Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Learning and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif Negative thinking versus positive thinking in a Singaporean student sample: Relationships with psychological well-being and psychological maladjustment Shyh Shin Wong ⁎ Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, Republic of Singapore a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 28 December 2010 Received in revised form 14 November 2011 Accepted 22 November 2011 Keywords: Negative thinking Positive thinking Stress Anxiety Depression Anger Life satisfaction Happiness a b s t r a c t This study examines the relationships of positive thinking versus negative thinking with psychological wellbeing and psychological maladjustment. Three hundred and ninety-eight undergraduate students from Singapore participated in this study. First, positive thinking were positively correlated with indicators psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness, and negatively correlated with indicators of psychopathology – stress, anxiety, depression, and anger. In contrast, negative thinking were positively correlated with indicators of psychopathology – stress, anxiety, depression, and anger, and negatively correlated with indicators of psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness. Second, hierarchical multiple regression results showed that females were more likely than males to be stressed and anxious at the first step of entry. However, there were no significant differences between the sexes in terms of depression, anger, life satisfaction, and happiness. Age did not significantly predict any of the criterion variables. Third, hierarchical multiple regression results showed that negative thinking accounted for more of the significant incremental unique variance in depression, stress, anxiety, life satisfaction, anger, and happiness in order of effect size. This is also found that positive thinking do accounted for a sizable significant incremental unique variance in happiness and life satisfaction, while a very small percentage of 1% significant incremental unique variance for stress, depression, anxiety, and anger. Implications and limitations of these findings were discussed. © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction The role of critical and creative thinking has been relatively well studied in educational research (Abrami et al., 2008; Simonton, 2000). However, with the growing importance of Social and Emotional Learning (SEL) (Elias et al., 1997; Goleman, 1995, Ministry of Education, 2009) and Emotional Literacy (Goleman, 1995), the role of other types of thinking in relation to Social and Emotional Learning and Emotional Literacy should be considered. The present study examined the role of negative thinking versus positive thinking in emotional or psychological maladjustment as expressed in various psychological problems and emotional well-being subsumed under the larger research area of psychological well-being. Negative thinking have been found to play a pivotal role in psychological maladjustment, such as depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979; Dobson & Breiter, 1983; Harrell & Ryon, 1983; Hollon & Kendall, 1980; Hollon, Kendall, & Lumry, 1986; Lightsey, 1994a, 1994b; Lightsey & Christopher, 1997; Muris, Mayer, den Adel, Roos, & van Wamelen, 2009; Olioff, Bryson, & Wadden, 1989), anxiety (Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985; Heimberg, Acerra, & Holstein, 1985; Szentagotai & Freeman, 2007, Wong, 2008), anger (Beck, ⁎ Psychological Studies Academic Group, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, 1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore 637616, Republic of Singapore. E-mail address: [email protected]. 1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.013 1999), and stress (Lightsey, 1994a). Negative thinking has also been found to be negatively associated with psychological well-being, such as happiness (Lightsey, 1994b). Positive thinking has also been found to be negatively related to psychological maladjustment, such as depression (Ingram, Kendall, Siegle, & Guarino, 1995; Lightsey, 1994a, 1994b; Lightsey & Christopher, 1997) and positively related to psychological well-being, such as satisfaction with life (Ingram et al., 1995) and happiness (Lightsey, 1994b). However, the issue of the relative importance of negative thinking versus positive thinking arises with the acceptance of positive thinking as a construct worthy of scientific investigation recently (Ingram et al., 1995). Ingram and Wisnicki (1988) proposed that there are at least two possible positions on this issue. First, the presence of positive thinking may be less important in determining psychological maladjustment than the absence of negative thinking (Kendall, 1984; Kendall & Hollon, 1981). This is the traditional position taken by theorists and researchers who emphasize the importance of psychopathological processes in influencing psychological maladjustment. For example, in the “vulnerability hypothesis” (Lightsey, 1994a) of the cognitive theory of depression, negative thinking and underlying negative schemas are hypothesized to contribute to depression (Beck, 1967; Beck et al., 1979). However, with the recent focus on the positive psychology movement (Seligman & Csikszentimihalyi, 2000; Simonton & Baumeister, 2005); a second position proposes that the presence of positive thinking rather than the S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 77 presence of negative thinking may be more important in determining psychological maladjustment as well as psychological well-being (Heimberg et al., 1985; Ingram, Smith, & Brehm, 1983). Lightsey (1994a, 1994b) proposed that this constitutes the “buffer hypothesis”, a logical corollary of the “vulnerability hypothesis”. consists of three hundred and twenty-nine (82.7%) Chinese, thirtyseven (9.3%) Malays, twenty-three (5.8%) Indians, nine (2.3%) identified with other racial groups (which includes all other ethnic groups not listed). Participation was voluntary and responses were anonymous. 1.1. The present study 2.2. Measures The purpose of the current study is to identify the relative incremental contributions of negative thinking versus positive thinking accounting for incremental unique variance in several important psychological outcome variables, that indicate either psychological maladjustment (e.g., depression, anxiety, anger, stress) or psychological well-being (e.g., life satisfaction and happiness) in an Asian sample. The design of the current study served to add to the knowledge based as prior research in two main ways. Previous research tends to predominantly involves European and American participants. Hence, cross-cultural convergent and divergent criterion-related validity for the constructs of both negative thinking and positive thinking in an Asian sample is not well known. Secondly, the current study looked at the explanatory powers of both positive thinking and negative thinking in multiple important mental health outcomes in both the negative as well as positive domains, i.e., both psychological maladjustment and psychological well-being. This is an improvement in research design since previous research tended to focus on just one type of thinking or one type of outcome domain, rather than multiple types concurrently. A number of hypotheses were examined in the current study. First, Hypothesis 1A states that negative thinking would be positively correlated with measures of psychological maladjustment – depression, anxiety, anger, and stress, while Hypothesis 1B states that negative thinking would be negatively correlated with measures of psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness. In contrast, Hypothesis 2A states that positive thinking would be negatively correlated with measures of psychological maladjustment – depression, anxiety, anger, and stress, while Hypothesis 2B states that positive thinking would be positively correlated with measures of psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness. Second, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses would be used to identify the relative unique contributions of negative thinking versus positive thinking accounting for the incremental variance in various mental health outcome variables after controlling for either positive thinking or negative thinking respectively in second step of entry after entry of age and sex at first step of entry (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). These hierarchical multiple regression results obtained could also be used indirectly to compare the viability of the “vulnerability hypothesis” and the “buffer hypothesis”. If the “vulnerability hypothesis” is more viable than the “buffer hypothesis”, we would expect that negative thinking would account for more incremental unique variance than positive thinking in the selected outcome measures, even after controlling for demographic variables and positive thinking (Hypothesis 3A). Conversely, if the “buffer hypothesis” is more viable than the “vulnerability hypothesis”, we would expect that positive thinking would account for more incremental unique variance than negative thinking in the selected outcome measures, even after controlling for demographic variables and negative thinking (Hypothesis 3B). 2.2.1. Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire-Negative (ATQ-N) The Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire-Negative (ATQ-N; Hollon & Kendall, 1980) was designed to measure the frequency of negative thoughts. These negative thoughts are hypothesized to play an important role in the development, maintenance and treatment of various psychological maladjustment, including depression (Beck et al., 1979) and anxiety (Beck et al., 1985), and anger (Beck, 1999). The thirty items on the ATQ-N are scored on a five-point Likert scale format (One = Not at all, Two = Sometimes, Three = Moderately often, Four = Often, Five = All the time). The total score is a sum of all thirty items. The ATQ has excellent internal consistency and has good concurrent validity for depression (Hollon & Kendall, 1980). 2.2.2. Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire-Positive (ATQ-P) The ATQ-P (Ingram & Wisnicki, 1988) was developed as the theoretical complement to the ATQ-N. The ATQ-P is a thirty-item selfreport instrument that measures the frequency of positive thoughts. The thirty items on the ATQ-N are scored on a five-point Likert scale format (One = Not at all, Two = Sometimes, Three = Moderately often, Four = Often, Five = All the time). The total score is a sum of all thirty items. The ATQ-P is reported to be reliable, able to discriminate between psychopathological and non-psychopathological states, unaffected by social desirability influences, has good convergent and discriminant validity, and is sensitive to change in affective states (Ingram et al., 1995). 2.2.3. Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) The BDI-II (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) is a twenty-one-item selfreport measure that assesses depressive symptoms. Each item contains four statements reflecting varying degrees of symptom severity. Respondents are instructed to circle the number (ranging from zero to three, indicating severity) that corresponds with the statement that best describes them. Total BDI-II score can range from zero to sixty-three. The BDI-II has demonstrated high internal consistency, good test-retest reliability, and good construct and concurrent validity with other common measures of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples (Beck et al., 1996; Whisman, Perez, & Ramel, 2000). 2.2.4. Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) The BAI (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988) is a twenty-one-item, self-report inventory for measuring severity of anxiety. Respondents are instructed to circle the number representing one of the four alternatives, which ranges from no anxiety (One= Not at all) to severe anxiety (Four = Severely or “I could barely stand it”). Total BAI score can range from twenty-one to eighty-four. The BAI has demonstrated high internal consistency and test-retest reliability and good concurrent and discriminant validity (Beck & Steer, 1991; Beck et al., 1988; Fydrich, Dowdall, & Chambless, 1992; Riskind, Beck, Brown, & Steer, 1987). Moreover, the BAI was carefully constructed to avoid confounding with depression. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Three hundred and ninety-eight college students from Singapore participated in this study. The mean age was 25.53 years (SD = 3.68). There were two hundred and fifty-three (63.6%) females and one hundred and forty (35.2%) males. Ethnic composition 2.2.5. Trait Anger Scale (TAS) The TAS (London & Spielberger, 1983) consists of fifteen items that that assess anger as a relatively stable personality trait. Trait anger is defined in terms of how frequently a respondent feels state anger over time. The trait-anger items are rated on four-point scales from Almost Never (One) to Almost Always (Four). Scores are the sum of the item ratings. The TAS has very good internal consistency. 78 S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 The TAS demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity (Corcoran & Fischer, 2000). 2.2.6. Perceived stress scale-four The PSS-Four (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) is the shortversion of the PSS-Fourteen, which is designed to measure the degree to which situations in one's life are appraised as stressful. The PSSFour is composed of four items (numbers two, six, seven, fourteen) that were correlated most highly with the fourteen-item PSS- Fourteen. Respondents are instructed to circle the number representing one of the four alternatives, which ranges from Never (Zero) to Very Often (Four). Total PSS-Four score can range from zero to sixteen. The PSS- Fourteen showed adequate internal and testreliability and is correlated in the expected manner with a range of measures (e.g., depressive symptoms, frequency of physical illness, health behaviors, life-events, health behaviors, physical illness, social anxiety). The coefficient alpha reliability estimate for the PSS-Four was .72. The test-retest reliability of the PSS-Four over a two month interval was .55. 2.2.7. Oxford Happiness Questionnaire-Short (OHQ-Short) The OHQ-Short (Hill & Argyle, 2002) is designed for measuring personal happiness. It is strongly correlated with the full OHQ. The OHQ-Short is a six-point Likert scale ranging from Agree Strongly (One) to Disagree Strongly (Six). Three items are reverse scored. Scores range from eight to forty-eight. Internal consistency and short-term test-retest reliability were found to be satisfactory. Both versions of OHQ have demonstrated validity with measures of happiness, personality, self-esteem, satisfaction, life orientation, and life regard. 2.2.8. Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) The SWLS (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) is a concise five-item measure of global life satisfaction and is suitable for all ages, from adolescents to adults. Respondents indicated their extent of agreement with each of the item (for example, “In most ways my life is close to my ideal”) on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from One (strongly disagree) to Seven (strongly agree). The reliability and validity of the SWLS has been considered adequate (Diener et al., 1985; Neto, 1993; Pavot, Diener, Colvin, & Sandvik, 1991). 3. Results Besides descriptive statistics and correlational analyses, they were two sets of six hierarchical multiple regression analyses for each of the six dependent variables – depression, anxiety, anger, stress, life satisfaction, and happiness using the same four predictors of age, sex, positive thinking, (positive automatic thoughts) and negative thinking (positive automatic thoughts) in each of the analyses. In set 1 (Equation 1) six hierarchical multiple regression analyses, age and sex were entered at the first step (Model 1) for predicting each of the six criterion variables, followed by negative thinking at second step (Model 2), and finally positive thinking at third step (Model 3). Model 3 also shows the results of each of the six standard simultaneous regression analyses when all four same predictors were entered together for each of the criterion variables. In set 2 (Equation 2), the sequence of entry is reversed at steps 2 and 3, after controlling for age and sex at first step (Model 1). In contrast to set 1, positive thinking is entered at second step and negative thinking is entered at third step. However, Model 3 for both set 1 and set 2 do present the same results for both sets of hierarchical multiple regression analyses since all same four predictors are entered together. 3.1. Preliminary statistics The means, standard deviations, and standardized coefficient alphas of the measures used in this study are shown in Table 1. The measures used in this study were also found to have internal consistency reliability estimates mostly of acceptable values, with standardized coefficient alphas ranging from .68 to .98. One item (item number 7) from the Perceived Scale Scale-4 was deleted because the item was found to have negative correlation coefficients with the remaining three items, and the standardized coefficient alpha was found to be .15. After deleting the item number 7, the PSS-3 was found to have a higher standardized coefficient alpha value of .68. The various correlation coefficients of the measures used in the study are reported in Table 2. Age was significantly correlated with sex only. Males (M = 27.286, SD = 3.226) tended to be older than females in the current sample (t = −7.462, p b .001). Sex was significantly correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress. Females tended to score higher than males on the BDI-II (Females: M = 12.303, SD = 8.278; Males: M = 10.293, SD = 9.136; t = 2.192, p b .05), BAI (Females: M = 31.615, SD = 8.466; Males: M = 29.007, SD = 9.523; t = −2.787, p b .01), and PSS-3 (Females: M = 5.613, SD = 2.278; Males: M = 4.600, SD = 2.541; t = 4.073, p b .001). Negative thoughts were inversely correlated with positive thoughts. 3.2. Thinking and depression Depression was significantly correlated with negative thinking, positive thinking, and sex in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 29% of the variance in depression scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive thinking, which significantly accounted for only 1% of the variance in depression scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 3). 3.3. Thinking and anxiety Anxiety was significantly correlated with negative thinking, positive thinking, and sex in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 20% of the variance in anxiety scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive thinking, which significantly accounted for only 1% of the variance in anxiety scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 4). 3.4. Thinking and anger Anger was significantly correlated with negative thinking and positive thinking in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 11% of the variance in anger scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive Table 1 Means, standard deviations, and standardized coefficient alphas. Variable n M SD Std. Coefficient Alpha Negative Thinking Positive Thinking Depression Anxiety Anger Stress Life Satisfaction Happiness 395 393 382 396 397 398 397 396 61.34 95.20 11.53 30.70 29.34 5.26 20.66 32.34 23.36 20.71 8.63 8.90 6.73 2.41 6.70 6.73 .98 .97 .91 .92 .87 .68 .88 .73 S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 79 Table 2 Bivariate correlations among variables. Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. Age 2. Sex 3. Negative Thinking 4. Positive Thinking 5. Depression 6. Anxiety 7. Anger 8. Stress 9. Life Satisfaction 10. Happiness – .35** – − .08 − .04 – .07 .09 − .60** – − .06 − .11* .75** − 54.** – − .05 − .14** .54** − .31** .63** – .0 .02 .47** − .35** .46** .41** – − .09 − .20** .66** − .50** .67** .56** .36** – .04 .02 − .58** .56** − .56** − .40** − .38** − 46** – .04 .02 − .66** .62** − .68** − .43** − .36** − .60** .63** – Note. ** p b .01 (2-tailed); * p b .05 (2-tailed). thinking, which significantly accounted for only 1% of the variance in anger scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 5). thinking, which significantly accounted for only 7% of the variance in life satisfaction scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 7). 3.5. Thinking and stress 3.7. Thinking and happiness Stress was significantly correlated with negative thinking, positive thinking, and sex in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 21% of the variance in stress scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive thinking, which significantly accounted for only 1% of the variance in stress scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 6). Happiness was significantly correlated with negative thinking and positive thinking in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 14% of the variance in life satisfaction scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive thinking, which significantly accounted for only 7% of the variance in life satisfaction scores after controlling for age, sex, and negative thoughts (see Table 8). 3.6. Thinking and life satisfaction 4. Discussion Life satisfaction was significantly correlated with negative thinking and positive thinking in descending order of magnitude. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that negative thinking significantly accounted for 10% of the variance in life satisfaction scores after controlling for age, sex, and positive thoughts; in comparison to positive The main objective of the present study to test the “vulnerability hypothesis” versus “buffer hypothesis” pertaining to the relative incremental contributions of negative thinking versus positive thinking in predicting depression, anxiety, anger, stress, life satisfaction, and happiness in an Asian sample. While the current study is unique in Table 3 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting depression. Table 4 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting anxiety. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p Depression 1) .01 − .08 − 1.87 .13 .99 − .03 − .10 − .61 − 1.89 .08 − 1.56 .28 .09 .65 .01 .04 − .09 .75 .98 − 2.40 22.08 .09 − 1.41 .25 − .06 .09 .64 .02 .02 .04 − .08 .68 − .13 Depression 1.03 − 2.19 16.00 − 3.10 .01 − .08 − 1.87 .07 .13 .99 − .03 − .10 − .61 − 1.89 .00 − 1.11 − .22 .11 .84 .02 .00 − .06 − .54 .03 − 1.32 − 12.21 .09 − 1.41 − .06 .25 .09 .64 .02 .02 .04 − .08 − .13 .68 1.03 − 2.19 − 3.10 16.00 2) .56 3) 1) .01 2) .28 3) .29 .07 .54 .06 .00** .33 .02* .00** .00** .31 .03* .00** .00** .54 .06 .00** .98 .20 .00** .00** .31 .03* .00** .00** Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p .02 .02* .94 .01* .00** .29 .00** .00** .00** .29 .00** .00** .55 Anxiety 1) .01 − 2.71 .13 1.01 .00 − .15 .07 − 2.69 .12 − 2.59 .20 .11 .85 .02 .05 − .14 .54 1.06 − 3.04 12.47 .12 − 2.63 .21 .01 .11 .86 .02 .02 .05 − .14 .55 .03 Anxiety 1.05 − 3.08 10.34 .60 .01 − 2.74 .13 1.01 .00 − .15 .07 − 2.69 .05 − 2.32 − .13 .13 .97 .02 .02 − .13 − .30 .42 − 2.41 − 6.17 .12 − 2.63 .01 .21 .11 .86 .02 .02 .05 − .14 .03 .55 1.05 − 3.08 .60 10.34 2) .28 3) .01 1) .02 2) .09 3) .20 .02* .94 .01* .00** .67 .02* .00** .00** .30 .00** .55 .00** Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. 80 S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 Table 5 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting anger. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p Anger 1) .00 − .03 .25 .10 .76 − .02 .02 − .33 .33 .04 .35 .14 .09 .67 .01 .02 .03 .47 .39 .53 10.45 .04 .45 .12 −.04 .09 .67 .02 .02 .02 .03 .41 − .11 Anger .41 .67 7.20 − 1.99 − .03 .25 .10 .76 − .02 .02 − .33 − .33 .00 .61 − .12 .09 .72 .02 .00 .04 − .36 .03 .39 .00 .04 .45 − .04 .12 .09 .67 .02 .02 .02 .03 − .11 .41 .41 .67 − 1.99 7.20 2) .22 3) .01 1) .00 2) .13 3) .11 .92 .74 .74 .00** .70 .60 .00** .05 .68 .50 .00** .05 .92 .74 .74 .00** .98 .39 .00** .00** .68 .50 .05 .00** Table 7 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for var. predicting life satisfaction. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p .00 .60 .35 .99 .00** .87 .83 .00** .00** .89 .46 .00** .00** Life Satisfaction 1) .10 .10 .11 .80 .05 .00 .95 .01 .02 − .14 − .18 .09 .65 .01 .01 − .03 − .58 .16 − .21 − 14.03 .01 − .46 − .12 .11 .08 .62 .02 .02 .01 − .03 − .39 .33 Life Satisfaction .14 − .74 − 7.84 6.65 .10 .01 .11 .80 .05 .00 .95 .01 .05 − .62 .19 .09 .67 .01 .02 − .04 .56 .53 − .94 13.20 .01 − .46 .11 − .12 .08 .62 .02 .02 .01 − .03 .33 − .39 .14 − .74 6.65 − 7.84 2) .34 3) .07 1) .00 2) .31 3) .10 .60 .34 .99 .00** .60 .35 .00** .00** .89 .46 .00** .00** Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. its sample composition and comparative approach to studying negative and positive thinking together across multiple negative and positive health outcomes, some limitations of the current study should be noted before discussing the findings. First, the findings may not generalize to clinical samples. Further studies using clinical samples are needed to replicate the results. Second, the cross-sectional design of the study limits the causal interpretation of the results. Future studies using experimental or prospective research designs may be helpful to illuminate this issue. Before discussing the study main findings, an examination of the demographic variables showed that they played a relatively minor explanatory role compared to negative thinking and positive thinking. Age did not significantly predict any of the criterion variables, even though the males in the current sample are much older than Table 6 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for variables predicting stress. Table 8 Summary of hierarchical multiple regression analyses for var. predicting happiness. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p Stress 1) .04 − .02 − .98 .04 .27 − .03 − .20 − .51 − 3.64 .02 − .93 .07 .03 .20 .00 .02 .03 .65 .56 − 4.59 17.45 .02 − .88 .06 − .02 .99 .03 .01 .01 .02 − .17 .57 − .14 Stress .58 − 4.37 12.25 − 3.10 − .02 − .98 .04 .27 − .03 − .20 − .51 − 3.64 − .00 − .80 − .06 .03 .24 .01 .00 − .16 − .49 − .07 − 3.37 11.09 .02 − .88 − .02 .06 .03 .20 .01 .01 .02 − .17 − .14 .57 .58 − 4.37 − 3.10 12.25 2) .43 3) .01 1) .04 2) .23 3) .21 .00** .61 .00** .00** .57 .00** .00** .05 .56 .00** .00** .00** .00** .61 .00** .00** .94 .39 .00** .00** .56 .00** .00** .00** Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. Variable Equation 1 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex NAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex NAT PAT Equation 2 Step 1 (Model Age Sex Step 2 (Model Age Sex PAT Step 3 (Model Age Sex PAT NAT B SE B Beta t R2 Change p .00 .54 .32 .91 .00** .96 .92 .00** .00** .99 .50 .00** .00** Happiness 1) .10 .09 .11 .77 .05 .01 .99 .11 .00 − .06 − .19 .08 .58 .01 .00 − .00 − .67 .06 − .10 − 17.33 .00 − .36 − .13 .11 .07 .54 .01 .02 .00 − .03 − .46 .34 Happiness − .01 − .67 − 10.29 7.62 .10 .09 .10 .77 .05 .01 .99 .11 .04 − .54 .20 .08 .61 .01 .02 − .04 .62 .47 − .89 15.27 .00 − .36 .11 − .13 .07 .54 .02 .01 .00 − .03 .34 − .46 − .01 − .67 7.62 − 10.29 2) .44 3) .07 1) .00 2) .38 3) .14 .54 .32 .91 .00** .64 .37 .00** .00** .99 .50 .00** .00** Note. ** p b .01; * p b .05; NAT = Negative Automatic Thinking; PAT = Positive Automatic Thinking; p = p value. S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 the females, due to compulsory military service for males before entering University. A person's sex was also found not to be related to either type of thinking. This means that males are as likely as females to think both negatively as well as positively, contrary to common gender stereotype. Similar to previous research findings in the West (Zahn-Waxler, Crick, Shirtcliff, & Woods, 2006), sex was significantly correlated with depression, anxiety, and stress. Hierarchical multiple regression results showed that females were more likely than males to be stressed and anxious. However, the gender effect sizes were weaker when compared to both types of thinking after thinking is entered into the equations. These results implied the importance of examining factors beyond sex when explaining individual differences in psychological maladjustment as well as psychological well-being. With regard to the hypotheses being tested, correlational results supported Hypothesis 1A, Hypothesis 1B, Hypothesis 2A, and Hypothesis 2B. As expected, negative thinking were positively correlated with measures of psychological maladjustment – stress, anxiety, depression, and anger (Hypothesis 1A), and negatively correlated with measures of psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness (Hypothesis 1B). As for positive thinking, the reverse pattern was obtained as expected. The positive thinking were negatively correlated with measures of psychological maladjustment – stress, anxiety, depression, and anger (Hypothesis 2A), and positively correlated with measures of psychological well-being – life satisfaction and happiness (Hypothesis 2B). The correlational results obtained were congruent with the theoretical expectations as well as past research findings. Moreover, they provided cross-cultural convergent and divergent criterion-related validity for the construct of both negative thinking and positive thinking in an Asian sample. Similar to the western studies (e.g., Beck, 1999, Lightsey, 1994a, 1994b; Lightsey & Christopher, 1997; Olioff et al., 1989; Szentagotai & Freeman, 2007), negative thinking was found to be positively related to depression, anxiety, anger, and stress; and negatively related to happiness. In addition, negative thinking was found to be negatively related to life satisfaction. The hierarchical multiple regression results supported Hypothesis 3A, instead of Hypothesis 3B. The set 2 hierarchical multiple regression results showed that negative thinking accounted for more of the incremental unique variance in depression (29% vs. 1% in set 1), stress (21% vs. 1% in set 1), anxiety (20% vs. 1% in set 1), happiness (14% vs. 7% in set 1), anger (11% vs. .8% in set 1), and life satisfaction (10% vs. 7% in set 1) in order of effect size after controlling for demographic variables and positive thinking in comparison with the set 1 hierarchical multiple regression results, which controlled for demographic variables and negative thinking. These results appeared to support the viability of the “vulnerability hypothesis” over the “buffer hypothesis”. This is consistent with the proposal by Kendall (1984) and Kendall and Hollon (1981) in that the presence of positive thinking may be less important in determining psychological maladjustment than the absence of negative thinking. This current finding collaborate the ongoing area of active research into the “vulnerability hypothesis” especially for psychological disorders, beyond maladjustments (Ingram & Price, 2010). Nevertheless, there was also evidence to support a role of “buffer hypothesis” for happiness and life satisfaction, since positive thinking also accounted for significant incremental unique variance of 7% in both measures. A smaller effect size of 1% was obtained for stress, anxiety, depression, and anger. The results were similar to those obtained in Western samples (e.g., Ingram, Slater, Atkinson, Scott, 1990; Lightsey, 1994b). The current study provided a number of implications. First, the results implied that negative thinking are relatively more important than positive thinking in explaining depression, stress, anxiety, happiness, anger, and life satisfaction. This means that evoking positive thinking alone may not be sufficient to protect oneself from depression, 81 stress, anxiety, and anger, or increase one's happiness and life satisfaction. It is also consistent with the “power of non-negative thinking” by a number of theorists such as Kendall (1984). Second, the results shed further light on the idea of “exclusivity hypothesis” (Haaga, Dyck, & Ernst, 1991, which states that “depressed people will report virtually no positive cognitions, only negative or neutral ones” (p. 217–218). Perhaps the severity of the depression may play a part in determining the frequency of positive thoughts present. In the current nonclinical sample, it was found that the presence of positive thinking do explain about 1% (vs. 29% for negative thinking) of the incremental unique variance in depression. Third, the results were consistent with the “negativity hypothesis” (Haaga et al., 1991), which states that “depressed persons’ thinking are more negative than are those of nondepressed people” (p. 216). Furthermore, the “negativity hypothesis” was also supported for other challenges such as stress, anxiety, and anger. A related area of discovery was that a logical corollary of the “negativity hypothesis” in the form of “positivity hypothesis” may be supported by the current research. This means that satisfied and happy people thinking may be more positive than those who are not. Finally, negative and positive thinking have directly opposite natures since they are negatively correlated, but they are not mutually exclusive. It is possible for one individual to have both types of thinking within a specific period of time or in a given situation/setting. However, individuals with high PATs do have low NATs, and vice versa. Acknowledgments Funding for this research is facilitated by a research grant RI 11/05 WSS from the National Institute of Education. References Abrami, P. C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Surkes, M. A., Tamim, R., et al. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills and dispositions: A stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 78, 1102–1134. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Beck, A. T. (1999). Prisoners of hate: The cognitive basis of anger, hostility, and violence. New York: Harper Collins Publishers. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893–897. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1991). Relationship between the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale with anxious outpatients. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5, 213–223. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory: Manual (2nd ed.). Boston: Harcourt Brace. Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385–396. Corcoran, K., & Fischer, J. (2000). Measures for clinical practice: A sourcebook: Volume 2, Adults. New York: Free Press. Diener, E., Emmons, A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale: A measurement of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. Dobson, K. S., & Breiter, H. J. (1983). Cognitive assessment of depression: Reliability and validity of three measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 107–109. Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Weisssberg, R. P., Frey, K. S., Greenberg, M. T., Haynes, N. T., et al. (1997). Promoting social and emotional learning: Guidelines for educators. Alexandria, Virgina: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Fydrich, T., Dowdall, D., & Chambless, D. L. (1992). Reliability and validity of the BAI. Journal of Anxiety Disorder, 6, 55–61. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books. Haaga, D. A. F., Dyck, M. J., & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215–236. Harrell, T. H., & Ryon, N. B. (1983). Cognitive-behavioral assessment of depression: Clinical validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 721–725. Heimberg, R. G., Acerra, M. C., & Holstein, A. (1985). Partner similarity mediates interpersonal anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 443–453. Hill, P., & Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1073–1082. 82 S.S. Wong / Learning and Individual Differences 22 (2012) 76–82 Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 383–395. Hollon, S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of depressotypic cognitions in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 52–59. Ingram, R. E., Kendall, P. C., Siegle, G., & Guarino, J. (1995). Psychometric properties of the positive automatic thoughts questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 7, 495–507. Ingram, R. E., & Price, J. M. (Eds.). (2010). Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan (2nd ed.). . New York: Guilford Press. Ingram, R. E., Slater, M. A., Atkinson, J. H., & Scott, W. (1990). Positive automatic cognition in major affective disorder. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2, 209–211. Ingram, R. E., Smith, T. W., & Brehm, S. S. (1983). Depression and information processing: Self-schemata and the encoding of self-referent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychological, 45, 412–420. Ingram, R. E., & Wisnicki, K. S. (1988). Assessment of positive automatic cognition. Journal of Counsulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 898–902. Kendall, P. C. (1984). Cognitive processes and procedures in behavior therapy. In G. T. Wilson, C. M. Franks, K. D. Brownell, & P. C. Kendall (Eds.), Annual review of behavior therapy: Theory and practice, Vol. 9. (pp. 132–179)New York: Guilford Press. Kendall, P. C., & Hollon, S. D. (1981). Assessing self-referent speech: Methods in the measurement of self-statements. In P. C. Kendall, & S. D. Hollon (Eds.), Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioral interventions (pp. 85–118). New York: Academic Press. Lightsey, O. R. (1994). Positive automatic cognitions as moderators of the negative life event-dysphoria relationship. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 353–365. Lightsey, O. R. (1994). “Thinking positive” as a stress buffer: The role of positive automatic cognitions in depression and happiness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 325–334. Lightsey, O. R., & Christopher, J. C. (1997). Stress buffers and dysphoria in a nonwestern population. Journal of Counseling and Development, 75, 451–459. London, P., & Spielberger, C. (1983 March). Job stress, hassles and medical risk. American Health, 58–93. Ministry of Education (2009). Social and emotional learning. Retrieved Febuary 3, 2009, from. http://www.moe.gov.sg/education/programmes/social-emotional-learning Muris, P., Mayer, B., den Adel, M., Roos, T., & van Wamelen, J. (2009). Predictors of change following cognitive-behavioral treatment of children with anxiety problems: A preliminary investigation on negative automatic thoughts and anxiety control. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40, 139–151. Neto, F. (1993). The satisfaction with life scale: Psychometrics properties in an adolescent sample. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 125–134. Olioff, M., Bryson, S. E., & Wadden, N. P. (1989). Predictive relation of automatic thoughts and student efficacy to depressive symptoms in undergraduates. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 353–363. Pavot, W., Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of the satisfaction with life scale: Evidence for the cross-method convergence of wellbeing measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 149–161. Riskind, J. H., Beck, A. T., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Taking the measure of anxiety and depression: Validity of the reconstructed Hamilton Scales. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 474–479. Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentimihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. The American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 151–158. Simonton, D. K., & Baumeister, R. F. (2005). Positive psychology at the summit. Review of General Psychology, 9, 99–102. Szentagotai, A., & Freeman, A. (2007). An analysis of the relationship between irrational beliefs and automatic thoughts in predicting distress. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 7, 1–9. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education. Whisman, M. A., Perez, J. E., & Ramel, W. (2000). Factor structure of the Beck Depression Inventory – Second Edition (BDI-II) in a student sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 545–551. Wong, S. S. (2008). The Relations of Cognitive Triad, Dysfunctional Attitudes, Automatic Thoughts, and Irrational Beliefs with Test Anxiety. Current Psychology, 27, 177–191. Zahn-Waxler, C., Crick, N. R., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Woods, K. E. (2006). The origins and development of psychopathology in females and males. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 76–138). Hoboken, New Jersey: Wiley.
© Copyright 2024