Childrearing Orientations in Mexican American Families: The Influence of Generation and Sociocultural Factors Author(s): Raymond Buriel Source: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 55, No. 4 (Nov., 1993), pp. 987-1000 Published by: National Council on Family Relations Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/352778 . Accessed: 25/10/2014 19:10 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Marriage and Family. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions RAYMONDBURIEL Pomona College ChildrearingOrientationsin Mexican American Families: The Influence of Generationand SocioculturalFactors This study compared the childrearing practices used by Mexican American parents of first-, second-, and third-generation adolescents. Effects of family sociocultural variables on childrearing were also examined. There were 317 adult respondents representing the parents of 186 adolescents. Multivariate and univariate analyses revealed significant generational differences in childrearing styles. Parents of first- and second-generation adolescents reported a more responsibility-oriented childrearing style, whereas parents of thirdgeneration adolescents reported a more concernoriented childrearing style. The relationship of family sociocultural variables to parents' childrearing practices also varied by generation. In general, the sociocultural variables of child's Spanish to parent, family income, and mothers' schooling showed the strongest relation to childrearing styles emphasizing either responsibility or concern. Results are discussed in relation to the effects that immigration and acculturation have on Mexican Americanfamilies. The generationalstatus of parentsand children contributesto socioculturalvariationsin Mexican American families that have implications for Psychology Department, Pomona College, Claremont, CA 91711. childrearing.Today,the Mexican-descentpopulation in the United States consists of persons in one of three generationalgroups:those born in Mexico, those born in the U.S. of Mexican-born parents,and those born in the U.S. of U.S.-born parents.There are importantsocial and cultural differences between these three groups (Buriel, Mercado,Rodriguez,& Chavez, 1991). Socially, Mexican-bornparents have less education and lower family incomes than their U.S.-borncounterparts.Culturally,Mexican-bornparentsspeak less English, which slows their acculturationby limitingcontactwith U.S. English-speakingsociety. An adequatestudy of culturalminorityfamilies musttakeinto accountboth social and cultural influences on human development (Laosa, 1989). A large body of literaturedocuments a consistentrelationshipbetweenchildrearingpractices and the socioeconomicvariablesof parents' education and family income (for reviews, see Hess, 1970;Laosa, 1981). A neglectedareaof research, however, concerns the relationship of childrearingpractices to culturalfactors (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Padilla& Lindholm,1983).The presentstudyexamines generationaldifferences in childrearing practices of Mexican Americanparentsand assesses the impactof socioculturalfactorsunderlying childrearingpracticesacrossgenerations. Journalof Marriageandthe Family55 (November1993):987-1000 This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 987 Journalof Marriageand the Family 988 The first generation being examined in this studyincludesthose personsbornin Mexico who later immigratedto the U.S. Some of these persons immigratedas single youngadultsor as marriedcouples,while otherswerebroughtto the U.S. as youngchildrenby theirparents.Familyincome is typicallylow in the firstgeneration,due to parents' lower educationand limited knowledge of English. First-generation children grow up in home environmentswhereSpanishis the primary languagebetweenparentsandchildren.The family's exposureto Englishoftencomes throughchilin the U.S. school system.The dren'sparticipation second generationrepresentsthe U.S.-bornchildren of immigrantparents.The family environchildrenare in many mentsof second-generation ways similar to those of their first-generation peers, owing to the foreign-bornstatus of their parents.Culturalschismsand synergismsare also mostapparentin the familiesof second-generation children.Thus, while Spanish is the native language of second-generationchildren,Englishbecomes theirdominantlanguageafterthe onset of schooling.However,Spanishcontinuesas the primary languageof their parents,which creates a strongenvironmentalpress for bilingualism.The thirdgenerationrefers collectively to persons of Mexicandescentwhose parentswere bornin the U.S. This includespersonsin the fourthand subsequent generations whose grandparents and were bornin this country.The great-grandparents thirdgenerationis distinguishedfromthe two previous generationsby the absence of any direct parentallinksto Mexico via immigration.Thus,in contrastto the first and second generationwhere socializationinvolveda Mexican-bornparent,persons in the thirdgenerationare socializedexclusively by parentswho were themselvesborn and raisedin the U.S. Personsin the thirdgeneration have also grown up in homes where all family membersare U.S. citizens, where English is the primary language (Lopez, 1982), and where parentalschoolinghas takenplace exclusively in U.S. schools. On average,years of schoolingand familyincomesarehighestin the thirdgeneration. ResearchindicatesthatimmigrationfromMexico is selective.Priorto immigrating,adultMexicans receivemoreyearsof schoolingthanthe nationalaveragefor Mexico (Portes,1979;Portes& Bach, 1985), and exhibit personalityand motivationalcharacteristics conduciveto achievementin U.S. society (Dinerman,1982; Fromm& Maccoby, 1970). Since immigrationis motivatedby a desire for change and upwardmobility, it is not surprisingthatMexicanimmigrantsexhibit lower ratesof delinquency(Buriel,Calzada,& Vasquez, 1982), less psychological distress (Burnam, Hough, Karno, Escobar, & Telles, 1987), and more maritalsatisfactionthan their later generation peers (Casas& Ortiz, 1985; de Anda, 1984). And despite a high drop-outrate due to limited English proficiency, while in school, immigrant adolescentsobtainhigherachievementtest scores (Nielsen & Fernandez,1981) and better grades (Landsman,Padilla,Leiderman,Clark,Ritter,& Dornbusch,1991)thanU.S.-bornMexicanAmericans.It is apparentthat,acrosssuccessivegenerations, MexicanAmericanfamilies undergosocial and culturalchangesthat do not always conform to a linearmodel of acculturation. Thatis, change is not alwaysin the directionof healthierpsychological adjustment.What is less obvious is how developmentalprocesseswithin Mexican American families change across generations.Recent discussionsof ethnicminorityfamilies (Harrison, Wilson, Pine, Chan, & Buriel, 1990; Lin & Fu, 1990) hypothesizethat parentsadapttheir socializationpracticesin responseto socioculturalvariations among family members. In immigrant groups,acculturativepressuresrepresenta major sourceof socioculturalchangeanddiversitywithin the family. Studies suggest that social and culturalvariables each impactdifferentlyon MexicanAmerican childrearingpractices.Laosa (1980a, 1980b) showed that maternalteachingstyles were affected by years of schooling, a social class variable. Motherswith less educationwere more likely to use modeling as a teaching style, whereasmore educated mothers used inquiry and praise. According to Laosa (1982), this shift in teaching styles occursbecausemorehighlyschooledmothers increasinglytake on the teachingstyle of the school, which emphasizes inquiry and praise. Chavez and Buriel (1986), on the other hand, found a relationshipbetween maternalreinforcement style and nativity,a variableoften used as a culturalmarker.The reinforcementsof foreignborn motherswere more contingenton the outcomes of children'sperformance(success or failure), whereasU.S.-bornmothersreinforcedmore on the basis of children's effort (high or low). These results were interpretedwithin a two-step culturalintegrationmodel (Buriel, 1984), which positsthat:(a) the highlyself-selectedcharacterof immigrantsresultsin the migrationof very taskorientedpersons,and, (b) prolongedexposureto lower societal expectationsfor minoritiesdimin- This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Childrearing in Mexican American Families ishes the probabilityfor success and encourages parentalcompensationin the form of reinforcing to success(i.e., effort). approximations Althoughthe aforementionedstudiesby Laosa (1980a, 1980b, 1982) and Chavez and Buriel (1986) are informative,thereare threelimitations in theirresearchconcerningthe uniqueinfluences of social and culturalvariableson childrearing. First, the inevitablecovariationbetween generation andeducationin these studies,resultingfrom smallsamplesizes, makesit difficultto unequivocally isolatethe separateeffects of these two variables on childrearing.Second,only two very specific categories of behavior were examined, teachingstyle and reinforcementstyle, thus leaving the relationshipof socioculturalvariablesto otherchildrearingdimensionsunexamined.Finally, the role of fathersin the child's development was not considered,and, along with it, the possible impactof fatherabsence.Only two published studies have examinedthe childrearingattitudes of MexicanAmericanfathers(Durrett,O'Bryant, & Pennebaker,1975; LeVine & Bartz, 1979), but neitherconsideredwithin-groupdifferencesrelated to socioculturalfactorsandgenerationalstatus. In additionto the aforementionedlimitations, studies have not exploredhow the culturalvariables of Spanish/Englishusage between parent and child and length of U.S. residence affect MexicanAmericanchildrearing.Especiallyin immigrantfamilies, parentsare often introducedto Englishand its attendantacculturativeinfluences throughchildren'sparticipationin school. Many are the Latino parents who complain that their children'sdiminishinguse of Spanishaffectstheir communicationwith them.On the otherhand,the longerimmigrantparentsare in this country,the more likely it is that they develop a workingunderstandingand use of English that facilitates communicationwith theirmore English-speaking children.Accordingto some social identitytheorists (Kim, 1988), minority languages serve as reservoirsof culturalvalues. Thus, to the extent thatimmigrantsocializationvalues are enmeshed in the semanticsof the native language,changes in languageusage may correlatewith changesin childrearingstyles. The presentstudywas designedto comparethe childrearingpractices used by parents of first-, MexicanAmerican second-, and third-generation adolescents.In addition,the contributionof socioculturalfactorsto childrearing,and theirpossible interactionwith generation,were examined.The social variablesincludedparents'educationand 989 family income. The cultural variables included Spanish/Englishusage in parent-childcommunication and length of U.S. residence. The Parent Attitude Research Scale or PARS (Cromwell, 1969; LeVine & Bartz, 1979) was used to assess the childrearingpracticesof mothersand fathers along seven dimensions: autonomy, productive use of time, equality, strictness,permissiveness, support,andcontrol.The seven PARSdimensions covermanyof the childrearingvariablesexamined in previousresearchcomparingMexican American and Euro-Americansamples. For instance, Durrettet al. (1975) found that,relativeto EuroAmericanparents,MexicanAmericansplace less emphasison their children'searly assumptionof responsibility.LeVineandBartz(1979) foundthat MexicanAmericanparentsstressedless egalitarianism while favoring more permissiveness. Kearns(1970) foundthatMexicanAmericanparents were stricterand more controllingwith their children.These between-groupdifferences were usuallyexplainedas the resultof traditionalMexican values, as if MexicanAmericanswere a culturallyhomogeneouspopulation.However,by focusing on between-group comparisons, researchers have ignored the tremendous within-groupdiversityin the Mexican American populationthatarisesfromgenerationalandsocioculturalfactorsthataffectchildrearing practices. Studies of Mexican American childrearing have focused exclusively on preadolescentchildren (Harrison et al., 1990; Ramirez & Arce, 1981). The presentstudy is the first to examine childrearingin a sample of Mexican American parentswith adolescentchildren.Researchwith MexicanAmericanparentsand adolescentsis importantbecauseit is duringadolescencethatchildrenof this groupare at greatestriskfor dropping out of school (Hirano-Nakanishi,1986), for engaging in delinquent behavior (Buriel et al., 1982), and for expressing confusion about their ethnicidentity(Vigil & Long, 1981). The present study thereforenot only extends childrearingresearchwith MexicanAmericansinto adolescence, but, in addition,establishesa context for understandingthe relationof childrearingvariablesto criticalareasof adolescentdevelopment. METHOD Subjects Therewere 317 MexicanAmericanadultrespondentsin the studyrepresentingthe parentsof 186 This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 990 Journal of Marriage and the Family seventh-gradestudents(96 boys and90 girls).The sample included 131 two-parentfamilies and 55 single-parentfamilies. Divorce or separationaccountedfor the single-parentfamilies,which,in all cases, were headedby women. The adultsample was stratifiedon the basis of theirchildren'sgenerational status. First-generation students (33 males and 33 females) were born in Mexico and also hadbothparentsbornin Mexico (53 two-parent families and 13 single-parentfamilies). Secstudents(34 malesand36 females) ond-generation werebornin the U.S. but had one or both parents born in Mexico (51 two-parentfamilies and 19 students single-parentfamilies). Third-generation (29 males and 21 females)were born in the U.S. andalso hadbothparentsbornin the U.S (27 twoparentfamiliesand23 single-parentfamilies). The presentstudyis partof a largerinvestigation of Mexican-descentseventhgradersandtheir parentsliving in three communities of the Los Angeles metropolitanarea. The communitiesincludedone in south-centralLos Angeles and two in the easternsuburbsof the city. The south-central community is a predominantly immigrant areathathas becomeMexicanizedwithinthe past 20 years. The socioeconomic make-up of the community is overwhelmingly working class, with many parentsworkingin the service industry. The two communitiesin the easternsuburbs of the city both have long-standingMexican-descent populationsdating to the 1930s. In recent years,bothcommunitieshave experienceda large influxof Mexicanimmigrants.The socioeconomic make-upof both areasincludes workingclass and lower middle class. Parents' occupations rangefromagriculturalworkersto managerialpositions. The three communities are typical of those inhabitedby Mexican Americansin southern California,the region with the largestMexican-descentpopulationin the U.S. In the course of answering questions about lengthof U.S. residence,many immigrantfathers notedthatthey initiallycame to the U.S. alone,in order to find work and housing, before sending for theirwives and childrenwho had remainedin Mexico. This demographicappearsin Table 1, which shows that, among immigrantparents,fathershave more averageyears of U.S. residence thanmothers.At the time of the interview,all fathers in the sample reported being employed. Abouthalf of the mothersin all three communities reportedemploymentoutside the home, althoughmany workedonly part-time.In response to an ethnicidentityquestion,all parentsandtheir seventh-grade children showed preference for ethnic labels expressing affiliation with their Mexicanancestry(Buriel& Cardoza,1993). Parental Measures Childrearing practices. The childrearing dimen- sions examinedin this study were identified by LeVineandBartz(1979) in theirstudyof low-income MexicanAmerican,AfricanAmerican,and Euro-Americanparents.LeVine and Bartzidentified seven childrearingdimensionsrepresentinga combination of items from the Parent Attitude ResearchInstrument(Schaefer& Bell, 1958) and the CornellParentBehaviorDescriptionquestionnaire (Devereaux, Bronfenbrenner,& Rodgers, 1969). The seven childrearingdimensionsincluded: (a) parentalpress for child's autonomy(i.e., expecting children to do things for themselves sooner), (b) parentalpress for child's productive use of time (i.e., not wasting time), (c) parental press for equalitybetween parentand child (i.e., encouragingdialoguebetween parentand child), (d) parentalstrictnesstowardchild (i.e., expecting obedienceto parentalrules), (e) parentalpermissiveness with child (i.e., deemphasizingthe need for harshdiscipline),(f) parentalsupportof child (i.e., offeringcomfortand solace to child),and (g) parentalcontrol of child (i.e., stressinghigh expectations for child's behavior at home and school). Twenty-fiveitems were used to measure the seven childrearingdimensions. The support and control items were measuredusing a fivepointLikertformat(neverto very often) while the five remainingdimensionswere measuredusing a four-pointLikertformat(stronglyagreeto strongly disagree). Parentswere instructedto answer each item as it pertainedto theirrelationshipwith their seventh-gradechild. Items on the childrearing scale were scoredin the directionof more of the parentalbehaviorunderconsideration. Socioeconomic variables. Parents' education and annualfamily income were used as measuresof socioeconomic status. The effects of these two variableswere examinedseparatelybecause previous researchsuggests they are differentiallyrelated to children's cognitive abilities (Laosa, 1982). Specifically,parents'educationis a better predictorof children'sliteracyskills than family income. Previous researchby Ortiz (1986) also indicates that parents'educationimpacts differently on achievementfor differentgenerationsof MexicanAmericans.She foundthatthe effects of This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Childrearing in Mexican American Families parents' education on students' educational attainmentwas greaterfor first- and third-generation MexicanAmericansand significantlyweaker for members of the second generation. In the presentstudy, the influence of mothers' and fathers' education was examined separately.The highest grade of school or year of college completedwas used to measureparents'education. Family income was measuredon a nine-step interval scale with income ranges of (1) under $7,500 to (9) $45,001 or more. Respondentsselected an income step (e.g., $15,001 to $20,000) andthe rankordervalue of thatstep (1-9) was assignedas the family income score for purposesof analysis.Whentherewere discrepanciesin the reported family income between parents in the same family, the father'sestimatewas used. Preliminaryfieldworkshowed the father's estimate to be a more accurateindicatorof family income thanaveragingmothers'and fathers'estimatesin the case of discrepancies. 991 homes of the 200 students, explaining to their parentsthatthey would be called to participatein a telephone interview. The letter explained that the school districthad approvedthe interviewbut that participationwas strictlyvoluntary.In addition, parentswere told that their responsewould be kept confidentialand that they would be paid $5 each for their participationin the interview. The parentletterwas writtenin both English and Spanish.Home telephonenumberswere obtained from the studentquestionnaireand from school records.Over 90% of the seventh-gradestudents had home phones. The validity of the telephone surveymethodwith Latinos,especiallyfor sensitive topics, has been empirically demonstrated (Marin& VanOssMarin,1989). Bilingual research assistants telephoned households and asked to speak to the students' mothersor fathers.Wheneverpossible, an effort was made to interview both parents during the same call. If one or both parentswere unavailable, the researchassistantasked for convenient Cultural variables. Parent-child Spanish/English times to reschedulethe interview.A minimumof threecall-backswere attemptedto locate and inusage and length of parents'U.S. residencewere used as culturalvariables.Two questionsassessed terview a missing parent.The responserate was parent-child Spanish/English usage. The first very high (93%),with only 16 householdsnot reaskedparentsthe ratioof Spanish-to-English they sponding,due primarilyto schedulingconflicts. used in speakingto theirseventh-gradechild. The Parentswere told that the purpose of the study second asked parentsthe ratio of Spanish-to-En- was to determine how parents influence the school achievementof theirchildren,specifically glish theirchild used when speakingto them.Retheirseventh-gradeson or daughter.Afterthe parsponses to both questionswere scored on a fivepointscale thatincludedthe followingcategories: ents had agreedto participate,the researchassisonly English, mostly English and some Spanish, tant told them that, at any time duringthe interEnglishand Spanishaboutequally,mostly Spanview, they could stop to ask questionsor ask for ish and some English, only Spanish. Higher clarificationof questions.The interviewthenproscores are in the direction of greater Spanish ceeded in either English or Spanish, depending usage. Parents' length of U.S. residence were upon the parent's preference. Interviews lasted codedin actualyearslived in this country. between 20 to 45 minutes, with Spanish interviews generallyrunninglonger. After the interview, parentswere thankedfor theirparticipation Procedure and asked for an address where their payment The adult respondentswere the parents of stucould be mailed.A substantialnumberof foreigndentsattendingone of threejuniorhigh schoolsin born parents declined payment but did request the greater Los Angeles area. Latino children copies of the resultof the study. were in the majorityat all threeschools. Student The parentinterviewquestionnairewas initialquestionnaireswere administeredduringrequired ly writtenin Englishand pilot testedwith a small Englishand Historyclasses. Froman initial pool pool of parentswith adolescentchildren.Parents of 500 Mexican American students, a stratified in the pilot-testingsample(3 malesand3 females) wereaskedto commenton the interviewquestions (generationand gender) sample of 200 was selectedandtheirparentswere contactedfor partic- and format. After making final adjustmentsin wording, the complete questionnairewas transipationin this study.Students'ethnicityand generationwere determinedby a combinationof ethlated to Spanish by a Mexican psychologist. nic identity and nativity questions answeredby Changesin wordingwerethenmadeby a Chicano bothstudentsandparents.A letterwas sent to the psychologist to fit the vernacularof the target This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Journal of Marriage and the Family 992 communities.Aftertheseadjustments,the Spanish questionnairewas translatedback into Englishto check for conceptual discrepancies that might have occurredduringtranslation.There were no apparentdiscrepanciesbetween the English and Spanishversionsof the parentquestionnaire. RESULTS Data analysisproceededin three steps. First,differences in childrearingwere examinedbetween parents of first-, second-, and third-generation students. Second, childrearingdata were factor analyzedto yield a minimalset of criterionvariables.Third,the relationshipof socioculturalvariables to childrearingwas examined. Table 1 presentsthe meansand standarddeviationsfor the socioculturalvariablesexaminedin this study.Althoughstatisticalanalyses were not carriedout on these data, it is apparentthat parents of first- and second-generationchildrendiffer from parentsof third-generationchildren in termsof all of the social andculturalvariables. TABLE1. MOTHERS'ANDFATHERS'MEAN ONEACH VARIABLEBY STUDENTS'GENERATION SOCIOCULTURAL Students'Generation SocioculturalVariable First Cultural/mother Mother'slanguage to childa 4.87 (.38) Child's language to mothera 4.55 (.73) Mother'syears in U.S. 9.63 (3.72) Socioeconomic/mother Mother's schoolingb 5.23 (4.01) 2.96 Family income (1.81) Cultural/father Father'slanguage to childa 4.47 (.78) Child's language to fathera 4.45 (.64) Father'syears in U.S. 12.30 (5.48) Socioeconomic/father 5.27 Father'sschoolingb (3.83) 2.96 Family income (1.81) Second Third 4.54 (.90) 4.35 (1.03) 16.68 (5.08) 1.64 (.83) 1.41 (.74) 6.48 (4.78) 3.65 (1.99) 11.79 (1.81) 5.05 (2.73) 4.44 (.71) 4.37 (.85) 18.24 (8.81) 1.37 (.82) 1.17 (.46) 6.65 (3.70) 3.65 (1.99) 12.51 (1.97) 5.05 (2.73) - - - Note: Numbersin parenthesesare standarddeviations. aHigherscores indicate greaterSpanish usage. bActualnumberof years of schooling completed. Parental Childrearing Practices The seven childrearingpracticeswere examined separatelyfor mothers and fathers using 3 x 2 (student generationx student sex) MANOVAs and ANOVAs. The multivariateand univariate resultsof these analysesarepresentedin Tables2 and 3 for mothersand fathers,respectively.The multivariateeffect of students' generation was significantfor bothmothersandfathers,while the multivariateeffect of students' sex was significant only for fathers.The multivariateinteraction effect of generationand sex was not significant for eitherparent,despite two significantunivariate interactionsfor fathers. UnivariateANOVAs yielded studentgeneration differences in six of the seven childrearing practices for mothers (see Table 2). For all six significant analyses, post hoc tests showed that mothersof third-generation studentsdifferedsignificantlyfrommothersof both first- and secondgenerationstudentswho, in turn, did not differ significantlyfrom each other.These findings are summarizedas follows: Mothersof first-and second-generationstudentsexpect earlier autonomy and more productiveuse of time, and are stricter andmorepermissive;mothersof third-generation studentsaremoresupportiveandcontrolling. UnivariateANOVAs for fathers'data yielded studentgenerationdifferencesin fourof the seven childrearingpractices(see Table3). In threeof the significant analyses, post hoc tests revealed the samepatternof differencesas the resultsfor mothers. Thus, fathersof first- and second-generation studentsdo not differfromeach otherand expect earlierautonomyandmoreproductiveuse of time, andaremorepermissivethanfathersof third-generationstudents.The only deviationfromthis general patternof findingswas for control.Fathersof studentsare similarin first- and third-generation theirdegreeof control,andbotharemore controlstudents. ling thanfathersof second-generation Following up on the significant multivariate effect for students'sex in the datafor fathers,univariatetests showed that fathersexpectedearlier autonomyfrom girls and were stricterwith girls. Althoughthe multivariateinteractioneffect was not significant,thereweretwo significantunivariate interactionsinvolvingautonomyand strictness that signal caution in interpretingthe sex difference in these variables.Examinationof the means in Table 3 suggest that these sex differencesare localizedin the datafor third-generation students, particularlyin the case of autonomy. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions TABLE 2. MEANS AND MANOVA RESULTS FOR MOTHERS' CHILDREARING VARIABLES: STUDENT GENERATION StudentGenerationby Sex Generation2 Generation1 Generation3 Ge ChildrearingaVariable Total Male Female Total Male Female Total Male Female Autonomy 3.28 (.46) 3.32 (.40) 3.23 (.54) 3.41 (.48) 3.38 (.41) 3.43 (.54) 2.81 (.69) 2.75 (.74) 2.92 (.59) Productiveuse of time 3.46 (.60) 3.51 (.62) 3.39 (.58) 3.59 (.55) 3.61 (.55) 3.57 (.55) 3.09 (.79) 2.96 (.78) 3.30 (.80) Equality 3.27 (.35) 3.21 (.35) 3.35 (.35) 3.40 (.33) 3.44 (.30) 3.37 (.36) 3.43 (.39) 3.42 (.38) 3.45 (.42) Strictness 3.40 (.52) 3.41 (.51) 3.39 (.54) 3.38 (.61) 3.25 (.61) 3.50 (.60) 3.01 (.62) 2.98 (.69) 3.05 (.51) Permissiveness 3.17 (.51) 3.24 (.38) 3.04 (.63) 3.12 (.56) 3.12 (.46) 3.11 (.64) 2.50 (.56) 2.50 (.55) 2.50 (.60) Support 4.11 (.55) 4.07 (.58) 4.17 (.53) 4.04 (.59) 3.94 (.49) 4.13 (.66) 4.32 (.54) 4.20 (.58) 4.51 (.41) Control 3.85 (.72) 3.86 (.66) 3.84 (.82) 3.86 (.58) 3.85 (.49) 3.87 (.66) 4.22 (.62) 4.17 (.64) 4.31 (.60) Note: Numbersin parenthesesare standarddeviations. aAll childrearingvariablesare scored on a 5-point scale, except supportand control which were scored on a 4-point scale. bMultivariateanalysis (Pillai's trace) for generation:F(14,322) = 5.87*** cMultivariateanalysis (Pillai's trace) for sex: F(7,160) = 1.05. dMultivariateanalysis (Pillai's trace) for G x S: F(14,322) = .96. *p<.05. **p<.01. ***p< .001. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions TABLE 3. MEANS AND MANOVA RESULTS FOR FATHERS' CHILDREARING VARIABLES: STUDENT GENERATION BY StudentGenerationby Sex Generation1 Generation2 Generation3 Gene ChildrearingaVariable Total Male Female Total Male Female Total Male Female Autonomy 3.38 (.33) 3.39 (.31) 3.38 (.36) 3.30 (.39) 3.27 (.43) 3.33 (.35) 2.87 (.54) 2.65 (.47) 3.28 (.44) Productiveuse of time 3.51 (.61) 3.55 (.57) 3.45 (.68) 3.54 (.61) 3.60 (.64) 3.48 (.58) 3.00 (.84) 3.00 (.86) 3.00 (.86) Equality 3.35 (.37) 3.31 (.39) 3.42 (.34) 3.38 (.37) 3.46 (.34) 3.31 (.39) 3.43 (.36) 3.37 (.40) 3.55 (.26) Strictness 3.40 (.58) 3.39 (.57) 3.42 (.61) 3.32 (.59) 3.24 (.61) 3.40 (.57) 2.94 (.69) 2.64 (.52) 3.50 (.66) Permissiveness 3.06 (.48) 3.13 (.42) 2.95 (.56) 3.09 (.60) 3.16 (.57) 3.02 (.63) 2.55 (.53) 2.55 (.34) 2.55 (.80) Support 4.25 (.55) 4.14 (.59) 4.41 (.46) 4.21 (.55) 4.21 (.50) 4.22 (.60) 4.48 (.40) 4.45 (.41) 4.52 (.40) Control 3.91 (.58) 4.06 (.62) 3.71 (.46) 3.61 (.69) 3.72 (.63) 3.50 (.75) 4.11 (.70) 4.14 (.72) 4.05 (.68) Note: Numbersin parenthesesare standarddeviations. aAll childrearingvariablesare scoredon a 5-point scale, except supportand control which are scored on a 4-point scale. bMultivariateanalysis (Pillai's trace)for generation:F(14,228) = 3.60***. analysis (Pillai's trace) for sex: F(7,113) = 4.55*** CMultivariate dMultivariateanalysis (Pillai's trace)for G x S: F(14,228) = 1.57. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions F Childrearing in Mexican American Families Factor Analysis of Parental Childrearing A separatefactoranalysis was performedon the childrearingdata for mothersand fathersusing a principalcomponentsanalysiswith varimaxrotation.This procedurereducedthe childrearingdata to the smallest numberof uncorrelatedfactors. Because the childrearingfactorswere to be used as dependentvariablesin subsequentmultipleregressionanalyses,the varimaxprocedurereduced the numberof necessaryanalyses.In addition,it avoided overlap in predictionbecause the same socioculturalvariableswere used as predictorsin all the regressionanalyses.This made the contribution of the socioculturalvariablesunique for each childrearing factor of each parent. Two childrearingfactors emerged from mothers' and fathers'data. The eigenvaluesassociatedwith the two factors emerging from the mothers' data explain 57% of the variancein the matrix.The first factor,whichexplains33%of the commonvariance, had its highest loadings with autonomy,productive use of time, strictness,and permissiveness. Becausethe childrearingpracticesincludedin this factor seem to describe a maternalsocialization style stressingassumptionof responsibility,this factoris referredto as Responsibility. The second factor, which accountedfor 24% of the common variance, loaded with support, control,andequality.These childrearingpractices seem to describematernalconcernfor the child, andthis factoris thereforereferredto as Concern. The eigenvalues associatedwith the two factorsemergingfrom the fathers'dataexplain52% of the variance in the matrix. The first factor, which explains 33.4% of the common variance, hadits highestloadingswith autonomy,strictness, permissiveness,productiveuse of time,andequality. With the exceptionof equality,this factoris identical to the first maternal factor. Because equalityhad the lowest loadingon this factor,and its inclusiondoes not appearto alterthe composite meaningof the othervariables,this first paternal factorwas also namedResponsibility. The secondfactor,which accountedfor 18.6% of the commonvariance,loadedwith supportand control. Again, with the exception of equality, this factor was identical to the second maternal factor.Therefore,this second paternalfactorwas also calledConcern. The reliabilityof the childrearingfactorswas assessedwith Cronbach'salpha.For Responsibility, the alpha coefficients were .74 and .67 for 995 mothersand fathers,respectively. For Concern, the alphacoefficientswere .59 and .33 for mothers and fathers, respectively. The alphas for parentalResponsibilitywere moderatelyhigh. On the other hand, the alphas for parentalConcern were lower, due perhapsto the fewer numberof measuresmakingup this factor for both parents. Cautionis thereforerecommendedin interpreting those portionsof the findingsinvolving Concern, especiallyfor fathers'data. Relation of Sociocultural Variables to Childrearing Four stepwise multipleregressionswere used to examinethe relationshipof the socioculturalvariables to the childrearingfactorsof Responsibility and Concern.Separateanalyses were performed on the datafor mothersand fathers.Each regression equationincludedthe five socioculturalvariables as predictors.In addition,generationwas includedas a variablein the regressionequationsto evaluateits uniqueeffects relativeto the socioculturalvariables,as well as its interactionwith these variables.Since MANOVA and ANOVA childrearingresultsshowed virtuallyno differencebetween parentsof first- and second-generation students, data for these two groups were combined into a single generationcategory.Thus,generation was dummycoded so that the combineddata for parentsof first- and second-generationstudents (coded 1) was contrastedwith datafor parentsof students(coded0). Finally,father third-generation absencewas coded and includedas a predictorto evaluateits possible effect on Responsibilityand Concernin the datafor mothers. REGRESSION ANALYSES TABLE4. STEPWISE MULTIPLE PREDICTING MOTHERS'ANDFATHERS'RESPONSIBILITY ANDCONCERNFROMSOCIOCULTURAL VARIABLES PredictorVariables Mother's responsibility Child's Spanish to mother Family income Mother'sconcern Mother's schooling Father'sresponsibility Family income Generation Father'sconcern Child's Spanish to father R2 Change F 1 .40 2 -.23 .26 .31 67.44* 41.92* 1 .33 .10 22.71" 1 -.31 2 .30 .17 .25 26.55* 21.15* 1 -.28 .08 12.10* Step *p<.001. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions P Journal of Marriage and the Family 996 Table 4 summarizesresults of the four stepwise multipleregressions.The table includesinformationaboutthe variablesthatmade a significantcontribution,the orderin which the variables enteredthe equation,and the amountof variance (R2)the variablesexplained. Results for mothersshowed Responsibilityto be positively associatedwith the child's Spanish to motherand negatively associatedwith family income. MaternalConcernshowed a significant relationship to only one variable, mother's schooling.The directionof this relationshipwas positive.Resultsfor fathersshowedResponsibility to be negativelyrelatedto family income. Responsibilitywas also significantlyrelatedto generation in the direction of more Responsibility amongfathersof the combinedfirst- and secondgenerations.Finally, paternalConcernwas negatively relatedto child's Spanish.The interaction of generationwith the socioculturalvariablesdid not make a significantcontributionto variancein the analysesfor mothersor fathers. DIscussION Resultsof this studyrevealimportantintergenerationaldifferencesin childrearingpracticesrelated to variationsin the socioculturalbackgroundsof Mexican American families. The proximity of Mexico to the U.S. and continued immigration clearly indicate intergenerationaldynamics as permanentfeaturesof MexicanAmericanculture. As the size of this populationcontinuesto grow, it is imperativeto understandthe extent and nature of within-groupdiversitythat influencessocialization. Parentsof first- and second-generation students,especiallymothers,were very similar in their socialization practices. This is not surprising,in that both groups of parents were born in Mexico and share an immigrantexperience. Nativity and the immigrationexperience thus distinguishthem from mothersof third-generation students, who, like their children, were bornand raisedin the U.S. Foreign-bornmothers stress earlier autonomy,productiveuse of time, strictness,and permissivenessmore thanmothers of third-generationstudents.On the other hand, mothersof third-generation studentsexpressmore concernand supportthantheirforeign-borncounterparts.All groupsof mothersarealike in the degree of equality that they encourage in parentchildrelations. The patternof socializationbetweenfathersof studentsmirfirst-, second-, and third-generation rorthe findingsfor mothersin the areasof productive use of time, equality,and permissiveness.In other areas, however, the similarity between spousesis greateramongfathersof first-and second-generationstudents.Thus, like theirspouses, fathersof first-and second-generation studentsdo not differin the areasof autonomy,strictness,and support.By contrast,fathersof third-generation studentsdifferfrom theirspousesin thatthey expect earlierautonomyonly for theirdaughtersand value less strictness only for their sons. The greatersimilarityin socializationbetweenforeignborn mothers and fathers may reflect stronger agreementand mutual supportfor childrearing goals amongparentsin immigrantfamilies.Immigrationremovesfamiliesfromthe supportbase of their native communities and places them into new environmentslacking extendedkinship networks(Keefe& Padilla,1987).Consequently,immigrantfamilymembersmayrespondby developing stronger interdependence (Casas & Ortiz 1985;de Anda, 1984) to help the family adjustto its new environment.Consensusin socialization goals may reflectan areaof domesticinterdependence conditionedby the immigrantexperience. and consensusmay be However,interdependence weakenedin later generationsthroughacculturation, which providesparentswith differentideas and options about childrearing.Thus, acculturation may lead to greatervariationsin socialization styles betweenU.S.-bornhusbandsandwives. Immigrantparentsgenerallyscored higher on those socializationpracticesmakingup the factor called Responsibility.This factoris characterized by an expectationfor earlierself-relianceand for adherenceto family rules within an open parentchild relationship.This factor resemblesthe authoritativeparentingstyle describedby Baumrind (1973) and others(Dornbusch,Ritter,Leiderman, Roberts,& Fraleigh,1987). In contrast,U.S.-born parents,particularlymothers,scoredhigheron the socialization practices that make up the factor called Concern. This factor is characterizedby emotionalsupportand the expectationof proper behaviorat home andschool. The self-selectionof Mexican immigrantshas been associatedwith such personalitycharacteristics as independence,responsibility,and deferred gratification (for a review, see Buriel, 1984). These characteristicsseem well-suitedto survival in an unfamiliarenvironmentand are probably encouragedby immigrantparentsin their socialization practices. First- and second-generation children are often requiredto serve as English- This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Childrearing in Mexican American Families speaking interpreters between their parents and societal institutions and to assume other family responsibilities such as the shared socialization of younger siblings (Quintanilla, 1990). The socialization style defined here as Responsibility therefore seems well-adapted to the life experiences of Mexican immigrants and their children. By virtue of growing up in the U.S., U.S.-born parents are more acculturated,which reduces some of the responsibility that they must place on their children for the family's survival. Consequently, a Responsibility style of socialization is expressed less than in immigrant families. In addition, by the second generation, and increasingly thereafter, Mexican Americans become cognizant of the socially imposed disadvantageous natureof their ethnic minority status (Buriel, Calzada, & Vasquez, 1982; Buriel & Vasquez, 1982; Dworkin, 1965; Matute-Bianchi, 1986; Ogbu & Matute-Bianchi, 1986). This awareness may motivate U.S.-born parents to be more supportive of their children and to encourage proper behavior at school as a means of overcoming their socially disadvantaged situation. Emphasis on a Concern style of socialization may also reflect a shift toward the childrearing norms of mainstreamEuro-American society, arising from acculturation. Future research comparing Euro-Americans could provide a more conclusive answer to this hypothesis. The sociocultural variables of Spanish usage, family income, and parental schooling bear the most theoretically interesting relationships to the childrearing styles of Responsibility and Concern. Parental values associated with a Responsibility style of childrearing may reflect the demands of the immigrant experience that are encoded in Spanish. Children's greater use of Spanish with mothers may reflect these children's receptivity to parental expectations for Responsibility. Because these children use English extensively outside the home, their retention of Spanish for speaking with mothers may represent a form of identity with parental values. As Spanish usage decreases, so, too, perhaps, do the cognitions and motivations engendered through a Responsibility style of childrearing. This may lead to a more Concern style of childrearing, which is negatively related to children's Spanish usage with fathers. Research documents a strong correlation between family income and acculturation (Moore & Pachon, 1985; Negy & Woods, 1992). Income and acculturation affect each other reciprocally, with more acculturation providing more opportunities for economic mobility, which in turn facilitates 997 social and cultural interaction with the larger society. The impact of family income on socialization may therefore be twofold. First, higher incomes may reflect more exposure to acculturating influences that compete with Responsibility as a socialization style. Second, greater financial resources may relieve families of some of the survival pressures attendant with immigration that initially encouraged a Responsibility style of childrearing. It should be noted that language also affects socioeconomic status because knowledge of English is an economic asset in this society. In this study, English/Spanish usage was assessed only in relation to parent-child interactions. However, parental English usage outside the home may facilitate socioeconomic development, which in turn affects childrearing styles. Future research should consider parents' English usage outside the home, as well as with children, to determine the independent effects of language on socioeconomic status and childrearing. Mothers' schooling encourages the development of a Concern style of childrearing. According to Laosa (1982), prolonged schooling imparts behavioral dispositions toward parenting styles resembling the academic style of the school. This includes reinforcement or praise for behaviors, which, in the present study, resemble the socialization style called Concern. The positive relationship of parental schooling to Concern is therefore to be expected. However, an alternative explanation that takes into account the more negative treatment of Mexican Americans in school should also be considered. Specifically, research shows that teachers behave less favorably toward Mexican Americans such that, relative to Euro-American students, they receive less teacher support and praise (U.S. Civil Rights Commission, 1973), even after answering correctly (Buriel, 1983), and are penalized for their lack of English proficiency (Laosa, 1977). The effects of prolonged exposure to such inequitable treatment may encourage a parental socialization style that attempts to compensate for the disadvantaged classroom situation of these students. Because parents of third-generation students were themselves schooled in the U.S., they have first-hand knowledge of the educational experiences of their children, and can empathize with their situation. Thus, more schooling may encourage a Concern style of socialization as a form of parental support and compensation for the disapproval that children receive in school. Father absence showed no relationship to the childrearing styles of Responsibility and Concern. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 998 Jourmalof Marriageand the Family Perhaps any effects of father absence are offset by the involvement of extended family in the socialization process. According to Keefe and Padilla (1987), U.S.-born families have the largest extended kinship networks. Schmidt and Padilla (1983) also found Mexican American grandparents' involvement in the socialization of grandchildren to be highest with the U.S.-born children of their daughters. It is this group of mothers and children that have the highest rate of father absence in this study. Delinquency and lower school achievement are two serious problems among Mexican American adolescents, particularly those in the later generations. The childrearing style of Responsibility that is more characteristic of foreign-born parents seems to encourage greater adherence to parental rules (strictness), within a context valuing efficiency (productive use of time) and self-reliance (autonomy). The greater press for Responsibility among parents of first- and second-generation students may partially explain why, in the literature, third-generation children experience higher rates of delinquency (Buriel et al., 1982; Vigil & Long, 1981) and lower grades (Landsman et al., 1991). Thus, among Mexican American adolescents, a childrearing style resembling Responsibility may be crucial in fostering healthy social and academic development. Future research should examine if a Responsibility style of childrearing is common among foreign-born parents with children of all ages, or unique to parents of adolescents owing to the particular developmental challenges of their teenage children. Previous research on Mexican Americans has ignored generational differences and sociocultural factors in families that contribute to variations in childrearing styles. By sampling generationally diverse families with sociocultural backgrounds representative of the Mexican American population, this study provides a more complete understanding of Mexican American childrearing. NOTE This study was supportedby a grantfrom the Social Science Research Council and The Inter-University Programfor Latino Research. Requests for reprints shouldbe sentto RaymondBuriel. REFERENCES Baumrind,D. (1973). The developmentof instrumental competence through socialization. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 3-46). Minneapolis:Universityof MinnesotaPress. U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a Bronfenbrenner, context for humandevelopment:Researchperspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723-742. interactionsandtheir Buriel,R. (1983). Teacher-student relationshipto studentachievement:A comparison of Anglo AmericanandMexicanAmericanchildren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 178-187. Buriel,R. (1984). Integrationwith traditionalMexican Americanculture and sociocuturaladjustment.In J. L. Martinez& R. Mendoza(Eds.), ChicanoPsychology(2nded., pp. 95-130). New York:Academic Press. Buriel,R. Calzada,S., & Vasquez,R. (1982). The relationshipof traditionalMexicanAmericancultureto adjustmentanddelinquencyamongthreegenerations of Mexican Americanmale adolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 4, 41-55. Buriel, R., & Cardoza,D. (1993). Mexican American ethnic labeling: An intrafamilialand intergenerational analysis. In M. Bernal & G. Knight (Eds.), Ethnic identity: Formation and transmission among Hispanics and other minorities (pp. 197-210). New York:SUNY Press. Buriel,R., Mercado,R., Rodriguez,J., & Chavez,J. M. (1991). Mexican American disciplinary practices andattitudestowardchild maltreatment: A comparison of foreign- and native-bornmothers.Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 13, 78-94. Buriel,R., & Vasquez,R. (1982). Stereotypesof Mexican descent persons:Attitudesof threegenerations of MexicanAmericanand Anglo Americanadolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 59-70. Burnam,M. A., Hough,R., Karno,M., Escobar,J., & Telles, C. (1987). Acculturationand lifetime prevalence of psychiatric disorders among Mexican Americans in Los Angeles. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,28, 89-102. Casas,J. M., & Ortiz,S. (1985). Exploringthe applicability of the dyadic adjustmentscale for assessing level of maritaladjustmentwith Mexican Americans. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 47, 1023- 1027. Chavez, J. M., & Buriel, R. (1986). Reinforcingchildren'seffort:A comparisonof foreignbornand native born Mexican American mothers. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 8, 127-142. Cromwell, R. E. (1969). Development of a parental attitude research scale for use with the lower and mid- dle classes. Unpublished master's thesis, Kansas StateUniversity. de Anda,D. (1984). Informalsupportnetworksof Hispanic mothers:A comparison across age groups. Journal of Social Service Research, 7, 89-105. Devereux,E. C., Bronfenbrenner, U., & Rodgers,R. R. (1969). Childrearing in England and the United States: A cross-national comparison. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 31, 257-270. Dinerman, I. R. (1982). Migrants and stay-at-homes: A comparative study of rural immigration from Michoacan, Mexico. San Diego: University of California, Center for United States-Mexican Studies. Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Liederman, P. H., Roberts, D. F., & Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Childrearing in Mexican American Families of parentingstyle to adolescentschool performance. ChildDevelopment,58, 1244-1257. Durrett, M. E., O'Bryant, S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1975). Child-rearingreportsof white, black, and Mexican American families. DevelopmentalPsychology,11, 871. Dworkin, A. G. (1965). Stereotypes and self-images held by native-born and foreign-born Mexican Americans.Sociologyand SocialResearch,49, 214224. Fromm,E., & Maccoby,M. (1970). Social characterin a Mexicanvillage. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall. Harrison,A. O., Wilson,M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan,S. Q., & Buriel,R. (1990). Familyecologies of ethnicminoritychildren.ChildDevelopment,61, 347-362. Hess,R. D. (1970). Social class andethnicinfluenceson socialization.In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology (Vol.2, pp. 457-557). New York:Wiley. Hirano-Nakaniski,M. (1986). The extent and prevalence of pre-highschool attritionanddelayededucation for Hispanics.HispanicJournalof Behavioral Sciences,8, 61-76. Kearns,B. J. R. (1970). Childrearingpracticesamong selectedculturallydeprivedminorities.TheJournal of GeneticPsychology,116, 149-155. Keefe,S. E., & Padilla,A. M. (1987). Chicanoethnicity. Albuquerque: Universityof New MexicoPress. and cross-cultural Kim, Y. Y. (1988). Communication adaptation.Philadelphia: MultilingualMatters. Landsman,M. A., Padilla,A. M., Leiderman,H., Clark, C., Ritter,P., & Dornbusch,S. (1991). Biculturalism and academicachievementamongAsian and Hispanic adolescents. Unpublishedmanuscript,StanfordUniversity,Stanford,CA. Laosa,L. M. (1977). Inequalityin the classroom:Observational research on teacher-studentinteractions. Aztlan:InternationalJournalof ChicanoStudiesResearch,8, 51-67. Laosa,L. M. (1980a). Maternalteachingstrategiesand cognitivestyles in Chicanofamilies.Journalof EducationalPsychology,72, 45-54. Laosa, L. M. (1980b). Maternalteachingstrategiesin Chicanoand Anglo-Americanfamilies: The influence of cultureand educationon maternalbehavior. ChildDevelopment,51, 759-765. Laosa,L. M. (1981). Maternalbehavior:Sociocultural diversityin modes of family interaction.In R. W. Henderson(Ed.), Parent-childinteraction:Theory, researchand prospects.(pp. 125-167). New York: AcademicPress. Laosa, L. M. (1982). School, occupation,culture,and family:The impactof parentalschoolingon the parent-childrelationship.Journalof EducationalPsychology,74, 791-827. Laosa,L. M. (1989). Social competencein childhood: Toward a developmental,socioculturallyrelevant paradigm.Journal of AppliedDevelopmentalPsychology,10, 447-468. LeVine, E. S., & Bartz, K. W. (1979). Comparative childrearingattitudesamong Chicano, Anglo, and black parents.HispanicJournalof BehavioralSciences, 1, 165-178. Lin, C. C., & Fu, R. R. (1990). A comparisonof childrearingpractices among Chinese, immigrantChi- 999 nese, andCaucasian-American parents.ChildDevelopment,61, 429-433. Lopez,D. E. (1982). Languagemaintenanceand shiftin the UnitedStatestoday:Thebasicpatternsand their social implications:VolumeL Overviewandsummary. Los Alamitos,CA: NationalCenterfor Bilingual Research. Maccoby,E. E., & Martin,J. A. (1983). Socializationin the contextof the family:Parent-childinteraction.In E. M. Hetherington(Ed.), Handbookof child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization,personality,and social development(pp. 1-102). New York:Wiley. Marin,G., & VonOssMarin,B. (1989). A comparison of threeinterviewingapproachesfor studyingsensitive topics with Hispanics.HispanicJournalof BehavioralSciences,11, 330-340. M. E. (1986). EthnicidentitiesandpatMatute-Bianchi, ternsof school success and failureamongMexicandescent and Japanese-American studentsin a Californiahigh school:An ethnographicanalysis.American Journalof Education,95, 233-255. Moore,J., & Pachon,H. (1985).Hispanicsin the United States.EnglewoodCliffs,NJ:Prentice-Hall. Negy, C., & Woods, D. J. (1992). A note on the relationshipbetween acculturationand socioeconomic status.HispanicJournalof BehavioralSciences,14, 248-251. Nielsen, F., & Fernandez,R. M. (1981). Hispanicstudents in Americanhigh schools: Backgroundcharacteristicsand achievement.Washington,DC: NationalCenterfor EducationStatistics. M. E. (1986). UnderstandOgbu,J., & Matute-Bianchi, ing socioculturalfactors:Knowledge,identity,and school adjustment. In CaliforniaStateDepartmentof Education(Ed.), Beyondlanguage:Social and culturalfactors in schoolinglanguageminoritystudents (pp. 73-142). Los Angeles:CaliforniaStateUniversity, Education,DisseminationandAssessmentCenter. Ortiz, V. (1986). Generational status, family background,andeducationalattainmentamongHispanic youth and non-Hispanicwhite youth.In M. A. Olivas (Ed.),Latinocollege students(pp. 29-46). New York:TeachersCollegePress. Padilla, A. M., & Lindholm, K. J. (1983). Hispanic Americans:Futurebehavioralscience researchdirections (OccasionalPaperNo. 17). Los Angeles: Universityof California,SpanishSpeakingMental HealthResearchCenter. Portes,A. (1979). Illegal immigrationand the international system: Lessons from recent legal Mexican immigrationto the UnitedStates. Social Problems, 4, 425-438. Portes,A., & Bach,R. L. (1985). Latinjourney:Cuban and Mexican immigrants in the United States. Berkeley:Universityof CaliforniaPress. Quintanilla, M. (1990, March 1). The littlest interpreters.Los AngelesTimes,pp. El, E7. ChiRamirez,O., & Arce,C. (1981). The contemporary cano family: An empirically based review. In A. Barron(Ed.), Explorationsin Chicanopsychology. New York:Praeger. Schaefer,E. S., & Bell, R. Q. (1958). Developmentof a parentalattituderesearchinstrument.ChildDevelopment,29, 339-361. This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 1000 Journal of Marriage and the Family Schmidt, A., & Padilla, A. M. (1983). Grandparentgrandchildinteractionin a MexicanAmericangroup. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 5, 181- 198. United States Commission on Civil Rights (1973). education study. Differences in teacher interaction with Mexican American and Anglo students. Wash- ington,DC:U.S. GovernmentPrintingOffice. Vigil, J. D., & Long,J. M. (1981). Unidirectionalor nativist acculturation: Chicanopathsto school achiev- Teachers and students: Report V. Mexican American NewPublication fromNCFR! Vision 2010: Families & Health Care IssueEditor: Barbara Elliott SeriesEditor: Sharon Price The Vision 2010series will examine trends,policy, program,and assessmentissuesfacingAmerican families both now and in the future. It will follow the format of NCFR's highly successful report, 2001: Preparing Families for the Future. Families and Health Care, the first issue in the Vision care 2010 health reform series, presents needs from the family perspective. By taking this perspective, the current needs can be seen more broadly,aandreformcanbefocused to improve the U.S.health caresystem and tostrengthen American families. Family and health experts provide analyses of 19 issues. Topics covered include: Health Promotion-Disease Prevention and the ment. Human Organization, 40, 273-277. NewNCFRPublication! FamilyHealth: FromDatato Policy Editors:Gerry E.Hendershot & FeliciaB.LeClere The Clinton administrationhas recognized that family issues are central to many of this nations health & welfare problems. However, federal agencies responsible for thestatisticaldataneeded to respond to the policy debates are ill prepared to provide integrated family health data topolicy makers. The offices of the Asst. Sec. for Planning & Evaluation (ASPE)and the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) in the Center for Disease Control & Prevention entered into an interagency collaborative effort to improve data collection and dissemination. Violence, Training Families,HealthExperiencesof MinorityFamilies, and Family Centered Health Policy. 44 pages. Thisbook summarizes a 3-day workshop aimed at increasing awareness of the need to improve federal statistics on families and health issues and to form consensus about what family and health statistics are needed. It encapsulates the discussion of members of government, agencies, family researchers, and policy makers! $12.95 for NCFR members and $14.95 for nonmembers. Foreignnand Canadian orders add $2.00 shipping and handling. U.S. funds on U.S. banks only. Canadian orders add 7% GST (123830-465). MN Residents add 6.5%sales tax. $17.95for NCFR members and $21.95for nonmembers. Foreign and Canadian orders add $2.00 shipping and handling. U.S.funds on U.S. banks only. Canadian orders add 7%GST(123830-465). MN Residents add 6.5% Sales tax. andtheFamily, Family, Reproduction Family Health Professionals About National Council on Family Relations 3989 Central Ave. NE, Suite 550 Minneapolis, MN 55421 (612) 781-9331 + FAX (612) 781-9348 National Council on Family Relations 3989 Central Ave. NE, Suite 550 Minneapolis, MN 55421 (612) 781-9331 + (612) 781-9348 FAX This content downloaded from 199.73.44.216 on Sat, 25 Oct 2014 19:10:27 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
© Copyright 2024