Journal of Research in Reading, ISSN 0141-0423 Volume 29, Issue 4, 2006, pp 400–417 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00315.x What is orthographic processing skill and how does it relate to word identification in reading? Jennifer S. Burt University of Queensland, Brisbane The role of orthographic processing skill (OPS) in reading has aroused the interest of many developmental researchers. Despite observations by Vellutino that current measures of OPS primarily are indicators of reading (and spelling) achievement, OPS commonly is distinguished from both reading achievement and phonological skills. An analysis of the reading literature indicates that there is no theory in which OPS meaningfully plays a role as an independent skill or causal factor in reading acquisition. Rather, OPS indexes fluent word identification and spelling knowledge, and there is no evidence to refute the hypothesis that its development relies heavily on phonological processes. Results of correlational studies and reader group comparisons (a) cannot inform about on-line processes and (b) may be parsimoniously explained in terms of phonological skills, reading experience, unmeasured language abilities and methodological factors, without implying that OPS is an aetiologically separable skill. Future research would profit from the investigation in experimental studies of the nature and development of orthographic representations. The individual differences approach to the investigation of reading acquisition has produced a substantial body of evidence that points to a causal role in reading and spelling acquisition for the phonological skills deployed in speaking and listening (Bradley & Bryant, 1985; Lundberg, Frost & Peterson, 1988; Perfetti, Beck, Bell & Hughes, 1987). Specifically, phonological sensitivity, the ability to discriminate and manipulate sound units of spoken words (cf. Stanovich, 1992), strongly predicts reading and spelling acquisition. These speech-based skills are thought to promote phonological coding of orthography (but see Castles & Coltheart, 2004), thus allowing readers to unlock the partially systematic way that English spelling patterns represent words that readers use in speech. Equipped with knowledge of the relevant abilities, researchers now are well placed to devise, test and apply theories about reading acquisition in training and other experimental studies. However, apart from a few exceptions (Cunningham, Perry, Stanovich & Share, 2002; Dixon, Stuart & Masterson, 2002; Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995; Share, 1999), the majority of developmental researchers continue to search for individual differences that might ‘explain’ differences in reading skills. One skill, termed orthographic processing, is the focus of the present paper. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006. Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 401 Interest here centres on the use of the term orthographic processing to describe a skill or facility that varies over individuals, rather than the act of processing orthography during reading. In this individual-difference sense it has been defined most broadly by Stanovich and West (1989) as ‘the ability to form, store, and access orthographic representations’ (p. 404). Wagner and Barker (1994) note that definitions vary according to whether they are expressed within the context of the dual route theory (Coltheart, 1978; Coltheart, Curtis, Atkins & Haller, 1993) or within alternative approaches (Perfetti, 1992; Vellutino, 1994). The purpose of the present paper is to examine the theoretical and empirical approaches relating OPS with reading, and to ask whether the current pursuit of individual differences in OPS is justifiable on scientific or practical grounds. The emphasis is on visual word recognition rather than syntactic analysis or comprehension of connected text. Several of the points made here are already in the literature. Notably, some researchers have argued that measures of OPS tap reading achievement (Vellutino, 1994; Vellutino, Scanlon & Tanzman, 1994), and that OPS exceeding expectations (based on individuals’ phonological skills) may reflect reading experience (Harm & Seidenberg, 1999; Snowling, Bryant & Hulme, 1996; Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo, 1997b). Of particular concern are inferences drawn by researchers apparently motivated by views (a) that OPS indexes a possibly heritable ability that is an independent causal factor in reading acquisition, or (b) that OPS is a component reading skill that is aetiologically distinct from phonological coding and print exposure. The position argued here is that there is no theoretical, practical or evidential justification for either of these claims. Theoretical issues Orthographic representations Identification of a visually presented word is thought to involve matching an input letter sequence with a representation of the word’s orthography that has been learned through experience with print. Recent evidence supports the common assumption that the orthographic representations acquired primarily during reading are used for both reading and spelling (Burt & Tate, 2002; Holmes & Carruthers, 1998). Phonological coding may contribute to word identification via a role in establishing orthographic representations, as suggested by Share’s self-teaching hypothesis (1995). For example, phonological coding may promote full decoding of a letter string, link it with experience with a word from speech and provide a structure to letter sequences that facilitates their learning. OPS as defined earlier, that is, the ability to form, store and access orthographic representations, can most directly be measured by assessing a child’s success in learning unfamiliar word-like letter strings. To date inferences about the status of OPS largely have been based on measures of OPS that tap a child’s current word-specific orthographic knowledge. This reliance on pre-existing knowledge as a measure is problematic because some individuals may have required many more learning trials than others to achieve similar scores on OPS. More critically, because OPS measures that rely on existing orthographic knowledge are heavily contaminated by reading achievement, it is unsurprising that they are good predictors of visual word identification. OPS as an independent causal factor or component skill Strong claims about an independent causal role of OPS in reading have been made in the recent developmental literature on reading. The basis of such claims is that OPS is an r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 402 BURT additional predictor of word reading over and above phonological skills (e.g. Juel, Griffith & Gough, 1986; Tunmer & Nesdale, 1985). For example, Wagner and Barker (1994) suggested that, across individuals with comparable print exposure and phonological coding, there are differences in the facility for forming, storing and manipulating orthographic images, and that these differences contribute to word identification. Bowers, Golden, Kennedy and Young (1994) noted that some problems in oral reading fluency may result from individual differences in a child’s ability to induce orthographic knowledge from the kind of exposure to print that usually is sufficient for it. An alternative justification for the predictive analyses involving OPS is that the analyses ascertain the relative importance of components of word identification. The dual route model’s distinction between sub-lexical (phonologically mediated) and lexical (orthographic) procedures for lexical access leads to the idea that individuals might differ in these component skills. OPS has been viewed as a distinct component process of word identification, perhaps indexing the effectiveness of the lexical procedure for lexical access (e.g. Manis, Szeszulski, Holt & Graves, 1990; Olson, Forsberg & Wise, 1994). The utility of this inference for understanding word identification is questionable because tests of orthographic knowledge do not provide a window into on-line processes in word identification. Visual word identification is thought to involve a cascading sequence of processes that cannot readily be separated empirically (e.g. Harm & Seidenberg, 2004), and the rapidity and efficiency of these processes cannot be tapped by ‘off-line’ tests of orthographic knowledge or learning. The limitations of current approaches to OPS are evident in the following illustration. Suppose that in a test analogous to the commonly used orthographic choice test, participants are asked to make speeded classifications about existing vs. fictitious athletic events. For example, given video clips of an athlete throwing a discus and a runner passing a discus to another runner, the correct choice is the first clip. An individual would be expected to respond correctly as a result of learning about the event, just as a child responds on the basis of prior learning in typical measures of OPS. In a test phase a number of additional athletic events might be depicted on video, with the participant being required to name as quickly as possible the athletic event. If scores in the event classification task explained a modest amount of unique variance in the event-naming task when relevant verbal abilities and sport-related experience were controlled, it is unlikely that a researcher would judge it useful to draw a conclusion that the ability to select actual from fictitious athletic events indexes a component process in athletic-event identification, or that event classification indexes a causal factor in event naming that is independent of factors already known to affect event-naming speed and accuracy. There is, of course, good reason to believe that there are abilities that contribute to orthographic learning, and that these abilities show a measurable variation over individuals (Reitsma, 1983). However, there are many already-identified skills and knowledge bases that predict reading and arguably, given enough learning opportunities, are sufficient for the acquisition of well-specified orthographic representations for word identification and spelling. These include an understanding of the alphabetic principle and familiarity with the letters of the alphabet, oral language skills (e.g. oral vocabulary, precise phonological representations) and phonological coding skill. The central argument of the present paper is that it is not necessary to reify unexplained variance by positing the existence of a separate ability to store orthography as an explanation for individual variation in word reading skill. A second argument of the r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 403 present review is that some analyses based on the component skills approach confuse processes occurring in word identification with the knowledge that supports word identification. Furthermore, the utility of OPS as a component skill is limited if it cannot be shown to be aetiologically distinct from other reading-related skills. As discussed below, the evidence does not indicate that there is a new, non-phonological skill that underlies OPS. Are there viable hypotheses about the distinct origins of orthographic processing skill? If OPS is a skill that is an independent causal factor in word identification, or an aetiologically separate component of word identification, then it must be empirically and conceptually distinguished from other predictors and from achievement in word identification. Measures of OPS that involve processing of words or orthographically legal letter strings cannot distinguish pre-reading skills from the consequences of reading. Because it is implausible to suppose that script-specific abilities have evolved (Diamond, 1998), any pre-reading orthographic processing ability must be constituted in some more generic skill or cognitive associated with language, visual processing or visual memory. There has been recent interest in rapid auditory processing (Tallal, Miller, Jenkins & Merzenich, 1997) and more general temporal processing (Farmer & Klein, 1995), cerebellar function (Nicolson, Fawcett & Dean, 1995) and rapid naming (Bowers & Wolf, 1993) as predictors of word identification skill. These plausibly are related to phonological skills but there is no plausible hypothesis linking them to OPS. Although Bowers & Wolf (1993) have suggested that rapid naming indexes the ‘automatic induction of good quality orthographic codes’ (p. 77), it is likely that the predictive utility of this measure reflects differences in children’s reading experience and their early familiarity with digits and letters. The only study reporting a substantial contribution of rapid naming when reading experience was controlled (Badian, 1993) used an insensitive index of reading experience (number of years of schooling). Some candidate abilities more relevant to the visual domain are considered briefly below. Skill in visual processing and visual memory Higher-order skills having to do with encoding and storing visually presented symbol sequences (e.g. Seymour & MacGregor, 1984) are likely candidates for abilities underlying OPS. However, available evidence overall supports the claim made by Vellutino (1979) that visual processing and visual memory do not play an important role in reading acquisition. There are a number of reports of a lack of association between visual search and memory tasks and OPS (Olson et al., 1994; Olson, Wise, Conners & Rack, 1990). In addition, poor readers do not show severe deficits on tasks requiring itemmatching or order judgements about visual symbols presented in sequences, and the deficits that have been found (e.g. Willows, Corcos & Kershner, 1993) have not been shown to be independent of the effects of reading experience and phonological skills. Generally, differences between good and poor readers on matching strings of visual symbols may be found when the symbols are letters or digits but not when they are rather unnameable symbols (e.g. Assink, Lam & Knuijt, 1998). r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 404 BURT Eye movements in reading In the substantial research literature on eye movements in the reading of connected text it is commonly reported that poor readers have less efficient eye-movement co-ordination than good readers (Biscaldi, Gezeck & Stuhr, 1998; see the review by Rayner, 1998). However, it is not clear whether these deficits affect single word reading, which has been the focus in the literature on OPS and reading. In addition, the current evidence is consistent with the common interpretation that in the vast majority of dyslexics these differences are consequences rather than causes of reading deficits. That is, inefficient eye-movement control, such as regressive eye movements and re-fixations, reflect decoding and comprehension difficulties (Olson, Kliegl, Davidson & Foltz, 1984; Rayner, 1998, 1999). Magnocellular system deficit A fundamental visual processing deficit that has received recent attention is the magnocellular system that predominates in peripheral vision and is thought to specialise in processing transient stimuli, thus detecting flicker or motion (Lovegrove, 1996). At present the evidence is inconsistent over studies and difficult to interpret (Hogben, 1996). If this processing deficit in perceptual tasks is to be tied to deficits in OPS, it is necessary to provide an empirically supported causal story that links a clearly identified magnocellular deficit with sequential processing of letters, and in turn, with OPS. One difficulty in making these links is that deficits in magnocellular function are more readily applied to reading connected text rather than individual word decoding, and thus may not apply to the single-word reading tests that are the focus of the literature on OPS (Lovegrove, 1993). The genetic basis of OPS Consistent with the above overview, there is no compelling evidence that OPS relies on a heritable skill that originates separately from phonological skills. Recent twin studies have reported significant heritability estimates for OPS (Gayán & Olson, 2001, 2003; Olson et al., 1994); but in these studies there also was a substantial bivariate heritability estimate for OPS and phonological skills. Also, selective deficits in OPS (surface dyslexia) have been found to have a smaller heritability component than deficits in phonological skills (Castles, Datta, Gayan & Olson, 1999). In any case, an independent genetic basis of OPS might reflect constitutional or temperamental factors that make a child more like to engage in reading rather than a specific ability in the visual learning domain. In summary, there currently is no clear theoretical rationale for claiming that OPS plays a causal role in reading acquisition independently of task-specific strategies and other method variance, phonological processing, more general language skills and vocabulary, motivational factors, reading experience and reading achievement. A number of authors have suggested that deficiencies in OPS result from a lack of detailed processing of word orthography during reading (e.g. Burt & Fury, 2000; Frith, 1985; Manis et al., 1990; Stanovich, 1992). Importantly, however, this reading style most parsimoniously may be linked with phonological coding (which promotes full processing of orthography) rather than with a constitutional cognitive factor (e.g. a global cognitive style) that is more general than reading. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 405 Measurement issues The observation already has been made that typical tests of OPS are alternative measures of reading and spelling achievement. This observation is substantiated here through a closer examination of the individual OPS tests most commonly used in the reading literature (Table 1). The most typical OPS measure is an orthographic choice test in which a visually presented word must be selected over a pseudohomophone distractor, as in boat vs. bote. Like other moderately common OPS tasks, such as homophone choice/verification, spelling recognition, lexical decision, masked orthographic priming in word recognition and word finding, this version of the orthographic choice task receptively assesses wordspecific orthographic knowledge. Occasionally productive assessments are made, for example, spelling to dictation. An alternative version of the orthographic choice test assesses abstract orthographic knowledge, that is, the ability to select orthographic patterns that are permissible in English (e.g. tilk) over letter sequences that are not found in English (e.g. tilv). It is likely that the tasks requiring correct memory for word-specific orthography are the most demanding in terms of their reliance on an individual’s reading skill and learning history (cf. Pennington, Lefly, Van Orden, Bookman & Smith, 1987). The remaining, less commonly used tests deal with recognition of letters and letter-like forms (Awaida & Beech, 1995; Badian, 1997), and recognition memory for letter clusters (Berninger, Yates & Lester, 1991). None of the above tests is independent of reading experience, print exposure and the concomitant familiarity with letters and permissible letter sequences of English. Among the visual processing tests, all those used by at least two laboratories involve letters, so all must require familiarity with letters that most obviously comes from reading experience or exposure to print. Examining the evidence Relationship between OPS, phonological coding and exposure to print Phonological coding appears to play a role in orthographic learning (see the review by Share, 1995), even for words with exceptional spelling–sound correspondences (Gough & Walsh, 1991; Manis, 1985). The magnitude of the association between phonological coding and OPS measures often is not reported, but available evidence suggests that the shared variance typically ranges from 10% to 25% in grade 1–4 children (Barker et al., 1992; Berninger et al., 1991; Braten et al., 1999; Cunningham et al., 2001, 2002). Similarly, OPS has been found to have a positive association with reading experience as assessed by print exposure measures (Stanovich & West, 1989; Stanovich et al., 1991). Nagy and Anderson (1984) have estimated that skilled readers in the middle grades of elementary school may read from 1,000,000 to as many as 50,000,000 words per year, whereas poor readers may read only 100,000. Phonological coding (measured by nonword pronunciation) also typically correlates with print exposure (Vellutino, 1994). In children, measures of print exposure have been found to account for significant variance in OPS measures after phonological skill and coding or phonological coding alone has been partialled out. The unique variance estimates range from 7% to 28% in a range of studies (Braten et al., 1999; Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990; Cunningham et al., 2001; McBride Chang et al., 1993; Stanovich et al., 1991). In skilled adult readers, r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 Braten et al. (1999); Hultquist (1997). walktreemother npmqsickqr yacht, eye Orthographic priming in lexical decision Embedded words Oral reading of irregular words Castles et al. (1999); Sprenger-Charolles, Siegel, Bechennec and Serniclaes (2003). Holmes and Standish (1996); Manis et al. (1990). Andrews and Scarratt (1996); Castles, Davis and Letcher (1999). Lexical decision Cunningham and Stanovich (1993); Foorman et al. (1996). Spelling production Manis et al. (1990, 1993); Stanovich and Siegel (1994). Gayán and Olson (2001). Streat? street? Manis et al. (1990, 1993). Barker et al. (1992); Cunningham et al. (2001); Cunningham and Stanovich (1990); Gayán and Olson (2001); Hultquist (1997); Patterson, Marshall and Coltheart (1985); Stanovich et al. (1991); Wagner and Barker (1994). Bailey, Manis, Pedersen and Seidenberg (2004); Barker, Torgesen and Wagner (1992); Braten, Lie, Andreassen and Olaussen (1999); Cunningham et al. (2001); Cunningham and Stanovich (1990); Foorman, Francis, Fletcher and Lynn (1996); Gayán and Olson (2001); Holmes (1996); Holmes and Standish (1996); Hultquist (1997); Jackson, Donaldson and Mills (1993); Manis, Seidenberg, Doi, McBride Chang and Petersen (1996); Manis et al. (1996, 2000); McBride Chang, Manis, Seidenberg, Custodio and Doi (1993); Newby, Recht and Caldwell (1993); Olson et al. (1984, 1990, 1994); Olson, Wise, Conners, Rack and Fulker (1989); Pennington, Gilger, Olson and DeFries (1992); Samuelsson, Gustafson and Ronnberg (1998); Sloboda (1980); Stanovich and Siegel (1994); Stanovich and West (1989); Stanovich, West and Cunningham (1991); Wagner and Barker (1994). Cunningham and Stanovich (1993); Cunningham, Perry and Stanovich (2001); Manis, Seidenberg and Doi (1999); Manis, Doi and Bhadha (2000); Massaro and Hestand (1983); Siegel, Share and Geva (1995); Stanovich and Siegel (1994); Stanovich et al. (1997a). Papers PIAT spelling test multi-choice spelling recognition Spelling recognition/orthographic verification Pair vs. pear Homophone choice A fruit: pear? pair? Bote vs. boat Orthographic choice word-specific; also termed spelling recognition. Homophone verification Tilk vs. tilv Example items where available Orthographic choice abstract Task Table 1. Commonly used tests of OPS. 406 BURT r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 407 Chateau and Jared (2000) found that print exposure differences were associated with the significant differences in the efficiency of orthographic processing in homophone choice, the LDT and form priming in the LDT. Consideration of the relationship between print exposure and OPS may shed light on the inconsistent findings reported when dyslexics and reading-level controls are compared on OPS. There are common reports of no difference between reading disabled and reading-level controls on OPS (Badian, 1997; Olson et al., 1984; Stanovich et al., 1997b), or lower OPS scores in disabled readers (Hultquist, 1997; Manis, Custodio & Szeszulski, 1993; Manis et al., 1990). Of more interest to researchers are occasional findings that disabled readers perform better on OPS than reading-level controls (Olson et al., 1990; Siegel et al., 1995), or better than predicted from their phonological skills (Foorman et al., 1996; Stanovich & Siegel, 1994). Reading-level controls typically are younger than their matched reading-disabled partners by at least several years. Consequently the disabled readers may have an advantage over the controls in OPS tests by virtue of their reading experience and skills that depend heavily on reading experience, as has been suggested previously (Foorman et al., 1996; Stanovich & Siegel, 1994). There is no compelling evidence for an alternative view (Holmes, 1996; Siegel et al., 1995) that those with deficits in phonological skills may be able to use OPS to compensate partially for deficits in phonological skills. This view about compensation implies that OPS is a facility that does not rely heavily on phonological skills. In summary, the finding that OPS is associated with phonological coding and print exposure is entirely predictable given the substantial literature documenting the association between these variables and visual word identification, and in turn, the dependence of word identification on orthographic knowledge. More fundamentally, phonological coding and reading experience can be expected to play a crucial role in the acquisition of new orthographic representations. Predictive studies: unique contributions of OPS to word identification Contemporary research in OPS generally has taken the form of assessing the unique contribution of OPS measures as predictors of visual word identification when other relevant predictors are controlled. Commonly controlled variables are phonological sensitivity and phonological coding, and less commonly, reading experience. It has been shown in hierarchical multiple regression analyses of data collected in a number of independent studies of children and adults that OPS accounts for unique variance in measures of word identification when phonological sensitivity (e.g. phoneme deletion) or phonological coding (e.g. non-word pronunciation) have been entered at a previous step in the analysis (Barker et al., 1992; Berninger et al., 1991; Conners & Olson, 1990; Cunningham & Stanovich, 1993; Cunningham et al., 2001; Holmes, 1996; Olson et al., 1994; Stanovich et al., 1991; Wagner & Barker, 1994). Although substantial variance in word identification might be explained when only phonological sensitivity is controlled (e.g. 30% in grade 1 children, Cunningham & Stanovich, 1993), the contribution of OPS to word identification is modest when the controlled variable includes phonological coding, which has a stronger and presumably more direct relationship with word identification. Estimates for children range from 7% to 10% (Barker et al., 1992; Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990; Olson et al., 1994; Stanovich et al., 1991). In the Stanovich et al. study the contribution for adults was 7%. It might be r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 408 BURT argued that the contribution of OPS is under-estimated because phonological coding tests measure orthographic processing and knowledge that is shared with OPS. However, in the absence of evidence for a specific orthographic processing skill this argument is circular. The overlap between phonological coding and OPS would merit investigation if there were an independent basis for proposing that OPS is a distinct skill. Adding print exposure to the set of predictors to be controlled in studies of the relationship between OPS and word identification has produced a decrement in the unique contribution of OPS. For example, Stanovich et al. (1991) found that the unique variance explained by OPS dropped 2% or 3% points to 4% (adults) and 6% (children) when a print exposure measure was controlled together with a composite of phonological coding measures. These decrements were not assessed for statistical significance but the information provided by the authors’ tables suggests that they were reliable. Olson et al. (1994) reported in their twin study that controlling the Title Recognition test in addition to phonological coding caused the unique contribution of OPS to word identification to fall by 2% points to 8%. In this study the age of twins ranged from 8 to over 18 years, and the authors commented on the rather low sensitivity of the Title Recognition test. In interpreting the unique contribution made by OPS in predictive studies, it is important to acknowledge the effect of method variance and task-specific factors, in addition to measurement issues. For example, appropriately difficult tests of wordspecific knowledge can be expected to better predict word identification than tests of abstract knowledge. Print exposure tests like the ART and the Title Recognition test lack sensitivity, because typically they tap primarily popular fiction. When the above procedural factors are considered together with the potential effects of unmeasured variables (e.g. oral vocabulary, motivation, instruction), it is a simple matter, without postulating that OPS is a distinct skill, to explain the modest prediction by OPS of visual word identification that has been reported in studies controlling phonological coding and print exposure. As noted, the present view is that OPS reflects orthographic learning that largely is shaped by phonological skills and reading experience. It must be acknowledged that commonality analyses would provide a more informative assessment of the viability of this hypothesis and the possible influences of underlying constructs. Such analyses would determine the overlapping variance between phonological skills and OPS, and between print exposure and OPS, in the prediction of word identification. Component skills analyses As indicated earlier, research largely motivated by dual route theories (Coltheart, 1978; Patterson & Morton, 1985) has treated phonological coding and OPS as distinct components of visual word identification. Although phonological coding may be interpreted as an on-line word identification process within the dual route model, OPS cannot be identified with on-line processes in visual word identification. Conventional tests of OPS and phonological coding do not provide information about the accrual of activation in lexical representations, or the interplay and time courses of processes occurring on-line in word identification. As noted, there are limits on the empirical separation of component on-line processes in word identification (e.g. Besner & Stolz, 1998). Thus component skills analyses are most accurately characterised as analyses of the skills that underlie or predict processes occurring on-line in word identification. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 409 Although it is consistent with available evidence to describe OPS as one of several component skills and knowledge bases that support word identification there is no compelling evidence that OPS is aetiologically separable from other component skills. Despite the lack of evidence, OPS frequently is assumed to be aetiologically separable from phonological coding (as illustrated below). Once this assumption is made, a component skills analysis effectively rests on the previously described strong claim that OPS is an independent causal factor in word identification skill. Typically researchers conducting component skills analyses have assessed the relative utility of OPS and phonological coding skills as predictors of word identification, or they have compared the skill pattern of normally achieving readers and dyslexics. A third approach, an attempt to identify reader subtypes based on relative strengths in component skills, is discussed in the next section. Manis et al. (1990) have delineated within the dual route framework the development of the lexical route, involving the acquisition and updating of orthographic entries, and development of the sub-lexical route, based on an ‘inductive process of rule creation’ (p. 213). In their analysis of a sample of dyslexic children, they note deficiencies in both phonological coding and OPS, coupled with reliance on a phonological decoding strategy for identifying words. They suggested that the children’s reliance on a weak phonological coding mechanism may have been a response to their deficiencies in OPS. This suggestion implies that the skills are aetiologically distinct, when a more parsimonious interpretation of the weakness of the two skills is that OPS relies on phonological coding. In an analysis of dyslexia Badian (1997) reported that the majority of the poorest readers in a sample of children aged 6–10 years had a double or triple deficit when assessed on phonological, naming-speed and orthographic skills. It was suggested that children with multiple deficits among these skills have fewer compensatory mechanisms to fall back on. The implication is that skills like OPS are separable factors in word identification, but no justification was given for this view. In light of reports of individual differences in OPS that are independent of reading experience, intellectual ability and phonological coding and awareness, Stanovich (1992) examined the small number of studies in which children had been trained on a set of unfamiliar words. He suggested that a lack of ability to form precise orthographic representations may be a significant factor in reading acquisition difficulties, but that isolating a specific problem in OPS requires a demonstration (not yet reported in training studies at that time) that a child has no deficit in phonological sensitivity or the required spelling-sound knowledge. A recent study does indicate that phonological skills predict the efficacy of learning orthographic representations (Cunningham et al., 2002). The authors reported, consistent with the self-teaching hypothesis of Share, that the ability of second-grade children to learn the orthography of pseudo-words (as assessed by a composite score over several measures) was predicted by their ability to pronounce the items in context (r 5 .52). In addition, they reported that orthographic choice accounted for significant variance in the orthographic learning composite when pronunciation accuracy for the pseudo-words was controlled. However, the conclusion that OPS was a unique predictor of orthographic learning was undercut by the results for their test of non-word pronunciation, which had a correlation with the orthographic learning composite (r 5 0.58) similar to that between orthographic choice and the orthographic learning composite (r 5 0.57). Pronunciation accuracy for the target items may have been unreliable because each child learned only 10 items and apparently only one pronunciation per item was taken as correct, although r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 410 BURT several of the 20 items in total had inconsistent word neighbours (e.g. trow–brow, crow; yait–plait, bait; kear–wear, near). Component skill analyses: subtypes and case study approaches Interest in OPS and phonological coding as component skills has led to attempts to subtype dyslexics and normally achieving readers on the discrepancy between orthographic and phonological skills. A review of the literature is beyond the scope of the present paper. It is worth noting, however, that there has been some debate about the validity of the posited typologies (Gough & Walsh, 1991; Sprenger-Charolles & Serniclaes, 2003; Stanovich et al., 1997b). Of particular relevance here are the postulated surface type in developmental dyslexia, and the sub-lexically reliant normal reader. In both categories individuals are characterised as having a specific or relative weakness in knowledge or use of whole-word orthography in reading (Olson et al., 1989). Equally, they may be described as having deficits in OPS. The diagnosis has been made in terms of a discrepancy between sub-lexical and lexical skill levels within an individual, or in terms of the relationship between lexical and sub-lexical skills in the target group compared with a reference group. Typically lexical skills are assessed via the naming of irregular words (which cannot be pronounced sub-lexically) and sub-lexical skills are assessed via the naming of non-words (which cannot be pronounced lexically). At issue is whether there is convincing evidence of a sub-lexically reliant reading typology that is based on either a deficit in OPS or a deficit in previously unidentified skills or knowledge. Sub-lexically reliant patterns of performance may be attributable to measurement error, impoverished reading experience, problems in reading instruction, motivation or other background variables. As indicated previously, lack of comparability of reader groups on reading experience has been flagged as an interpretational problem by a number of authors. The literature on reading typologies relating to the lexical and sub-lexical routes of the dual route model reveals some concerns about the status of the sub-lexically reliant type. For example, when reading age is taken into account, the surface dyslexic pattern that was moderately common in the Castles and Coltheart (1993) analysis based on chronological age is rare (Manis et al., 1996; Stanovich, Siegel & Gottardo, 1997a). In addition Manis, Seidenberg, Stallings, Joanisse, Bailey et al. (1999) reported that classification of the surface dyslexic profile was not stable over a year. There is some agreement that the surface developmental dyslexic profile is best characterised as a delay, in that it resembles normal reading at a younger age (Manis et al., 1996; Stanovich et al., 1997b). Stanovich et al. (1997b) noted that surface dyslexia may arise from a milder form of phonological deficit conjoined with ‘exceptionally inadequate reading experience’ (p. 124). Inadequate reading experience is likely to be reflected in more marked deficits for irregular than regular words because the orthography of irregular words is more difficult to learn than the orthography of regular words. Some support was provided by Gustafson (2001), who reported that in addition to having more general cognitive weaknesses, his surface dyslexic group had fewer books at home. The hypothesis about the dependence of surface dyslexia on impoverished reading experience was challenged in a recent study (Bailey et al., 2004) on the grounds that surface dyslexic children did not score lower on print exposure than phonological dyslexics. Although it is not clear what r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 411 group comparison was implied by Stanovich et al., it seems more appropriate to compare surface dyslexics with their age-matched peers. The Bailey et al. study found that 13 surface dyslexics tended to have lower print exposure percentage scores (29.5) than 13 chronological age controls (39.9) although the difference fell short of reliability. In the Manis et al. (1999) longitudinal study a delayed reading group showed a small and unreliable deficit in print exposure relative to the chronological age controls. However, a subgroup of nine delayed readers, classified as surface dyslexics based on their exception word reading, showed a marginally reliable (po.10) deficit in print exposure compared with the chronological-age controls. Admittedly, the above evidence for an effect of print exposure in surface dyslexia is not clear-cut, but it suggests that studies of larger samples may provide support for the hypothesis that surface dyslexia reflects deficient reading experience. At present there is no strong evidence that this group has a specific deficit in orthographic learning. A study by Castles and Holmes (1996) that found an impairment in the learning of irregular nonwords in surface dyslexia is problematical because the irregular non-words were devised on the basis of irregular words that may have been unfamiliar to some of the group (e.g. yacht, corps, cf. Bailey et al., 2004). A critical case: superior OPS with weak phonological skills If OPS depends substantially on phonological skills and reading instruction and experience then it will be difficult to find a developmental pattern of excellence in OPS with a deficiency in phonological coding or sensitivity. Furthermore, any individual with such an ability pattern would be expected to have a history of intensive reading instruction or reading experience, together with high motivation. Although studies of reader subtypes have found groups of children who have better than expected irregular word reading given their phonological skills, these lexically reliant groups rarely show OPS superior to mixed strategy readers. For example, the subgroup with stronger OPS and weaker phonological coding in the study of Samuelsson et al. (1998) showed weaker OPS than controls. In other studies the irregular word performance of the lexically reliant or sub-lexically impaired groups has been reported to be within the normal range, for example, within 1 standard deviation (SD) of the performance of normally achieving controls (Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Castles & Holmes, 1996; Manis et al., 1996). Against a background of a strong relationship between reading experience and OPS, Braten et al. (1999) found a group of five children who had phonological coding scores in the lowest quartile but OPS scores above the median, together with reading experience scores below the mean for their sample of 117 children. They inferred that these children may have a unique talent to ‘establish, store and/or access orthographic representations in memory’ (pp. 83–84). These results are intriguing but not compelling, given that available measures of print exposure are not sufficiently direct and sensitive to serve as robust indicators for individual children, and all five children scored close to or within 1 SD of the mean on the reading experience measure. Indeed, there appears to be no unambiguous case of a developmental weakness in phonological skills accompanied by clearly superior OPS. Holmes and Standish (1996) described a young woman (K.Q.) who excelled at orthographic processing despite deficient phonological processing and oral word reading. However, although K.Q. was highly proficient at a word-specific orthographic choice task and lexical decision, she was r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 412 BURT a poor speller relative to her undergraduate student peers, a problematic finding given that poor spelling is likely to be diagnostic of imprecise orthographic representations. Importantly, K.Q. had above average reading experience, liked reading, and indicated that she had spent a great deal of time rote learning the spellings of words. A similar observation about intensive reading practice was made in relation to a case study of an individual with poor phonological skills by Olson et al. (1994). In summary, OPS and phonological coding have been found to have at least a moderate positive association but also to contribute independently to the prediction of visual word identification. This finding is equally compatible with dual route and alternative, integrative approaches to understanding reading processes. An imperfect association does not provide evidence for an aetiological distinction between the two skills. Similarly, with respect to individuals or subtypes with an imbalance in sub-lexical and lexical skills, imbalances provide little information about the origin of the skills and their role in on-line reading. Certain clear-cut cases, for example, superior OPS with deficits in phonological skills, have the potential to provide compelling evidence of the independence of the two skills. However, such clear-cut cases have not been reported, and the evidence is more consistent with the proposal that OPS development depends substantially on already identified skills, especially the effective use of phonological coding in the learning of word orthography. It may be possible to make a case for the aetiological separation of OPS by showing that it is more heavily dependent on reading experience and individual background factors (e.g. motivation) than is phonological coding. However, a substantial contribution of reading experience to orthographic learning is already accepted, and few researchers would disagree that background variables may play a significant role in orthographic learning directly, as well as indirectly (through print exposure). Finally, the dual route framework that drives the component skills approach does not address developmental issues. For example, the approach does not address how the two procedures are acquired and it does not explain why there is a moderate or strong association between them. Summary A selective review of the reading literature indicates that measures of OPS tap reading and spelling achievement. There is no theoretical account of variation in reading skill that provides a viable causal hypothesis about a visual or memory skill (distinct from reading achievement and phonological skills) that might underlie OPS. Reading experience is known to be associated with OPS, and method variance plus differences in reading experience, reading achievement, oral vocabulary, motivation and instruction may explain why OPS scores do not always closely track the level of phonological coding skill in dyslexic and normal readers. The formation and fluent access of well-specified orthographic representations are viewed here as fundamental processes of word identification in reading, rather than independent predictors of word identification skill. Component skills analyses and classification into subtypes have not found clear-cut cases (e.g. strong OPS accompanied by inadequate phonological sensitivity) that would justify inferences that OPS is a distinct skill that is aetiologically independent of phonological coding. Predictive and subtyping approaches employing measures of existing knowledge are limited in that they cannot provide information about on-line processes in reading. In addition, component-skills approaches to reading skills that are not informed by theories about the development of word identification skills have limited application to reading remediation and instruction. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 413 Future research on OPS The current interest in OPS is well founded in the sense that word recognition and literacy research has been limited in its value for reading instruction and perhaps its progress in theory development by a neglect of the nature and acquisition of lexical representations (but see Ehri, 1986; Perfetti, 1992). Training studies that assess the skills and processes involved in the effective learning of unfamiliar letter strings assess directly the development of orthographic (and lexical) representations and as such they are ideal for devising and assessing remedial programmes (see the studies by Cunningham et al., 2002; Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995; Share, 1999). Additionally, the implications of OPS for spelling should not be overlooked. Acquiring the orthographic knowledge tapped by measures of OPS essentially is the same problem that writers face when they set about learning to spell. Also, recognition that OPS as assessed by reading researchers is a reading achievement measure highlights the theoretical and remedial utility of using OPS measures for tracking the development of orthographic representations (cf. Castles et al., 1999). Finally, it was suggested by Stanovich et al. (1991) and others that OPS deficits may result from a habitual shallow and non-analytic processing style in readers when encountering words (see also Frith, 1985; Barker et al., 1992). Consistent with this proposal, good adult spellers perform better than poor spellers in tests of orthographic processing accuracy in silent reading (Baron & Strawson, 1976; Burt & Fury, 2000). This non-analytic style may result from less effective use of phonological information during reading by poor spellers, a possibility that awaits empirical investigation. Conclusion The purpose of the present paper was to make a case that progress in reading theory and practice depends upon the experimental evaluation of causal models of reading development. Statistical partialling of variance in word identification scores is a tool for testing hypotheses about relevant individual differences rather than a justification for positing causal variables or distinct component skills to account for unexplained variance. Task analyses indicate that visual word identification, spelling and OPS as currently measured rely on knowledge of the orthography of words, and that this fact is sufficient to explain their prediction of word identification that is independent of phonological coding. Investigation of the conditions and skills governing the acquisition of new orthographic representations promises to increase researchers’ understanding of OPS and more generally, the development of visual word identification and spelling. References Andrews, S. & Scarratt, D.R. (1996). What comes after phonological awareness? Using lexical experts to investigate orthographic processes in reading. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48(3), 141–148. Assink, E., Lam, M. & Knuijt, P. (1998). Visual and phonological processes in poor readers’ word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(3), 471–487. Awaida, M. & Beech, J.R. (1995). Children’s lexical and sublexical development while learning to read. Journal of Experimental Education, 63(2), 97–113. Badian, N.A. (1993). Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing, 5(1), 87–100. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 414 BURT Badian, N.A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 69–87. Bailey, C.E., Manis, F.R., Pedersen, W.C. & Seidenberg, M.S. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: Evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 125–154. Barker, T.A., Torgesen, J.K. & Wagner, R.K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(4), 334–345. Baron, J. & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 386–393. Berninger, V.W., Yates, C. & Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing, 3(2), 115–149. Besner, D. & Stolz, J.A. (1998). Unintentional reading: Can phonological computation be controlled? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52(1), 35–42. Biscaldi, M., Gezeck, S. & Stuhr, V. (1998). Poor saccadic control correlates with dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 36(11), 1189–1202. Bowers, P.G., Golden, J., Kennedy, A. & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 1: Theoretical and developmental issues, Vol. 8. (pp. 173–218). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Bowers, P.G. & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 5(1), 69–85. Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press. Braten, I., Lie, A., Andreassen, R. & Olaussen, B.S. (1999). Leisure time reading and orthographic processes in word recognition among Norwegian third- and fourth-grade students. Reading and Writing, 11(1), 65–88. Burt, J.S. & Fury, M.B. (2000). Spelling in adults: The role of reading skills and experience. Reading and Writing, 13, 1–30. Burt, J.S. & Tate, H. (2002). Does a reading lexicon provide orthographic representations for spelling? Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 518–543. Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180. Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91(1), 77–111. Castles, A., Datta, H., Gayán, J. & Olson, R.K. (1999). Varieties of developmental reading disorder: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 73–94. Castles, A., Davis, C. & Letcher, T. (1999). Neighbourhood effects on masked form priming in developing readers. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 201–224. Castles, A. & Holmes, V.M. (1996). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia and lexical acquisition. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 130–135. Chateau, D. & Jared, D. (2000). Exposure to print and word recognition process. Memory and Cognition, 28(1), 143–153. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. New York: Academic Press. Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P. & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and paralleldistributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608. Conners, F. & Olson, R.K. (1990). Reading comprehension in dyslexic and normal readers: A component skills analysis. In D.A. Balota, G.B.F. d’Arcais & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading. (pp. 557–579). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence-Erlbaum Associates. Cunningham, A.E., Perry, K.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing. Reading and Writing, 14(5–6), 549–568. Cunningham, A.E., Perry, K.E., Stanovich, K.E. & Share, D.L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185–199. Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 733–740. Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1993). Children’s literacy environments and early word recognition subskills. Reading and Writing, 5(2), 193–204. Diamond, J. (1998). Guns, germs and steel: A short history of everybody for the last 13,000 years. London: Vintage. Dixon, M., Stuart, M. & Masterson, J. (2002). The relationship between phonological awareness and the development of orthographic representations. Reading and Writing, 15, 295–316. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 415 Ehri, L.C. (1986). Sources of difficulty in learning to spell and read. In M.L. Wolraich & D. Routh (Eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics, Vol. 77. (pp. 121–195). Greenwich, CT.: JAI Press. Ehri, L.C. & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing, 7, 295–326. Farmer, M.E. & Klein, R.M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 460–493. Foorman, B.R., Francis, D.J., Fletcher, J.M. & Lynn, A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: Comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level-match designs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(4), 639–652. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia. (pp. 301–330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gayán, J. & Olson, R.K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on orthographic and phonological skills in children with reading disabilities. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(2), 483–507. Gayán, J. & Olson, R.K. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(2), 97–123. Gough, P.B. & Walsh, M.A. (1991). Chinese, Phoenicians, and the orthographic cipher of English. In S.A. Brady & D.P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. (pp. 199– 209). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gustafson, S. (2001). Cognitive abilities and print exposure in surface and phonological types of reading disability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(4), 351–375. Harm, M.W. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106(3), 491–528. Harm, M.W. & Seidenberg, M.S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720. Hogben, J.H. (1996). A plea for purity. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48(3), 172–177. Holmes, V.M. (1996). Skilled reading and orthographic processing. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48(3), 149–154. Holmes, V.M. & Carruthers, J. (1998). The relation between reading and spelling in skilled adult readers. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 264–289. Holmes, V.M. & Standish, J.M. (1996). Skilled reading with impaired phonology: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13(8), 1207–1222. Hultquist, A.M. (1997). Orthographic processing abilities of adolescents with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 89–114. Jackson, N.E., Donaldson, G.W. & Mils, J.R. (1993). Components of reading skill in postkindergarten precocious readers and level-matched second graders. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 181–208. Juel, C., Griffith, P. & Gough, P. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255. Lovegrove, W. (1993). Do dyslexics have a visual deficit? In S.F. Wright & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation: Studies in visual information processing, Vol. 3. (pp. 33–49). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers. Lovegrove, W. (1996). Dyslexia and a transient/magnocellular pathway deficit: The current situation and future directions. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48(3), 167–171. Lundberg, I., Frost, J. & Peterson, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284. Manis, F.R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 78–90. Manis, F.R., Custodio, R. & Szeszulski, P.A. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(1), 64–86. Manis, F.R., Doi, L.M. & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 325–333. Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S. & Doi, L.M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(2), 129–157. Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., Doi, L.M., McBride Chang, C. & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia. Cognition, 58(2), 157–195. Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., Stallings, L., Joanisse, M., Bailey, C., Freedman, L. et al. (1999). Development of dyslexic subgroups: A one-year follow up. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 105–134. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 416 BURT Manis, F.R., Szeszulski, P.A., Holt, L.K. & Graves, K. (1990). Variation in component word recognition and spelling skills among dyslexic children and normal readers. In T. Carr & B.A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches. (pp. 207–259). New York: Academic Press. Massaro, D.W. & Hestand, J. (1983). Developmental relations between reading ability and knowledge of orthographic structure. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 174–180. McBride Chang, C., Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., Custodio, R.G. & Doi, L.M. (1993). Print exposure as a predictor of word reading and reading comprehension in disabled and nondisabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 230–238. Nagy, W.E. & Anderson, R.C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330. Newby, R.F., Recht, D.R. & Caldwell, J. (1993). Validation of a clinical method for the diagnosis of two subtypes of dyslexia. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 11(1), 72–83. Nicolson, R.I., Fawcett, A.J. & Dean, P. (1995). Time estimation deficits in developmental dyslexia: Evidence for cerebellar involvement. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 259, 43–47. Olson, R.K., Forsberg, H. & Wise, B. (1994). Genes, environment, and the development of orthographic skills. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge: Theoretical and developmental issues, Vol. 1. (pp. 27–71). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. Olson, R.K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B.J. & Foltz, G. (1984). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In G.E. MacKinnon & T.G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, Vol. 4. (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic Press. Olson, R.K., Wise, B., Conners, F. & Rack, J. (1990). Organisation, heritability, and remediation of component word recognition and language skills in disabled readers. In T.H. Carr & B.A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches. (pp. 261–322). New York: Academic Press. Olson, R.K., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J. & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–348. Patterson, K., Marshall, J. & Coltheart, M. (1985). Surface dyslexia. London: Erlbaum. Patterson, K. & Morton, J. (1985). From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pennington, B.F., Gilger, J.W., Olson, R.K. & DeFries, J.C. (1992). The external validity of age- versus IQdiscrepancy definitions of reading disability: Lessons from a twin study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 562–573. Pennington, B.F., Lefly, D.L., van Orden, G.C., Bookman, M.O. & Smith, S.D. (1987). Is phonology bypassed in normal or dyslexic development? Annals of Dyslexia, 37, 62–89. Perfetti, C.A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition. (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Perfetti, C.A., Beck, I., Bell, L. & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. Rayner, K. (1999). What have we learned about eye movements during reading? In R.M. Klein & P.A. McMullen (Eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia. Language, speech, and communication. (pp. 23–56). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 321–339. Samuelsson, S., Gustafson, S. & Ronnberg, J. (1998). Visual and auditory priming in Swedish poor readers: A double dissociation. Dyslexia, 4, 16–29. Seymour, P.H. & MacGregor, C.J. (1984). Developmental dyslexia: A cognitive experimental analysis of phonological, morphemic, and visual impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 43–82. Share, D.L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. Share, D.L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95–129. Siegel, L.S., Share, D. & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6(4), 250–254. Sloboda, J.A. (1980). Visual imagery and individual differences in spelling. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling. (pp. 231–248). London: Academic Press. r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006 ORTHOGRAPHIC PROCESSING AND READING 417 Snowling, M.J., Bryant, P.E. & Hulme, C. (1996). Theoretical and methodological pitfalls in making comparisons between developmental and acquired dyslexia: Some comments on A. Castles & M. Coltheart (1993). Reading and Writing, 8(5), 443–451. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Reliability of phonological and surface subtypes in developmental dyslexia: A review of five multiple cases studies. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition. Special Issue on Language Disorders and Reading Acquisition, 10, http://cpl.revues.org/ document248.html. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L.S., Bechennec, D. & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 194–217. Stanovich, K.E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition. (pp. 307–342). Hillsadle, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Stanovich, K.E. & Siegel, L.S. (1994). Phentotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53. Stanovich, K.E., Siegel, L.S. & Gottardo, A. (1997a). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 114–127. Stanovich, K.E., Siegel, L.S. & Gottardo, A. (1997b). Progress in the search for dyslexia subtypes. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention. (pp. 108–130). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd. Stanovich, K.E. & West, R.F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433. Stanovich, K.E., West, R.F. & Cunningham, A.E. (1991). Beyond phonological processes: Print exposure and orthographic processing. In S.A. Brady & D.P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. (pp. 219–235). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Tallal, P., Miller, S.L., Jenkins, W.M. & Merzenich, M.M. (1997). The role of temporal processing in developmental language-based learning disorders: Research and clinical implications. In B. Blachman et al. (Eds.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention. (pp. 49–66). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tunmer, W.E. & Nesdale, A.R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417–427. Vellutino, F.R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and Research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vellutino, F.R. (1994). The increasingly inextricable relationship between orthographic and phonological coding in learning to read: Some reservations about current methods of operationalizing orthographic coding. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 2: Relationships to phonology, reading, and writing. (pp. 47–111). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M. & Tanzman, M.S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In G.R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. (pp. 279–332). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company. Wagner, R.K. & Barker, T.A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 1: Theoretical and developmental issues, Vol. 8. (pp. 243–276). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Willows, D.M., Corcos, E. & Kershner, J.R. (1993). Perceptual and cognitive factors in disabled and normal readers’ perception and memory of unfamiliar visual symbols. In S.F. Wright & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation. Studies in visual information processing, Vol. 3. (pp. 163–177). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers. Received 17 October 2005; revised version received 3 May 2006. Address for correspondence: Jennifer S. Burt, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, QLD 4072, Australia. E-mail: [email protected] r United Kingdom Literacy Association 2006
© Copyright 2024