• W hy is Burst®:Reading designed the way it is? • W hy is it so important to target instruction? I knew Jim and Amy had different needs from Carlos and Sally, but now, I know exactly what they are. Burst:Reading Early Literacy Intervention Table of Contents Why Burst®:Reading? ....................................................................................................... 1 The Burst:Reading Skills-Based Model ..................................................................... 1 Burst:Reading Instruction ........................................................................................ 5 Fitting Together the Burst:Reading Skills-Based Model and the Instructional Hierarchy ... 7 The Structure of Burst Activities............................................................................... 8 Reading Comprehension ................................................................................................. 10 Phonological Awareness Instruction .............................................................................. 15 Letter-Sound Instruction .................................................................................................. 20 Sounding-Out Instruction ................................................................................................ 26 High-Frequency Irregular Word Instruction.................................................................... 30 Letter Combination Instruction ....................................................................................... 35 Advanced Phonics Instruction ......................................................................................... 39 Reading Fluency Instruction ............................................................................................ 44 Vocabulary Instruction ..................................................................................................... 50 Comprehension Strategies Instruction............................................................................ 54 Recommended Reading ................................................................................................... 59 User Guide: Why Welcome to the Why book of your Burst®:Reading guide set. To help struggling readers, teachers like you need a model of reading that helps you identify what skills and abilities each student currently lacks — and what skills and abilities he or she does have on which you can build. This guide describes the research-based model used in Burst:Reading and then explains the component skills. For each component skill this guide explains: Why it’s important, How to identify it in a child, and What the research says about teaching the skill. What the skill is, The guide also uncovers the structure within Burst sequences (known as strands) — such as Phonological Awareness or Connected Text — and the simple structure within an individual Burst activity. You can read this guide cover to cover; or just dip into it to read about a particular skill area. Either way, we recommend you start by reading this introductory article. The Burst:Reading Skills-Based Model The ultimate goal of reading instruction — as Catherine Snow1 of Harvard Graduate School of Education has said — is for students to “be able to read books with enjoyment while lying in a hammock under elm trees.”2 But reading is an enormously sophisticated skill that must be explicitly learned. No child fails to learn to speak; yet millions across the world fail to learn to read. Even with careful, systematic instruction, learning to read is a real challenge for many. While it’s hotly debated exactly how many component skills there are to reading, recent research has shed light on the subject. A helpful starting point is the Simple View of Reading:3 Reading with comprehension User Guide: Why 1 Macro-structure Midi-structure Micro-structure How strands strands fit How fittogether; together; the Burst Burst Skill Skill Model. Model. the The The sequence sequenceof ofactivities activities within withinaastrand; strand; the the Instructional Instructional Hierarchy. Hierarchy. is seen as the product of (i) decoding, and (ii) listening comprehension. The Burst Skill Model takes each of those areas and breaks out the constituent skills as shown in the diagram on the next page. hexagons in the Skill Model as shown in the table on Page 4. Each of these strands is described in more detail later in this guide. This model shows how Burst:Reading works: Each hexagon represents a component skill of reading. (To keep the model simple, writing skills are not included.) Skills to the left of the diagram are generally precursors of skills to the right. Of course, there are exceptions; some children might become fluent readers without phonological awareness. And there is reciprocity between skills; for example, reading fluency practice will improve phonological awareness. Identifying the earliest skills that the students in Each strand of instruction in the Burst you are sent for your student groups corresponds to one of the 2 The The structure structurewithin within aa single singleactivity; activity; Prepare/Model/Practice/Adjust. Prepare/Model/Practice/Adjust. Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention your group have not yet acquired; Teaching those skills; Building on that foundation with later skills; and All the while, connecting each component skill to the overall goal of reading with comprehension. For example, for a group of students who can identify most letter sounds but are not blending orally or from print, the Burst system would recommend instruction in Phonological Awareness and Sounding Out and Blending. Burst:Reading Skills-Based Model Phonological Awareness Regular Word Recognition Phonological Awareness Sounding Out & Blending Letter-Sound Knowledge Alphabetic Principle Letter Combination Knowledge Reading Fluency Reading Comprehension High-Freq. Irregular Word Recognition Advanced Phonics User Guide: Why 3 Skill 4 Strand Abbrev. Strand Strand Goal Phonological Awareness PA Phonological Awareness Instruction Students can blend and segment sounds in spoken words. Letter-Sound Knowledge LS Letter-Sound Instruction Students can say the most common sound for single letters. Sounding Out & Blending B Sounding-Out Instruction Students can sound out and read regular words with simple patterns such as VC and CVC. Regular Word Recognition RW Connected Text Instruction Students can read and understand short sentences that are 100% decodable. Irregular Word Recognition IW High-Frequency Irregular Word Instruction Students can read 120 high-frequency irregular words. Letter Combination Knowledge LC Letter Combination Instruction Students can decode words containing common letter combinations (such as sh and oa) as well as VCe words. Advanced Phonics AP Advanced Phonics Instruction Students can decode words containing advanced text features such as common word families, contractions, compound words, etc. Reading Fluency Flu Reading Fluency Instruction Students can read connected text with grade-level automaticity. Vocabulary Voc Vocabulary Instruction Students know the meaning of grade-level words and high-frequency prefixes and suffixes. Comprehension Strategies CompS Comprehension Strategies Instruction Students know when and how to apply core comprehension strategies such as summarizing, making inferences, and identifying the author’s purpose. Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Burst Instruction The Burst Skill Model tells you what instructional areas (or strands) to focus on for a given group of students. There is also a structure to the instruction within a strand — that is the sequence of activities that comprise the strand. It has its foundations in a 30-year-old theory of how people learn skills. According to this theory4 — for which considerable evidence has accumulated5 — anyone learning a new skill passes through four stages: acquisition, fluency, generalization, and adaptation. This theory — known as the Instructional Hierarchy (ih) — says that students encountering a new skill, such as sounding out words, adding fractions, or driving a car, need first to learn how to execute the skill with reasonable accuracy (this phase is called acquisition). Only then is it profitable to focus on building automaticity (fluency) in the skill, usually through practice, practice, and more practice. Once fluent in a skill, cognitive resources are freed to attend to higher-level concerns such as performing the skill in different settings (generalization) and problem-solving in novel situations (adaptation). As in sports, or playing the piano, or driving a stick-shift, basic abilities must be mastered first. Similarly, first graders learning to read need some basic ability in word recognition — as the experience of countless Instructional Hierarchy Description Example Acquisition Reasonably accurate performance of a skill Given a written regular word, sound out and then say the word ( sat g“sssaaat” g “sat” ). Fluency Performing a skill rapidly, consistently, and with proficiency Given a printed word, say its sound without hesitating ( sat g “sat” ). Generalization Performing the skill in multiple settings Given a written decodable passage, read it aloud. Adaptation Adapting the skill to novel demands; problem-solving Given a written passage, make inferences about the author’s intent. User Guide: Why 5 teachers, frustrated that students did not arrive in their classroom with the necessary prerequisite skills, attests. To be specific: A student who cannot produce the most common sound for each letter (acquisition) will not profit much from instruction in sounding out regular words (generalization). Instead, the student should be taught letter-sound correspondences until she reaches a reasonable level of accuracy before proceeding. Otherwise, it is as if the student is attempting to play Rachmaninoff before having practiced her scales sufficiently. Making Sense of the Instructional Hierarchy (From E. J. Daly, III, F. E. Lentz, & J. Boyer (1996). The instructional hierarchy: A conceptual model for understanding the effective components of reading interventions. School Psychology Quarterly, 11, 369–386.) The first stage in the skill development process The next stage is fluency — the ability to perform The next step in the Adaptation is the final hierarchy, generalization, step of the learning is acquisition, “the the skill rapidly and is the process of hierarchy. The learner period between the with proficiency (that is, displaying a recently must be able ultimately first appearance of the consistently, at a set level, acquired behavior either to modify learned desired behavior and and with a high degree of in multiple settings, or in responses in the face the reasonably accurate accuracy). the appropriate context of novel environmental or environment. demands. Haring et al. performance of that behavior” (Haring et al., 1978, p. 25). (1978) equate adaptation to problem-solving where it is necessary to provide the learner with as many novel instances of applications of the skill as possible to promote adaptation. 6 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Fitting Together the Burst Skill Model and the Instructional Hierarchy Here’s how the model works in practice: Let’s say that a kindergarten student begins by acquiring some facility with letter-sound correspondences — when presented with the grapheme p and asked “What sound?” she replies /p/. But when shown d she responds /b/ and when shown b she responds /d/. After some instruction, let’s say she reaches 90% or better accuracy in lettersounds, correctly identifying the phoneme for 90% or more of the graphemes. (Research has shown that one of the most effective ways of getting a student to this stage is modeling.6 For example, using the “I do; we do; you do” approach, a teacher models a performance, then asks students to repeat it, and finally lets students perform the skill on their own.) According to the Instructional Hierarchy, having achieved accuracy in letter-sound correspondences, a student can move on to building automaticity — through the key technique of practice. For instance, the teacher might ask students to quickly produce the sounds for the letters they know — perhaps by using a stack of flash cards, or a game where students race to the board to touch the correct letter, or a computer- based practice system. Some good research supports these techniques,7 but other research8 shows that practice alone can lead to poor generalization: For example, the student who excels at identifying letters on index cards or on a white board may struggle to identify the same letters presented as part of a word. So, in Burst:Reading, we recommend practicing the skill in its natural context — identifying letters in printed words and short phrases. Once a student has reached a certain level of proficiency in a skill — meaning accuracy, fluency, and some level of generalization and adaptation — we move on to the next skill: in this case, sounding out and blending regular words. Exactly what level of proficiency is optimal is an open research question and, most likely, varies from skill to skill and from student to student. For weaker students, a stronger foundation in the basic skills is probably helpful before moving on. But notice something interesting here: we said the next skill is sounding out and blending. As usual, we start by building accuracy. Again, the teacher might use modeling and “I do; we do; you do,” showing the student simple, regular words and having him or her sound out each letter and then blend to form the word. User Guide: Why 7 Clearly, this lesson in sounding out and blending would also increase a student’s fluency in letter-sound correspondences. In other words, teaching soundingout accuracy — a later skill — has the side effect of improving fluency and generalization in letter-sounds — an earlier skill. We can do both at once! What this reinforcement loop means is that we can move more rapidly through the component skills. We don’t have to wait for a student to fully master lettersounds before moving on to sounding out, since sounding out itself provides excellent, in-context practice for letter-sounds. And that’s exactly what Burst:Reading does: it uses subsequent lessons to continue to move students up the Instructional Hierarchy for skills taught in earlier lessons. Thanks to this reinforcement loop, moving through each step of the ih for each skill can in practice be quite rapid. The Structure of Burst Activities Every Burst activity has the same structure: Prepare, Model, Practice and, if needed, Adjust. Prepare: How to get ready to teach the activity. Model: How to model a skill for the group. Practice: How to provide students with chances to perform the skill — with you, as a group, or indi- vidually. Adjust: How to make the activity easier or harder depending on student proficiency, using the support and challenge sections. As you can see, this structure follows the “I do; we do; you do” approach supported by the research. Every Burst activity has these same parts every day so that you, and the students, can get into a fast-paced rhythm. Now that you are familiar with the structure of Burst instruction, the rest of this guide provides a plainEnglish introduction to each major area in the Burst Skill Model. 8 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention MODEL PREPARE EASIER HARDER ADJUST PRACTICE User Guide: Why 9 Reading Comprehension: The Big Picture Everyone knows that reading comprehension is important. But how does it relate to all the other skills of reading? Reading comprehension is the ultimate goal of all reading skills. When all skills are working well, reading comprehension will generally be high. When some skills aren’t working well, reading comprehension will generally be lower. CS Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp This graphic shows the breakdown of reading comprehension into two main groups of skills. The Decoding group is on the top, and the Language Comprehension group is on the bottom. Together these two groups enable readers to make sense of what they read. OK. But what does making sense really mean? What do readers who make sense of a text actually do, think, and feel? Great question! A second grader once asked this of Ellin Oliver Keene (co-author of Mosaic of Thought)9, and she recently spent six years working out a response. Here are some of the key answers she 10 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention highlights in her new (2008) book, To Understand: New Horizons in Reading Comprehension:10 You remember what you read. Sometimes you think about it for a long time. Sometimes you work hard to understand an idea. Sometimes you talk to other people, read more, and re-read. Sometimes you ask people questions. Sometimes you change your mind and your first thoughts. You see how things fit together. You see patterns and can predict things based on the patterns. You get the same emotions in your heart that the characters feel. How exactly does poor fluency lower reading comprehension? Imagine that the amount of mental energy a child has for reading a text is like the amount of juice in a battery. When children have poor fluency, they need a lot of mental energy to decode a written text. If they use all or most of their juice for decoding, they won’t have enough left for the language comprehension part before the battery dies. I thought reading was a great way for children with poor vocabulary to improve their vocabulary. How exactly does poor vocabulary lower reading comprehension? Children can learn new vocabulary from reading, but if there are too many unknown words in a text, they won’t be able to make sense of it, whether the text is written or spoken. Their comprehension will suffer. You know enough about the characters’ problems to think of your own ways to solve them. What’s the special role of comprehension You decide which ideas matter most and add them to what you already know. Children need comprehension strategies for times when they can’t make sense of what they decode just by using the oral language skills they’ve developed for understanding everyday speech. By learning comprehension strategies and their uses, children strategies? User Guide: Why 11 can bolster the language comprehension part of the equation when ordinary oral language skills aren’t enough. This allows them to make sense of a wider range of texts. How does motivation fit in? Researchers have described children who have good decoding and language comprehension skills but fail to comprehend a text at their level. How does this happen? The culprit could be poor motivation. If children are bored with a text and let their mind wander, they can decode all the words but essentially decide not to send any energy to their language comprehension skills. To them, that energy is not worth using, because they don’t care what the text says. As a result, their reading comprehension for that piece of text will be low.11 Similarly, researchers have described children who have difficulty with the fluent decoding of a text but are passionately interested in what the text says. These readers can pour extra mental energy into the language comprehension portion of the equation, and if they know how to use comprehension strategies, such as re-reading and clarifying, they can compensate for their low fluency. They can end up with relatively high comprehension for a piece of text that is relatively difficult for them to fluently decode … if they are motivated enough.12 12 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Could there be other culprits at work if a child has good decoding and language comprehension skills but fails to understand a particular text? Yes – another culprit could be background knowledge. All texts require readers to make inferences based on their knowledge about the situation being described. If children are reading a text about a situation they are completely unfamiliar with, they may fail to understand it. OK, this all sounds pretty simple: Good decoding skills plus good language comprehension skills plus good motivation plus good background knowledge are all necessary for good comprehension. But if it’s really that simple, then why is it so darn hard to help some children become good at it? Don’t confuse Simple with Easy. Instead, think of Simple as Clear. The way to reading comprehension is clear. But it’s not easy for any teacher to help all children get there. Two things make it especially hard: Time issues Differences across students Here’s what we know from research. Becoming good at decoding and language comprehension takes enormous amounts of time. And different kids need to spend time on different things. Research clearly shows that even in late elementary school, some children need more time working on decoding parts of the equation. Others need more time working on language comprehension parts. The lowest skilled students often need both.13 Unfortunately, time in school is limited. And children don’t come to school with labels that identify the way they need to spend their reading time. Matching children to the right activities, and giving children enough time on the right activities, is not a snap. OK, but teachers already have lots of assessments that identify skill needs, and there are tons of curriculum activities. We all know that the federal government has spent millions of dollars on programs that are supposed to help teachers match skills to activities. Yet too many students still fail to show good reading comprehension at the The reading comprehension needs of our lowestskilled students are so large, that gaps in reading comprehension cannot be closed unless we make the most efficient use of instructional time possible.14 To achieve this goal, we need two main things that historically have been hard to get: 1. Instructionally intensive activities that give us maximum effectiveness for the limited instructional time we have. We need these activities for all the components of reading comprehension (in both the decoding and language comprehension parts of the equation). 2. E xtremely accurate ways for matching the needs of different students to the instructional activities that target those needs. The effectiveness of reading comprehension instruction will be determined by how well these two components are achieved. We can even state this in an equation: end of third grade. Why is it still so hard to help Effectiveness = Intensity x Fit. our lowest-skilled students? What’s still missing? The effectiveness of reading comprehension instruction Typical curriculum and assessment packages simply don’t yet achieve enough learning for the lowestskilled students in the limited time of the school day. is the product of Intensity-of-Instruction and the Matching-Fit Between Instruction and Student Need. User Guide: Why 13 If either of the components on the right side of the equation is low – for any reason – the effectiveness of reading comprehension instruction will be low, and the ultimate reading achievement of our lowest-skilled students will remain low. For example: If a program’s individual lessons are effective, but they require more instructional time than is possible to bring our lowest-skilled students up to grade level, then they will lack sufficient intensity, reduce the total effectiveness of instruction, and fail some students. If a program’s activities are not motivating enough for our lowest-skilled students, then they will lack sufficient instructional intensity, reduce the total effectiveness of instruction, and fail some students. If the matching fit between what students need and their activities isn’t accurate enough, then this will reduce the effectiveness of instruction and fail some students. Students would sometimes be spending instructional time on what they need and other times they wouldn’t – and there simply is not enough time for that to happen and for our lowest skilled students to still get enough instructional time on the skills they need. 14 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention However, if both components are high, the effectiveness of instruction will be high. And our lowest-skilled students will finally succeed at reading comprehension throughout their educational careers. Clearly Simple? Yes. Easy? No. Easier with Burst? Yes. Phonological Awareness Instruction What’s the first thing I should know about phonemic awareness? The first thing to know is that phonemic awareness refers to skills with spoken language, not printed language. Okay. What can children with phonemic awareness skills do? CS Children with phonemic awareness can identify and manipulate phonemes in spoken language. Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp Hold on: What’s a phoneme? A phoneme is the smallest sound in a word that can affect the word’s meaning. In the word bat, there are three phonemes: /b/ /a/ /t/. If I change the first phoneme to /m/, I’ve changed the word’s meaning: Now it’s mat, not bat. So do words always have the same number of phonemes as they do letters? No. For example, deep has four letters but three phonemes: /d/ /ē/ /p/. User Guide: Why 15 Can you give me some examples of what it What are some examples of phonological aware- means to identify and manipulate phonemes? ness skills with sounds larger than phonemes? Sure. Here are several specific phonemic awareness (pa) skills: Take rhyming and syllable skills, for example. When children can tell you that cat and bat rhyme, they only have to focus on the -at chunk of the word, a chunk that is larger than a single phoneme. And when children can tell you that Pete has one syllable, but people has two, they’re also focusing on larger units than phonemes. Recognizing individual sounds in words. “Tell me the first sound in paste.” “/p/” Blending: listening to a sequence of separate sounds and combining them into a word. “What word is /k/ /l/ /a/ /p/?” “clap” Segmenting: breaking a word into its sounds. “How many sounds are in the word ship?” “Three: /sh/ /i/ /p/” What’s the difference between phonemic aware- I’ve also heard the term phonological awareness. what’s the link between them? How is phonological awareness different from A key difference is that phonemic awareness instruction targets students’ skills with spoken words, and phonics instruction targets skills with printed words. But in both, phonemes play a key role. For example, in a phonemic awareness lesson, a teacher may say a set of separate phonemes (for instance, /p/ /a/ /t/) and then ask students to blend those phonemes into a spoken word. In a phonics lesson, a teacher may show students phonemic awareness? Phonological awareness is a broader term; in other words, phonemic awareness is just part of phonological awareness. When researchers talk about phonological awareness, they’re talking about phonemic awareness skills plus skills with sounds that are larger than phonemes. 16 As children develop phonological awareness, they generally develop skills with larger units of sound first. So children usually learn to identify rhymes and syllables before they can identify phonemes. Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention ness instruction and phonics instruction? And how to match single letters to phonemes — for example, asking them to matching the letter p to the sound /p/. In later phonics lessons, teachers may show students how to match multi-letter patterns to phonemes — for example, matching sh to /sh/ and matching igh to /ī/. Phonemic awareness programs and phonics programs also share a focus on blending and segmenting: Most phonics programs explicitly teach students to blend sounds from printed letters into words while learning to read, and to segment spoken words while learning to write them down. So it stands to reason that if students have trouble with phonemes, blending or segmenting, they’ll have trouble in both phonemic awareness lessons and phonics lessons. When researchers began to realize that many children had no phonemic awareness skills upon entering school, they investigated the value of phonemic awareness instruction as a way to support children’s success in phonics instruction and in their overall literacy development. Research on Phonemic Awareness How much do researchers really understand about phonemic awareness? A lot. Studies on phonemic awareness received major funding in the ‘80s and ‘90s. When the National Reading Panel conducted its 2000 analysis of the work in this area, it looked for studies that used designs with experimental and control groups. (Studies employing control groups give educational researchers the highest confidence that what they are testing is actually causing learning growth.) Such studies are often the most expensive and difficult to run. But in the case of phonemic awareness, the Panel found 52 such studies from 1976 through 1999 — the most for any area of reading development. The combined evidence from these studies clearly established that instruction in phonemic awareness (pa) can have significant, positive effects on children’s reading and spelling. The evidence also helped clarify some of the specific features of effective pa programs that offer the most benefits for reading growth. User Guide: Why 17 What were some of the findings that convinced the National Reading Panel to recommend phonemic awareness instruction as a key element in every early reading program? Here are some of the big ones: The beneficial effects of pa training on reading lasted well beyond the end of training. pa instruction produced positive effects on both word reading and pseudoword reading, indicating that it helps children remember both how to read familiar words and decode words they’ve never seen before. pa instruction helped all types of children improve their reading, including normally developing readers, children at risk for future reading problems, disabled readers, preschoolers, kindergartners, first graders, children in second through sixth grades (most of whom were disabled readers), children across various ses levels, and children learning to read in English as well as in other languages. 18 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Teaching Phonemic Awareness What advice do researchers have for teachers? Here are the key nuggets of advice, based on research about what makes pa instruction most effective: F ocus on one or two pa skills, rather than trying to explicitly teach all of them. Blending and segmenting, for example, are good choices. D on’t wait until after pa instruction to teach letters. As you teach students the names, shapes, and sounds of letters, use visual letters to help students picture the sounds. Let students use manipulatives like letter cards or magnet letters as you teach them to manipulate sounds. S how students how to apply pa skills in reading and writing. Show them how blending helps them decode, and how segmenting helps them spell. If students see and hear a new word they haven’t read before, show them how to segment the spoken word and look at the letters in the printed one, so they can figure out how the letters and sounds go together. This can make it easier for them to read the word the next time they see it. Children vary in how much pa instruction they need. In the beginning of kindergarten, when most children are nonreaders, pa instruction will usually benefit everyone. Later, it’s important to use assessments to figure out which students need more instruction. Teach pa to these students in small groups. O verall, plan to spend no more than 20 hours of your instructional time with any student teaching phonemic awareness. Assessing Phonemic Awareness You can informally check for students’ development of phonemic awareness by asking them questions like the ones in the beginning of this article, in the list of examples for different types of pa skills. Formal assessments such as dibels Phoneme Segmentation Fluency can tell you which students need additional, small-group pa instruction. sedl, a private, non-profit education corporation has an unbiased database of reading assessment tools for students in grades K-2. You can search the database at www.sedl.org/reading/rad/ for tools that assess phonemic awareness, and you’ll find at your fingertips information about each tool’s cost, administration procedures, and other important features. User Guide: Why 19 Letter-Sound Instruction What’s the difference between letter-sound instruction and phonics instruction? Letter-sound instruction is part of phonics instruction, in which students learn how to match a single letter (like m) to a sound (like /mmm/). CS Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp Phonics instruction is broader, including lessons involving single letters, letter combinations (like ea and sh), sounding out, and segmenting spoken words into sounds in order to write them down. I’ve also heard researchers talk about the alphabetic principle. Is that the same thing as letter-sound instruction? The alphabetic principle is the idea that letters in printed words stand for individual sounds (known as phonemes) in spoken words. Children who understand the alphabetic principle know the general operating instructions for written English: 20 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention To write a word, you assign a letter or group of letters to each of its individual sounds. To read a word, you assign sounds to each letter or group of letters in the word and blend those sounds together. Letter-sound instruction teaches students both the alphabetic principle, and letter-sound knowledge. We use the phrase letter-sound knowledge to refer to students’ knowledge of which specific sound usually goes with each letter of the alphabet. (And we use the term letter combination knowledge to refer to the more skilled level of knowing how to decode multiple-letter patterns, like ph and er.) Basal programs, in contrast, may have a scope and sequence for teaching some letters and sounds, but they are less likely to strictly focus the teaching of a letter to one specific sound. For example, a basal program may use all of these words to teach the letter o: orange, of, on, once, open, off, and out. Whole-language instruction, on the other hand, does not use a predetermined sequence. Teachers teach a particular letter-sound combination when it’s needed for words or sentences that students want to read or write. What about differences in how students practice their letter-sound knowledge while reading? For example, I know teachers who don’t love decodable books and those who do. How do phonics programs, basal programs, and whole-language programs differ in how they cover letter-sound instruction? One major difference is that most phonics programs tend to teach letter-sound knowledge explicitly and systematically. There’s a specific sequence in the program for teaching the letters and sounds, and the lessons are structured to ensure lots of practice with every letter and its sound. Systematic phonics programs also typically teach just one sound for each letter early on. Students in literature-based basal programs typically practice reading selections from trade books. These selections often have many low-frequency words with complex letter patterns.15 As a result, beginning readers in such programs encounter many words that are hard for them to read. Similarly, students in whole-language classrooms typically practice their letter-sound knowledge as they write stories or read material from sources they will encounter in the “real” world (for example, User Guide: Why 21 signs, grocery lists, poems, or trade books). As with literature-based basals, students often practice reading words for which they have not yet learned all the English letter-sound correspondences. Some researchers believe that children can develop poor reading habits — educational psychologist Kerry Hempenstall calls them catastrophic strategies16 — when the material they read is only loosely connected to their letter-sound lessons. These habits include ignoring the interior spelling of unfamiliar words, and over-using guessing strategies; they are linked to the habits of poor readers in older grades.17 Systematic phonics programs typically avoid these problems by giving students decodable text that primarily contains words with the letter-sound correspondences students have already learned. The problem with decodable texts, though, is that too often, the writers fail to make them authentic and interesting. This raises the serious concern that some students could develop poor — even catastrophic — habits of ignoring the meaning of what they read, becoming word callers. To avoid this second problem, some phonics programs have tried to make these texts sound more natural 22 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention and use more interesting content. Another option for phonics programs is to use a co-reading model in the early stages of instruction. In a co-reading model, teachers and students read sentences together: The students read familiar words and new words with familiar letter-sound patterns, and teachers supply the rest of the words. In this way, even beginning readers can practice reading natural sentences in the course of activities where the primary purpose of reading is comprehension. Research on Letter-Sound Instruction How much do researchers really understand about letter-sound learning? A great deal. For instance, for its 2000 report, the National Reading Panel studied questions including these: Does systematic phonics instruction help children learn to read more effectively than other types of instruction (such as basal, whole-language, and whole-word instruction)? Is this instruction beneficial for a wide range of children, including children who are having difficulty learning to read, and children who are at-risk for developing reading problems in the future? To answer these questions, the National Reading Panel looked for studies employing experimental and control groups, because such studies give educational researchers the highest confidence that what they are testing is actually causing learning growth. The combined evidence from 38 such studies clearly showed that systematic phonics programs were more effective than other programs in helping children learn to read. The panel concluded that when children learn letter-sound correspondences systematically, their reading growth is greater than in programs that don’t use systematic phonics lessons. (You may also want to check out the box on Page 32 in the Letter Combinations article with a description of the data from cognitive science and neuroscience about why systematic phonics programs help children learn to read better than other programs.) Teaching Letter-Sound Knowledge What advice do researchers have for teachers? Here are researchers’ key points: H elp students apply their letter-sound knowledge in daily reading and writing activities. B e sure to supplement the phonics portion of your reading program with other important elements. Be sure, for example, to spend time reading quality literature to students to build a sense of story and to develop vocabulary and comprehension. Whenever possible, integrate portions of your phonics lessons into these activities. I f kindergartners have trouble with letter-sounds, help them create a mnemonic (memory aid) that links the letter’s shape to its sound. For example, draw s as a snake, or h as a house with a chimney. Students will vary in the amount of letter-sound knowledge they acquire outside of school. It’s preferable to assess your students’ needs to select the suitable amount and type of phonics lessons. Evaluate your students’ reading competence in many ways, not only by their phonics skills but also by their interest in books and their ability to understand information that is read to them. User Guide: Why 23 What were some of the specific findings that convinced the National Reading Panel to recommend phonics instruction (including systematic letter-sound learning) as a key element in every early reading program? Phonics instruction helped children at all ses (Socio-Economic Status) levels make significantly greater gains in reading than did non-phonics instruction. Phonics instruction improved the reading performance of disabled readers (that is, children with average IQ but poor reading abilities). Phonics instruction taught early on in a student’s education proved much more effective than phonics instruction introduced after first grade. It produced the biggest impact on growth in reading when it began in kindergarten or first grade, before children had learned to read independently. Phonics instruction improved children’s ability to decode regularly-spelled words and pseudowords. It also produced growth in the ability to read irregularly-spelled words. Phonics instruction also boosted growth in reading comprehension for younger students and reading-disabled students. (Whether phonics instruction produces growth in reading comprehension for students above first grade was less clear.) 24 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Assessing Letter-Sound Knowledge You can informally check for students’ development of letter-sound knowledge by asking them to say the sounds for each of a series of letters. Many students find the vowels and the letters b and d especially difficult so those are good letters to assess. Formal assessments can tell you which students need additional, small-group letter-sound instruction. These assessments often test letter-sound knowledge by asking students to read short nonsense words, such as wub. sedl, a private, non-profit education corporation, has an unbiased database of reading assessment tools for students in grades K-2. You can search the database at www.sedl.org/reading/rad for tools that assess lettersound knowledge (listed under what sedl calls cipher knowledge), and you’ll find at your fingertips information about each tool’s cost, administration procedures, and other important features. User Guide: Why 25 Sounding-Out Instruction What’s the difference between sounding out and phonics? Sounding out is part of phonics instruction, in which students learn how to see a printed word, say the sounds associated with the letters, and blend the sounds together to say the word aloud. CS Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp Phonics instruction is broader; it includes lessons involving single letters, letter combinations (like ea and sh), sounding out, and segmenting spoken words into sounds in order to write them down. What’s the difference between blending in phonemic awareness tasks and sounding out? Sounding out combines the spoken-word blending skill that students learn in phonemic awareness tasks, with the skill of translating the printed letters of a word into sounds, in order to blend them together. 26 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Does sounding out have to always be sound-bysound, letter-by-letter? No, some phonics programs teach students to sound out using larger chunks. For example, students may sound out the word cat using the first letter (or onset) and the rest of the word (or rime), like this: /c/ /at/. Recently, though, several researchers have concluded that children’s ability to sound out words with rimes typically develops after children have learned how to sound out individual letters and sounds.18 Does sounding out only refer to when students do so out loud? No. You can teach students to sound out aloud at first, and then later encourage them to sound out new words silently, “in the head.” However, when the brain automatically recognizes the word, the process is unconscious and instant, and even involuntary; we can’t choose not to recognize a word. But I thought that as children developed, they shifted from a stage of sounding out to a stage of automatic word recognition. Are you saying they will always need to sound out new words? The shift from sounding out to automatic recognition happens for every word the child reads, throughout life. As children develop, they shift from being unable to recognize any words, to being able to recognize a small number, and so on. Part of a child’s growth as a reader lies in building an ever-larger mental dictionary of words he or she can automatically recognize. One researcher recently put the point this way: What’s the difference between what happens in The question becomes ‘On what words is this child the brain when students sound out a word, and fluent?’ rather than ‘Is this child a fluent reader?’19 when they recognize the word? When students sound out a word, they use a conscious strategy to figure out what the word is. When they become skilled at this, they may be able to do it fairly quickly, but not instantly. So even fluent readers still sound out new words? Yes. Just see for yourself what happens when you try to read an unfamiliar word like: bathysiderodromophobia (fear of subways). User Guide: Why 27 Of course, skilled readers are more likely to use larger units of letters (and sounds) to sound out an unfamiliar word, instead of going letter by letter, sound by sound, as beginning readers do. Right? Right! unfamiliar word before the brain will automati- Research on Sounding-Out Instruction cally recognize it? How much do researchers really understand There isn’t yet a clear answer, but it seems likely that it takes more times for this to happen for a beginning reader and fewer for a skilled reader. However, one experiment with Dutch children who had been through a half-year of first-grade reading instruction showed that it may happen fairly quickly. Even after only reading a word a few times in a meaningful sentence, three days later the children could read that word significantly faster than a misspelled form of it — a form that would still be pronounced the same. This happened when children practiced reading a word four or six times, but not when they only practiced reading it two times.20 about the effectiveness of sounding-out How many times do people need to sound out an All this makes it sound like it’s really important for children to read a lot, because — for one thing — doing so should increase the number of words they can automatically recognize, versus 28 the number of words they have to sound out. Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention instruction? Enough to recommend that it be part of early reading instruction. The National Reading Panel concluded in its report (published in 2000) that when children learn to sound out new words, their reading growth is greater than in programs that don’t use systematic phonics lessons. (See the box on Page 19 in the Letter-Sounds article for a description of the National Reading Panel’s findings on the advantages of explicit, systematic phonics instruction.) Teaching Sounding-Out Skill Assessing Sounding-Out Skill What advice do researchers have for teachers on You can informally check for students’ development of sounding-out skill by asking them to read short, regular words, especially words they have not yet seen in lessons. What’s more, formal assessments can tell you which students need additional, small-group sounding-out instruction. These assessments often test sounding-out knowledge by asking students to read short nonsense words, such as wub. sounding-out instruction? Here are researchers’ key points: H elp students understand the purpose of sounding out and help them apply their soundingout skill in daily reading and writing activities. S tudents will vary in the amount of sounding-out skill they acquire through reading outside of school. It’s preferable to assess your students’ needs and select the amount and type of phonics lessons suited to those needs. (To find out more about current research on sounding out, go to www.burstbase.net. You can also help future research efforts by contributing your own insights there — we’ll show you how.) Nonsense words have become undeservedly controversial in some classrooms. If you are wary of them, the following points may help: First, nonsense words are useful only as a testing device for decoding skill; they shouldn’t necessarily be incorporated into your instructional program. Second, some teachers have expressed concern that testing students on nonsense words isn’t an authentic task; students will never have to do so outside the class room. That’s true, but the purpose of the test is to check for students’ ability to decode a totally unfamiliar real word, when there is no help from an adult, a picture, or a sentence. (Unfortunately, it’s impossible for test developers to be absolutely certain, for any real word, that the word is totally unfamiliar to every child. So, their only choice is to use nonsense words.) User Guide: Why 29 High-Frequency Irregular Word Instruction What are high-frequency irregular (hfi) words? hfi words have the following features: They are words that English speakers and writers use frequently. Young readers are highly likely to encounter these words as they read beginning-level sentences and texts. CS Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp They are difficult words for beginning readers, because they do not use simple one-letter/onesound spelling patterns, or they use patterns where the letter-sound relationship is unusual. So young readers are likely to have trouble figuring these words out independently, using their sounding-out skills alone. Examples of these words include: the, was, her, is, and they. Tell me more about how you define difficult. We start by taking the point of view of young readers who have learned basic blending and letter-sound skills. These readers know the most common sounds for each letter of the alphabet, but haven’t yet learned more complex patterns, such as letter combinations (like th 30 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention and er). We think of these students as first-threshold readers, because they’ve reached the first threshold of knowledge they need for independent reading. help from the letter-sound connections. Fortunately, there are very few of these words. Examples include: eye, one, once, and hour.) When these readers see a normal English sentence, they’ll likely encounter several types of difficult words. One type will be words that use letter combinations, such as the word her. This word wouldn’t be difficult for more advanced readers who have learned the er pattern, but our first-threshold readers could be stumped. Okay. How exactly do you define “frequent”? A second type of difficult word is one that uses an unusual letter-sound correspondence, and one that our first-threshold readers may have never seen before. An example is the word is. First-threshold readers may not know that we sometimes say the sound /z/ for the letter s, so they could again be stumped. When it comes to vowel letters, we assume that our first-threshold readers have only learned the short vowel sounds. So a word like he could also stump a first-threshold reader. A third type of difficult word is one that’s difficult for both of the above reasons. The classic example is the, which uses both a letter combination (th) and an unusual sound for the letter e. (There’s a fourth type of difficult world, too: a word that is so irregular that students just have to learn it without We use lists that researchers have compiled based on analyses of written material in English.21 These lists order words according to how frequently they appeared in the samples of text that the researchers analyzed. (The number-one word, by the way, is the.) These analyses of highly frequent words map pretty well onto other common lists of high-frequency words you may have seen in textbooks, including the Dolch and Fry lists.22 The diagram below gives examples of words that are difficult (irregular), frequent, both, and neither. Irregular piece rind the was Regular cast tam and it Infrequent Frequent User Guide: Why 31 What’s the difference between hfi words and hfi word is a sight word for that child. sight words? It depends what you mean by sight words — different people use the phrase differently. We use it to mean any word that students have learned to read automatically, instead of needing to sound it out. This could include any of the words in the above diagram. We take the view — supported by research23 — that every word a student learns goes through a shift from being a word the student can’t instantly recognize, to being one the student does recognize, by sight. The shift happens when that particular word has been practiced enough that the brain automatically recognizes it. In our view, a key part of a child’s growth as a reader consists of building an ever-larger mental dictionary of sight words: words that he or she can automatically recognize. The difference between the terms hfi words and sight words is that hfi words are those that first-threshold readers are likely to be stumped on the first time they see them. In order for the child to practice an hfi word enough for it to become a sight word, the child is likely going to need some help from another reader who can help him identify and learn the word. Once the child learns the word and can recognize it automatically, the 32 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention How do you recommend teaching hfi words? We recommend that you teach hfi words by explicitly drawing children’s attention to how the letters in the word relate to the word’s sounds. Here’s an example for the printed word the: For this word (the) we say [th] for these letters (write out th). And we say /uh/ for this letter (write out e a little apart from the th). We blend it together and say the (point to whole word). Wow, that’s a word we use all the time! For instance, I might say, “The thing I like best about Joey is his smile!” or “The picture that Jenny drew is wonderful!” What a cool word! Won’t children object if they’ve learned to say /e/ for the letter e, and now in the word the they say /uh/ for the letter e? If they do object, you should simply respond that we usually say /e/ for the letter e, but in this word we say /uh/. It’s fine to reassure students that /e/ is still a great sound to try when they see the letter e in a new word. Here are some key features of the English language that first-threshold readers need to learn: S ome letters are associated with more than one sound. For example, although we usually say /e/ when we see the letter e, sometimes we say a different sound (like /uh/ in the). S ome sounds are associated with more than one letter. For example, although we often use the letter u to write the /uh/ sound, sometimes we use a different letter (like the e in the word the.) Why not just teach students to memorize hfi they miss an opportunity to focus attention on the word in the way that helps the brain best remember it. This belief is supported by evidence from cognitive science and neuroscience about how the brain learns to recognize printed words: When good readers see a new word, they pay close attention to all the letters in it, and think about how the letters match up to the sounds. The brain stores this information and makes connections between information about the word’s letters (sometimes called the word’s orthography) and information about the word’s sound and meaning. We recommend that you not teach hfi words by sight, because there isn’t any evidence that our brains need to learn these words differently from other words. In fact, scientists have been able to use computer models to simulate children’s word learning — including the difficulties children face — using models where all words are learned in a similar way: through thinking about how the sounds map onto the word’s letters.24 Each time a reader sees and decodes a particular word, paying close attention to the letters, the neural connections for that word get strengthened. When the connections are strong enough, the brain can automatically recognize the word, and the reader no longer has to decode it. Research also suggests that poor readers tend to skip over letters, particularly those in the middle of words, and this makes it hard for them to develop the neural connections necessary for automatically recognizing words. Further, many researchers strongly believe that when children ignore the way that letters match to sounds, (For a description of the research behind this view, see the box on Page 32 in the section on Letter Combinations.) words by sight? User Guide: Why 33 Research on HFI Instruction Teaching HFI Words How much do researchers really understand What advice do researchers have for teachers? about the effectiveness of hfi instruction? As described above, researchers suggest that you help students focus attention on the relationship between the letters and sounds in hfi words. When the National Reading Panel analyzed the evidence on effective reading instruction for its report (published in 2000), the panel did not specifically look at how different synthetic phonics programs (that is, those that teach sound-by-sound decoding) dealt with teaching irregular words. So it’s probably fair to say that the field’s understanding of hfi instruction is less well-developed than its understanding of other aspects of phonics instruction (including phonemic awareness, letter-sound learning, and letter combination instruction). However, the panel’s review of basic research on word learning did lead it to the following conclusions: P rocessing letter-sound relations in words through decoding or analogizing creates alphabetic connections that establish the words in memory as sight words. A lthough alphabetic knowledge is not in itself sufficient for decoding irregularly spelled words, it does help children remember how to read these words. 34 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Researchers also suggest that you follow the general advice from the National Reading Panel report about what makes phonics instruction most effective — described on Page 19. Assessing High-Frequency Irregular Word Skill You can informally check for students’ development of hfi word skills by looking for patterns of errors in hfi words as students read text aloud. You can also ask students to read the set of irregular words taught in Burst:Reading (which is listed at www.burstbase.net). Formal assessments can also help you determine which students need additional, small-group hfi instruction. For example, Wireless Generation’s mCLASS®:DIBELS® Now What?® Tools can identify for you how well students read irregular words in text passages. Letter Combination Instruction What’s the difference between letter combination instruction and phonics instruction? CS Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp Letter combination instruction is part of phonics instruction. Usually, letter combination instruction refers to lessons where students learn how to match multiletter patterns to a sound (like matching ph to /f/ and ai to /ā/). Phonics instruction is a broader term, including lessons with single letters, letter combinations, blending sounds, and lessons on segmenting spoken words into sounds in order to write them down. Sometimes I hear researchers use the term grapheme-phoneme knowledge. I know that phonemes are individual sounds in words, but what is a grapheme? Grapheme refers to a particular letter or group of letters used to represent a particular phoneme. So when researchers talk about children’s knowledge of how both single letters and multiple letters match up to phonemes, they use the term grapheme-phoneme knowledge. User Guide: Why 35 What’s the difference between a letter Is there any research that can help me decide combination and a blend? whether to teach phonics rules for letter In general, letter combinations are two or more letters representing a single sound (also known as digraphs and trigraphs) such as sh, ea, oa, ph, or igh. Blends, in contrast, are multiple consonants representing multiple sounds blended together, such as tr, bl, or scr. combinations, like When two vowels go walking, Letter combination instruction can also include the l-controlled vowel patterns, such as ol (as in cold). In this case, the pattern is a multi-sound one that often confuses young readers, so it’s worth teaching explicitly. Research on Letter Combination Instruction How much do researchers really understand the first one does the talking? Researcher Theodore Clymer published a classic study on phonics rules back in 1966 (The Reading Teacher reprinted the article in 1996). He showed that most rules don’t actually work most of the time. The two vowels walking rule, for example, only works about 45% of the time. (It does work better for some vowel pairs. For example, it works 95% of the time for the oa pair; but it doesn’t work well for oi or au — think of soil and haul.) Even the silent-e rule only works about 63% of the time. (Again, it works better for some patterns: about 77% of the time in a words like cake, but only about 58% of the time in o words like stove.)25 about letter combination learning? A lot. The National Reading Panel concluded (for its 2000 report) that when children learn graphemephoneme correspondences systematically and explicitly, their reading growth is greater. (See the box on Page 19 in the Letter-Sounds section for a description of the National Reading Panel’s findings on the advantages of explicit, systematic phonics instruction.) 36 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Teaching Letter Combination Knowledge What other advice do researchers have for teachers? The Letter-Sounds article lists key advice from the National Reading Panel’s report about what makes phonics instruction in general most effective. This important advice also applies to letter combination instruction. Do research scientists know why systematic phonics programs help children learn to read better than other programs? Many scientists believe that this question can be This view suggests that of two possible approaches, one answered by looking at evidence from cognitive is decidedly better. We might encourage students to science and neuroscience about how the brain learns to look only at a few letters in a word and then guess its recognize printed words. identification from pictures or context. But students will Research evidence suggests that when good readers see a new word, they pay close attention to all its letters and think about how the letters match up to the sounds. The brain stores this information and makes learn to recognize words more easily if we instead focus their attention on all the letters in a new word, as well as the match between the letters and the sounds. One group of researchers recently explained why: connections between information about the word’s Phonics instruction may simply point children in letters (sometimes called the word’s orthography) and the direction of looking deeply into the printed information about its sound and meaning. form of the word as they attach sound to it, rather Each time a reader sees and decodes a particular word, paying close attention to the letters, the neural connections for that word get strengthened. When the connections are strong enough, the brain can automatically recognize the word, and the reader no longer has to decode it. than looking for clues outside the printed word. Indeed, each time the child skips over scrutinizing the internal structure of a printed word in favor of using contextual cues or illustrations to identify the word, the child loses an opportunity to imprint the orthography.26 User Guide: Why 37 In addition, many researchers add these pieces of advice specifically targeted at letter combination instruction: W hen deciding how many letter combinations to teach explicitly, keep in mind that the most effective approach will likely depend on the child’s skill level. Children with the weakest phonics skills are likely to benefit most from a curriculum that devotes lots of teacher time to explicitly teaching a large number of letter combinations. In contrast, children with higher phonics skills are likely to benefit most from curriculum that gives them more time with book-reading practice. F rom first grade onward, all readers benefit from — and need — lots of time to practice their letter combination knowledge through reading connected text. So be sure to include this practice time, even for your lowest students. 38 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Assessing Letter Combination Knowledge You can informally check for students’ development of letter combination knowledge by asking them to read words that use these combinations, especially words they have not yet seen in your reading lessons. You can also look for patterns of errors on these words as children read trade book or textbook selections aloud. If you suspect a problem on a particular type of letter combination, try creating your own mini-assessment by asking the student to read five words of that type in isolation. Formal assessments — such as dibels® Deep — can tell you with more precision which students need additional, small-group letter combination instruction. Advanced Phonics Instruction What exactly is advanced phonics instruction? Advanced phonics activities help students learn about the following word patterns: Word families: back, lack, black Double-letter words: fill, miss, chill CS Silent-letter words: lamb, sock, answer Phonological Awareness Compound words: bedbug, sandbox, firefighter Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS Flu Comp Contractions: it’s, I’m, can’t -ed words: melted, filled, kicked IW AP Voc -s words: bats, bugs The lessons in advanced phonics go beyond soundby-sound decoding. They encourage students to think about: 1. Larger pattern chunks in words (as in word families). For example, a child can use his knowledge of miss and kiss to read the new word hiss. When students read new words this way we call it reading by analogy — a skill that typically User Guide: Why 39 develops after children have developed and practiced skills in sound-by-sound decoding. 2. The way that certain patterns (like s and ed) are related to meaning. These types of patterns are among the morphemes. Researchers cannot yet clearly delineate how our brains use morphemes when we read a new or familiar word, but they do suggest that morphemes may play a role in children’s automatic word recognition, even as early as second grade.27 The goal is to continue to encourage students to look deeply at the spelling — also known as orthography — of words as they read, and to enjoy discoveries about how spelling patterns are related. Do all children need advanced phonics instruction? When it comes to reading instruction, one size never fits all. Research suggests that children who enter first grade with middle or high literacy skills (and engage in lots of independent book reading) need a lower amount of phonics instruction.28 Your best bet is to use assessments for deciding which children need advanced phonics lessons. 40 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Research on Advanced Phonics Instruction How much do researchers really understand about advanced phonics instruction? When the National Reading Panel analyzed the evidence on effective reading instruction for its 2000 report, it did not specifically look at the difference between synthetic phonics programs (that is, those that teach sound-by-sound decoding) that extended their lessons into advanced phonics, and those that did not. So it is probably fair to say that there currently is not as clear an understanding about advanced phonics instruction as there is about earlier stages of phonics instruction. So, are there research-based principles from basic phonics lessons that might be important for advanced phonics lessons? Possibly. The panel’s report on effective reading instruction clearly states that for the earlier stages of phonics learning, “Reading programs that are highly systematic, explicit, and comprehensive in teaching children how to read words based on their spelling are more effective than programs that do not share these features.” Advanced phonics lessons are systematic, explicit, and comprehensive and so more effective. (See the box on Page 19 in the Letter-Sounds section for a description of the National Reading Panel’s findings on the advantages of explicit, systematic phonics instruction.) pattern you teach. An explicit and systematic philosophy is perfectly consistent with encouraging students to be “pattern detectives” who find and celebrate these examples. The National Reading Panel’s conclusions don’t Teaching Advanced Phonics Knowledge seem to fit with what I know about motivating students. In my teaching experience, most “explicit and systematic” scripted programs are boring and What advice do researchers have for teachers? involve lots of “telling.” My students get more Here are the key nuggets of advice, based on research about what makes phonics instruction in general most effective: excited when they discover things for themselves. Your confusion is understandable. The terms explicit and systematic are sometimes misunderstood by curriculum developers as the only important features of a program. They’re also sometimes mistakenly interpreted to mean boring and mindless by teachers who think they can’t mesh such an approach with their views on authentic, motivating classrooms. Both views are mistaken. Explicit and systematic instruction can — and should — also be joyful and authentic. For example, you can use an explicit and systematic phonics program and still celebrate when students make their own discoveries about language. Even with scripted programs, you’ll never include all of the words in a phonics lesson that relate to a particular pattern, and there will always be exceptions to any H elp students understand the purpose of learning advanced phonics patterns and help them apply their advanced phonics knowledge in daily reading and writing activities. B e sure to supplement the phonics portion of your reading program with other important elements. Phonics lessons alone are not sufficient, and they should not take up the lion’s share of the time you devote to your reading program. Be sure, for example, to spend time reading quality literature to students to build a sense of story and to develop vocabulary and comprehension. Whenever possible, integrate portions of your phonics lessons into these activities. User Guide: Why 41 S tudents will vary in the amount of advanced phonics knowledge they acquire outside of school. Typically, at-risk readers will need more explicit instruction on a greater number of text features such as those covered in advanced phonics. It’s preferable to assess your students’ needs and, based on them, select the suitable amount and type of phonics lessons. E valuate your students’ reading competence in many ways, not only by their phonics skills but also by their interest in books and their ability to understand information that is read to them. By emphasizing all of the processes that contribute to growth in reading, you will have the best chance of making every child a reader. Assessing Advanced Phonics Knowledge Decoding assessments for advanced phonics knowledge range from highly formal, diagnostic instruments — such as the dibels® Deep assessment — to less formal instruments, some of which are available for free download. (See, for example, www. balancedreading.com/assessment.html). If you suspect that a student could benefit from advanced 42 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention phonics lessons, try some of these assessments and look for patterns of errors that map onto advanced phonics content. You can also take an even more informal route to assessment and look for patterns of errors on these words as students read trade book or textbook selections aloud. If you suspect a problem on a particular type of word, such as contractions, try creating your own mini-assessment, asking the student to read five words of that type in isolation. Australian education expert Brian Cambourne recently outlined some key considerations when integrating an explicit and systematic approach into classrooms that also — and crucially — teach children to love reading.29 He advocates that you: DO: DON’T: Teach explicitly and systematically about the Teach scripted (explicit and systematic) lessons often invisible ways we write, spell, and learn. without being consciously aware of what’s going on. Otherwise, students may be confused about how In other words, be careful not to sound robotic and reading and writing work. They may also miss skills, keep in touch, as we are sure is your goal, with your understandings, and know-how that some students students and how they are responding, feeling and discover on their own and others don’t. reacting. Talk explicitly about your personal likes and dislikes Forget to help students understand the meaningful about the texts you read, as well as how you use and authentic purposes for reading in the world reading in your personal life. outside of school. Tell students explicitly about why you’re asking them to do the activities in the lessons and how it will fit together for the main purposes of learning. Systematically (and rationally) plan what you’re doing with students. Challenge yourself to justify your planning decisions. User Guide: Why 43 Reading Fluency Instruction What exactly is reading fluency? Reading fluency is the ability to read a text quickly, accurately, and with proper expression. What’s the relationship between reading CS fluency skills and word recognition skills? Phonological Awareness Alphabetic Principle PA RW LC B LS IW AP Voc Flu Comp The relationship is an extremely close one, particularly for beginning readers. Recent research suggests that for readers in Grades 1-3, their level of fluency is determined almost exclusively by their sight-word skills.30 (By sight words, we mean any words that students can automatically recognize instead of having to sound them out.) So, the more words students can recognize automatically, the better their fluency. Of course, readers also have to know how to interpret punctuation in order to read with proper expression. Many researchers also believe that as children get older, and as the texts they read get more complex, children’s fluency skill is more influenced by comprehension-related skills: skills that use knowledge about how words and phrases in sentences go together based on their meaning.31 44 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention What’s the relationship between reading fluency and comprehension ability? In general, children’s fluency skill is an extremely good predictor of their comprehension ability. Why? Because when children can recognize words automatically, they have many more mental resources to use in comprehending what a text means. If children have to struggle to read words, they spend most of their mental resources just identifying what the words are, with few resources left over for comprehension. As a result, their comprehension ability suffers. But I’ve seen students who seem fluent when they read something, and then have no idea what they just read. I’ve also seen students struggle through a book that’s way above their level, yet then be able to tell me lots of things about it. So I have a hard time accepting that fluency is all that matters for comprehension. You couldn’t be more right: Fluency is not all that matters for comprehension. For one thing, motivation plays an important role in the relationship between fluency and comprehension for any particular text. If children are bored with a text’s content, they may read it quickly without paying attention to meaning. Likewise, readers with weak word-reading skills can still understand a text they are highly motivated to read, if they use such strategies as re-reading, looking back, or even reading aloud to minimize distractions.32 Lack of background knowledge (including vocabulary knowledge) can also throw off comprehension for fluent readers. If children are reading about a totally unfamiliar situation, they may be able to read the words but be unable to make the kind of inferences needed for good comprehension. On the other hand, children with a lot of background knowledge and vocabulary about a topic (such as outer space) may be able to use these abilities to figure out words that are difficult for them to decode. This knowledge may also help them make inferences and comprehend well, even though their reading is slow and effortful. In rare cases, children may have a general cognitive deficit that impairs their ability to comprehend oral language. Children apply their spoken-language comprehension abilities to reading, so these children could have the ability to read text quickly and accurately but have poor reading comprehension skills. User Guide: Why 45 How does fluency develop? reach late elementary school. These differences Through practice, practice, practice with reading texts. That’s one thing researchers agree on. Of course, students can’t practice fluency with a particular passage until they have a basic ability to decode most of the words in the passage. in reading practice emerge during the earliest How much practice? difficult for children, once significantly behind in the A lot. In fact, Joseph Torgesen of Florida State University is convinced that this is one reason it’s extremely hard to help older low-skilled readers catch up to their peers on fluency, even after you’ve helped them catch up on phonics skills. It’s just very difficult to give these older readers enough reading practice time to catch up on the amount of reading practice they’ve missed and keep up with the amount of reading practice their fluent peers are continuing to get. He writes: growth of their sight word vocabulary, to close the We have proposed elsewhere several possible explanations for the difficulty we have experienced in helping older children to close the gap in reading fluency after they have struggled in learning to read for several years. The most important factor pronounced as the children advance across the grades in elementary school…[if] typical readers are continually expanding their sight vocabularies through their own reading behavior, it should be very gap in reading fluency.33 What kind of reading practice helps children most? How can I support this practice in my classroom? Those are the million-dollar questions researchers have been studying closely in recent years. The next sections describe their findings, particularly those relevant to elementary teachers. Research on Instruction for Developing Reading Fluency How much do researchers really understand about the effective instruction for developing appears to involve difficulties in making up for reading fluency? the huge deficits in reading practice the older Enough to conclude that repeated oral reading practices can definitely help increase children’s fluency. children have accumulated by the time they 46 stages of reading instruction and they become more Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention For its report published in 2000, the National Reading Panel found 16 studies, conducted from 1970 to 1996, that used experimental-control group designs for testing the effectiveness of repeated oral reading practices. (Control-group studies give educational researchers the highest confidence that what they are testing is actually causing learning growth.) The students in these studies ranged from Grades 2 through 9. The resulting evidence convinced the panel that repeated oral reading practices helped improve students’ reading ability, from Grade 2 through at least Grade 5, and helped improve the reading of students with learning problems much later than Grade 5 as well. In addition, some programs (called guided repeated oral reading) give teachers carefully designed feedback routines to use in guiding the reader’s performance. Did the panel find that some programs were more effective than others? No, the panel did not believe there was enough evidence to recommend one program over another. Instead, it concluded that all of the procedures seemed to have a reasonably high likelihood of success. Teaching Reading Fluency What advice do researchers have for teachers? What are repeated oral reading practices? These are programs that generally have the following key features: 1. Students read and re-read a text. 2.Students get increased amounts of practice reading aloud — compared to the amount they get in traditional whole-group, “round-robin” instruction — through either one-on-one instruction, tutors, audiotapes, peer guidance, computer-based voice recognition software, or some other means. Here are the key nuggets of advice, based on research about what makes fluency instruction most effective: D o not rely solely on word-list reading practice. Children need to develop fluency through practice reading connected, meaningful text. F ind ways to incorporate repeated oral reading practices as part of your reading program. Have students read passages orally multiple times while receiving guidance or feedback from peers, parents, or yourself. User Guide: Why 47 D on’t emphasize fluency without comprehension. Continue to emphasize to students that the goal of reading is comprehension. A s a general rule, aim for material that students can read with a 92-95% accuracy level. Traditional notions of the right “instructional level” held that children should be able to read materials with greater, 95-98% accuracy. However, recent research with second graders suggests that the increased support provided by repeated reading practices can help children gain instructionally from more difficult texts, even those children reading at below the 95% accuracy level. (Indeed, researchers found benefits when children worked with text that they initially read at only an average 85% accuracy level.34) C onsider using texts such as poems or plays that students can perform in front of an audience. The goal of delivering a real performance can give students a natural motivation to re-read and practice texts, and to work on their expression.35 D o not rely on currently popular programs that encourage students to increase the amount of reading they do on their own. Research has not yet proventhat children actually do read more as a result 48 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention of these programs, or that they do so enough to affect their reading achievement. It’s not that encouraging children to read more is bad or wrong, it’s just that we don’t yet know enough about how to do this effectively. The National Reading Panel’s review of research on current popular programs, including those that use points or prizes to motivate reading, has not found that these programs can be effective. Assessing Reading Fluency You can informally check students’ fluency development by asking them to read aloud texts that you think are at their level. Formal assessments can help you determine which students need the most help with fluency, and whether their fluency is improving. Curriculum-based measurement (cbm), such as dibels® Oral Reading Fluency, is a widely-studied form of classroom assessment that includes a passage-reading fluency measure. cbm is used over time, and includes enough equivalent tests to cover most or all of the school year, even if students are tested weekly. Each passage fluency test contains text written at an end-of-grade difficulty level. So for example, first graders use cbm tests written with an end-of-first-grade level. Each time students are tested, they individually read aloud a different passage for one minute, and the teacher marks each student’s number of words read correctly. Since every passage is of the same difficulty, the teacher can tell if students are improving from test to test. There are over 200 research studies showing the validity, reliability, and instructional usefulness of cbm.36 User Guide: Why 49 Everything You Wanted to Know About Vocabulary But Were Afraid to Ask The Basics: In Plain English I teach young, at-risk students. What are the top three things I should know about vocabulary? 1. The size of your students’ vocabularies when they come to school may be dramatically smaller than that of children from more advantaged homes. By first grade, children from homes with high-income levels know anywhere from two to five times as many words as children from low-income homes.37 Researchers have labeled this problem word poverty.38 2. Closing the gap in vocabulary knowledge must start early if we are to help children from disadvantaged homes succeed in reading comprehension. Too many children progress in early reading only to fall behind in the fourth-grade slump. Many researchers are convinced that this problem is due in large part to the failure to adequately build children’s vocabularies in earlier grades.39 By fourth grade, many children are encountering so many unknown words in text that they cannot understand what they read. 50 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Unfortunately, the word poverty that some children bring to school is so large that it will take years to address. Your school’s attempts to remedy the vocabulary gap must start as soon as possible, rather than waiting until after children have learned to read. 3. Knowing a word is not all-or-none. Children usually only pick up bits of information about a word each time they encounter it. They need to encounter a word in many different contexts in order to fully know it. Children who fully know a word can use it precisely, understand it quickly, and use it for different purposes.40 What are the best words for vocabulary instruction? As usual, there’s no one-size-fits-all answer. You need to consider the age of the children, their level of need, and the type of instruction. For older children, particularly those near middle school level, it’s important to include academic words. Words like infer, deny, prove, and factor are highly critical for understanding texts and thinking processes in different disciplines.41 For younger children, many researchers suggest that for general whole-class instruction, teachers pull words from grade-appropriate read-aloud books. You can think of words from these books as falling into three tiers: Tier one words are words that children already know. They occur very frequently in everyday talk. Tier two words are not as common in everyday talk but are relatively frequent in children’s books. Examples from kindergarten read-alouds are words like disappear and foolish. First grade tier-two words include clutching and announce. Researchers recommend that you use tier two words like these for your vocabulary lessons. Tier three words are rare words, such as peninsula, that are less common across read-alouds. They are better suited for learning in content areas.42 For the most at-risk younger children, you should consider small-group intensive instruction with a focus on words that may derail learning in decoding lessons and independent reading. Researchers report that children in vocabulary crisis, particularly those from other language backgrounds, often do not fully know common words in early reading instruction, such as hog and thorn.43 So does knowing a word’s meaning really affect how children decode and recognize it? Yes. Children who know a printed word in their oral vocabulary can more easily and quickly sound it out and read it in their texts.44 Research on Vocabulary Instruction What do researchers know about effective instructional methods for teaching vocabulary? The National Reading Panel45 concluded that teachers should use a variety of methods for most effectively teaching vocabulary. These methods should include: Direct instruction that gives definitions or other attributes of words Repeated and multiple exposures to a word in many contexts Rich and authentic contexts that can help link word use to content areas Active engagement Multimedia methods, including pictures and computer technology Incidental exposure outside of vocabulary lessons User Guide: Why 51 Teaching Vocabulary I already include lots of vocabulary discussions and activities in my day, especially when I read to children. But often the words just don’t stick. What else can I do? Researchers are continuing to study what makes words sticky; that is, what makes vocabulary instruction most meaningful and memorable. Their work suggests that instruction is stickier if the teacher includes analytic talk. For example, if you are reading a book in which frogs quarrel, don’t just talk about the word in the context of the story or ask children about times when they quarreled. You should also help children analyze word meanings, and you should recycle word knowledge by doing this across multiple days. Ask questions like: What would quarreling sound like? What do people quarrel about? I f you were quarrelling with a friend over a game, would you think the same as your friend or different? Is quarrelling peaceful and friendly? If someone quarreled in our classroom, what would it look like? 52 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Use the word quarrel in a sentence by saying, “People quarrel over ___.” Give a thumbs-up if this sentence is true, and a thumbs-down if it’s false: A party is more fun when everyone is quarrelling.46 You could also consider ways that computer technology can help add richness to your lesson contexts. For example, one study showed impressive gains in students’ vocabulary learning when they used digital cameras as part of a program to help them notice and recycle words.47 What do researchers suggest that I do outside of my vocabulary lessons to help students’ vocabulary learning? Researchers strongly encourage you to insert uncommon words into your speech as much as you possibly can: For example, rather than reminding a student that he didn’t quite close the door, the teacher might tell the child to close the door because it is ajar. Rather than asking a student to water a drooping plant, the teacher might say that the plant is becoming dehydrated. Rather than telling students to line up faster, the teacher might ask them to stop dawdling.48 How can I teach my children to be independent word learners? Here are several strategies that have shown positive effects in research studies:49 Model for your students how to look up a difficult word in a dictionary. Think aloud as you choose the best definition for the context where you and your students encountered the word. Model how to use context alone. Some contexts can be vague or misleading, so be sure to use examples where the context is specifically helpful and contains strong clues. Point out the clues you are using. In Grades 3 – 5, teach children common pre-fixes and suffixes in activities where students read texts with these words in them. Create activities that foster students’ curiosity about and awareness of prefixes, suffixes, and root words, rather than asking students simply to recite meanings. Assessing Vocabulary Vocabulary assessments can vary on whether they tap breadth of word knowledge (how many words are known)50 or depth of word knowledge (how well words are known). Assessments also vary on whether they measure children’s receptive vocabulary knowledge (the ability to understand a spoken or written word) or their expressive vocabularies (the ability to use words). Some vocabulary assessments focus on breadth by asking children to match a word to a picture of its most common meaning. An example of a researchbased assessment of this type is the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). The PPVT measures receptive vocabulary. Usually children’s ability to understand a word is greater than their ability to produce and use a word. The Expressive-Vocabulary Test (EVT), asks children to produce words in response to pictures and questions. The EVT is normed with the PPVT so that you can make comparisons between children’s receptive and expressive vocabularies. You can also informally check for children’s knowledge of depth of word meaning by asking them questions that go beyond the word’s simplest or most common meaning and contexts. Many words, such as duck have multiple meanings–as in quack or watch out! User Guide: Why 53 Everything You Wanted to Know About Comprehension Strategies But Were Afraid to Ask The Basics: In Plain English What are comprehension strategies? Comprehension strategies are mental procedures that children can use on their own as they read to improve their understanding. What are some examples of comprehension strategies for young readers? Here are just a few examples, from studies with students as young as kindergarten:51 Making predictions Inferring Drawing conclusions Finding main ideas 54 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention What’s the difference between comprehension strategies and comprehension skills? These terms are commonly confused, and researchers have only recently suggested a way to make the distinction clear.52 We can think of comprehension strategies as mental actions that readers take deliberately, in an attempt to control and improve understanding. Once readers have used a particular strategy over and over, so that its use becomes natural and automatic, then we can say the strategy has evolved into a skill. For example, some young readers may need to deliberately remind themselves to visualize information after reading each sentence, particularly if the text is hard or confusing. After much practice, visualization may become something they do automatically when reading interesting material. At that point, visualization has evolved from a strategy into a skill. Research on Comprehension Strategy Instruction How much do researchers really understand about the effectiveness of comprehension strategies instruction? For Grades 3 – 6, they understand a lot; for Grades K – 2 somewhat less. Research on comprehension strategies began in the 1970’s and became a hot topic in the field soon after. For its report published in 2000, the National Reading Panel reviewed 203 studies. The vast majority were conducted with students in Grades 3 – 8. They found that seven types of comprehension instruction appeared to be effective for classroom instruction with these older students: Comprehension monitoring instruction Cooperative learning instruction U se of graphic and semantic organizers (including story maps) Question-answering instruction Question-generation instruction Summarization instruction Instruction that combined strategies (multiple-strategy instruction) also appeared effective. So do researchers recommend waiting until Grade 3 before beginning comprehension strategy instruction? No! Researchers agree that more studies are needed, but they recommend that teachers not wait on research to integrate some level of comprehension strategy instruction into their K – 2 classrooms. Here’s a quote from leading researchers David Pearson and Nell Duke: It may be that some strategies – perhaps relating text to prior experiences or making predictions about text content – are so foundational that they should be emphasized a great deal very early in schooling. It may be that other strategies, such as summarizing, are so difficult for young learners that they should be either backgrounded or given more intensive instruction in primary-grade education. Unfortunately, presently available research does little to suggest priorities for different comprehension strategies or for the particular challenges young learners may face in learning them … These significant shortcomings notwithstanding, we know enough about what to teach and how to teach comprehension to our very youngest readers User Guide: Why 55 to make a good start at doing so. To delay this sort of powerful instruction until children have reached the intermediate grades is to deny them the very experiences that help them develop the most important of reading dispositions – the expectation that they should and can understand each and every text they read.53 Teaching Comprehension Strategies What advice do researchers have for teachers? Here are some key pieces of advice for teaching comprehension strategies in K – 2 classrooms: H elp young students visualize and remember to use strategies by combining them with hand motions. Research shows that hand motions can be extremely effective in helping young students understand and trigger their use of comprehension strategies.54 M ake sure that children understand the purpose for comprehension strategies. Explain that strategies are things that good readers do to better understand and enjoy what they read. The more explicit a teacher is in teaching and stipulating the purpose of comprehension strategies, the better the students will achieve.55 Model, model, model. Most researchers advocate 56 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention that with beginning readers, you can use comprehension strategy instruction with readaloud text. As you read (or during a discussion of the text), “think out loud” to model the strategy you’re teaching. Then give children a chance to practice using the same strategy as they listen to more of the text and additional read-alouds. D on’t overdo the use of any particular strategy. For example, don’t ask children to make predictions on every page. Research shows that even good readers don’t make predictions constantly – they’re doing other things too. U se comprehension strategies instruction with both story and informational read-alouds. Research suggests that children don’t get enough practice in comprehending informational text in the early grades, and that they enjoy these texts as much as or even more than stories. By fourth grade, children with lots of practice in comprehending informational text will have a definite advantage over those who don’t.56 Some genre-specific strategies for story and informational texts include: • Stories: Understanding story elements, sequencing events, retelling, finding the underlying message • Informational texts: Understanding visual and textual elements, text structure, and summarization of non-fiction H elp children organize information and concepts from text with graphic organizers. Recommended structures include Venn diagrams, hierarchical maps, flow charts, lists, and KWL charts.57 W hen children are ready to read and write independently, encourage them to put an occasional sticky note on pages where they’ve used a strategy. After some practice with marking the page, teach children to jot down a note about the strategy, so they can share it later with a reading partner or with you. S caffold instruction to include a gradual release of responsibility to students when having them apply a strategy. Research suggests that teachers should explain, model, and scaffold instruction before releasing the responsibility onto students to practice and apply a strategy.58 E stablish a set of organizational and management routines in the classroom and create an atmosphere of support and encouragement. When students know what to expect during instruction and are not afraid of taking risks, they will perform better. Assessing Comprehension Strategies Comprehension assessments that ask children to answer questions quickly and fluently can help you assess strategies that have developed into skills. For assessing children as they are developing comprehension strategies, you can also use informal observation: If a child cannot retell a story, ask the child to identify the order of key events or use a graphic organizer to query if the child understands narrative elements and their relations. If children cannot answer multiple-choice questions quickly, ask them to think aloud as they read the stem and response options and ask them to show you how they search for confirming or disconfirming evidence in the text…The main reason for assessing strategies is to find clues about what the student is not doing or what is being done incorrectly so that teachers can reteach better strategies.59 Kathy Collins, a researcher and first grade teacher, offers several additional ideas for assessing early readers: During a reading conference I might choose to assess a child’s comprehension or engagement with the text User Guide: Why 57 by asking, “What have you been thinking about as you read this?” The child’s response can provide a lot of information. I also ask children to share some of the places where they have put sticky notes so that I can get an idea of the kinds of thoughts they have as they read. If I notice that a child always seems to predict or make personal connections, I may remind her of another kind of thought that readers may have as they read.60 58 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Recommended Reading Overcoming Dyslexia: A New and Complete Science-Based Program for Reading Problems at Any Level by Sally Shaywitz (Vintage, 2005). Direct Instruction Reading by Douglas Carnine, Jerry Silbert, and Edward Kame’enui (Prentice Hall, 2003). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: an evidencebased assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Report of the sub-groups. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000) (nih Publication No. 00-4754). Washington DC. http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/nrp/ smallbook.cfm. Information in this guide is partially excerpted from the Panel’s report. Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print by Marilyn Jager Adams (The mit Press, 1994). A Focus on Vocabulary (Research-Based Practices in Early Reading Series) by Dr. Elfrieda Hiebert, Fran Lehr, Jean Osborn. (Pacific Resources for Education and Learning, 2004). Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction by Isabel Beck, Margaret McKeown, and Linda Kucan (The Guilford Press, 2002). Comprehension Instruction: ResearchBased Best Practices edited by Cathy Collins Block and Sheri R. Parris (The Guilford Press, 2008). User Guide: Why 59 Recommended Reading (continued) Comprehension Process Instruction: Creating Reading Success in Grades K – 3 by Cathy Collins Block, Lori L. Rodgers, and Rebecca B. Johnson (The Guilford Press, 2004). Growing Readers: Units of Study in the Primary Classroom by Kathy Collins (Stenhouse Publishers, 2004). Reading and Writing Informational Text in the Primary Grades: Research-based Practices by Nell K. Duke and V. Susan Bennett-Armistead (Scholastic Teaching Resources, 2003). To Understand: New Horizons in Reading Comprehension by Ellin Oliver Keene (Heinemann, 2008). 60 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention Notes 1. Professor Snow is also a member of the FreeReading advisory board. FreeReading (www.freereading.net) contains the open source instructional materials on which the Bursts program is based. 2. Snow, C. (2007). Word Generation. Transcript from a presentation at the CREATE conference, October. Oak Brook, IL. 3. Hoover, W. A. & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127-160. 4. Haring, N.G., Lovitt, T.C., Eaton, M.D., & Hansen, C.L. (1978). The Fourth R: Research in the Classroom. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill Publishing Co. 5. See Daly, E. J., III, Lentz, F. E., & Boyer, J. (1996). The instructional hierarchy: A conceptual model for understanding the effective components of reading interventions. School Psychology Quarterly, 11, 369–386. A very similar concept, called “stages of learning” is described in Bos, C.S. & Vaughn, S. (2002). Strategies for Teaching Students with Learning and Behavior Problems. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 6. Espin and Deno (1989) found that modeling (i.e., the teacher models accurate reading of the entire word) outperformed prompting (i.e., the teacher says only the initial part of a word) in building accuracy of sight word reading. Espin, C. A., & Deno, S. L. (1989). The effects of modeling and prompting feedback strategies on sight word reading of students labeled learning disabled. Education and Treatment of Children, 12, 219231. See also: Grossen, B., & Carnine, D. (1991). Strategies for maximizing reading success in the regular classroom. In G. Stoner, M. R. Shinn, & H. M. Walker (Eds.), Interventions for Achievement and Behavior Problems (pp. 333 — 356). Silver Spring, MD: The National Association of School Psychologists. 7. For instance, Word Drill (i.e., having the student repeat a misread word following examiner modeling of accurate reading) was found to be more effective at increasing accuracy and fluency in word recognition compared to no error correction procedures, modeling followed by a single repetition, or prompting. Rosenberg, M. S. (1986). Error-correction during oral reading: A comparison of three techniques. Learning Disability Quarterly, 9, 182-192. 8. Shapiro and McCurdy found that found that teaching words in isolation did not lead to generalization in passage reading. Shapiro, E. S., & McCurdy, B. L. (1989). Direct and generalized effects of a taped-words treatment on reading proficiency. Exceptional Children, 55, 321 — 325. 62 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention 9. Keene, E. O., & Zimmerman, S. (1997). Mosaic of thought: Teaching comprehension in a reader’s workshop. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. 10. Keene, E. O. (2008). To understand: New horizons in reading comprehension. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. 11. Walczyk, J.J., & Griffith-Ross, D.A. (2007, March). How Important Is Reading Skill Fluency for Comprehension? The Reading Teacher, 60, 560–569. 12. See note 11. 13. Valencia, S. W., & & Buly, M. R. (2005). Behind test scores: What struggling readers really need. In S. Barrentine & S. Stokes (Ed.), Reading assessment: Principles and practices for elementary teachers, 2nd edition. International Reading Association. 14. Allington, R. (2001). What really matters for struggling readers: Designing research-based programs. New York: Longman 15. Hiebert, E.H. & Fisher, C.W. (April, 2002). Text matters in developing fluent reading. Paper presented at the Preconvention Institute, “Tools for Global Understanding: Fluency, Comprehension, and Content Knowledge” at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, San Francisco, CA. 16. Hempenstall, K. (2004). Abracadabra phonics: Balanced magic. Education News.org. Retrieved 8/18/08 at www.ednews.org/articles/43/1/ Abracadrabra-phonics-Balanced-magic/Page1.html 17. Foorman, B., R., Breier, J. I., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). Interventions aimed at improving reading success: An evidence-based approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24, 613-639. 18. Hempenstall, K. (2003). Phonemic awareness: What does it mean? A 2003 Update. EducationNews.org: Retrieved 8/11/08 from www.ednews. org/articles/523/1/Phonemic-awareness-What-does-it-mean/Page1.html. 19. Foorman, B., R., Breier, J. I., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). Interventions aimed at Improving reading success: An evidence-based approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24, 613-639. 20. Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 321-339. 21. See: (i) Carroll, J.B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). The American Heritage Word Frequency Book. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. (ii) Fry, E. B., Kress, J. E., & Fountoustudentis, D. L. (2000). The Reading Teacher’s Book of Lists. NJ: Prentice Hall. (iii) Kucera, H. & Francis, W. H. (1967). Computational analysis of presentday American English. Providence, RI: Brown University. (iv) Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. New York: Touchstone Applied Science Associates. 22. See also www.wordlistgenerator.net for the Dolch list (originally published in Problems in Reading, The Garrard Press, 1948) and Fry list (cited above). 23. Perfetti, C. A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.) Reading Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 24. Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491-528. 25. Clymer, T. (1963). The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 16, 252-258. Referenced in: Johnson, F. (2001). The utility of phonics generalizations: Let’s take another look at Clymer’s conclusions. The Reading Teacher, 55, 132-143. 26. Snow, C. and Juel, C. (2005). Teaching children to read: What do we know about how to do it? (pp. 501-520). In M. Snowling & C. Hulme, (Eds.) The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. See also: Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31 – 74. Wolf, M. (2007). Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain. New York: HarperCollins. 27. Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 428-449. 28. Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and instructional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458-492. 29. Cambourne, B. (2002). Explicit and systematic teaching of reading – a new slogan? In R. Allington (Ed.) Big Brother and the National Reading Curriculum: How Ideology trumped evidence. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. 30. Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E. B., Wisenbaker, J. M., Kuhn, M. R., Strauss, G. P., & Morris, R. D. (2006). Becoming a fluent and automatic reader in the early elementary school years. Reading Research Quarterly, 41, 496-522. 31. National Reading Panel (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature in reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the sub-groups. (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. 32. Walczyk, J.J., & Griffith-Ross, D.A. (2007). How Important Is Reading Skill Fluency for Comprehension? The Reading Teacher, 60, 560–569. 33. Torgesen, J. K. (2004). Lessons learned from research on interventions for students who have difficulty in learning to read. In P. McCardle and V. Chhabra (Eds.) The Voice of Evidence in Reading Research. (pp. 355-382). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company. 34. Stahl, S. A., & Heubach, K. M. (2005). Fluency-oriented reading instruction. Journal of Literacy Research, 37, 25 – 60. 35. Rasinski, T. (2008). Teaching fluency artfully. In R. Fink., & S. J. Samuels, (Eds.) , Inspiring reading success: Interest and motivation in an age of high-stakes testing (pp. 117-140). Newark, DE: International Reading Association, Inc. 36. Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2004). Monitoring early reading development in first grade: Word identification fluency versus nonsense word fluency. Exceptional Children, 71, 7-21. 37. Graves, M. F., Brunetti, G. J., & Slater, W. H. (1982). The reading vocabularies of primary-grade children of varying geographic and social backgrounds. In J. A. Harris, & L. A. Harris (Eds.), New inquiries in reading research and instruction (pp. 99-104). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Moats, L. (2001, Summer). Overcoming the language gap. American Educator, 5 – 9. 38. Moats, L. (2001, Summer). Overcoming the language gap. American Educator, 5 – 9. 39. Chall, J. S., & Jacobs, V. A. (2003). Poor children’s fourth-grade slump. American Educator: Research Round-Up. Retrieved from http://www.aft. org/pubs-reports/american_educator/spring2003/chall.html. Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. User Guide: Why 63 40. Lehr, F., Osborn, J., Hiebert, E. (2004). A Focus on Vocabulary. Research-Based Practices in Early Reading Series. Pacific Resources for Education and Learning. Available at: http://www.prel.org/products/re_/ ES0419.htm. 41. See http://www.wordgeneration.org/snow.html. 42. Beck, I., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. New York: Guilford. 43. Juel, C., & Deffes, R. (2004). Making words sticky. Educational Leadership, 61 (6), 30 – 34. 44. See note 40. 45. See note 31. 46. Juel, C., & Deffes, R. (2004). Making words sticky. Educational Leadership, 61 (6), 30 – 34. Labbo, L.D., Love, M.S., & Ryan, T. (2007, March). A vocabulary flood: making words “sticky” with computer-response activities. The Reading Teacher, 60(6), 582–588. 47. Labbo, L.D., Love, M.S., & Ryan, T. (2007, March). A vocabulary flood: making words “sticky” with computer-response activities. The Reading Teacher, 60(6), 582–588. 48. See note 40. 49. See note 40. 50. Adams, M., & Spoehr, K. T. (2006). Framework Paper for the Vocabulary Addition to the National Assessment of Adult Literacy (NAAL). See http://cog.brown.edu/~spoehr/Vocabulary_Project/VocabMain.html 51. Block, C. C., Parris, S. R., & Whiteley, C. S. (2008). CPMs: A Kinesthetic Comprehension Strategy. The Reading Teacher, 61(6), pp. 460 – 470. 64 Burst:Reading Literacy Intervention 52. Afflerbach, P., Pearson, D. P., & Paris, S. G. (2008). Clarifying differences between reading skills and reading strategies. The Reading Teacher, 61(5), 363-373 53. Pearson, P. D. & Duke, N. K. (2002). Comprehension instruction in the primary grades. In C.C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.) Comprehension instruction: Research-based Best Practices. Guilford. 54. Block, C. C., Parris, S. R., & Whiteley, C. S. (2008). CPMs: A Kinesthetic Comprehension Strategy. The Reading Teacher, 61(6), pp. 460 – 470. 55. RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for Understanding: Toward an R&D Program in Reading Comprehension. 56. Duke, N. K. & Bennett-Armistead, V. S. (2003). Reading and writing informational text in the primary grades: Research-based practices. New York: Scholastic. 57. Duke, N. K. & Bennett-Armistead, V. S. (2003). Reading and writing informational text in the primary grades: Research-based practices. New York: Scholastic. 58. Carlisle, J. & Rice, M. (2002). Improving Reading Comprehension: Research-based Principles and Practices. USA: York Press. Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2005). A Focus on Comprehension. Pacific Resources for Education and Learning. 59. Afflerbach, P., Pearson, D. P., & Paris, S. G. (2008). Clarifying differences between reading skills and reading strategies. The Reading Teacher, 61(5), 363-373 60. Collins, K. (2004). Growing readers: Units of study in the primary classroom. Portland, ME: Stenhouse. User Guide: Why III WHAT HOW • Welcome to Burst®:Reading! • How should I prepare for Burst sessions? • Introducing Burst:Reading • How do I make my Burst:Reading sessions effective for • What’s in a Burst? • Smarter Instructional Materials • Where does the instructional content come from? • What is the need for Burst:Reading? WHY all students in the group? • W hat should the rest of my class be doing while I work with the Burst group? • H ow do I change a Burst if I feel it doesn’t match my students’ needs? • H ow can I keep students on task and motivated during a Burst? • Why Burst:Reading? • How far can I depart from the steps specified in the Burst? • Reading Comprehension • What are the important roles teachers and others on • Phonological Awareness Instruction the instructional team may play in delivering Bursts? • Letter-Sound Instruction • Can I add students to my Burst group? • Sounding-Out Instruction • H ow should I respond when some of the students in • High-Frequency Irregular Word Instruction • Letter Combination Instruction • Advanced Phonics Instruction • Reading Fluency Instruction • Vocabulary Instruction • Comprehension Strategies Instruction • Recommended Reading my group make great progress but, for others in the same group, progress is slower? • H ow do I handle a Burst session that takes more than (or less than) 30 minutes? • How do I use Burst:Reading with my ell students? • H ow do I use Burst:Reading as part of Response to Intervention (rti)? • H ow do I use Burst:Reading with my Special Ed students? © 2012 Wireless Generation, Inc. Wireless Generation®, Burst®:Reading, and mCLASS® are registered trademarks of Wireless Generation, Inc. All other registered and unregistered trademarks are the sole property of their owners. Corporate: 55 Washington Street, Suite 900, Brooklyn, NY 11201-1071 • Customer Care: (800) 823-1969 • Sales Inquiries: (866) 212-8688 • www.wirelessgeneration.com powered by
© Copyright 2024