britain invited over some Saxon mercenaries. It did not take long for the mercenaries to work out that ruling was more fun. So, the north and east of the country had strong Viking settlements, the south had Saxon settlements and the west was largely Celt. This was how things stood until 1066, when William the Conqueror arrived. He was a Norman speaking French, with a bunch of Norman barons. They quickly settled the country under their rule and for the next 100 years the ruling elite all spoke French. Even to this day, the French words in our language are viewed as being less attractive and real than the native equivalents. If you Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 146 want to write “attractive” English to the ordinary Englishman, exclude all those high-fallutin French imports, because at some subconscious level, they don’t go down well! It is a measurable effect. All of this makes English by far the hardest of all Indo-European languages to learn to read. It is due to our history, not any defect in our school system. We were crossed by waves of invaders until 1066. The Celts, the Romans, the Danes, the Vikings, the Angles, the Saxons and the Normans all came by and each one has deposited some of their language. Then in the last 300 years we have gone out around the world and collected a whole new lexicon from our colonial period. Bungalow (Hindi), coffee (Turkish), guru (Sanskrit), orangutan (Malaysian), paparazzi (Italian), pyjamas (Persian), safari (Swahili), shampoo (Hindi) and yoghurt (Turkish) are all imports for instance. All of this mixing has made English a rich language, but tricky to learn to read. It is termed morphophonemic, because some of the spelling is derived from the original meaning root of the word, while it is also phonetic in the use of the letters. For instance, the English word muscular is derived from the Latin word musculus. The c in muscular is phonetic, to give the hard /c/ sound. In the word muscle the c is only present in order to link the word back to its root, and therefore its meaning, through its morphology. Until Samuel Johnson wrote his dictionary in 1755 there was not even a generally agreed spelling to many of the words in the language. Sometimes there were half a dozen different spellings of each word. This was increased by the way printers would adjust Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 147 the length of words to achieve justification of the text, rather than adjusting the spacing between them. It was linguistic chaos! Dr Johnson effectively set the rules that we live by now. But it has led to much heartache ever since. There have always been calls for the redesign of English spelling around a set of fixed rules. So far they have been ignored and probably always will be, unless some dictator takes control and enforces it as part of his legacy. English Today We have 26 letters, but there are around 44 individual phonemes (sounds) in English and over 70 if you count all the little variations in these 44. These 70+ sounds can be represented by over 200 letter patterns (graphemes). By contrast, Spanish has 34 phonemes and 40 graphemes. You can guess which is the easier to learn to read! This constant cycle of failure has led to huge disagreements on the best way to teach reading. So we need to have a look at the history of the Reading Wars to understand what has been going on. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 148 The History of Teaching Literacy “The 400-Years War” Introduction Most people think that the Reading Wars are quite a recent phenomenon and that the systems being proposed are new. So, it is interesting to look back and see just how new they are. Synthetic phonics was first presented as the best way to teach reading by John Hart, in his book A Methode or comfortable beginning for all unlearned, whereby they may bee taught to read English in a very short time, with pleasure, published in 1570. It is funny to see reading systems being oversold even 400 years ago! Whole word approaches to reading were viewed as very trendy in th the 20 century. But the first recorded proponent of that approach was Jan Amos Comenius in the early 1600s. Those two main beliefs in the right way to teach reading have been battling it out ever since, with the tide of opinion swinging back and forth over roughly a 40-year cycle. There have been other interventions tried, like the ITA (Initial Teaching Alphabet). So this section of the book will have a look in more detail at this history, to give us some perspective on the current situation. The Oral Tradition All languages start purely oral. Writing is always a secondary development, if it is recording a spoken language. That may seem Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 149 obvious, but it is critical to an understanding of how reading was learnt in the past. An essential part of an oral culture is the ability to memorise, because all knowledge is held in memory. Nowadays, that facility is much reduced for most of us. Most exams are now based around the interpretation of knowledge and very little emphasis is put on pure memorisation of knowledge. Literacy weakens the oral memory because it is less exercised. In an information rich environment like today, that makes sense as an approach. All information is available, in various recorded forms, and is so vast as to be beyond the possible scope of a single human being. Therefore, accessing, manipulating, interpreting and judging information have become far more important skills. But the human brain is still exceptionally good at memorising. Even today, it is not thought extraordinary that a professional actor should be able to recall most of the plays of Shakespeare verbatim. It is also thought a standard measure of the devotion of a Muslim to be able to recite the Koran. People can recite the value of pi to many thousands of digits. And memory specialists can store whole telephone directories, even if their reason for doing so is less clear. But in any society with limited literacy memorisation is a central measure of intelligence and is widely practised. All early forms of literacy teaching were based on that premise. First and foremost a child would be learning most of their knowledge through memorisation. So, the most basic form of literacy teaching was to get a child to copy a text that was already familiar. In the days of cuneiform, this was done with tablets that had been inscribed by the teacher on one side and were then copied on the reverse by the student. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 150 It was clearly a slow and tedious process. For Chinese school children it still is. The written language is effectively learnt by pure repetition, which is famous for its dullness. In recent times, writing lines was viewed as an excellent punishment, for this very reason. I was trying to improve my own poor writing style a few years ago and I was advised to develop it by copying the texts of good authors. I am sure it would be effective as a technique, but I know that it is beyond my limited capabilities to stick with such an essentially dull task. Children, in particular, do not naturally apply themselves to dull tasks, except under duress. The result was that teaching writing and reading was connected with a corporeal punishment regimes. Sumerian apprentice scribes were routinely beaten. And that has happened very recently too. Until around 1900 elementary school teachers were largely paid by results. Any child who did not learn to read was not paid for. So you can well imagine the power of this incentive on the already low-paid teachers. School was a harsh and bitter experience for most children. Abceebooks As we know, writing had changed substantially between early Sumerian and the first writing of English in around 1200. The alphabet had been invented and English is written using the Roman alphabet. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 151 th So, when we pick up the story in the 16 century, the alphabet was recognised as a key element of our language. Children were then learning to read by the ABC method. To achieve this, a child had an abceebook, which was an early English reading primer. The abceebook was a wooden paddle. It had a printed sheet attached to it and often had a thin sheet of horn covering it as protection. They were often known as hornbooks. In the hands of children, these hornbooks inevitably got used as bats for playing ballgames and so were also known as battledores. XXXXXXIMAGE OF ABCEEBOOKXXXXXXXXX On the printed sheet were the alphabet, the 9 digits and some familiar religious texts such as the Lord’s Prayer. The most fundamental aim of education, at this time, was for the child to be able to read the Bible. So, the children would be orally very familiar with the text on the abceebook. They would learn the alphabet and then study the text printed on it, already knowing it by heart. Part of this process was to stand and read the text out. This was done by saying the letters of the word (ie spelling it out) and then saying the word. It may seem amazing that anybody could learn to read in this way. But the brain is actually very good at making connections through familiarity. By using this rather laborious process, the child was becoming familiar with the interior structure of the word, albeit using the letter names. One has to remember that the idea of entertaining a child to help the learning process was still a long way off becoming mainstream. Once a child could read, writing might be introduced. Writing was a considerable and messy task in those days, with knives, quills, Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 152 inkpots, blotters and parchment. It was really viewed as a craft, more than the obvious and inevitable extension of reading. In 1746 Benjamin Collins invented a cardboard booklet form of the abceebook, which replaced the hornbooks. He still called it a battledore, even through it was now useless for actually playing battledores and shuttlecocks. The alphabet also used to appear in biscuits, loaves of bread, needlework tapestries and other designs. And even today, a plastic alphabet set is one of the earliest educational toys a child will receive. The First Phonics System While we can see the main approach being used, within this I am sure that many teachers were aware of the more complex phonetic structure of the words and were guiding their students through it. The first to record this by publishing a book about it was John Hart, who wrote A Methode or comfortable beginning for all unlearned, whereby they may bee taught to read English in a very short time, with pleasur. He was the founding father of the entire phonics teaching movement. Here are some pages from his groundbreaking book: XXXXXXXXImages of A Methode..XXXXXXX You can see just how close he was to the current phonics reading systems. He had analysed all the different sounds and graphemes and developed a system of presentation of the text to make it easier for the child to see the phonetic structure of the words. He was building on the existing approach of using a familiar text, but tried to break it open for the child. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 153 Since 1570 there have been many dozens of similar attempts to do the same thing. But they all struggle with the same conundrum. The Phonics Conundrum John Hart developed his system because he could see that some children were not engaging with the phonetic structure of the word. As they worked through the primer, they knew the words of the text. So they had no need to try to relate the structure of the word to the sounds in it. They just saw the words as pictures. And this is the phonic dilemma. If a child naturally takes to phonics, all is well. If a child does not take to phonics, they tend to memorise words instead. So, you can teach them phonics, but when they are faced by an unfamiliar word, they can either read it or not. If they can’t then they have almost no route to decoding it. They are just stuck. English is by far the worst language for this, because it is so phonetically irregular. This is the conundrum that we have addressed with Guided Phonetic Reading and Easyread TrainerText. The First Whole Word Warrior As I have described, school was a pretty dismal experience in the th 16 Century. Even an enormously gifted student like Shakespeare wrote about hating it. But some people could see that it was far easier to teach a happy and engaged child than a battered and frightened one. The first to write about this was Jan Amos Comenius. He is often referred to as the “father of modern education”. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 154 Comenius was born in 1592 in Moravia. He lived through the horrors of the 30 Years War and as a Protestant ended up as a migrant exile much of his life, having fled his homeland in 1628. He believed in universal education of men and women and that the experience was more efficient if it was a pleasant one. He also believed in education following the principles of nature, which were themselves, after all, created by God. One element of this was his belief in children learning to read text through familiarity with whole words, rather than the rote learning of the conventional abceebooks. Comenius has also been the inspiration for the movement towards the modern “primary school ethos”. It is the aim of most primary schools nowadays that the experience should be, first and foremost, a pleasant one for every child. If the children are not enjoying school, then the potential for successful education is much reduced. The logical extension of this has been a belief that the stresses of winning and losing, being better or worse, should be removed as far as possible. This feeds into gestalt psychology, which views the whole of a person as being more than the collection of the person’s parts and every part of the person having an influence on the whole. The education of the child begins with the holistic aim of general happiness, which then feeds into maximum progress in each of the parts of their education. When you are not familiar with all of this, it can catch you out. I can remember an incident when I was first helping in our local primary school as a parent. It illustrates the fundamental contrast in this approach to how life works outside the primary school environment. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 155 I had gone on a residential trip to the south coast with the 9 yearolds of the school and we were gathered outside the building before the next activity. The children were in a gang of about 50 and were all chatting volubly. The teacher needed to brief them and started saying “quiet please” in a voice barely louder than I would use to pass a comment to my neighbour in the pew of a church, mid-sermon. After about 20 seconds of this, my army training got the better of me and so I shouted, as pleasantly as I could, “OK… shut up and listen to Mrs White”. It certainly worked… but I don’t know who was more shocked; the children or Mrs White. The whole word approach to literacy fits extremely well with this psychology. First, it gets very quick results. The human brain is very good at memorising images and so a child can often learn a few sight-words quite quickly. So, that is very good for their psychology. Then you are also straight into reading real text. Comenius actually wrote books for children, which was evidently a huge step forward to going over the Lord’s Prayer for the hundredth time, beautiful as it may be. By contrast, the phonics approach to reading has become more and more focused on the development of awareness of the individual phonemes and the letter patterns that can represent them. Reading text is kept back as a potentially dangerous activity in the early stages. So, phonics tends to be a very mechanistic teaching approach, which is very uncomfortable for many primary school teachers, who are often more naturally comfortable with a whole word approach. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 156 The Whole Word Conundrum Early progress normally goes well with the whole word approach. However, as the complexity of the text increases, reading by whole word recognition gets proportionately more difficult. By contrast, once a child can decode words phonetically, reading gets easier in proportion to the experience of text accumulated. Our text is not designed for this type of sight memorisation. As a result, most children will reach a plateau, beyond which their reading cannot progress, unless they make the leap to phonetic decoding themselves. You will find many adults who have been taught by this method skip through the text, picking up words they are familiar with, working out others from various cues (such as the context and first letter) and jumping others completely. It is a hugely inefficient process, which leaves them effectively just partially literate. The Camps Form on the Battlefield th th From these early beginnings in the late 16 and early 17 centuries, the camps for the future Reading Wars began to form. Comenius was joined in the whole word camp by major luminaries such as Rousseau, Herbart, Froebel and Parker, who encouraged greater attention to meaning in all education and especially th th reading. Through the 18 and 19 centuries, this overarching aim led to the development to more and more sophistication around the word approach to reading. Here is a quote from a specialist writing in The Common School Journal in 1842: “My theory is that words can be more easily remembered than letters, for two reasons. In the first place they are not such minute Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 157 objects, and the faculty by which the forms are distinguished can therefore more readily perceive them; in the second place, every word that a child learns, will (if judiciously selected) convey a distinct image of a thing, or an act, to his mind, and can be more easily remembered than the name of a letter with which he can have no natural associations. What is invaluable in this method is, that the children are always happy to learn thus… it is altogether an unnatural one to learn writing signs to which nothing already known can be attached. Until I was convinced that this was the best method, I always found myself instinctively helping innocent children along, through their first steps, by means which, at the time, I half thought were tricks and pernicious indulgences. I feared I was depriving them of some wholesome and desirable discipline… but will never again force helpless children of three and four years, to learn the alphabet and the abc, until reading is so charming to them that every letter is interesting from its position and its association with the word it helps to form. When letters are learned in the ordinary manner, they are often associated artificially with some image as a stands for apple, b for boy etc; and these associations are so many hindrances to the next step in the process, because they must all be unlearned before the letters can be applied to other words… I frequently tell little children who know the alphabet to look at the word, rather than spell it over with the lips, and then tell me, without the book, what the letters are, assisting them by saying, “Observe what two letters are in the middle, or what two are at the end, or in the beginning. I therefore say to those who make the objection, that this mode of teaching to read leaves spelling out of the question, that it only defers spelling a little, and that the first words spelled should be those which are already perfectly familiar to the eye. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 158 It is desirable that in the first book there should be many repetitions of the same words, and experience has convinced me that nothing can be learned easily or remembered well that is not so arranged as to have some natural sequence. For this reason I would arrange the first words in natural groups. (In a story of a bird, for instance, after some of the principal names of this, such as bird, tree, wings, feathers, bill, etc have been mentioned, several actions may be introduced. These are the things the birds do; - build, fly, sing, etc. Then the names of the colours are easily associated, and even the words the, which, and and.) After learning a few groups of words often repeated on a page, let these be combined in short sentences. These sentences children will learn with great ease, and they will remember the particles that necessarily connect the names of things and the actions. They will of their own accord, turn back to the pages where they first became familiar with the words; and when this process of comparison has gone on a little while, if no pain is associated with it, the improvement will be rapid. Children of six, who begin to read thus by learning words instead of letters, will be able in three months to read simple stories very easily… One or two instances have quite astonished me, however… My present class of that age are beginning to emerge from all their puzzles, and their desires to read are perfectly insatiable. One boy of seven, who has been much neglected is still on the Slough of Despond, and cannot yet read better than little L., who began three of four months ago. His knowledge of letters does not help him forward; but light is dawning upon him since he has been made to spell only the words he knows… I would have the stories in the first book attractive to the imagination, that they may be frequently recurred to with pleasure, Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 159 - and the first words well impressed before the vocabulary is much enlarged. Repetition is a great secret of real progress..; After the process of spelling has become familiar, classes of words of similar pronunciation and appearance should be given; as boy, toy, joy. It is also an admirable exercise to let children spell over their reading lesson from the book after reading, pronouncing each word distinctly before they read the letters. The good effect of this mode of drilling will soon be perceived both in oral spelling and writing. Let me impress it upon all teachers, if I have not already made it sufficiently clear, that spelling is only to be deferred a little while, till it can be begun with advantage; and then there cannot be too much of it. A quick ear for music will assist some children, a good organ of form will aid others, and frequent repetition of the sight and sound of letters, in their various combinations, will help all. Above all, let the whole process be made agreeable, and there is no fear of want of cooperation on the part of the little people.” That is a pretty succinct description of the whole word teaching method. It was little changed in the 1940s and 1980s, the two high-water marks of whole word and real books theory in the last 100 years, except that the spelling drills have been much reduced. Interestingly it was only signed “M”. I guess the Reading Wars were already raging. No teachers have been burnt at the stake over this issue, but many have feared losing their jobs. th Through the middle of the 18 Century, there was a growing body of materials designed for this approach. For instance, J. Russell Webb published “A New Method Of Teaching Children To Read: Founded on Nature and Reason” in 1850, with new editions in 1856 and 1874. Here is an extract from the “Directions for Teaching” from part 1 of Webb’s system: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 160 “It is a lamentable fact, and one which the community is beginning to understand, that children have been wrongly taught their first lessons in reading… It may be asked by some, why we have commenced with words instead of letters. We answer, a word can as easily be learned as a letter; and, in addition, a word has some meaning – a letter none. A word conveys to the mind an idea, the mind acts to receive it; the letter has no such effect. The former necessarily teaches a child to think; the latter teaches - nothing. The child, in this part, is not to be taught a letter, or to spell a word, but is simply to learn the words by their forms, the same as he learns to the names of animals, by looking at them as a whole as an animal – associating the name with the form. The child reads this naturally, by sight, the same as all persons read, and understanding the meaning of every word, of course, reads with ease and pleasure.” In part 2 Webb goes on to “Teaching New Words, Reading, Spelling, the Alphabet, and the Sounds of the Letters.” So, in exactly the same way as XXXM, he believed that the essential difference between the two approaches was not what was being taught, but the order of teaching. For them, the first stage was to get the child reading and then start to explore the internal structure of the words. That is known as a “top-down” approach. A phonics teacher will view it the other way. In phonics teaching the internal structure of the word is taught first and an ability to deal with the whole word emanates from that. The same arguments are heard again and again. The disciples of phonics have always said: • English text is based on an alphabet, not logographs Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 161 • It is impossible for most children to accurately memorise more than a few words • If the child becomes familiar with the phonetic structure of the text then decoding any word becomes possible The disciple of whole words and real books have always replied: • English has a very irregular phonetic structure with over two hundred letter patterns representing the 44 phonemes. • The letters of the alphabet, the phonemes and the various grapheme-phoneme relationships are very abstract and therefore dull to a child. It is a form of child abuse to make them wade through hours of phonics instruction. • Even when a child does learn to read through phonics it is a mechanistic process with little joy or benefit derived, because the attention to meaning in the text has been pushed into a subsidiary position to the decoding of the text. • Many children struggle to follow phonics and never develop an ability to blend the individual sounds into words. Everything they say, in both camps, is true. Here is a preface from a book published in 1870: “In his experience as a teacher and superintendent of schools, it became evident to the author, many years ago, that there was something fundamentally wrong in the ordinary methods of teaching reading, writing and spelling. Viewed from the standpoint of economy, the result bore no just ratio to the time and effort devoted to these branches; and viewed from the standpoint of education, the first years of instruction seemed imperfect and unsatisfactory. This conviction, which he shared with many Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 162 teachers throughout the country, led to examination and experiment. In 1858, the phonetic system was introduced into the schools of Syracuse, N.Y., and for a time it was thought that the true method of teaching children to read had been discovered. After a trial of five years, however, it was seen that while pupils learned to read by this method in much less time than usual, and attained a high state of excellence in articulation, their reading was nearly as mechanical as before and few of them became good spellers. The two systems of analysis, phonic and graphic, had so little in common that permanent confusion was produced in the mind. The word method, next tried, was much more productive of good results than any that had preceded it; yet by this method words were treated as units, independent of sentences, and oral reading almost of necessity became a series of independent pronunciations, perpetuating the mechanical results of the old methods.” This is taken from The Sentence Method of Teaching Reading, Writing and Spelling, A Manual for Teachers (1881) by Farnham. It is a good example of the madness that can grip people in their efforts to solve the perpetual problems described by him. He continues: “These experiments and their results led to further investigation, especially in the line of psychology. From a close observation of the action of the mind, and of the relations of language to thought, it was seen that the unit of thinking is a thought, and therefore that the sentence ought to be made the basis of reading exercises. In 1870 a series of experiments was instituted in the schools of Binghampton, N.Y., to subject this theory to a practical test. The results far exceeded expectation in the direct teaching of reading, spelling and writing, and led to other results in awakening the mind and in influencing conduct, which were unexpected and gratifying. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 163 It is safe to assume that the problem, how to teach these branches successfully, has been solved. Reading consists: first, in gaining the thoughts of an author from written or printed language: - second, in giving oral expression to these thoughts in the language of the author, so that the same thoughts are conveyed to the hearer. It is important that this two-fold function of reading should be fully recognised. The first, or silent reading, is the fundamental process. It is often called “Reading to one’s self,” a phrase significant as indicating a wrong conception of the true end to be accomplished. The second, oral reading, or “reading aloud,” us entirely subordinate to silent reading. While oral expression is subject to laws of its own, its excellence depends upon the success of the reader in comprehending the thought of the author. The importance of these distinctions is so great that I will consider them in detail. It is scarcely possible to exaggerate the importance of correct “eye reading;” – of the ability to look over the written or printed page, and with the least possible consciousness of the words used, to fully comprehend the thoughts expressed. A common process is indicated by the expression, “reading to one’s self.” This means the translation of written into oral language. The reader either pronounces each word so that he can actually hear it, or he thinks of the pronunciation. In either case the thought is not formed in his mind directly through the written language, but indirectly after the written words have changed into oral expression. This process is slow and laborious, it becomes painful when long continued; and its practice will account for the antipathy which so many persons have to reading books and articles of considerable length. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 164 The object in teaching should be to make every pupil an eye reader, - to give him the ability to look directly through the written expression to the meaning, or to at once detect the unknown elements that prevent the accomplishment of this object… The child has come into possession of his powers, both of thought and of expression, by a gradual and unconscious process. He has simply been shaped by his surroundings. By association with those who talk, he has acquired the power of understanding speech and of speaking. The kind of speech, whether perfect or imperfect, which he hears and reproduces. This fact should be distinctly understood and realized. The powers of speech and of understanding what is said, both come to the child by a process so simple and natural that he is conscious of no effort to acquire them. Speech, objectively considered, is only a combination of sounds uttered in quick succession, having not the slightest resemblance to the thoughts represented; but by the child it is understood with exactness and uttered with precision. The whole complicated process is matured without effort, and without the intervention of teachers. To make the eye perform the office of the ear, and the hand that of the organs of the voice, is the problem that presents itself in attempting to teach a child to read and write. The vital point is to so change the function of the eye that it will look upon written or printed characters, not as objects to be recognized for their own sake, but as directly calling into conscious being past experiences, and so becoming representative of thought. All the efforts of the teacher should be directed to this end. At this point our education has often failed. The process of translating the written language into speech is so slow and difficult that a large share of the pupils in our schools are condemned to comparative ignorance. The words as they appear have no Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 165 meaning to them. One who acquired the power of directly receiving thought from the printed page, is endowed with a new intellectual faculty. His eye flashes along the pages of a book, and he comprehends whole sentences at a glance. It would not do to say that these rapid readers do not understand what they read. The fact is they understand much better than the slow reader. The mental power, being relieved from the necessity of translating, concentrates itself upon the thought, and the thought is understood and remembered. Our endeavour should be to give pupils this power of eye reading from the first so that they may continually profit by it, and have no evil habits to overcome.” It is difficult to put into the right words, what utter rubbish this is from beginning to end. First, he suggests that children develop the facility of understanding words and then speaking “without effort and without the intervention of teachers.” That will have any mother either apoplectic with rage or laughing derisively. Hopefully we can all do the latter rather than get cross, because the poor fool knew not of what he wrote. Learning to understand and reproduce the spoken word is one of the longest and most laborious processes of our development. Without a dedicated parent or carer, it does not happen. He then goes on to suggest that absorbing meaning from text can be achieved directly, without any focus on the structure of the text, either phonic or in the form of the words. That is patently and completely obviously incorrect. I would have loved to sit him in front of some text converted into another script (as I did earlier in the book for you), to see how long it took him to learn to absorb the meaning directly. But there is a crucial lesson to be taken from this madness; he was not a ranting loony in the periphery of education, but a major Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 166 player in East Coast USA. He was taken seriously; both his theories th and his “gratifying results”, right into the 20 Century. His Sentence Method is a respected form of teaching. Success with the approach he suggests is clearly impossible, for all except the occasional savant. Therefore, his results must have been a misrepresentation of what really happened. I am afraid he was not the first or the last person to do that. It is virtually impossible to avoid distortion of results in favour of your theory. Mendel, the father of genetics, did it, even though his theory was absolutely correct and his results reflected that. He could not help tidying them up a bit. Don’t think he is the only scientist to have done that! I think it is a deep psychosis, because people have a great need for their theories to be correct. Most people are actually unaware of the selective way they are treating the information around them. In other realms it can be even more serious. In the Second World War, General Montgomery developed a bold plan to grab the bridges behind the German front lines near Arnhem, so that they couldn’t be blown up as the Germans retreated during a coordinated assault. It was going to be a glorious venture that he would be remembered for in the history books of the European campaign. A few days before the operation was due to launch, an aerial reconnaissance aircraft picked up signs of a German Panzer brigade on furlough in a wood near one of the bridges. This was hugely serious since paratroopers are no match for tanks. The plan was utterly compromised. Montgomery ignored the intelligence and went ahead anyhow. It was clearly suicidal for the troops involved and doomed to failure. And he had the experience and training to know that. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 167 But his desire for the plan to work and for his star to rise on it, pushed him towards making this massive mistake. He certainly made it into the history books, but not in the way he had hoped. His decisions were driven by emotions not rationality. The results are portrayed in the film A Bridge Too Far. And educationalists are human too. They desperately want to achieve a better result and sometimes will see it when the reverse is true. If Farnham did get good results, it was almost certainly in direct proportion to the degree that the teachers in the classrooms ignored his suggestions. And this leads us to one of the unstated realities of education; most experienced teachers ignore the fad of the day. While the educational strategists battle each other and publish their theories, teachers are spending the day in class, managing groups of 30+ children. You can only appreciate the enormity of that task by doing it. They are controlling, educating, entertaining and supporting all these children for 7 hours a day. So, they do that the way they know how. It may not be perfect, but at least they know it and know the results they can get with it. Many of the theories that are offered up are pretty complex and difficult to apply. Also, teachers are familiar with the realities of teaching. They cannot give more than a few minutes of individual attention to each child each week. Every child is different. Many children do not follow what is going on in a group setting. It is just a tough job. I personally think they do wonders to keep the children heading in roughly the right direction, follow all the directives they have to follow, deal with all their colleagues/school management/parents Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 168 and all the while remaining sane most of the time. I know that I couldn’t achieve that. th So, through the 19 century, while these different competing theories were flying around, most teachers seem to have taught reading in the way that they always had done. That still involved a lot of use of abceebooks and the group reading of known texts. Children were routinely required to read out loud from a book. When I was a child it was very common to hear people stammering, and it is very rare nowadays. I think the stammering habit was probably triggered by this routine of having to read aloud, while learning to read. It is almost unheard of as a technique now. The Development of Linguistics th During the 19 century, there was the first study of linguistics; the study of languages as a science. People had always studied languages, of course. Samuel Johnson’s dictionary was based on his understanding of the roots of our language. But the nature of this study took a new turn with Erasmus Rask’s Investigation on the Origin of the Old Norse or Icelandic Language, in 1818. Rask began to take the study away from personal theory and into the field of impersonal and cumulative knowledge, that can be independently verified by peers. Over the next 50 years, this academic study of linguistics led to the publication in 1875 of Life and Growth of Languages: An outline of Linguistic Science, by William Dwight Whitney of Yale University. At the same time as all of this linguistic research, the first Oxford English Dictionary was being written. They started collecting Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 169 evidence for it in 1858. They began editing it in 1878 and published the first part in 1884. It was not completed until 1928. It took a fundamentally different approach to Dr Johnson’s dictionary of 1755. That recorded the current meaning of words in th use in the mid 18 century. The aim of the Oxford English Dictionary was to chart the use of the words through history, ending up with the current usage. th This was only possible through the linguistic studies of the 19 century. And it gave a much deeper understanding of the phonetic origins of each word, through analogical creation and borrowing from other languages. Analogical creation was the adoption of a word structure across the language. For instance, we are all now familiar with the use of an s at the end of a noun to indicate the plural form. This originated with just male “-a stem” words. But has spread across the whole language from that base. An understanding of these transformations, and borrowings from other languages, began to patch up the very inconsistent nature of the understanding of the sound laws operating in the language. This led to the idea of “phonetic laws without exceptions” for the English language. All of this academic activity fed new energy into the phonics camp, leading to a new surge of reading development ideas in the late th th 19 century and early 20 century. This led in three directions. The first was spelling reform, the second was the development of special alphabets and the last was a redoubling of the efforts behind the classic phonics approach. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 170 Spelling Reform and the IPA There has always been a rumbling call to change the way we spell English words. In fact John Hart published a “newe maner of writing” in 1569. It is an obvious solution. He was followed over the centuries by Sir Thomas Smith, William Bullokar, Edmund Coote, Alexander Gill, Charles Butler, John Wilkins, Benjamin Franklin, A J Ellis and Isaac Pitman. They have all developed or suggested the use of different writing systems, alphabets or spelling codes to try to simplify the inconsistencies of English. Obviously, none of their suggestions has been taken up. The movement is still represented even today by mad Masha Bell, author of Learning To Read, who wrote in the Times Education Supplement (29 August 2009) “The bottom line is that coping with English spelling conventions requires a higher level of basic intelligence than roughly 20 per cent of learners at the lower end of the ability range are endowed with.” Hmmm… that is her rationale for changing English. Nice. Of course they are all correct that a simpler writing system would make English easier to read. That can be seen in the far higher literacy rates of languages with a consistent phonetic code. But it isn’t going to happen. However, one product of all of this is the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) published in 1888. XXXXImage of the IPA It detailed twenty-six vowel sounds and fifty-two different consonants, used in different languages. It is still used today in many dictionaries to indicate the phonetic structure of the words. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 171 Special Alphabets Similar, but different, to the idea of spelling reform was the idea of special alphabets and writing systems for learners. These are designed to be a stepping-stone for the learner to gain an ability to read, which would then be transferred over to normal text. th All the thinking for this was done in the mid 19 century and the most famous, the Initial Teaching Alphabet (ITA) was designed then. But they only came into their own a hundred years later. So we will look at them in more detail in due course. 1875-1925 Phonics Surge In addition to the IPA, and ITA the development of Linguistics as a science, gave the phonics fanatics a new level of academic gloss to th their systems and strategies. In the late 19 century there was a big surge of new systems and the mother of modern phonics systems is Nellie Dale, who published her phonic reading programme in 1899. Of course, we can now see that she was basing her work on a wealth of prior development of phonics, running all the way back to 1570. She did bring a couple of innovations to the process. The first was to make it more multisensory. Children would learn to engage with the letters and phonemes with ear, hand and eye. And this was also used to judge whether a child was “phonic ready”. It is certainly true that a certain level of maturity seems to be needed to begin identifying and blending phonemes. The second was that she added colour to the text to highlight vowels (red), voiced consonants (black), unvoiced consonants (blue) and silent letters (yellow). Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 172 This colouring approach was also picked up by others (eg Moxon and Gattegno in the 1960s), but has since been dropped by most phonic systems, as a distraction. th In general however, the first two decades of the 20 century did belong to the phonic fanatics. Whole Wordies Get Science Too th Durng the early part of the 20 century there was a tremendous pressure building for schooling to move away from the rather grim practices of the past. Comenius was finally coming to the fore and was the inspiration for a whole group of educational reformers. In England the 1870 Elementary Education Act created the state education of children, with free provision of education for every child from 5 to 12 and compulsory education until 10. The curriculum was controlled by the Revised Code, established in 1862, which also set the framework of payment by results for the teachers. So there was tremendous focus on achievement of a pass in the final tests, to be sure that the grants were paid. You can imagine the psychology of the teachers. Although the code was dropped in 1895 and the curriculum dropped in 1902, the style of education was well ingrained and took several decades to change. A key force for change was the Newbolt Report in 1921. It said that the rote learning of the past should be replaced by: • the universal need for literacy as the core of the curriculum • the developmental importance of children’s self expression Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 173 • a belief in the power of English Literature for moral and social improvement • a concern for the ‘full development of the mind and character’ This was followed by a series of Hadow Reports (1926, 1931 and 1933) taking this line of revolutionary thinking even further: “We are of the opinion that the curriculum of the primary school is to be thought of in terms of activity and experience rather than of knowledge to be acquired and facts to be stored.” A great range of new thinking was taking hold, based on the th writings of Comenius and gestalt theory of the 19 century. Gestalt psychology proposes that the brain operates in a holistic way, concentrating on the form of the whole, rather than analysing the components making up the whole. For animals for instance, this is certainly true. A man sat on a horse is not recognised as a human being by most animals. Tourists sat in an open Landrover are not recognised as human beings by lions in African reserves. However it is much less true of humans. We are always searching for the parts of the whole. If you have ever taken your car into the monkey enclosure of a safari park, you will know that all primates seem interested in taking things apart. It has now lost influence as a theory in psychology, but just as linguistic science gave phonics an academic home to work from, gestaltism was the new academic stronghold for the whole word believers. It could have been written for them. th In fact, through the middle of the 20 century, the whole word concept spread into a number of related but different approaches and by the end of the 30s phonics was in retreat everywhere. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 174 Look & Say The most basic use of gestalt theory was Look & Say. The underlying premise of Look & Say is that the bulk of early reading is based on a skeleton of around 100 very frequent words, many of which are phonetically irregular. So, the most economical and easy way for a child to get into reading is just to memorise those words. Then, with this skeleton in place, the child’s vocabulary can be extended through contact with interesting texts. So, Look & Say uses flash cards to present this skeleton vocabulary. And the child is then encouraged to engage with text in specially written books, that slowly extend the vocabulary used. Helen Davidson published a study in 1931 investigating how children recognise a whole word from its shape, using the outline of the word. While it can be clearly seen from any distance that this would be an extraordinarily inaccurate and inefficient way to detect words in text, it has been in use ever since and I have seen special needs teachers using packs of cards with word shapes printed on them, during the last 10 years. The Kinaesthetic method In 1921 Fernald and Keller published their Kinaesthetic method. This, to my mind was really just a rip-off of much of Maria Montessori’s methods, which have been in and out of favour for over a century. Anyhow, they got the children to physically trace their fingers over the word while saying it. It does clearly help the word familiarisation process and writing is now viewed as part of the process of reading development. However, it is not true to say that it is some panacea for all reading Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 175 difficulties, particularly because it is a boring, laborious activity for the children. The Sentence Method th We have already discussed the 19 century work of Farnham in the USA. This now got picked up by Dewey and Decroly, who reversed the process. They got the child to create the sentence. It is not clear to me how the child could do this, without any knowledge of text, unless they were each coached through on an individual basis. Having done this first step, they would build on it with whole word methods and phonics. So, it was evidently more practical than the quasi-spiritualist approach of Farnham! A London school inspector called J H Jagger published The Sentence Method of Teaching Reading in 1929. He explains that he views the key unit in writing is the sentence, which carries a single thought. And the essence of writing is the communication of meaning between author and reader, very like Farnham. This leads him to believe that the analysis of the sentence is the essential foundation of learning to read. So, he proposes that the children look at a picture and then suggest sentences to describe it. These then get written in under the picture, and the children practise reading them. Now the picture is taken away and the child is asked to match each written sentence with an identical sentence, presented in a group of options below it. And finally the child reads the sentence without the help of the picture. What is really going on here is a format for whole word instruction. The sentence is just a framework for that, which maximises the sense of meaning in the words. It gained a considerable following during the 30s and 40s. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 176 It does have one major advantage and that is the involvement of the child through their own creation of the sentence. But I cannot see the practicality of that in a class of 30. If it is done in a whole class setting, is the sentence created by one child, John say, any more interesting to his friend Jane than a sentence created by a professional author? Almost certainly not. Furthermore, Jagger refused to allow teachers to guide children towards any particular method of word building. He believed the children would develop the technique that suited themselves best, of their own accord. They certainly do, but what seems the best approach to a child is not always the best technique to be using long term, as we have discussed. Anyhow, all this experimentation with whole word strategies led to a steady decline in reading standards. The literacy rate of UK primary school leavers by the end of the 1940s was 48%. A similar decline was happening in the USA. A backlash was waiting to happen. The “Why Johnny Can’t Read” Phonics Backlash In 1955 the American Rudolf Flesch published his book “Why Johnny Can’t Read”. This was evidently targeted at the worried parents of children who were not progressing in literacy. It was a very clearly stated challenge to the then prevailing conventional wisdom on literacy teaching, and got a great reception from the public, remaining in the bestseller lists for over 6 months. He proposed a complete return to the phonics methods of the turn of the century. He was soon followed by other writers such as Sibyl Terman and Charles C Walcutt. In England the phonics banner was picked up out of the mud by J C Daniels and Hunter Diack. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 177 The base material for these resurgent phonic fanatics was the work of Leonard Bloomfield, published in the early 1940s. Bloomfield was a linguist and questioned the top-down, meaning-first approach of the whole word and sentence method systems. He believed that the children should first be given instruction in the relationship between the spoken word and the written word, using phonetically regular words initially. He wanted to expose the child to the alphabetical principle of our writing system, but without the synthetic phonic approach of conventional phonics. He believed that if the child was exposed to regular spelling, then the alphabetical system would be discovered by the child by default. He was against phonic sounding out of words and the attempt to blend sounds into words. He suggested spelling words out using the letter names. In many ways, what he suggested was similar to th the systems used in the 16 century, before Hart. He was the first of a series of academics proposing a linguistic approach to reading. A leading member of that group was Charles Fries, who wrote Linguistics and Reading. His view was that there were three phases of learning to read; the initial “transfer” stage when it is a process of transfer of text to auditory information, then the “productive” stage when the decoding becomes automatic and finally the “imaginative” phase when the text can be effectively lived through, as a virtual experience. All of these built structured reading texts that drew the child through these different stages by presenting text thought to be appropriate to their stage of reading. They did not believe in explicit phonic instruction, but a more autodidactic approach. In the UK this was led by Daniels and Diack, who published the Royal Road reading scheme. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 178 The philosophy behind it can still be seen, even now, in the colour coded-book systems, such as the Oxford Reading Tree, present in most schools. However, Flesch completely distorted the basis of Bloomfield’s work as justification for pure phonics. Flesch’s book evidently resonated with the wider parent population, even if it was at odds with the beliefs of most of the education establishment. And there was a renewed flush of phonics systems in schools and home literacy packs through the 1960s. Special Alphabets In a parallel development to the IPA there was a series of special th alphabets developed through the mid 19 Century, designed to make reading easier to follow for the learner. The most famous of these was the ITA (Initial Teaching Alphabet), launched in 1844. Interest in this approach picked up through the 1960s and several large scale experiments were undertaken. The principle behind all of them was to have at least one symbol or letter pattern (grapheme) for each phoneme. This can be achieved in three possible ways: • Having a clear set of spelling rules and adjusting the English spelling to fit them • Extending the alphabet to create sufficient letters to cover every phoneme and then spelling the words in a consistent way using this new alphabet • Using conventional spelling but with diacritical marks above the letters in order to indicate what sound the letter is representing on each occasion. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 179 You can see that this is very similar to the spelling reform movement, except that these systems were designed as steppingstones towards reading normal text. They were widely tested with the ITA by far the most common. XXXXXXImage of ITA text and symbols (see Modern Inventions in the teaching of reading). XXXXXXimage of Wijk’s Regularized Inglish The whole thing was a famous failure. It was true that the children could learn to read more easily, but they were learning a quite different form of text to written English. So, having learnt the interim text, they had to unlearn it and learn conventional text instead. Many never managed the switch and were left unable to read. I met a woman at a conference last year who claimed to be following the ITA system. It was a bit like meeting a Flat Earther (a member of the very small club who deny the spherical nature of the world) or someone in a time-warp. I have since gone online to find out more, but the ITA website seemed to be non-operational. Another current form of this was shown to me recently as well. It is called the DISTAR Reading Program, by Siegfried Engelmann. This is a diacritical system with lots of coded symbols to indicate the sound of each letter. There are certainly superficial similarities between the ideas behind this approach and the Guided Phonetic Reading (GPR) system that we use. There are two key differences, however. First, in GPR the English text is left unchanged and unadulterated, in a conventional font with normal spelling. Second, the phonetic images we float above the text are instantly recognisable, once the learner has been introduced to them. There is no process of “learning the system”, which is a major Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 180 element of most of these special alphabet approaches. And the use of imagery as opposed to symbols, involves far less processing on the part of the learner, leaving more mental capacity for the actual reading. New Rise of the Whole Word Warriors Through the 60s a split developed between the direction of mainstream teachers and that of child psychologists. The former were being trained in teacher training colleges and were generally uncomfortable with the nature of a pure phonics approach to early reading and tended to use whole words strategies leading into a low intensity phonic explanation of word structure. The child psychologists were university based and ran experiments, which showed that the children who struggled with reading had a very low “phonemic awareness”, meaning that they found detecting phonemes within words very hard, in comparison to their peers. So the child psychologists tended to gravitate more and more towards phonic remedial methods. The teachers viewed phonics as a purely remedial approach and remained with a mixed approach for mainstream class teaching. However, in the early 70s there was a group of new standardbearers for the “meaning first” approach, led by Frank Smith and Kenneth Goodman. They were strongly anti-phonics and as a result of their work phonics-first teaching became virtually banned in mainstream classrooms through to the mid 1990’s. The basis of their theories was that a child “learns to read by reading”. That reading is a “psycholinguistic guessing game” Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 181 where the child was using a whole range of cues to draw meaning from the text. Their suggestion was to purely link text with meaning and to surround the child with really great reading materials. For this reason, it was called the “real books” movement. Books should be well illustrated and exciting. Phonics should be absolutely avoided, since it was a way to permanently disincline the child to reading. In Smith’s words: “Readers have non-visual information about the choices available to the author, and make full use of their knowledge to reduce their own uncertainty about what successive words might be… In other words, the reader knows so much for every word the author supplies, the reader can provide the next himself, without even looking.” And on reading unfamiliar words: “The first alternative and preference is to skip over the puzzling word. The second alternative is to guess what the unknown word might be. And the final and least preferred alternative is to sound the word out. Phonics, in other words comes last, and with good reason, for phonics is the least efficient choice.” The results can only be described as disastrous. It is quite possible that individual teachers can get good results with this approach through their particular force of personality, but nationally it dropped millions of children into a semi-literate state. Reading proficiency at 11 slumped back down to 48% of children, the same level reached in the late 1940s, shortly before Flesch hit the headlines. Given that probably a third of children would eventually teach themselves to read with a bit of parental help and no formal instruction, that is a pitiful result. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 182 The reasons are simple and I have personal experience of this having been a father of young children through the 90s. A primary school teacher has roughly 20 hours of teaching time through the week. With a class of 30, ten minutes with each child would take up 5 hours of solid teaching. So there is virtually no time for individual coaching. And yet, with the real book method, how in real terms is the child going to develop any ability to read? He or she was sitting down with a book during set aside quiet reading times, but had no strategy to be able to make sense of the text. At least with a whole word strategy you can have books with objects and the names of those objects beside them. But real books books are just that; real books with stories. We certainly found our children showing absolutely no progress at all in reading during their time at school. We taught them to read entirely at home. While I was doing that I heard a radio programme talking about phonics and phonemes. It was a term that was unfamiliar to me and seemingly totally out of use in schools at the time. The Pendulum Swings Yet Again In 1996 Martin Turner and Tom Burkard wrote Reading Fever: why phonics must come first. It was a swingeing attack on the conventional wisdom of real books: “Essentially, the tenets of the Whole Language movement have been that language cannot be split up into pieces, that learning to recognize words out of context offends against the wholeness of the text, and that top-down processes moving from the comprehension of the whole to deciding what individual words might be, placing meaning at the centre of the enterprise, are of paramount Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 183 importance. Such an emphasis upon hallucination rather than evidence readily identifies itself as belonging to the intellectual milieu of the late 1960s.” You can see why they are called the Reading Wars! Of course, while the debate raged again, practice in most schools just carried on in the pragmatic way of most teachers. It has to be remembered that the vast bulk of primary school teachers are in the age range 22-40. So they had all been through the real books system themselves as children. Few had had any formal phonics training and many were using whole word reading strategies to read themselves. So, while the education system isn’t exactly the fabled supertanker, it is quite similar to an armada of 20,000 ships sailing along. In order to change the sailing technique used in every ship it takes more than a signal from Whitehall. Studies also found that, while literacy was viewed as an important part of the education process, surprisingly little time was directly devoted to it. And even less time was given to any systematic process for the development of literacy in the pupils. It seems amazing, but most teachers at that time had had little or no literacy training while going through teacher training college. And the knowledge of the causes of potential difficulties with literacy was virtually non-existent and largely still are. The Special Needs Coordinator in most schools is just that, a coordinator. They have often received no training in support strategies. They just know the administrative procedures. So the main driver of literacy teaching technique was the stock of physical materials held in each school. These normally represented a substantial investment for the school and the teacher had no choice but to follow the pattern of teaching presented by the structure of the materials. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 184 At the same time there was virtually no formal assessment of the state of literacy achieved by the children. 1990s UK National Literacy Strategy In the UK a Literacy Task Force was assembled and began to review all this, including the newly introduced national tests. It is interesting that in 1995 the number of children achieving level 4 (basic proficiency) at 11 was 48%. Just one year later that had leapt to 57%. It is a good example of how tests distort the teaching process towards passing of the test, because there is clear evidence (reported by Tymms in 2004) that the actual growth in literacy did not match the dramatic improvements reported over this period. In the UK the Labour Party began governing the country in 1997 and immediately set the target of 80% of children reaching the expected standard by 2002. This was to be achieved through the development of a National Literacy Strategy (NLS). This set out six major policy principles: • Education will be at the heart of government • Policies will be designed to benefit the many, not just the few • Standards matter more than structures • Intervention will be in inverse proportion to success • There will be zero tolerance for under-performance • Government will work in partnership with all those committed to raising standards So they meant business…! And in practical terms the NLS was based on three main pillars: Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 185 • A framework for teaching – setting out the range and progression of literacy learning • An agreed and common repertoire of generic teaching methodologies based on good practice and embedded in a daily Literacy Hour • A national entitlement programme of professional development for every primary school teacher The National Literacy Hour was originally based on the Searchlights model. This was founded on the idea of redundancy and used a metaphor of searchlights being used to guide a plane in to land. If you have several searchlights tracking an airplane, when one or two lose track of it, the plane can still follow the path to the airfield with the others. So, in the case of literacy, the searchlights available were phonic ability, context knowledge, grammatical cues and word recognition. In other words it was a blend of all the strategies available, based on the hope that at any particular moment one of them would see the child through. This aspect of the National Literacy Strategy came under strong and consistent attack from the phonics fanatics, who felt that the mixed approach was quite wrong. They proposed a strategy heavily weighted on “phonics first and fast”. At this point Jim Rose was commissioned to do the first of a series of reviews of literacy strategy. And he recommended in 2006 a switch to the “simple model” of reading, proposed by Morag Stuart. The simple model suggests that there are two distinct elements of reading; word decoding and meaning comprehension. And it claims that it is wrong to put the cart (meaning) before the horse (decoding ability). Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 186 The phonic fanatics are very clear about this; there should be no reading of text unless it is already known to be decodable by the child. However, Jim Rose was a bit more circumspect than that in his report. He suggested that purely decodable texts might be useful if well written… but that: “..there is no doubt that the simple text in some recognized favourite children’s books can fulfil much the same function … it may be possible to use these books in parallel or in place of them. In any event, the use of decodable books should not deny children access to favourite books and stories at any stage and particularly at the point when they need to read avidly to hone their skills” The Current Situation Phonics clearly has the upper hand in 2009. Reading levels have risen as a result, as they always do, to roughly 80% reaching basic proficiency at 11, with small variations between countries (depending to a degree on where you draw the line). That leaves a staggering one in five children failing. Most teachers are still uncomfortable with too prescriptive an approach and it is clear that conventional phonics still leaves some children unable to read. It won’t be long before the whole word warriors regain freshness to their arguments and begin a new assault. With reading levels on a plateau, the government of the day will want to be seen doing something about it. The real books arguments are very beguiling. And small-scale tests almost always get much better results, whatever approach you use. The trick is developing a system that supports teachers in a way that can be successfully rolled out to an entire school system and still achieve the aim of 98%+ literacy. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 187 That has been our aim with Guided Phonetic Reading. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 188 Guided Phonetic Reading and Easyread “The new dawn breaks…!” Easyread is our version of Guided Phonetic Reading (GPR). I expect and hope there will be many copies of it over the coming decades and centuries. I believe it is the closest we will come to a universal solution to learning to read English. If you are a publisher considering developing a version of GPR, do please get in touch because I would be happy to consult and advise you on it. We have learnt many lessons over the years, some of which may not be obvious from our materials. In addition, we have the opportunity to create a unified image set that will inevitably aid the children using the different versions. Anyhow, what the hell is Guided Phonetic Reading? The world is dominated by two things; theories and practical solutions. Grand theories can be things of great beauty but practical solutions are what make things work. What happens all around us is always more complex than any theory. I did a degree in Mechanical Engineering and you quickly come to realise that the purity of theory is confined to school education, except in mathematics. And one of the most exciting things, for me, is seeing real mechanisms at play. Everything is cause and effect. Everything that happens has a mechanism. Nothing, not even the splash of a raindrop is just Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 189 random. If you see a result, there was always a mechanism behind it. Now it is always easier to respond to the result you see. Symptoms are in your face and often scream out to be treated, like a skin rash. But the underlying mechanism is much more interesting. Understanding it gets far greater results. German car engineers know this. They demonstrate highly compulsive behaviour in their attempts to understand the mechanisms at work in a car, so that they can control them. And you know the result. A good gardener will know the mechanisms of nutrition of the different plants, so that the environment of the plant can be optimised. Plants don’t die or thrive through chance. And the exact mechanisms by which we learn things are the same. The neurochemical processes in the brain are the key to learning. Our brain is still largely a mystery, but that does not mean that there is not an underlying mechanism for everything that goes on in it. At all times it has been a fundamental aim of GPR that is should be first and foremost a practical tool for almost any child to learn to read with. That is its most elemental concept. As I go through the stages of GPR, it is this pragmatic utility that we are always aiming for and any grand theory is subordinated to it. Phonemic Awareness English is constructed around an alphabet. So, despite the fact that it has very inconsistent spelling patterns, it is essential to approach any alphabet-based text in the way it has been designed to be read. I can show you any number of examples of how children and adults Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 190 who are restricted to pure sight-reading remain at a considerable disadvantage to those who can decode text phonetically. Any reading system that does not aid the child in developing phonemic awareness is fundamentally flawed and will leave at least 30-40% of children poor readers. So, the first task of GPR is to develop a phonemic awareness in the learner. Indeed, the aim in any phonic instruction should be to build direct links between the visual cortex and the auditory cortex. That is not a natural pathway and requires some hard work to develop. However, at no stage does any child doing GPR chant letter sounds. Voicing individual phonemes is an almost impossible task anyhow. It can cause considerable confusion for the child and is about as dull an activity as you can imagine. Nor is there ever a spelling or pronunciation rule to learn. Entertain First It is one of the critical flaws of conventional phonics systems is that they are incredibly dull. It doesn’t matter whether you call a system “jolly”, or whatever else, that does not really make it fun. Boredom is the death of education, at the primary level particularly. Every cart needs a horse and the driving force of primary education is interest. So Rule No 1 of GPR is “no boring stuff”. We give the child access to phonemic awareness in two ways. First, the child should be playing games with the sounds of words. This is something that any preschool or reception class should be doing anyhow, with rhymes and alliterations. We have a particular game called “Easyread I Spy”. It is exactly the same as I spy with Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 191 my little eye, except that you use the initial sound of the word, rather than the initial letter name. This may sound simple, but actually it is a powerful little tool. Children love playing I spy, beyond one’s imaginings. And you can play it anywhere. Second, we introduce imagery to link to each phoneme. Of course we are not the first to do that. But our imagery has a quite different aim to the conventional apple, banana, carrot images so often used. For one thing, it is quite crazy. We start with the ants in pink pants, the bear with long hair, the cat having a chat and the duck covered in muck. Then we quickly move on through eggs with little legs all the way through to the Zuto from Pluto. In case you were unsure, the planet Pluto is inhabited by the Zutos. Easyread Characters We can teach the alphabet in under a week with this imagery. Conventional approaches often struggle with that for months. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 192 All the long vowels have their own Easyread images and the digraphs as well (eg ch and sh). The images are crazy, active and colourful for one simple reason; it makes them far, far easier to remember. How long will it be before you forget the image of the egg with little legs? And I haven’t yet told you that he has bit of a problem. You see… he has a rather embarrassing issue with eggy flatulance. Poooeee. Part of the reason that they are easy to remember is that they entertain. I mean genuinely entertain, at least if you are a child (which includes me I guess). If a child has not laughed out loud in the first 10 minutes of Easyread, we view that as a failure. I love receiving messages from parents saying their child has chosen to get up early to do Easyread before going to school. Some of the most emotional complaints that we get have been sent by a parent having trouble logging on, with their child beside them throwing a fit about it. Entertainment is much under-rated in education. Too many lessons are just crushingly dull. You may say that some things are just not naturally entertaining. But I refuse to accept that. I was taught double-entry bookkeeping with a video presented by John Cleese (star of Monty Python, Life of Brian and Fawlty Towers). I can remember almost all of it to this day. Entertainment engages. Blending Some people ask me when the right time to start a child reading is. My answer is “when the child can blend individual sounds to form a simple word.” Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 193 When that is varies enormously. It seems to be a part of the maturing process in the brain. If a child cannot yet take three sounds and blend them into a word, they cannot really start learning to read. They should just stick to playing with sounds and rhymes. We start the child blending simple consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) words in the first 10 minutes of Easyread. That is what reading is all about. There are three tricks we bring into play at this stage. Our first trick is a simple one. We start with nonsense words. The key to nonsense words is that you cannot guess them. They could be anything so trying to guess them is futile. A lot of children will try to guess a word from the rough length, any context they have and the first letter of the word. Nonsense words are a key tool to blocking this habit. The next trick is that we actually read the word out and get the child to pick the right one from a choice of three. The child still has to blend the words to see if they match what has been read out. But it is evidently easier than reading a word from scratch. Throughout Easyread we try to use the Vygotskian principle of scaffolding. Everything is build around creating a framework that allows the child to succeed, but without spoonfeeding. And our third trick is our unique TrainerText. TrainerText I would love to say that I thought up TrainerText. But in truth it was a gift from God, or something like that. I can only give thanks because I created it by mistake. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 194 I accidentally created the first TrainerText just by using the imagery from the interior of our alphabet book for the title page. Anyhow, TrainerText has the phonetic images floating above the conventional text. The conventional text is completely unadulterated, but as the child decodes it, the phonetic images are always there to help. I cannot overemphasise the beauty of this solution. The child is now empowered to read anything, once familiar with all the imagery. And the images are designed to be almost instantly recognisable. When a child is learning to read with ordinary text and gets stuck on a word, what on earth is he or she supposed to do? They end up guessing. So TrainerText is a truly revolutionary tool. It frees the child to work words out without needing help and so the process of reading can be practised. It has a huge psychological impact as well. Most children develop their reading by reading out loud to an adult. Each time they get stuck and need help, they view it as a little moment of failure. So reading practice is a serial failure experience. No wonder they aren’t keen! Now that moment of difficulty is turned from failure to triumph. The child works it out and overcomes the obstacle. It pumps their self-esteem instead of denting it. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 195 Games Most of the early stages of Easyread are built around various games. Whole word warriors would say that reading should start with the meaning of the author’s thoughts, in the form of sentences. Well hold on there a bit, I say! Proust can wait. It is clear to me that there is a skill to be learnt first. In line with real book philosophy however, our games are constantly pushing the text-meaning links in the child’s brain. We don’t want any “dumb decoding”. It is something that I would not believe without having seen it, but some children can learn to decode perfectly, without being able to understand what they have read. It sounds bizarre, but if they read out “point at the red square” they would look at you blankly, even if you said “OK, which square should you point at”. But if you say to them “point at the red square” (ie what they have just read out to you) they will do it without any problem. So we are often switching between the two to force the child to decode and understand the decoded word. The relationship between text and meaning is a very important one to keep emphasising. The use of a computer and games on the computer also feed into one of our fundamental concepts; entertain, entertain, entertain. Even though the games work very well, we quickly move from games into reading real text, with phonetic guidance. That is what reading is all about and it brings its own challenges. We are always trying to get the balance between the two right, but essentially, we use the games to engage the children and give them a foundation. They then move onto reading text and the motivation comes more from their inner desire to learn to read. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 196 Short, Correct Practice Reading is a skill. The test for whether something is a skill or not is whether you can describe exactly how you are doing it. If you do a complex mathematical calculation, you can explain how you are doing each step. In fact you are usually made to do that as part of the process. When you catch a ball, ride a bike or drive a car, you have no awareness of how you are doing them. The whole process has moved into procedural memory. As you read this text, the same is true. The text flows through your brain quite seamlessly. In fact, you can’t even stop it. Try staring at the next few words without reading them… there, what did I tell you! I was listening to an interview with a basketball coach a few years ago. He had been coaching a top university team that had won the national trophy several times during his tenure. He had then switched to another university who were in a secondary league and within 3 years they had won the national trophy as well. What this coach said was very interesting. He made two key points. First, most games are won in the last 10 minutes. So any team of his was going to be fitter than the opposition. That isn’t very relevant to literacy, but useful to think about if you coach a sports team. The second thing he said was that basketball relied on four key skills with the ball. All of his players, no matter how naturally brilliant, would spend at least 2 hours a day practising those four elemental skills. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 197 That is relevant to us, because reading is a skill too, with a number of discrete elements to it. All of Guided Phonetic Reading is about honing the skill of reading and we use games to work on these discrete elements of it. As the basketball coach knew, the key to any skill is practice. Practice is the mother of ability. But what effective way has there ever been for children to practice reading, while learning to read? We have not given them the tools they need to do that. Learning to catch a ball, you need some tips on good technique plus a ball and a wall. Keep chucking the ball and eventually you will learn to catch it. A child starts riding with a tricycle. Then they get their first bicycle, with trainer wheels to balance it. Then the trainer wheels get disconnected and a parent runs around, risking permanent disability by holding the bike upright. Meanwhile the child is learning the sensation-response patterns for balancing the bike until one day you seem them peddling madly off across the grass, unaware that you are no longer holding on. No Rules Nobody, in the whole course of human history, ever learnt to ride a bike by studying the engineering and forces involved, while sat in a classroom. It is an absurd idea. And yet, we have been teaching children English phonemegrapheme relationships. There are over 200 potential graphemes (letter patterns) to represent the 44 main phonemes (sounds) used in our million-plus words. That is an average of four for each one. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 198 Most letters can be silent and so that creates even more options to choose from. And many of the graphemes can represent several phonemes. We have thousands of words that have two pronunciations and meanings and yet more words that are spelled differently but pronounced the same. It is chaos! A language like German or Spanish can be taught by its spelling rules. I have learnt to read both languages by learning their rules. English spelling rules are a cruel joke and a misuse of the word, since each “rule” only operates around 50% of the time, at best. That is the reason why literacy levels are so much lower in Englishspeaking countries that the continental European countries of similar economic wealth. Around 80% of children do learn to read when taught with conventional synthetic phonics. But that is because they have taken their phonemic awareness and managed to apply it to the code through experience. It is not because learning English spelling rules are helpful (with the exception of the one I use, “i before e except after c”, of course!). However, we now have the solution to making the exercising of the right neural processes an easy thing to do for everyone; TrainerText. TrainerText is the tool the children need to actively practise reading and we use it to build their experience and confidence day by day. It is the key to Guided Phonetic Reading. It allows them to genuinely practise the process of decoding. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 199 Correct Learning Patterns The one thing that we try to get every parent, teacher and child involved in Easyread to understand is that a daily rhythm is absolutely critical. Many people feel our lessons are too short, but that is something I learnt from Keda Cowling, author of the ToeByToe system. She is absolutely rigorous that the lessons should never last more than 15 minutes and that they should be as close to daily as possible. She is absolutely right. Sometimes less is more. The reason is that nobody can concentrate on an intense learning task, like learning to read, for more than a few minutes. Educational psychologists can measure this. Longer sessions will lead to poor practice, which is potentially worse than no practice. I have heard some music tutors say that a learner should only play for 5 minutes, before taking a short break. As you learn a skill it is essential to train your neurones to follow the right patterns. If you do not practice each day, your skill level will steadily decay between sessions and so you make progress harder for yourself. Sleep is also an important element of this whole pattern. Your brain sorts new information overnight and is very good at making links between the different things that you have learnt. We force an overnight break between each lesson. Managed Psychology I have touched on this before, but I would estimate psychology management as 50% of Easyread. If a child is stressed, the frontal lobe begins to shut down and the lizard brain of basic emotions and fight/flight/immobility takes over. You must have seen that with a child under stress. They either go very quiet, sullen and withdrawn Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 200 (a form of flight) or they get aggressive. These are simple stress responses. Now reading is a higher brain activity. So any stress is going to make it harder. And yet even the parents describe the intense stress they feel during reading practice, let alone their children. Generally we try to remove stress as much as possible. That alone can increase the effective brain capacity of the child enough to make handling the text easier. Occasionally we introduce it artificially again in the games we use. One of the reasons for that is that we want to make reading standard text seem relatively easy, without the time pressure of our games. And sometimes the “fun stress” of the game distracts the child from the stress of the reading task. Skiing and horse-riding coaches often use this principle. The learner is stressed by the physical experience and so the coach takes the focus away from the fundamental task by organising games. The learner soon forgets to worry once focused on winning the game. The opposite of stress is a relaxed spiral of fun, success and growing self-esteem. That is our aim and we are managing the child’s psychology throughout the process, by presenting a Vygotskian series of little steps. Asking a learner to take achievable steps is the key process and we deliver regular praise and recognition of that progress through praise, prizes and certificates. Some people may think this is all rather woolly and touchy-feely. That is just wrong. It is based on hard-won practical experience. Sometimes we have fixed children’s reading just by changing their psychology. And trying to achieve anything with a negative psychology in place is like pushing water up hill. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 201 School Results Statistics in literacy are very unreliable and statistics are often tricky at the best of times! I would always take any statistics you read on literacy with a good pinch of salt. I know the temptations of number massaging as well as anybody. However, there are two things which are useful to know about the figures I am going to give you below. First, they come from schools around the country. I can put you in touch with any of them. They are purely their numbers, not mine. We have plenty of internal results, but I am not presenting those. Second, they ran their trials quite independently, without any coaching or input from me. I only visited one of the schools involved. You will find many small-scale trials of reading systems have had a very forceful luminary of the system in close attendance. That creates a result that is quite irrelevant to the realities of a full-scale rollout across thousands of schools. The Children The children involved were in groups of 5 in different schools. Their average age was 8 and their average reading age was 6. So they were substantially behind their peers and not progressing, despite intervention using a range of the most respected approaches available. Range of Response No reading system is going to resonate equally with every child and Easyread is no different. 84% managed to accelerate to a rate of progress equal to their peers. The results within that group varied Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 202 from progress of one month per month of trial up to 8 months of progress per month of trial. Average Progress The average for all the children was an advance of 5 days in reading age for each Easyread lesson completed. That equated to an advance of 2.5 months in reading age for each month of the trial. Having been 2 years behind, around 2/3rds were on target to be in the middle range of their peers (ie with no detectable reading difficulty) within 12 months. Home Results I can’t give equivalent statistics for the children using Easyread at home. Formal assessment is not our expertise and is not part of the Easyread process. However, we offer an unconditional guarantee on every subscription to Easyread and our refund request rate in around 12%. Most of those were children aged 10 or above who we did not engage over the first 2-3 weeks. Their sight reading was fluent enough for them to bypass our efforts to get them engaging their auditory cortex. Plans For the Future Easyread is currently designed as an intervention programme for 611 year-olds. We are in the process of making two new versions; for reception class children and for youths/adults. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 203 The three versions will be named Foundation, Accelerator and Maximizer. The new versions are due for release in 2010. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 204 The Shannon Trust Story “Changing the world, one person at a time” The key to this whole journey for me was the Shannon Trust. It is an organisation that I have great fondness for and which does an amazing job. When my father retired, in 1990, my mother was quite worried. He had always enjoyed work and had worked hard all his life. Now, she feared, he was going to test the old saying – “I married you for better or worse, but not for lunch”. She was a very resourceful woman, however, and quietly looked around for things he could expend his energies on. I think this was on her mind when she came across something in the papers. It was an advertisement from the Prison Reform Trust, inviting readers to enrol in a penfriend scheme. They were putting outsiders in touch with long term prisoners in the hope that corresponding would give them a window on the world outside. As far as I know, my family has been lucky so far and no one had seen the inside of prison, although I think my father once went to a staff dance at Strangeways. Anyway, knowing nothing of what might lie ahead, both my parents enrolled and in due course received a package of instructions of a thoroughly practical kind – what to do, how to do it and, above all, what not to do. The main advice was to keep a polite distance, not reveal one’s address and not show too much emotion. So they each sent out their first letters. My mother soon received a reply from a very got-together man called Ray who was making the best of a bad job in prison. He was Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 205 insistent that he hadn’t meant to kill her, but the police and the court took the view that “he would say that, wouldn’t he?” Meanwhile, my father got no reply. By the time my mother was reading out her third letter, my father’s vanity was coming under stress, so he wrote again. This time he did receive a letter from a man in for murder, called Tom Shannon. Tom had lost his temper and hit a friend with a hammer, putting him into hospital. Once in hospital he got MRSA and died. The next morning, the police called on Tom’s digs. “It’s not GBH now, Mr Shannon… it’s murder. You put him in harm’s way and he’s died”. Nobody really believed it was murder or that any court would say so – except for Tom. He pleaded guilty. The whole court tried to persuade him to change his plea, but he had made up his mind. He had done this to his best friend. Prison would purge his sin. He had been in prison for about six years when he first wrote to my father. The judge had given him the lightest possible tariff, the suggested period that a “lifer” should actually serve. But prison is no place to shelter a violent temper. In no time he was in all sorts of trouble. He is a small man and his crime was not one to win him the respect of other prisoners. And prison can be a hard place. He developed the opinion that sudden attack was his best form of defence. He soon joined the small group of difficult prisoners, continually on the free transfer list. This is a system for exchanging such prisoners when they have reached the limit of a prison’s ability to cope. And yet, there was a certain concept of honour behind his outbursts. Once he started writing, he warmed to the project. His letters were wildly ungrammatical but they were vivid, often rather poetic Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 206 and perpetually entertaining. They were a real eye-opener for someone unfamiliar with prison. We all think we have an idea of what prison life is like. My father found that he had been quite wrong in his imaginings. Anyhow, it was not long before Tom was in trouble again. A knife had gone missing, which was resolved but one thing lead to another until, within a year, virtually every awful thing that one could imagine had happened to him. My father had kept his letters and even drafts of his own as a sort of defence should any misunderstandings arise, but now he began to wonder whether they didn’t constitute a document of interest to the public at large. So he collected them into a book called “The Invisible Crying Tree”, a phrase from one of Tom’s letters. The book attracted press attention and sold quite well. I can recommend it as an entertaining, moving and enlightening read. The question soon arose as to what to do with the royalties. Tom Shannon was not allowed to receive them and my father didn’t feel he should pocket his half. So they went to form the Shannon Trust. My father spent some of it trying to re-establish contact between Tom and his family but Tom wasn’t really interested. There were still some thousands of pounds waiting for a project. I think my father’s solution came from something in Tom’s letters. He clearly obtained satisfaction from helping other vulnerable prisoners – terrified youths or foreigners at sea. This led my father to the thought of mentoring. He was already beginning to be vaguely aware of the appalling problems of illiteracy in our prisons. Tom had mentioned it and there was some press comment at the time as well. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 207 It seemed that half our prisoners couldn’t read – a huge problem and surely partly responsible for getting them locked up. But it also meant that the other half could read. Maybe they could be matched up. It was just one of those ideas that come to you soaking in the bath, but the more he thought about it, the more enthusiastic he became. Luckily he had one or two contacts. He knew Sir David Ramsbotham, who had recently become Inspector General of the Prison Service. So Sir David knew Sir Richard Tilt, the Director General. My father put his idea to the Director General, who said he thought it was a thoroughly bad one. Prisons already had enough trouble with the stronger prisoners “grooming” the weaker ones. However, he said, if my father really wanted to try, he should take his plan to Wandsworth. If he could get it to work there, it could work anywhere. I think he was pretty sure it wouldn’t. Again helped by Sir David, my father met a clergyman, a retired governor, who was quite intrigued by the idea. “It’s never been done but don’t let that stop you.” He had a friend, another clergyman who had resigned as protest against women priests. He was looking for useful employment. His wife was a head teacher at a local school and he had an ambition to teach in prisons on a freelance basis, his specialist subject being Michelangelo’s sonnets. He believed that much valuable wisdom could be derived from their close study. My father was invited to join these two at a meeting already arranged with the Governor of Wandsworth, a Mr McKnight. My father said that the meeting was amiable, but he was evidently puzzled by his visitors. I would love to have been there. Anyway, my father entrusted his precious idea to the Michelangelo expert. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 208 And… nothing happened. Eventually my father decided to go it alone with his idea, and went back into Wandsworth. Wandsworth had a new governor by this time, a completely different kind of man, who was determined to purge its image. He started by removing the dogs, which meant confronting the prison staff. He was a courageous man, but he did not seem to have an easy natural touch with people. The BBC heard of the confrontations going on at the prison and got permission to make a film. This film, more than anything else, taught my father about prison life and prison politics. He now met the Special Educational Needs Coordinator for the prison, a woman called Gill Gander. Gill is one of the heroines of the story. She was very enthusiastic and advised my father to adopt a book called ToebyToe. ToebyToe had been produced by a retired primary school teacher in Yorkshire, with the help of her eldest son. It was the fruit of 30 years teaching, concentrating on the 20% of the pupils in each class that found reading difficult. It breaks down the problems into manageable bites, which gave baffled children a sense of progress, until they eventually realized that, yes they could read! It gives teachers rigid and explicit instructions as to how to handle each exercise. For my father, who was proposing to use prisoners as untrained teachers, it was ideal. The adoption of ToebyToe was one of the key developments towards success. He also managed to persuade its author, Keda Cowling, to come to Wandsworth to talk about her method, which was a memorable day in the annals of the prison. She is a true Yorkshire Lady, in the finest style. But still, the idea refused to take off. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 209 Every quarter my father would attend a meeting in the prison with a group of “interested” staff. Plans would be agreed, roles allocated and my father would bicycle home, with revived hope. Prior to the next meeting, a second copy of the minutes would be sent to all involved so that they knew what they had promised to do. All was in vain. At the next meeting, a completely new set of staff members would file in. My father came to wonder whether they just came because they were most easily spared from other duties at that moment. The whole plan had to be explained all over again By the start of 2000, he had been trying for three years and did not seem to be getting anywhere. Everyone was very nice about the Plan, but noone could see how to get it going. Early that year my father fell very ill with an unusual form of vasculitus called Wegeners Disease. He recovered sufficiently to go to Wandsworth for the April meeting at which he withdrew his offer. He could not go on forever coming to meetings when nothing happened. He had come to concede that Richard Tilt had won. However, at this meeting there was a new face at the table. This was a young officer on one of the wings called Neil Lodge. Neil is a huge Zimbabwean, with an utterly irrepressible character. Of every link in the chain that finally brought the Shannon Trust Reading Plan into life, Neil’s was the most critical. He asked if he could be allowed to have one final go at making it work, on the Onslow Wing. This was the wing for “vulnerable” prisoners. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 210 Many of these were relatively well-educated prisoners, inside for failing to control their libidos. My father did not expect that there would be a literacy problem. He was wrong. Furthermore, a VPU (Vulnerable Prisoners Unit) offered certain advantages. There was less churn (the constant movement of prisoners from prison to prison). And the atmosphere on a VPU is also more tranquil. If it requires courage to admit that one cannot read (and it certainly does), it is less stressful on a VPU because it is a less macho environment. In any case, my father had no inhibitions about encouraging Neil Lodge. He welcomed his offer and gave him all the help he could provide. In the event, this was not great because he soon had to return to hospital and did not emerge for many months, and then in a much enfeebled state. As he was driven away, he asked me to look after the Shannon Trust for him. It proved to be a privilege and a pleasure for me to have been in charge during this critical moment when, at long last, someone with vision, enthusiasm and courage had taken up the running inside the prison. I made sure that Neil lacked nothing, but it was his daily supervision that turned my father’s idea into something real. As with so many things in human history, everything literally turned on one person’s drive. Neil had gathered a team of 5 mentors, who each took on six students. It became their entire way of life. The system had just three rules: • The lessons had to be daily • They had to be one-on-one • They were limited to 20 minutes Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 211 The mentors had a leader who organized everything. In return the mentors were given various small concessions, such as having their cell door open through the day. And they had a fulfilling way to spend their day. Once recruited some of the mentors developed the same level of energy and commitment to the project as Neil. He was now operating as a facilitator. He kept in touch with everyone daily, having a chat with the prisoners involved as he crossed with them in the wing. All the heavy lifting was done by the mentor team. In the Autumn David Ramsbotham carried out an inspection at Wandsworth and reported that the Onslow Wing was alive with enthusiastic mentors and mentees, teaching and learning how to read. They were wandering round the wing proudly displaying their bright red ToebyToe manuals. My father’s reaction was that someone must have been taking the piss out of Sir David. After so many barren years, he simply did not believe it. But it was true. Richard Tilt’s challenge, then, had been met. It was working at Wandsworth! We now also had a second project running near Oxford, at Bullingdon Prison, where I had introduced it. My father gradually resumed control as his health returned. Sometime during 2001, he took to the road, visiting whatever prison would receive him to tell them the good news. They were all very friendly but only one took any action, a high security prison in Durham called HMP Frankland. This was surprising because life in a high security prison is very restricted, but in late 2002, after two years on the road, my father had another acceptance, thanks to a lady in the Frankland education department. She is called Karen Bruce. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 212 But one prison in two years is not impressive. There was evidently a problem. My father began to wonder whether it might be that prison governors were wary of the Prison Officers Association. The POA is the prison officers’ trade union and it had a somewhat militant reputation. Contemplating seeking their support, he asked Sir David’s advice, which was “you really are crazy”. At the other end of the scale, he also asked Neil Lodge and his advice, which was equally negative. Neil’s view was that the project had nothing to do with the POA. Their job was to look after their members. However, my father had had many dealings with trades unions in his time and felt there was nothing to lose. So he set off on the long journey to Cronin House where he got a friendly reception. He explained his idea to two members of their National Executive Council, who instantly understood. By the time he was trudging back to the station, my father was assured of the POA’s support. Their reasoning was simple. They knew that the system was changing the mentality of the prisoners for the better. It gave the inmates a new sense of personal pride and hope in the future. That made them easier to deal with for the prison officers. They knew this from the prison officers involved. Not just Neil but all his colleagues as well. It had been a surprising and wonderful moment for me as well, when a very experienced older officer said to me in a meeting “Frankly David I thought this whole thing was a load of tosh when I first heard about it. I certainly don’t now!” So Neil was right about the role of the POA, but they saw that as the very reason for supporting the project, not the reverse. In one way or another, this was the turning point. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 213 It was not so much the active support of the POA members but rather the fact that prison managements now lost their fear. They no longer felt that if they supported the idea of ToebyToe, they could expect a visit from the POA Branch Secretary with questions about their officers’ job descriptions. At any rate, by the end of the year, ToebyToe was operating in 69 prisons. There was another factor that allowed this sudden expansion to happen. Back in 2000 we had had a “graduation” ceremony for a group of prisoners who had learnt to read on the system. There were about 30 students, plus the mentors and various staff involved, all due to be at the meeting, along with my father and me. So I phoned around the various national papers and eventually managed to get the Saturday Telegraph Magazine to send a journalist over for the morning. The journalist had come along, but his article never appeared. Now, 18 months later and just at the right moment for us, it was finally printed. It aroused great interest. The Trust was soon receiving fan mail from people all up and down the country asking to help. Oh yes, of course they could! We welcomed them in. Suddenly, in a couple of weeks, my father’s lonely plod around the country was replaced by the Shannon Field Force, made up of volunteers resolved to sell ToebyToe into their local prison. This ragtag group were soon to be the new driving force of the Shannon Trust. Prisons are very difficult places to run. They have to deliver so many things over and above the safe containment of their inmates. If they are going to experiment with something radically new, such as what my father was proposing, they need consistent support both in terms of materials but also in terms of encouragement. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 214 It was soon evident that my father’s early intuition that ToebyToe would only work if it was voluntary and run by people who believed in it, had been perfectly right. It was the volunteer’s job to find the necessary enthusiast from amongst the junior staff in the prison. There is always one key individual in every site where it runs well. It is the Neil Lodge syndrome, replayed again and again. Our volunteers had to find that person, quite a challenge in itself, and then remain in regular contact, to keep the energy in the project flowing. The facilitator in the prison always has a tricky balancing act to achieve. The voluntary principle does not apply to prison staff. The prisoners who take part, either as mentors or as mentees, need to feel that they are doing it of their own volition. Any hint that the governor wants them to do it is a turn-off. There was an interesting moment in Birmingham prison, early on, when the governor discovered all these ToebyToe mentors on his wings and proposed to pay them. After some discussion, the mentors asked him not to. They did not want their mentees to think they were doing it for money. All of this makes the promotion of ToebyToe a delicate operation. It is important not to allow it to become part of the box-ticking system that the prisons have to play by. On the other hand, it is important that everybody who is a link in the chain between the Trust and the newest mentee should feel appreciated and involved in something worthwhile. Measuring progress and achievements is clearly part of that. Perhaps the most difficult aspect of this was to achieve the support of the Department of Education. My father had some very disappointing meetings with staff in the prison education system, which were only too easy to understand. Teachers go through lengthy and rigorous training before they are Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 215 let loose. And here was my father proposing to use untrained criminals for the same purpose. So it was natural for them to feel it couldn’t, or even shouldn’t, work. However, we were delivering something that they couldn’t. This was and still is for several reasons. The main one is that adults who can’t read are embarrassed and will only respond if offered private, one-to-one lessons by someone they trust. They will almost never go into a classroom, after years of frustration and torment at school. Secondly, it is the sheer scale of the problem. We are talking of over 50,000 prisoners who can’t read at any one time. Most of these will only learn to read if given sufficient daily one-to-one coaching. This is far beyond the capacity and resources of the prison education departments. Using our army of inmate mentors is the only affordable way to tackle the problem. When my father’s experiment began to succeed, he went to see the new Director General of the prison service, by this time Phil Wheatley. He asked him why he would not support his efforts and Wheatley replied that he was now much inclined to, especially as the POA was doing so. However, he would first have to obtain the agreement of the Department of Education. When the Department replied, it was negative. No, they said, he must not support ToebyToe because it diverged from the principles laid down by the National Curriculum. Wheatley felt he had to accept their professional opinion, but to give him his due, he replied by saying that, as the system seemed to be working, the Department should research it before they turned it down. The research was approved and was conducted by an outstanding civil servant called Liz Lawson. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 216 The outcome was that, while ToebyToe did indeed diverge from the National Curriculum, it was proving an ideal medium for the Shannon Trust. And the Trust was helping non-readers whom the official methods had been unable to reach. The final report by the various researchers had to be toned down by Liz, because she felt it wouldn’t be taken seriously as delivered. The researchers had gone native! Indeed two of them joined as volunteers for the Trust. So the Trust was encouraged to continue and was officially requested to help out with those non-readers who could not be helped by the prisons’ education departments. A special manual was commissioned to form a bridge between the national curriculum and what we were doing with ToebyToe. It is a funny window into how society operates that this bridging manual cost more than our entire operating budget for that year. And at about this time we were trying to work out how to finance our future operations and were advised to drop “teaching people to read” as our aim, since there was no funding for that. However there was lots of money available for mentoring and so we should present ourselves as a mentoring charity instead and line up in the queue! Anyhow, we took the green light with enthusiasm and continued to grow our operations, focusing on just our literacy aims. We were lucky that the money appeared when we needed it, from a variety of very generous individuals and organizations. The Trust has not yet reached its full potential and probably never will. It is currently operating in some 130 prisons – but there are 150. Furthermore, in so far as we can tell, it is probably teaching about 10,000 prisoners during the course of a year and there are about 50,000 in prison at any one time who effectively can’t read. There is also the obvious international potential for the process. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 217 A spell in prison has proved to be a potentially unique opportunity and turning point in people’s lives. While not all of the 10,000 will finish the course, which takes on average 4-6 months, all will obtain benefit and a realization that learning to read is not impossible. The system has also delivered a secondary and equally invaluable benefit. All those involved seem to gain in self-esteem, whether they are mentors or mentees. As something to do in prison, it rates very highly on the value scale. All involved learn that they are worth something. There is an irony, in this. Where Sir Richard Tilt was afraid that ToebyToe would lead to abusive practices, the opposite has happened. One of the Trust’s standard techniques with a new site has been to hunt out the biggest and toughest inmates as either mentors or mentees. They are often the trend setters within the inmate community and can open the door to success. Indeed, the Prison Service is now seeking more ways to release the hidden talents of its inmates, not just to benefit from their skills but also to release their impulse for good. Pre-publication Manuscript Not for Sale or Reprint Copyright Oxford Learning Solutions Ltd, 2009 For more information, visit www.easyreadsystem.com 218
© Copyright 2024