UMEÅ UNIVERSITY MEDICAL DISSERTATIONS N ew Series N o 136 - ISSN 0346-6612 From the Department o f Environmental Medicine, University o f Umeå, Umeå, Sweden and the N ational Board o f O ccupational Safety and H ealth, Technical unit, Box 6104, Umeå, Sweden Vibration Exposure of the Glabrous Skin of the Human Hand by RONNIE LUNDSTRÖM Umeå University VIBRATION EXPOSURE OF THE GLABROUS SKIN OF THE HUMAN HAND av Ronnie Lundström Fil. kand. Institutionen för Hygien och Miljömedicin, Umeå Universitet, 901 87 Umeå och Arbetarskyddsstyrelsen, Forskningsavdelningen Tekniska enheten, Box 6104, 900 06 Umeå. Akademisk avhandling som med vederbörligt tillstånd av Medicinska Fakulteten vid Umeå Universitet för avläggande av doktorsexamen i medicinsk vetenskap kommer att framläggas för offentlig granskning i Sal A 5, Farmakologiska institu tionen, Umeå Universitet, fredagen den 29 mars 1985, kl 09.00 Avhandlingen baseras på följande rapporter: I. Local vibrations - Mechanical Impedance of the Human Hand's Glabrous Skin. R Lundström. Journal of Biomechanics, 17:2(1984) pp. 137-144. II. Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin dis placements . R S Johansson, U Landström & R Lundström. Brain Research, 244(1984) pp. 17-25. III. Sensitivity to egdes of mechanoreceptive afferent units innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand. R S Johansson, U Landström & R Lundström. Brain Research, 244(1982) pp. 27-32. IV. Acute impairment of the sensitivity of skin mechano receptive units caused by vibration exposure of the hand. R Lundström & R S Johansson. Ergonomics (submitted for publication). V. Effects of local vibration on tactile perception in the hands of dentists. R Lundström & A Lindmark. Journal of Low Frequency Noise and Vibration, 1:1(1982), p p . 1-11. VI. Effects of local vibration transmitted from ultrasonic devices on vibrotactile perception in the hands of therapists. R Lundström. Ergonomics, (1984) (In press)*. ABSTRACT Lundström, Ronnie, 1985. Vibration exposure of the glabrous skin human hand. Umeå University, Medical Dissertions, New Series, 136. of the An occupational exposure to hand-arm vibration can cause a complex of neu rological, vascular and musculo-skeletal disturbances, known as the 'vibra tion syndrome'. However, the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms are not at all clear. Early signs of an incipient vibration syndrome are often inter mittent disturbances in the cutaneous sensibility of the fingers, i.e. numb ness and/or tactile paresthesias. At later stages, a vasoconstrictive phe nomenon appears, usually as episodes of finger blanching. When using a vibratory tool, all mechanical energy entering the body has to be transmitted through, or absorbed by, the glabrous skin in contact with the handle. Therefore, the aims of this study was to investigate: (i) mecha nical responses of the skin to vibrations, (ii) the response properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors to vibrations, and (iii) influences of vibration exposure on touch perception. It was found by measuring the mechanical point impedance (0.02-10 kHz) that the skin is easy to make vibrate within the range of 80 to 200 Hz. Within or close to this range are the dominant frequencies of many vibratory tools. Thus, strong mechanical loads, such as compressive and/or tensile strain, can appear in the skin which, in turn, may induce temporary or per manent injuries. Recordings of impulses in single mechanoreceptive afferents, while the skin as exposed to vibrations, were obtained using needle electrodes inserted into the median nerve. The 4 types of mechanoreceptive afferents (FA I, FA II, SA I, and SA II) in the glabrous skin exhibited different response cha racteristics to vibrations. The FA I units were most easily excited at vibra tory frequencies between ca 8 and 64 Hz and the FA II units between ca 64 and 400 Hz. The SA units were most sensitive at lower frequencies. At high sti mulus amplitudes, such as may occur while using vibratory tools, a consi derable overlap existed between the frequency ranges at which the units were exited. Evidence was also provided, that mechanical skin stimuli produced by edges of a vibrating object, compared to flat surfaces, more vigorously excited the FA I and particularly the SA I units. Thus, a marked edge en hancement, essential for tactile gnosis and precision manipulation, seems to exist already within the peripheral nervous system. Acure impairment of tactile sensibility caused by vibrations, proved to be due to a reduced sensitivity of the mechanoreceptive afferents. A loss of manual dexterity a*vi an increased risk for accidents may therefore appear, both during and after a vibration exposure. Percussive tools, high speed drills and ultrasonic devices are known to generate mechanical energy at frequencies above 1 kHz, i.e. frequencies usually not felt. At these frequencies, it is known that most of the energy, entering the body, is absorbed by the skin. Therefore, it was investigated whether a long-term exposure to high-frequency vibration may have a detri mental effect on the cutaneous sensitivity. One group of dentists and one of therapists, professionally exposed to high-frequency vibrations, were studied with regard to vibrotactile thresholds in their hands. The study showed that deleterious effects on tactile sensibility, at local exposure to high fre quency vibration, can not be excluded. Key words: Vibration, hand, skin, mechanical tactile perception, high-frequency. Ronnie Lundström Dept. Environ. Med. Univ. Umeå, S-901 87 Umeå, impedance, Sweden. mechanoreceptors, UMEÅ UNIVERSITY MEDICAL DISSERTATIONS New Series N o 136 - ISSN 0346-6612 From the Department of Environmental Medicine, University of Umeå, Umeå, Sweden and the National Board of Occupational Safety and Health, Technical unit, Box 6104, Umeå, Sweden Vibration Exposure of the Glabrous Skin of the Human Hand by RONNIE LUNDSTRÖM K • u m X e* Umeå University 1985 2 ABSTRACT Lundström, Ronnie, 1985. Vibration exposure of the glabrous skin human hand. Umeå University, Medical Dissertions, New Series, 136. of the An occupational exposure to hand-arm vibration can cause a complex of neu rological, vascular and musculo-skeletal disturbances, known as the 'vibra tion syndrome'. However, the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms are not at all clear. Early signs of an incipient vibration syndrome are often inter mittent disturbances in the cutaneous sensibility of the fingers, i.e. numb ness and/or tactile paresthesias. At later stages, a vasoconstrictive phe nomenon appears, usually as episodes of finger blanching. When using a vibratory tool, all mechanical energy entering the body has to be transmitted through, or absorbed by, the glabrous skin in contact with the handle. Therefore, the aims of this study was to investigate: (i) mecha nical responses of the skin to vibrations, (ii) the response properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors to vibrations, and (iii) influences of vibration exposure on touch perception. It was found by measuring the mechanical point impedance (0.02-10 kHz) that the skin is easy to make vibrate within the range of 80 to 200 Hz. Within or close to this range are the dominant frequencies of many vibratory tools. Thus, strong mechanical loads, such as compressive and/or tensile strain, can appear in the skin which, in turn, may induce temporary or per manent injuries. Recordings of impulses in single mechanoreceptive afferents, while the skin as exposed to vibrations, were obtained using needle electrodes inserted into the median nerve. The 4 types of mechanoreceptive afferents (FA I, FA II, SA I, and SA II) in the glabrous skin exhibited different response cha racteristics to vibrations. The FA I units were most easily excited at vibra tory frequencies between ca 8 and 64 Hz and the FA II units between ca 64 and 400 Hz. The SA units were most sensitive at lower frequencies. At high sti mulus amplitudes, such as may occur while using vibratory tools, a consi derable overlap existed between the frequency ranges at which the units were exited. Evidence was also provided, that mechanical skin stimuli produced by edges of a vibrating object, compared to flat surfaces, more vigorously excited the FA I and particularly the SA I units. Thus, a marked edge en hancement, essential for tactile gnosis and precision manipulation, seems to exist already within the peripheral nervous system. Acute impairment of tactile sensibility caused by vibrations, proved to be due to a reduced sensitivity of the mechanoreceptive afferents. A loss of manual dexterity and an increased risk for accidents may therefore appear, both during and after a vibration exposure. Percussive tools, high speed drills and ultrasonic devices are known to generate mechanical energy at frequencies above 1 kHz, i.e. frequencies usually not felt. At these frequencies, it is known that most of the energy, entering the body, is absorbed by the skin. Therefore, it was investigated whether a long-term exposure to high-frequency vibration may have a detri mental effect on the cutaneous sensitivity. One group of dentists and one of therapists, professionally exposed to high-frequency vibrations, were studied with regard to vibrotactile thresholds in their hands. The study showed that deleterious effects on tactile sensibility, at local exposure to high fre quency vibration, can not be excluded. Key words: Vibration, hand, skin, mechanical tactile perception, high-frequency. impedance, Ronnie Lundström Dept. Environ. Med. Univ. Umeå, S-901 87 Umeå, Sweden. mechanoreceptors, 3 ORIGINAL REPORTS This thesis is based on the following papers, which in the text are referred to by their Roman numerals: I. Local vibrations - Mechanical Impedance of the Human Hand's Glabrous Skin. R Lundström. Journal of Biomechanics, 17:2(1984) pp. 137-144. II. Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements. R S Johansson, U Landström & R Lundström. Brain Research, 244(1982), pp. 17-25. III. Sensitivity to egdes of mechanoreceptive afferent innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand. R S Johansson, U Landström & R Lundström. Brain Research, 244(1982), pp. 27-32. IV. units Acute impairment of the sensitivity of skin mechano receptive units caused by vibration exposure of the hand. R Lundström & R S Johansson. Ergonomics (submitted for publication). V. Effects of local vibration hands of dentists. R Lundström & A Lindmark. on tactile perception Journal of Low Frequency Noise and Vibration, pp. 1-11. VI. 1:1 in the (1982) Effects of local vibration transmitted from ultrasonic devices on vibrotactile perception in the hands of therapists. R Lundström. Ergonomics (In press). 4 CONTENTS 1. GENERAL BACKGROUND OF THE STUDY 1.1 Vibration syndrome 1.2 Risk assessment 5 5 9 1.3 Mechanical response of the hand-arm system 1.4 Tactile sensibility 12 14 1.5 Acute impairment of tactile sensibility following vibration exposure 19 1.6 Effects of high frequency vibration on tactile sensibility 20 2. SPECIFIC AIMS OF THE PRESENT STUDY 21 3. RESULTS 22 3.1 Mechanical response of the glabrous skin to vibration 3.2 Neural responses to vibration 3.3 Neural sensitivity to vibrating edge contours 3.4 Acute impairment of tactile sensibility following vibration exposure 3.5 Effects of high frequency vibration on tactile perception threshold 22 25 27 29 31 4. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS 34 5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 38 6. REFERENCES 39 5 1. GENERAL BACKGROUND OF THE STUDY 1.1. Vibration syndrome The vibrations in many electrically or pneumatically powered handheld tools or workpieces may cause a complex of neurologi cal, vascular and musculo-skeletal disturbances. These pheno mena, occurring together or independently, are becoming widely recognized as an important occupational disease known as the vibration syndrome (among others Hamilton 1918, Dart 1946, Agate and Druett 1947, Pyykkö 1974, Taylor 1974, Griffin 1980, Taylor and Brammer 1982, Gemne and Taylor 1983). The best documented and most easily observed condition connect ed to hand-arm vibration exposure is a vasoconstrictive pheno menon, appearing as episodes of fingers blanching together with tingling and numbness in the exposed hands. However, similar symptoms of "white fingers" were first described by Raynaud in 1862 on healthy non-vibration exposed individuals (Primary Ray naud's Disease). Since then many causes of "white fingers" have been recognized, including vibration, and are referred to as "Secondary Raynaud's Phenomenon". Vibration-induced white fingers were first described about stone cutters and rockminers (Loriga 1911). The literature relevant to this injury has ever since increased tremendously and it has been denoted by diffe rent names, "Raynaud's Phenomenon of Occupational Origin", "Traumatic Vasospastic Disease" (TVD) and "Vibration-Induced White Fingers" (VWF) are the most commonly used (for refs, see Taylor and Pelmear 1975, Taylor and Brammer 1982). The degree of severity of VWF are often assessed according to a scale presented by Taylor and Pelmear 1975 (Table 1). In the early stages of VWF, a person ususally reports symptoms of neu rological disturbancies, such as slight tingling and numbness of his fingers. Later, after regular prolonged exposure, the tips of one or more fingers suffer attacks of finger blanching. The episodic vasoconstrictions are usually precipitated by cold. With further exposure, in the magnitude of years, the area of finger blanching extends proximally to the base of those fingers exposed to vibration. During the attacks the 6 Table 1. Grading scale for the severity of VWF according to Tai lor and Pelmear (1975). Symptoms and signs Stage of disorder 0 Oip Condition of digits Work and social inter ference No blanching No complaints Intermittent tingling No interference without blanching °N Intermittent numbness No interference without blanching °TN Intermittent tingling No interference and numbness without blanching 1 Blanching of one or more fingertips without tingling and/or numbness No interference 2 Blanching of one or more complete fingerS/ Slight interference at home or in social activi usually during winter ties but not at work Extensive blanching, usually all fingers bi laterally. Frequent epi Definite interference at work, at home and in so sodes during all seasons tions of hobbies Extensive blanching of all fingers, both winter and summer Occupation changed be cause of severity of signs and symptoms of VWF 3 4 cial activities. Restric 7 fingers feel swollen, painful and inflexible, and there is a reduction in tactile sensibility, in manual dexterity and in grip strength 1972, Färkkilä (Taylor and et.al. Brammer 1982). The 1982, Bannister vasoconstriction and Smith terminates after some minutes up to about one hour being due to the degree of severity of the VWF attack and the possibility of warming of the hands. During this period a reactive hyperaemia occurs, usually painful and unpleasant. The mechanisms behind these vasomotor reactions are still not known but several theories have been suggested, based primarily on changes in peripheral bloodvessels and/or in the peripheral nervous system (e.g. Ashe et.al. 1962, Magos and Okos 1963, Stewart and Goda 1970, Gurdjian and Walker 1945 and Hyvärinen et.al. 1973). One theory, suggested by Hyvärinen and coworkers (1973), is that a sympathetic reflex, triggered by exposition to cold, regards loud noise vibration, or vibration, the activity entails vasoconstriction. originating in Pacinian As cor puscles was suggested to elicit the reflex. These mechanoreceptors are particularly sensitive to vibrations at frequencies be tween ca. 50-500 Hz (see below). Apart from vasomotor reactions, other types of injuries seem to be associated with the use of vibratory hand-held tools. Peri pheral neuropathy (Seppäläinen 1972), changes in nerve conduc tion velocity (Kumlin and Seppäläinen 1969) and changes in tac tile perception threshold of the hand (e.g. Lidström et.al. 1982) have earlier been reported. Musculo-skeletal changes such as unnormally strong muscle fatigue, and vacuoles, cysts and décalcification of bone tissues, are somewhat less commonly reported (Färkkilä et. al. 1982, Kumlin et. al. 1973). The correlation between generated injuries and the physical cha racteristics of the vibration source has proved to be very com plex. Man's response to vibration appears to be determined by several physical, operational, ergonomical, environmental and personal factors (Guignard 1979, Taylor and Brammer 1982, ISO/DIS 5349.2 1984). Some of them are listed in Table 2. Table 2. Some factors known or believed to influence the severi ty of vibration syndrome. EXTERNAL AND OCCUPATIONAL FACTORS Physical factors Intensity, directionality and durability of vibration entering the hand. Dominant frequencies and amplitudes that enter the hand. Total duration and temporal pattern of exposure each work day. Operational and ergonomic factors Work cycle and method of using the tool. Weight of tool. Configuration of handle. Hand grip force required to guide tool. Working posture. Use of gloves of soft texture. Surface area in contact with the tool. Environmental factors Prevailing climate (temperature, humidity etc) at worker's home and workplace. Surrounding noise. INTERNAL AND PERSONAL FACTORS Years of employment involving vibration exposure. Entry age. Level of training, skill and familiarity with the tool. Body constitution and biological susceptibility. Vasoconstrictive agents (smoking, medication etc). 1.2 Risk assessment The proposed international standard ISO/DIS 5349.2 (1984) speci fies methods for measuring and reporting hand-transmitted vibra tion in three orthogonal axes within the frequency range of about 5 to 1500 Hz. All measurements and assessments are to be based on 'broad-banded frequency weighted acceleration values' determined with a specified frequency weighting network or by conversion of one-third or octave band data. The nominal gain of the network is to be zero from ca 6-16 Hz, and further on, up to 1250 Hz, the acceleration signals will be attenuated by 6 dB per octave. The attenuation at even higher frequencies should be at least 12 dB per octave. The risk of getting vibration injuries is thus considered to decrease with increasingfrequency, * assuming that man is less sensitive to high frequency compo nents. If the characteristics of the weighting network, on the other hand, is expressed in terms of vibration velocity, the hand-arm system is range of 16 to 1250 considered as equally Hz (cf. Figure 1). sensible within the The guidelines for the assessment (ISO/DIS 5349.2 - Annex A), specified in terms of frequency weighted acceleration for the dominant axes and daily exposure time, is based on the present knowledge on the dose-effect relationship (Brammer 1982). The exposure times in years for different percentiles of a popula tion before the onset of finger blanching may thus be estimated. A recent study, where the vibration of 110 tools were measured (Lundström and Burström 1984), shows that large variations, as regards the time interval from vibration exposure to the onset of vascular disorder is to be expected both within and between different types of tools (Table 3). The shortest estimated time intervals were found, nibblers. for instance, among chipping hammers and Mean 1/3-octave band spectras expressed in both vibration acce leration and velocity have also been calculated for each type of tool, and the result is shown in Figure 1. For the grinders and the nibblers the dominant frequencies, which were related to their rotational speeds, were near 100 and 63 Hz, respectively. For percussive tools, such as chipping hammers and wrenches, 10 the frequency spectras appeared to be more broadbanded. As can be seen, the shape of the acceleration spectras differs from those expressed in terms of velocity. The high-frequency com ponents are more dominant in the acceleration spectras. Table 3. Mean frequency weighted acceleration for various types of hand-held tools and corresponding estimated exposure times before the onset of vascular symptoms for diffe rent percentiles of an exposed population in accordance with ISO/DIS 5349.2 (1984). The dose-effect relation ship given is based on a daily exposure of 4 hours. Data gathered from Lundström and Burström (1984). Type of tools Number of tools Grinders 54 Weighted accelera tion in m/s^(rms) for dominant axes Mean ± SD 3.3 2.4 Years before the onset of finger blanching Percentile of population ± SD 10% 50% ± SD ‘ 13 5 20 >25 >25 2.5 7.2 Chipping hammers 4.7 12 7 Wrenches 27 2.8 1.7 11 >25 2.5 Nibblers 6.8 4.7 . 14 9 f 16 24 i 1L> 25 [ 6 10 I 1L >25 4.5 Others 15 4.2 2.3 .. 15 6 L 25 1[ ^ 10 i 1.>25 According to guidelines based on 1/3-octav band-spectras given in a previous proposal for international standard, ISO/DIS 5349 (1979), the daily use of both riveting hammers and nibblers should, on average, not exceed 4 hours. A daily exposure of be tween 4 and 8 hours is acceptable for grinders. No limitations in exposure time seems to be required for wrenches. It should be noted that large variations in vibration levels exist between each group of tools, why any generalisation as regards recommen ded daily exposure time is difficult. The assessment in a parti cular case should therefore be based on the actual vibration en vironment . 11 V E L O C I T Y , mm/s ACCELERATION, m/s 100 100 31.5 GRINDERS 31.5 n«20 10 10 3.1 1 0 .3 0.1 T I I I I I i i i i 100 CHIPPING HAMMERS 31.5 n =8 100 100 31.5 WRENCHES 31.5 n =26 10 3.1 1 0.3 0.1 100 NIBBLERS 31.5 n =6 F R E Q U E N C Y , Hz Figure 1. One-third octave band spectras expressed in both vib ration acceleration and velocity for four different types of hand-held tools frequently used in industry. The upper and lower limits in the frequency spectras indicate the maximal and minimal average level, res pectively, for any of the three directions. The fi gures also show exposure guidelines for hand-trans mitted vibration, for 4 to 8 hours' daily exposure, continuous or not regularly interupted. The graphs are in accordance with a previous draft of international standard, ISO/DIS 5349 (1979). For more information see text. 12 1.3. Mechanical response of the hand-arm system Due to its mechanical complexity, the response characteristics of the human hand-arm system to different dynamic inputs, such as vibration, is difficult to describe, both in quantitative and qualitative terms. This system can be regarded as consisting of a large number of separate mechanical elements, such as masses held together with springs and viscous dampers. However, more or less sophisticated hand-arm models can be found in literature, and a fairly good idea how the system responds to vibration can therefore be obtained under specific conditions with regard to hand-arm posture, squeeze force etc. (Dieckman 1958, Reynolds and Keith 1977, Mishoe and Suggs 1977, Wood et.al. 1978, Byström et.al. 1982). The mechanical characteristics of man's hand-arm system have proved to be affected by several factors, for example bodily constitution, vibration direction, muscle tension, arm's posi tion and squeeze force. The resonance frequency, (or fre quencies) is influenced by these factors. Impedance measurements taken in earlier studies, by among others Abrams and Suggs (1970), Zaveri (1974), Mishoe and Suggs (1977), Reynolds and Keith (1977) indicate that it is below 500 Hz. It is known that vibrations entering the body, through the hand, are highly attenuated as they are propagated through the hand and arm. According to Bekesy (1939), at 50 Hz an attenuation of the vibration amplitude of approximately 15 dB occurs from the handle to the elbow and another 25 dB to the head. Furthermore, it is also known that a vibration input is more heavily attenua ted the higher the frequency. As can be seen from Figure 2, dif férencies of up to about 60 dB between a low and a high frequen cy, 31.5 Hz and 2000 Hz, respectively, have been reported as re gards the attenuation taking place in the hand (Suggs, 1970). Thus, at frequencies above ca. 50 Hz most of the vibration ener gy entering the hand-arm system has dissipated in the hand. This is more pronounced the higher the frequency (see also Reynolds and Angevine 1977, Reynolds and Keith 1977). Up to about 500 Hz the mechanical energy is propagated as shear waves within the skin. Higher frequencies are probably propagated as bulk shear waves through underlying tissues (Potts et.al. 1983). Handle, input to hand 20 6.3 CO -1 0 pi 2.0 -20 .63 -30 .20 -40 .063 -50 ATTENUATION, . 020 -60 dB cr Radius, < .0063 -70 O) z O - < £ I LU o O .0020 31.5 125 500 FREQUENCY, 2000 -80 Hz Figure 2 . Vibration attenuation in the hand and arm. from Suggs 1970). (Modified 14 1.4. Tactile sensibility In addition to provide a mechanical protection of the deeper tissues of the hand, the glabrous skin has a number of func tions. One of them is to lodge the huge number of sense organs, which provide the central nervous system with information about tactile, thermal and noxious events on the skin surface. Such information is necessary when using the hand in explorative tasks, but is also essential for the motor control of the hand. The tactile information clearly plays the most important role for the more or less automatic processes within the central ner vous system, and are of paramount importance in precision move ments carried out by the fingers (e.g. Moberg 1962; Johansson and Westling 1984). The tactile signals provide, at every mo ment, the central nervous system with an image of the mechanical state of the skin and thereby also information related to mecha nical properties of hand-held objects. The term tactile unit is applied to a single mechanoreceptive neuron, including its affe rent nerve fibre and all its peripheral branches (terminals), connected to the appropriate end organs in the skin. This unit responds to skin deformations within a specific skin area de noted the unit'sreceptive field. The signals arising in the nerve terminals are transmitted through the afferent nerve fibres to the central nervous system as nerve impulses, ca. 1 ms in duration. The tactile units have large-diameter myelinated fibres (Aq£) with conduction velocities of ca 30-70 m/s. With the introduction of the microneurographic method for re cording impulses from single afferent nerve fibres in awake human subjects (Vallbo and Hagbarth 1968), it became possible to study the peripheral tactile neuronal mechanism in man. This method also provided the possibility to combine directly neurophysiological and psychophysical responses. After Knibestöl and Vallbo (1970) first explored the tactile units in the glabrous skin of the human hand using this technique, several investiga tions of their functional properties have been carried out. Their main results are briefly reviewed in a following section. The reader may consult some recent reviews for more detailed information 1984). (Johansson and Vallbo 1983, Vallbo and Johansson 15 There are about 17000 mechanoreceptive units of four different types supplying the glabrous skin area of the hand (Johansson and Vallbo, 1979). They have been classified into two major ca tegories on the basis of their response to a sustained indenta tion of the skin. Furthermore, within each of these categories two different types of units can be distinguished on the basis of the size and structure of their receptive fields (Figure 3). ADAPTATION Fast , no st at i c r e s po n s e Slow, st ati c r e s po n s e Small _l LU LL shar p bor ders LU > I- Q_ LU O LU CC Large obscur e borders Figure 3. Types of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the gla brous skin of the human hand classified on the basis of their adaptation and receptive field properties. The graphs show schematically the impulse discharge (lower trace) to ramp indentation of the skin (upper trace). The black patches and dashed areas of the drawings of the hand show the extent of typical re ceptive fields for type I and type II units respec tively. For further information, see text. 16 Two types are fast adapting (FA I and FA II) and respond only when the stimulus is in motion. The FA I units seem to be most dependent on the velocity of the skin indentation, whereas the FA II units are more sensitive to higher derivatives, acceleration (Knibestöl 1973). The fast adapting such as units can respond during both the increasing and the decreasing part of the skin indentation. The FA I units constitutes about 43% of the total number of tactile units within the glabrous area of the human hand and are characterized by small, usually round or oval, and well defined receptive fields (Johansson 1978). The median size of the fields, when measured with von Frey hairs at an intensity of 4-5 times the threshold force of the unit, is 13 mm2 (Johansson and Vallbo 1980). The second type, FA II, has larger receptive fields with obscure borders. The median size has been approximated to about 101 mm2, determined with the same technique (Johansson and Vallbo 1980). They constitute about 13% of the total number of tactile units in the hand. An interesting feature of these units is their extreme sensitivity to mechani cal transients even when the stimulation is applied remotely from the receptor. The two types of slowly adapting units, SA I and SA II, respond only during increasing skin indentation but, in addition, with a sustained discharge during a constant deformation of the skin (Knibestöl 1975). The receptive fields for the SA I units are small with sharp borders while the SA II units have large field with no distinct borders. The median fields for the SA I and SA II units are 11 and 59 mm2, respectively, estimated with the same method as for the FA units (Johansson and Vallbo 1980). SA I and SA II units constitutes about 25% and 19%, respectively, of the unit population in the glabrous skin area of the hand (Johansson and Vallbo 1979). Some characteristic features of the SA II units are their exquisite sensitivity to remote lateral skin strech, and many of' them are also continously discharging in the absence of skin indentation (Johansson 1978). In Figure 4, the structure and location of the proposed end organs are schematically indicated. The end organs of the FA I units are most likely Meissner corpuscles, located in the der mal papillae. The Pacinian corpuscles, located in the deep 17 dermis and subcutaneous tissues, and the smaller and simpler lamellated Golgi-Mazzoni bodies in the dermis are probably the endings of FA II units. The endings of SA I units are Merkel cell neurite complexes which intermediate epidermal ridges. are located Finally, at the tip of the the SA II units most likely terminate with the spindle shaped Ruffini endings located in the dermis. I FA I Epidermis SA I Dermis S A II F A II i Subcutis Figure 4. Vertical section through the glabrous skin of the hu man hand schematically showing the location of the end organs of four types of mechanoreceptive units. (Adapted from Johansson & Vallbo, 1983). It has been shown that the density of tactile units in the glabrous skin differs between unit types as well as between dif ferent regions of the glabrous skin area (Johansson and Vallbo 1979). As shown in Figure 5, the density is highest in the distal part of the finger tip. The density decreases stepwise towards the wrist. There are two steps; between the distal and proximal half of the distal phalanx and between the palm and the bases of the fingers. It has been suggested that type I units, with small and well-defined receptive fields, together with their high innervation density particularly at the finger tips, account for the high spatial acuity of the tactile sense. Thus, 18 a strong correlation between the density of these units and the spatial discrimination capacity has been shown (Vallbo and Johansson 1978). For all unit types there is a huge overlap be tween receptive fields, especially on the finger tips (Johansson and Vallbo 1980). FA I FA II SA I 21 70 37 10 30 25 9 S A II Figure 5. Estimated average densities of mechanoreceptive affe rent units in three different areas of the glabrous skin of the human hand innervated by the median nerve. Figures indicates the average unit density per cm^ for each region based on the results from Johansson and Vallbo 1979. It should be noted that in the present summary, a modified ter minology for the fast adapting units have been used compaired to paper II and paper III, where the FA I units are denoted RA, and the FA II are denoted PC. The motives for a change to the pre sent names have been further discussed by Johansson and Vallbo (1983). 19 1.5. Acute impairment of tactile sensibility following vibration exposure As previously pointed out (cf. Table 1) early signs of an in cipient vibration syndrome are neurological symptoms primarily affecting the sensibility of the hand, e.g. paraesthesias and impairment of the tactile sensibility (Taylor and Brammer 1982, Lidström et.al. 1982). Vibration exposure is known to account for a temporary increase in vibrotactile thresholds on healthy human subjects, not previously exposed to vibrations (e.g. Tominaga 1973, Verrillo and Schmeidt 1974, Nishiyama and Watanabe 1981). The magnitude of the temporary threshold shifts (TTS) as well as the time for recovery depend on several factors, such as the frequency, amplitude and duration of the vibration exposure. In general, longer duration and/or higher amplitudes increase the threshold shifts and the time for recovery. How the fre quency effects the thresholds is not yet that clearly documen ted. Neither are the physiological mechanisms accounting for the depression of the tactile sensibility known. Interestingly, the TTS elicited by a given vibration exposure is higher and the duration of the recovery is longer for subjects showing a previous history of work with vibrating tools (Lid ström et.al. 1982). This is true even for workers with normal absolute thresholds. With increased exposure, a permanent de crease in tactile sensibility may appear. Thus, it appears as if the mechanisms accounting for the TTS and the permanent impair ment of the sensibility would somehow be related. Therefore, it might be fruitful to study further the TTS in terms of under lying physiological mechanisms since it may provide clues to the pathophysiology of the vibration syndrome. 20 1.6. Effects of high frequency vibration on tactile sensibility At the end of the 1970s complaints about numbness and stiffness in the fingers were heard from dentists. Some of them had been forced to resign due to reduction in tactile sensibility and in motor skill. Thus, the complaints resembled those to be expected among industrial workers using vibratory hand-held tools. Most handpieces used in modern dentistry have high rotational speed, over 100000 rpm. The outcome of a vibration measurement showed vibration levels as high as about 100 m/s^ (rms) within the fre quency range of 1-50 kHz (Lundström and Liszka 1979) . Similar results, have been obtained in later investigations as well (Andersson et.al. 1983). High levels of high frequency vibra tions have also been measured in grinders, drills, riveting ham mers and at pedestal grinding (Lundström and Liszka 1979, Dandanell 1984, Starck 1984). A high prevalence of VWF has been re ported for personal using pedestal grinders and percussive tools (Dandanell 1984, Starck 1984). It is known that the greater part of mechanical energy, at high frequencies, is absorbed by superficial tissues in direct con tact with the vibrational source (cf. section 1.3). Cutaneous mechanoreceptors may therefore be affected with a loss in sensi bility and motor precision performance as a consequence. Distur bances of the tactile sensibility indicate influences on the nervous system and may be an early sign in the development of more serious injuries, such as VWF (cf. Table 1). However, according to ISO/DIS 5349.2 (1984) all assessments shall be limited to vibrational frequencies below ca. 1.5 kHz, assuming that man is regarded as being not adversely influenced by higher frequencies. Against this background it seems important to exa mine whether a reduction in tactile sensibility can be objecti vely established among people, proffessionally exposed to vibra tions only containing high-frequency components. 21 2. SPECIFIC AIMS OF THE PRESENT INVESTIGATION The scientific literature relevant to the injurious effects of hand-transmitted vibration probably amounts to hundreds of pub lications. Despite of that, still a paucity of firm experimental and epidemiological data exist as regards the causal relation ship between vibration exposure and associated injuries. When using a vibratory hand-held tool, all mechanical energy entering the hand-arm system has to be transmitted through, or absorbed by, the skin in contact with the vibrating handle. To fully understand how man is influenced, basic knowledge of how the skin responds, both physically and physiologically, is thus needed. Therefore the principal aims of the present investiga tion were to answer the following questions: - How does the human hand's glabrous skin, in contact with the vibrating surface, mechanically behave when exposed to vibra tions? - How does the mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand respond to vibrations of different fre quencies and amplitudes? - Is there a temporary reduction in sensitivity among cutaneous mechanoreceptors after a powerful vibration exposure? If so, how does it relate to the reduction in tactile sensibility observed with phychophysical methods? - Does a long-term exposure to vibration with very high fre quencies, above 1 kHz, have a deleterious effect on tactile sensibility? 22 3. RESULTS 3.1. Mechanical response of the glabrous skin to vibration (I) When using a vibratory tool all mechanical energy entering the hand has to be transmitted through, or absorbed by, the glabrous skin in contact with the vibrating handle. In the present study, the mass-like, the spring-like and viscous properties in diffe rent areas of the glabrous skin of human hand were investigated by measuring the mechanical point impedance (I). The mechanical impedance (Z), defined as the ratio between force (F) and velocity (v), describes the mechanical structure's re sistance to follow an applied vibration. If, as in this inves tigation, force and velocity is measured at the same point is is called point impedance. Sinusoidal vibrations within the frequency range of 20 to 10 000 Hz were delivered to the glabrous skin of the human hand through an impedance head mounted on a small vibrator whereas the force and the velocity applied to the skin were continously recorded. The magnitude of the point impedance could then be calculated. The vibrations were superimposed on two static loads (0.5 and 1 N). Two cylinder-shaped test probes, with plane surfaces in con tact with the skin were used (0.5 and 1 cm^). The vibration amp litudes varied between 1 and 10 m/s^ (rms). In each of 6 adult subjects the skin impedance were determined on ten different points located on the right hand (Figure 7). It was found that the skin behaved as a mechanical system con sisting of a spring, a damper and a mass element coupled in parallel. For such a system, the magnitude of the impedance de creases with increasing frequency down to a minimum value, after which it increases again. The frequency at the turning point defines the system's resonance frequency, at which the magnitude of the impedance is mostly dependent on damping properties of the system. At lower frequencies the skin mainly acts like a spring element causing the impedance to decrease with increasing frequency. At higher frequencies, at which the mass element do minates, it will increase with the frequency. 23 Figure 6 shows separate impedance curves referring to two test points (LI and L10, see Fig. 7) and an average curve referring to all testpoints (L1-L10). As can be seen there are small differences between testpoints as regards the skin as a damper and a mass element (right limbs of the curves). More pronounced regional différencies were found regarding the skin's spring like properties (left limbs of the curves), i.e. the skin of the fingers showed a higher stiffness than the palmar skin. -1 0 UJ - 3 0 L1-10 L 10 -40 < - 5 0 -60 -70 FREQ U EN CY, Hz Figure 6. Mechanical impedance for the skin and the hand-arm system. DX and DZ are hand-arm impedance curves deriv ed by Dieckmann (1984). The lower triplet of graphs show the hand's skin impedance for two individual test-points (LI and L10, 6 subjects pooled) and an average for all points (Ll-10, 6 subjects pooled). The locations of the testpoints are indicated in Figure 7. As a consequence, the resonant frequency varied between test points which are illustrated in Figure 7. The highest and the lowest resonance frequencies, ca 80 and 200 Hz were found at the distal part of the finger and at the thenar eminence, respecti vely. No difference between male and female subjects could be discerned. 24 m oo|?o & *00&|Sd * co 0 °*°j® * o£ I 20 50 100 200 Frequency, Figure 7. 500 1k 2k Hz Mechanical resonant frequencies at different skin areas on the hand's inside for three female ( ) and three male ( O ) subjects. Straight lines indicate the position of an average resonant frequency. A biodynamic model of the hand-arm system has recently been constructed, based on impedance data from several investiga tions. This work has been initiated by the member bodies within ISO TC 108/SC 4/WG 5 (Biodynamic modelling) and carried out by Dieckmann (1984). The results from these calculations are shown in Figure 6 for two directions (DX and DZ ) together with the present results on the impedance data for the skin. It may be noted that for frequencies above ca 100 Hz the shapes of the impedance graphs of the hand-arm system as a whole and of the skin show some similarities. For this reason, it seems resonable to assume that the mechanical impedance of the total skin area in contact with a handle is close to the impedance of the whole hand-arm system. This supports the idea that vibrational ener gies of frequencies above ca 100 Hz is mostly dissipated into the skin and the superficial tissues of the hand. 25 3.2. Neural responses to vibration (II) The purpose of this part of the study was to investigate syste matically the responses in tactile afferent units innervating the glabrous skin of the human hand to vibrations with different frequencies and amplitudes (II). Single unit nerve impulses were recorded from adult human sub jects using the microneurographic technique developed by Vallbo and Hagbarth (1968). The tungsten needle electrodes were insert ed in the median nerve on the upper arm about 10 cm proximal to the elbow. The recording electrode was then adjusted manually in small steps until the activity from a single unit could be dis tinguished from the background activity of the nerve fascicle. After isolation and classification of the unit sinusoidal skin displacement were delivered through a small perspex probe (dia meter: 6-8 mm) (Westling et.al. driven by 1976). a feed-back moving coil A set of computer stimulator controlled test sequencies, consisting of sinusoidal vibrations with different frequences (0.5-400 Hz), and amplitudes (0.001-1 mm, peak to peak) were executed. The neural responses, expressed as the average number of impulses elicited per sine wave cycle and de noted as the cycle response, were then analysed. It was showed that the cycle response varied with unit type, frequency and amplitude of the stimuli (Figure 8). The FA II units were most easily excited at high frequencies, above ca 50 Hz. At 256 Hz the stimulus amplitude, required for evoking a discharge, could be as low as 1 ,pmp_p, which corresponds to a vibration acceleration amplitude of ca 0.9 m/s^(rms). The FA I units were most easily excited at frequencies between ca 8 and 50 Hz. The lowest amplitude, ca 8 ^imp_p(0.11 m/s2(rms)) for evoking discharges were at 32 Hz. A striking finding as re gards both FA I and II units was that the frequency range, with in which the units could be excited, quite rapidly increased with the stimulus amplitude. Furthermore, as the stimulus ampli tude was increased the frequency for the maximal cycle response decreased. 26 7 FA FA SA 6 SA 5 4 3 2 1 0 2n 1 0 0.5 2 8 32 128 512 S i n e w a v e f r e q u e n c y , Hz Figure 8. Relation between the sine wave frequency and the aver age number of impulses described per sine wave cycle for the four types of mechanoreceptive afferent units innervating the glabrous shin of the human hand. Pool ed data from 8 FA 1, 4 FA II, 4 SA I and 8 SA II units. The graphs represent two different stimulus amplitudes; 500 and 62 ^im (peak). For more details, see text. 27 The slowly adapting units were particularly sensitive at low stimulus frequencies. As to the SA I units they could be excited most easily within the frequency range of 2-32 Hz and especially at 8 Hz with an absolute threshold of about 16 jamp_p (0.014 m/s^ (rms)). The SA II-units were most easily excited at frequencies below ca 8 Hz, with the maximal cycle response at the lowest test frequencies. As can be seen in the upper graph of Fig. 8, at high stimulus amplitudes, well above the thresholds of the units, a big over lap existed between FA I, FA II and SA units, with regard to the frequency ranges at which they responded. Therefore it was con cluded that a selective stimulation of a particular type of unit by selecting its "best" frequency is mulus amplitude is fairly only possiblewhen the sti close to the absolute thresholds of that unit population. 3.3. Neural sensitivity to vibrating edge contours (III) The handle of a vibrating order to avoid slipping with tool is often grooved basically in a risk for accidents as a con sequence. It is therefore of great interest to know how rough and/or grooved surfaces influence the ability of the tactile units to respond to stimuli. Thus, the purpose of the third paper (III) was to examine whether edges of vibrating objects in contact with the skin can alter the response patterns compared to flat surfaces. By using similar experimental technique as previously described (paper II) the cycle response from FA I and SA I units to sinus oidal skin displacements were studied with regard to two diffe rent locations of the stimulus probe in relation to the recep tive field (Fig. 9A): (1) The stimulus probe completely covered the receptive field (No edge condition); and (2) the probe covered only a part of the receptive field (Edge condition), i.e. the edge of the probe cuts through the field. The outcome showed that the FA I units, and especially the SA I units, responded more readily during the edge condition (Figure 28 9 A ) . The edge response was stronger within the entire ranges of stimulus frequencies and amplitudes studied and this tended to be most pronounced at lower stimulus frequencies. Furthermore, for most units the threshold was lower in the edge condition. This implies, as schematically illustrated in Figure 9 B, that afferent signals from the population of FA I and SA I units con tains distinct information about the presence and location of edge contours. A similar edge enhancement has also been demon strated for the SA I units in the monkey (Phillips and Johnson 1981). Hence, mechanisms accounting for enhancement of spatial contrast and our distinct awareness of edges of objects seem to occur not only in the central nervous system due to lateral NO EDGE □ p ro b e ■ r e c e p tiv e fie ld Skin in d en ta tio n EDGE ) t /\/\/\/\/V_ /\/\/\/\/\_ 4 A fferen t S tim u lu s A fferen t Ï Perip h eral nerve R esp on se m a g n itu d e Figure 9. Edge sensitivity of SA I units. (A) The afferent res ponses are shown for two different stimulus conditions (Edge and No edge condition). The skin displacements were mechanical sinusoids (16 Hz, 0.2 mm (peak) super imposed on a static preindentation (0.5 mm). The dia meter of the flat stimulus probe was 6 mm. As can be seen a stronger response is obtained when the edge of the stimulus probe cuts the receptive field. (B) A schematic illustration of the response profile when a group of SA I units is stimulated by a block indenta tion. A marked edge enhancement would appear. (Adapted from Johansson and Vallbo, 1983). 29 inhibitory mechanisms as earlier proposed (Bekesy 1967), also at the level of the tactile afferent units. Moreover, but it may be inferred that the vibration transferred to the hand via a grooved handle on a vibrating tool will account for a stronger excitation of the type I units than would be the case with a smooth handle provided that the intensity of the vibration would be the same. 3.4. Acute impairment of tactile sensibility following vibration exposure (IV) The use of vibratory hand-held tools is known to cause an acute, but reversible, impairment of the tactile sensibility as mea sured by psychophysical methods (cf. 1.5)*. A fundamental quest ion adressed in the present study is whether the tactile loss is caused by an influence on mechanoreceptive afferents. If not, it has to be explained by processes taking place within the central nervous system. The microneurographic method previously mentioned was used to study the thresholds of tactile units in the glabrous skin be fore, and after a powerful vibration exposure (IV). Correspond ing psychophysical threshold determinations were also carried out while recording the responses from single afferent units. Three frequencies were used during the threshold determinations, 2, 20 and 200 Hz. They were chosen on the basis of their capa city to activate preferentially SA, FA I and FA II units, res pectively. The unit thresholds, as well as the psychophysical thresholds were determined by a modified version of a threshold tracking method first introduced by von Bekesy (1939). The ex perimenter or the subject, respectively, exerted control of the stimulus amplitude through a pushbutton, making it to decrease while the button was pressed and to increase while the button was released. During the determination of the unit threshold, the experimenter pressed the button as soon as one nerve impulse was evoked per sine wave cycle and released it again as soon as the impulse discharge disapperared, i.e. the atunal interval (cf. Talbot et. al. 1968) was tracked. During the psychophysical 30 measurements the subject was asked to press the button as soon as a vibration could be perceived (upper limen) and not release it until the sensation of movement completely disappeared (lower limen). The perception threshold and the unit threshold were defined as the midpoint of the atunal interval and the midpoint between the upper and lower limen, respectively. The frequency of the vibration exposure was either 2, 20 or 200 Hz and the amplitude was preset to 1.5, 1 and 0.25 mm(p_p) at these frequencies, respectively. These amplitudes, which cor respond to accelerations of ca 0.08, 5.6 and 139 m/s^ (rms), were approximately 20-40 dB above the normal perception thresh old at 2 and 20 Hz and 30-50 dB above it at 200 Hz. The results showed that the vibration exposure could account for an acute depression of the sensitivity of tactile units. This depression was temporary and the recovery rate was initially rapid but it declined with time. With the exposure parameters used in the present study, a nearly complete recovery took place within one or a few minutes. Moreover, the magnitudes and the time courses of the recovery of the encountered neural threshold shifts were approximately the same as observed for the corres ponding psychophysical threshold shifts, provided that the threshold was measured at the appropriate frequency, i.e. 200 Hz, 20 Hz and 2 Hz for the FA II, FA I and SA I units, respec tively. Thus, it was concluded that the acute impairments of the tactile sensibility caused by vibration exposure as observed in psychophysical studies most likely is explained by an influence on the peripheral nervous system, excitability of the tactile units. i.e. by a depression of the 31 3.5. Effects of high frequency vibration on tactile perception threshold (V, VI) In the light of the suspicions that hand-transmitted vibrations at frequencies above 1 kHz may have a detrimental effect on man's peripheral nervous system, (cf. 1.6) two groups, one con sisting of dentists (V) and the other of therapists (VI), using high-speed handpieces and ultrasonic therapy devices, respecti vely, were tested with regard to the vibration perception in their hands. For the sake of comparison two reference groups, one for the dentists and the other for the therapists, were also examined. The experimental procedure and apparatus used in the study of dentists respective therapists were quite similar except that two different methods for perception threshold determination were used. The first, used on dentists (V), was based on a continous in crease of the vibration amplitude (10 dB/s) up to the perception level. As soon as the vibration was perceived the subject pres sed a remote control button after which the amplitude momentari ly dropped to zero and started to increase again when the button was released. The perception threshold was defined as the ampli tude at which the button was pressed. Ten test frequencies with in the range of 40 to 400 Hz were used. At each frequency six to eight separate threshold determinations were made and their average formed the basis for the data analysis. The second method used on therapists (VI) was based on the same technique as used in paper IV. In this study twenty test frequencies with in the range of 5 to 400 Hz were used, each during a period of twenty seconds. For the dentists a 3 dB higher vibration amplitude in average was needed for perception for the exposed hand compared to the other. For the corresponding controls no difference between hands could be found. When comparing the non-exposed hands of either group somewhat higher perception thresholds, ca 3 dB, were observed among the dentists, probably due to an age diffe rence of 7 years between the tested groups (cf. Verrillo 1980). An analysis of variance (Anova, SPSS) has recently been carried out as regards the threshold data with respect to both groups 32 (Table 4) . As can be seen a significant difference between controls and dentists (Source: G) and between hands existed (Source: H ) . There was also a significant interplay between groups and hands (Source: G by H) implying that the significant difference between hands could have existed within any of the tested groups. However, as no difference was found between the hands of the controls it was concluded that the difference be tween hands has to be within the group of dentists. For the therapists a slightly higher perception threshold was seen for their right hands compared to their left hands (all therapists were right-handed). The difference was about 2-6 dB and most pronounced at test frequencies above 40-50 Hz. As ex pected there was no or little difference between hands of the reference group. It could, however, be established that two thirds of the therapists also used their left hands on tiring occations why both hands have to be considered as exposed to vibrations. This might explain the slight difference of about 1-2 dB between the therapists left hand and the hands of the reference group. Thus, it seems likely that the reduction in tactile sensibility has developed due to contact with ultrasonic devices. It can be concluded, that both the dentists and the therapists showed a reduced tactile sensibility for vibration exposed hands. Even if these reductions in sensitivity have to be con sidered as quite small, they support the idea that vibrations with high frequencies (> 1 kHz) have a detrimental effect on man. Table <N VO CO 00 CO VO II) 00 (Tv (Error 4. Analysis of variance (Anova SPSS) of the vibrotactile threshold data for individual test frequencies with respect to dentists and corresponding controls. Effect names: G (Group), S (Subject) and H (Hand). For further information, see text. 33 34 6. While DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS using entering the vibratory hand-arm hand-held tools system has all to be mechanical transmitted energy through, and/or dissipated in, the glabrous skin in contact with the vib rating handle. In the hope of getting closer to an understanding of the pathophysiological mechanisms behind the development of the vibration syndrome, the present study has been addressed to problems related to the interplay between the vibrating sur face and the mechanical properties of the skin as well as the responses properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors while exposed to vibratory stimuli. Furthermore, possible deleterious effects on tactile sensibility at long-term exposure to vibrations at frequencies above 1 kHz were also considered, i.e. at fre quencies usually not felt. From the frequency spectras presented in Figure 1, it is clear that many vibratory hand-held tools used in industry have fre quency components within the range of 40 to 160 Hz, i.e. within or close to the range where the skin's resonant frequencies are to be found (80-200 Hz) (I). During excitation at resonances the skin is fairly easy to make vibrate, why strong mechanical loads, such as compressive and tensile strain, will appear. Therefore, such tools may have particularly harmful mechanical influence on the skin tissues. Moreover, the extremly low psychophysical threshold to vibration within this frequency range (Verrillo 1980, Lidström et. al. 1982, Löfvenberg and Johansson 1984) indicate that tactile afferent units are easily excited by the vibrations produced by such tools. From a compa rison between the vibrational intensities produced by the tools and the sensitivity characteristics and densities of tactile units in the glabrous skin, it can readily be inferred that a huge number of mechanoreceptive afferents are strongly excited. Thus, it is not surprising that these tools evoke intense vibra tory sensations. While directly recording the signals in the tactile units, it appears that the 4 unit types exhibit diffe rent response characteristics to vibrations (II). An interesting thing is, that one type of units, the FA II units connected to Pacinian corpuscles, is particularly easy to excite within approximately the same frequencies as where the resonance fre 35 quencies of the skin are found. Indeed, experiments with isola ted Pacinian corpuscles (from the cat's mesentery) have shown that the corpuscle respond to vibration within a wide range, but the optimal frequency being between 150-300 Hz depending on the temperature (Sato 1961). Thus, under the assumption that the Pacinian corpuscles of the mesentery and those of the glabrous skin have similar properties, the findings suggest an almost perfect impedance match between the skin tissue and the relevant receptors in the high frequency range. In contrast to this, the mechanical impedance of the skin is high at the frequency ranges in which the FA I units (ca 8-50 Hz), the SA I units (ca. 4-16 Hz) and the SA II units (< 8 Hz) are most easily excited. Their regions of best sensitivity must therefore be the results of mechanical properties of the receptors themselves, and not the transmitting properties of the skin (cf. Talbot et.al. 1968). Moreover, due to the considerable overlap of the frequency ranges at which the units respond at higher stimulus amplitudes, it can be inferred that all types of mechanoreceptive afferents most likely will be excited while using most vibratory hand-held tools. It has been shown that vibration causes a vasoconstrictive reflex among intact blood vessels which is most easily provoked within the frequency range of 60-200 Hz (Färkkilä and Pyykkö 1979, Welsh 1980), i.e. within the range where the FA II units are most sensitive. Pyykkö and Hyvärinen (1973) have suggested that this reflex is sympathetically triggered by afferent acti vity from the Pacinian corpuscles, i.e. the FA II units. A repetetive activation of these vasoconstrictive reflexes, like during long-term daily use of vibratory tools, probably produces a hyperthrophy of the muscle layers of the vessel walls which makes them react more strongly to the sympathetic drive (Ashe et.al. 1962). At this stage an exposition to vibration, noise and/or particularly to cold, accounting for a vasoconstriction, the level for the critical opening pressure of the vessel may be more easily reached, triggering a strong vasoconstrictive attack, such as a VWF attack. Hence, it seems likely that the FA II units may play an important role in phatophysiology of VWF. For more detailed information about this hypothesis, the reader may consult a recent review (Pyykkö et. al. 1982). 36 The present result provide evidence that the temporary losses in tactile sensibility appearing after vibration exposure, pre viously shown in psychophysical studies, to a large extent de pends on a depression of the excitability of the tactile units. (For a brief discussion of possible mechanisms, see paper IV). During the post-exposure depression of the sensibility, the control of the precise movements of the hand may be impaired together with disturbancies in the regulation of grip forces (cf. Johansson and Westling 1984). An impaired control of the hand is not only a serious handicap in most activities, but may also imply increased risks of accidents. Consequently, it cannot be excluded that the depression of the tactile sensibility occuring after work with a vibrating tool account for many acute injuries usually not connected to the vibration, e.g. knife cuts due to grip slip. The risk for such injuries may be particular ly pronounced during tasks requiring alternations between use of non-vibrating (knives, hatches, screw drivers etc) and vibrating tools. Most previous studies concerning temporary or permanent losses in vibrotactile sensibility have appearently been limited to frequencies above 40-50 Hz, i.e. frequencies normally detected by the FA II units (e.g. Wedell and Cummings 1938, Hahn 1966, Gescheider and Wright 1969, Streeter 1970, Tominaga 1973, Lid ström et.al. 1982, Lundström and Lindmark 1982). Possible im pairments of the sensibility at lower frequency detected by FA I and SA units has thus been neglected. The present findings have showed that these units also can be depressed by a vibration exposure. Vibrotactile measurements as an index for vibration damages should therefore include test frequencies not only focussing on the FA II units, but also on other types of mechanoreceptive units, i.e. they should be carried out at fre quencies below ca 50 Hz as well. The present results provide evidence that the distinct awareness about the presence and location of edge contours in contact with the skin may be due to a pronounced edge sensitivity of the FA I and particularly the SA I units (III). Thus, a marked edge en hancement seems to occur already at the level of afferent response patterns of the type I units and not only due to late 37 ral inhibitory processes within the central nervous system (Bekesy 1967). It is tempting to speculate that grooved handles improve the accuracy in the precision control of hand-held tools. In addition, such handles increase the frictional force against the skin which, in turn, reduces the squeeze force need ed for a firm grip. Consequently, with a vibrating tool, the dissipation of vibrational energy into the hand may be lowered, at least for vibration components above ca 100 Hz (cf. Mishoe and Suggs 1977). On the other hand, a more vigorous response from the type I units may increase the risk of a more rapid sen sitivity depression (IV). At the present stage, it is difficult to argue whether this negative effect overweights the advantages of a grooved handle. Finally, the present findings support the idea that a long-term exposure to very high-frequency vibrations (> 1 kHz) may also have an untoward effect on man's tactile sensibility. However, the sensory loss seems to be quite small and the affected per sons probably fully adapt to the loss. This might explain that non within the exposed groups had any subjective complaints indicating an injury. The mechanisms behind the observed sensory losses are not at all clear. According to the guidelines given in ISO/DIS 5349.2 (1984) man's hand and arm is considered to have an uniform sensibility be tween 16 to 1250 Hz when the vibration amplitude is expressed in velocity and very little attention is paid to vibration at higher frequencies (cf. 1.2). Considering the present results it seems resonable to conclude that there are strong reasons for changing the shape of the frequency weighting network. In the author's opinion, the frequency ranges for low mechanical impe dances of the skin (ca 80-200 Hz) and for high tactile sensiti vity (ca 100-300 Hz) should be taken into more serious conside ration. Moreover, further studies concerning the exposure to vibration at high frequencies (> 1kHz) are also of utmost necessity to obtain reliable basic data for setting the upper limiting frequency of future guidelines. 38 7. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work has been carried out at the Department of Environmen tal Medicine, University of Umeå, in cooperation with The Swe dish National Board of Occupational Safety and Health, Technical unit in Umeå, and with the Department of Physiology, University of Umeå. I express my most sincere gratitude to these depart ments for providing the outmost satisfactory facilities at my disposal and to all employers who in many ways have contributed to this thesis. I wish to thank Prof. Axel Ahlmark and my supervisor Doc. Ludwik Liszka for their guidance, support and interest throughout this work. Special thanks are due to Doc. Roland Johansson, for his super vision, guidance and for many fruitful discussions within the fascinating field of neurophysiology. Special thanks are also due to Mrs Asta Lindmark and Mr Bertil Nordström for their technical assistance. Excellent typing work has been done by Miss Barbro Johansson. This work has financially been supported by grants from the Swe dish Work Environmental Fund (Project ASF 78-156 and ASF 79-104) and is greatly acknowledged. Finally, I .am greatly indebted to my wife, Agnetha, and my children, Anders and Anna, for their support, encouragement and patience throughout this work. 39 8. REFERENCES ABRAMS, Jr. C.F. and SUGGS,. C.W. (1970) Mechanical impedance simulation of vibrational characteristics of the human system. American hand-arm Society of Agricultural Engineers, Paper No. 70-523, 1-10. AGATE, J.N. and DRUETT, H.A. (1947) A study of portable vib rating tools in relation to the clinical effects which they produce. British Journal of Industrial Medicine. 4, 141-163. ANDERSSON, R., BERGSTRÖM, B., DANDANELL, R., GRAVE, B., MAGNUSSON, L., NILSSON, L. ENGSTRÖM, K., and SJÖSTRAND, O. (1983) Undersökning av vibrationsmiljön och skaderisker vid arbete med tandläkarborr med höga varvtal och ultraljudverktyg. SAAB SCANIA, TH 83:64. (In S w e dish) . ASHE, W.F., COOK, W.T. and OLD, J.W. (1962) Raynaud's pheno menon of occupational origin. Archives of Environmen tal Health, 5, 333-343. BANNISTER, P.A. and SMITH, F.V. (1972) Vibration-induced white fingers and manipulative dexterity. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 29, 264-267. BEKESY, v o n G. (1939) Ü b e r d i e V Zeitschrift, 4, 316-334. BEKESY, von G. i b r a t i o n s e m p f i n d u n g . Akustische (1967) Sensory inhibition, Princeton University Press, Princeton. BRAMMER, A.J. (1982) Relations between vibration exposure and the development of the vibration syndrome. In Vibration effects on the hand and arm in industry. (Eds.) Brammer, A.J. and Taylor, W. JohnWiley and Sons, New York, 283290. 40 BYSTRÖM, B.-O., NILSSON, A. and OLSSON, E. (1982) Development of artificai hands for use in chain saw vibration measure ment. Journal of Sound and Vibration, 82(1), 111-117. DART, E.E. (1946) Effects of high speed vibrating tools on operators engaged in the airplane industry. Occupational Medicine, 1, 515-550. DANDANELL, R. (1984) Vibration measurements and analysis of per cussion tools such as riveting hammers and bucking bars. United Kingdom Informal group on human response to vibra tion, Edinburgh, 21-22 September 1984, 13-23. DIECKMAN, D. (1958) Ein mechanisches Modell für das Schwingungs erregte Hand-Arm-System des Menschen. Internationale Zeitschrift für Angewandte Physiologie, 17, 125-132. DIECKMAN, D. (1984) Draft proposal on the impedance and analogue model of the hand-arm system. International Standardiza tion organization, ISO/TC 108/SC4/WG5, N 32. FÄRKKILÄ, M. and PYYKKÖ, I. (1979) Blood flow in the contra lateral hand during vibration and hand grip contractions of lumberjacks. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 5, 368-374. FÄRKKILÄ, M.A., PYYKKÖ, I., STARCK, J.P. and KORHONEN, O.S. (1982) Hand grip force and muscle fatigue in the etio logy of the vibration syndrome. In Vibration Effects on the Hand and Arm in Industry. (Eds.) Brammer, A.J. and Taylor, W» John Wiley and Sons, New York 1982., 45-50. GESCHEIDER, G .A. and WRIGHT, J.H. (1969) Effects of vibrotactile adaptation on the perception of stimuli of varied inten sity. Journal of Experimental Phychology, 81,449-453. GEMNE, G. and TAYLOR, W (Eds.) (1983) Hand-arm vibration and the central autonomic nervous system. Journal of Low Fre quency Noise and Vibration (A special volume). 41 GRIFFIN, M.J. (1980) Vibration injuries of the hand and arm: their occurrence and the evolution of standards and limits. Health and Safety Executive, Research Paper 9, 1-36. GUIGNARD, J.C. (1979) Evaluation of exposures to vibrations. In Patty's Industrial Hygiene and Toxicology, Vol. III. (Eds.) Cralley, L.V. and Cralley, L.J. John Wiley and Sons, New York, 465-524. GURDJIAN, E.S. and WALKER, L.E. (1945) Traumatic vasospastic disease of the hand. Journal of the American Medical Association, 129, 668-672. HAHN, J.F. (1966) Vibrotactile adaptation and recovery measured by two methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 655-658. HAMILTON, A. (1918) A study of spastic anemia in the hands of stonecutters. U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labour Statistics, Bulletin 236 (Industrial Accidents and Hy giene Series, No. 19), Washington, D.C. 53-66. HYVÄRINEN, J., PYYKKÖ, I. and SUNDBERG, S. (1973) Vibration fre quencies and amplitudes in the aetiology of traumatic vasospastic disease. Lancet,.April 14, 791-794. ISO/DIS 5349.2 (1984) Guidelines for the measurement and the assessment of human exposure to hand-transmitted vibra tion. JOHANSSON, R.S. (1978) Tactile sensibility in the human hand: receptive field characteristics of mechanoreceptive units in the glabrous skin area. Journal of Physiology (Lon don), 281, 101-123. JOHANSSON, R.S. and VALLBO, Å.B. (1979) Tactile sensibility in the human hand: relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in the glabrous skin. Journal of Physiology. (London), 286, 283-300. 42 JOHANSSON, R. S. and VALLBO, Å.B. (1980) Spatial properties of the population of mechanoreceptive units in the glabrous skin of the human hand. Brain Research, 184, 353-366. JOHANSSON, R.S., LANDSTRÖM, U. and LUNDSTRÖM, R. (1982 a) Res ponses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements. Brain Research, 244, 17-25. JOHANSSON, R.S., LANDSTRÖM, U. and LUNDSTRÖM, R. (1982 b) Sensi tivity to edges of mechanoreceptive afferent units inner vating hand. Brain Re the glabrous skin of the human search, 244, 27-32. JOHANSSON, R.S. and VALLBO, in the Å.B. (1983) Tactile sensory coding glabrous skin of the human hand. Trends in Neuro- Sciences, 6(1), 27-32. JOHANSSON, R.S. and WESTLING, G. (1984) Influences of cutaneous sensory input on the motor coordination during precision manipulation. In Somatosensory mechanisms. (Eds.) Euler, C., Franzén, O., Lindblom, Macmillan Press, London, 249-259. KNIBESTÖL, M. (1973) Stimulus-response U. and Ottoson, functions of M. (1975) Stimulus-response functions adapting mechanoreceptors in the human area. Journal of Physiology , 2 54, 63-80. D. rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors in the human glabrous area. Journal of Physiology, 2 32, 427-452. KNIBESTÖL, von of glabrous skin slowly skin KNIBESTÖL, M. and VALLBO, Å.B. (1970) Single unit analysis of mechanoreceptor activity from the human glabrous skin. Acta Physiologcia Scandinavica, 80, 178-195. KUMLIN, T. and SEPPÄLÄINEN, A.M. (1969) Neurophysiological studies of workers using vibrating tools. XVI Interna tional Congress on Occupational Health, Tokyo, 149-150. 43 KUMLIN, T., WIIKERI, M. changes in carpal SUMARI, P. (1973) Radiological metacarpal bones and phalanges and and caused by chain saw vibration. British Journal of Indus trial Medicine, 30, 71-73. LIDSTRÖM, I.-M., HAGELTHORN, G. and BJERKER, N. (1982) Vibration perception in persons not previously exposed to local vibration and in vibration-exposed workers. In Vibration Effects on the hand and arm in industry. (Eds.) Brammer, A .J . and Taylor, W. John Wiley and Sons, New York, 59-65. LORIGA, G. (1911) II labora coir martelli pneumatici. Boll. Ispett, lavoro 2, 35. LUNDSTRÖM, R. and LISZKA, L. (1979) High frequency vibrationsultrasound in hand-held tools. The Swedish National Board of Occupational Safety and Health, Investigation report 1979:8, 1-27 (In Swedish). LUNDSTRÖM, R. and LINDMARK, A. (1982) Effects of local vibration on tactile perception in the hands of dentists. Journal of Low Frequency Noise and Vibration, 1(1), 1-11. LUNDSTRÖM, R. (1984) Local vibrations-mechanical the human hand's glabrous nics, 17(2), 137-144. LUNDSTRÖM, R. and BURSTRÖM, L. skin. Journal (1984) Vibrations impedance of of Biomecha in hand-held tools. The Swedish National Board of Occupational Safety and Health, Investigation report 1984:24, 1-111. (In Swedish with an english summary). LUNDSTRÖM, R. (1984) Effects of local vibration transmitted from ultrasonic devices on vibrotactile perception hands of therapists. Ergonomics (In press). in the 44 LUNDSTRÖM, R. and JOHANSSON, R.S. (1985) Acute impairment of sensitivity of skin mechanoreceptive units caused by vib ration exposure of the hand. Ergonomics (Submitted for publication). LÖFVENBERG, J. and JOHANSSON, R.S. (1984) Regional differences and interindividual variability in sensitivity to vibra tion in the glabrous skin of the human hand. Brain Re search, 301, 65-72. MAGOS, L. and OKOS, G. (1963) Raynaud's phenomenon. Archives of Environmental Health, 7, 341-345. MISHOE, J.W. and SUGGS, C.W. (1977) Hand-arm vibration, Part II. Vibrational responses of the human hand. Journal of Sound and Vibration. 53(4), 545-558. MOBERG, E. (1962) Critisism and study of methods for examining sensibility in the hand, Neurology, 12, 8-19. NISHIYAMA, K. and WATANABE, S (1981) Temporary threshold shift of vibratory sensation after clasping vibrating handle. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 49, 21-33. PHILLIPS, J.R. and JOHNSON, K.H. (1981) Tactile spatial resolu tion. III. A continuum mechanics model of skin predicting mechanoreceptor responses to bars, edges, and gratings. Journal of Neurophysiology, 46, 1204-1225. POTTS, R.O., CHRISMAN, Jr. D.A. and BURAS, Jr. E.M. (1983) The dynamic mechanical properties of human skin in vivo. Journal of Biomechanics, 16(6), 365-372. PYYKKÖ, I. (1974) The prevalence and symtoms of traumatic vaso spastic disease among lumberjacks in Finland. A field study. Scandinavian Health, 10, 36-47. Journal of Work, Environment & 45 PYYKKÖ, I. and HYVÄRINEN, J. (1973) The physiological basis of the traumatic vasospastic disease: a sympathetic vasocontrictor reflex triggered, by high frequency vibration? Work Environmental Health, 10, 36-47. PYYKKÖ, I. HYVÄRINEN, J. and FÄRKKILÄ, M. (1982) Studies on the etiological mechanism of the vasospastic component of the vibration syndrome. In Vibration effects on the hand and arm in industry. (Eds) Brammer, A.J. and Taylor, W. John Wiley and Sons, New York 1982, 14-23. RAYNAUD, M. (1862) Local Asphyxia and Symmetrical Gangrene of the extremities. M.d. Thesis, Paris. REYNOLDS, D.D. Part I. and KEITH, Analytical R.H. model (1977)‘ Hand-arm of the characteristics of the hand. Journal Vibration, 51(2), 237-253. REYNOLDS, D.D. Part hand and ANGEVINE, E.N. (1977) vibration, vibration of response Sound and Hand-arm vibration, II: Vibration transmission characteristics of the and arm. Journal of Sound and Vibration. 51(2), 255-265. SATO, M. (1961) Response of Pacinian corpuscles to sinusoidal vibration. Journal of Physiology, 159, 391-409. SEPPÄLÄINEN, A.M. (197 2) Peripheral neuropathy in forest workers A field study. Work, Environment and Health, 9, 106-111. STARCK, J. (1984) High impulse acceleration levels in hand-held vibratory tools. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environ ment & Health, 10, 171-178. STREETER, H. (197 0) Effects of localized vibration on the human tactile sense. American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, Jan-Feb, 87-91. 46 STEWART, A.M. (1970) Vibration Syndrome. British and GODA, D.F. Journal of Industrial Medicine 27, 19-27. SUGGS, C.W. (1970) Vibration of machine handles and controls and propagation through the hands and arms. 4th International Ergonomics Congress, 6-10 July, France. TALBOT, W.H., DARIAN-SMITH, I., KORNHUBER, H.H. and MOUNTCASTLE, V.B. (1968) The sense of flutter-vibration:comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. Journal of Neuro physiology, 31, 301-334. TAYLOR, W. (1974) The vibration syndrome. Academic Press, don, New York. TAYLOR, W. and PELMEAR, P.L. (1975) Vibration white finger in industry. Academic Press, London, New York, San Francisco. Lon TAYLOR, W. and BRAMMER, A.J. (1982) Vibration effects on the hand and arm in industry: An introduction and review. In Vibra tion effects on the hand and arm in industry. (Eds.) Brammer A.J. and Taylor, W. John Wiley and Sons: New York. TOMINAGA, Y. (1973) Effects of localized vibration of the vibra tory sense at the fingertips. Journal of Science of La bour, 49, 17-25. VALLBO, A.B. and HAGBARTH, K.-E. (1968) Activity from skin mecha noreceptors recorded percutaneously in awake human sub jects. Experimental Neurology, 21, 2 70-289. VALLBO, Å.B. and JOHANSSON, R.S. (1978) The tactile sensory innervation of the glabrous skin of the human hand. (Ed) In Active touch. (Ed.) Gordon, G., Pergamon Press, Oxford, 29-54. VALLBO, A.B. and JOHANSSON, R.S. (1984) Properties of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the human hand related to touch sen sation. Human Neurobiology 3, 3-14. 47 VERRILLO, R.T. (1980) Age related changes in the sensitivity to vibration. Journal of Gerontology, 35, 185-193. VERRILLO, R.T. and SCHMIEDT, mulatory R.A. (1974) Vibrotactile poststi shift. Bullentin of the Psychonomic threshold Society, 4, 484-486. WEDELL, C.H. and CUMMINGS, Jr. S.B. (1938) Fatigue of the vibra tory sense. Journal of Experimental Phychology, 22, 429438. WELSH, C.L. (1980) The effect of vibration on digital blood flow. British Journal of Surgery, 67, 708-710. WESTLING, G., JOHANSSON, R.S. and VALLBO, Å.B. (1976) A method for mechanical stimulation of skin receptors. In: Sen sory functions of the skin. mon Press, Oxford, 151-158. WOOD, L.A., SUGGS, vibration, C.W. Part and ABRAMS, III: A (Ed.) Zotterman, Y., Perga Jr. C.F. distributed (1978) Hand-arm parameter dynamic model of the human hand-arm system. Journal of Sound and and Vibration, 57(2), 157-169. ZAVERI, K. (197 4) Measurement of the dynamic mass of the handsystem. Brüel & Kjaer Technical Review, No. 3, 26-36. UMEÅ UNIVERSITY MEDICAL DISSERTATIONS N ew Series N o 136 - ISSN 0346-6612 Editor: the Dean of the Faculty of Medicine Centraltryckeriet Umeå, 1985
© Copyright 2024