Orthographic Awareness and Metacognitive Management thereof. Including Marginalised Aspects of Literacy Instruction:

Including Marginalised Aspects of Literacy Instruction:
Orthographic Awareness and
Metacognitive Management thereof.
Dr Susan Galletly B.Sp.Thy, Grad Dip Teach, M.Ed., PhD
CQUniversity & Education Queensland
Email: [email protected]
Include & Impact
2011 LSTAQ/LDA/SPELD Conference
Brisbane 16-17 September 2011
Please read our theorising on Orthographic Advantage
Theory and Transition-from-Early-to-Sophisticated Literacy
Do discuss it with your peers: It’s either rubbish, or
possibly this century’s biggest paradigm shift !




Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2004). The high cost of
orthographic disadvantage. Australian Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 9, 4-11.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2011). Transition from Early to
Sophisticated Literacy (TESL) as a factor in cross-national
achievement differences. Australian Educational Researcher.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (In press b). Because trucks aren‟t
bicycles: Orthographic complexity as an important variable in
reading research. Australian Educational Researcher.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2011). A theory of differential
disadvantage of Anglophone weak readers with language and
cognitive processing weakness. Australasian Journal of Special
Education.
PART 1: THE THEORY
Q: Do we need to leverage literacy development?
A: YES!
What’s the evidence?

100%
Lev5
80%
Lev4
60%
Lev3
40%
Lev2
20%
Lev1
Germany
Taiwan
Switzerland
Belgium
Estonia
Poland
Sweden
Netherlands
Australia
Liechtenstein
NZ
Ireland
Canada
Hong Kong
Finland
0%
Korea
% of cohort at each level

Schools & Ed associations calling for higher literacy
resourcing to reduce proportions of lower achievers.
Internationally we do well, but still >> low achievers,
e.g., compare Australia with Korea &Finland in PISA 2006
<Lev1
Q: What’s Orthographic Awareness? A: Awareness/use of spelling patterns: Spelling,
Orthographic Awareness & Reading Accuracy relate reciprocally to boost each other.
Key instructional implication: Build them together in a co-ordinated manner!
Orthographic
Awareness
(Knowledge
& Skills)
Q: What’s Orthographic Advantage Theory?
Step 1. Dream a little! Imagine if…



Every child had parents who read well & love
reading.
All children had proficient reading accuracy &
spelling in 4 to 18mths, i.e. Most by End-Yr1?
No students above Grade 2 had reading
accuracy or spelling difficulties:
• Even students with intellectual challenge.
Q: What’s Orthographic Advantage Theory?
Step 1. Dream a little! What if…

Every child had parents who
read well & love reading.

Step 2. Stop dreaming!
 That’s reality in
All children had proficient
transparent reading accuracy & spelling in
4 to 18mths, i.e. Most by
orthography
nations.
End-Yr1?


No students above Grade 2
had reading accuracy or
spelling difficulties:
• Even students with
intellectual challenge.
This creates the reality of
massive differences in
resourcing needs between
Anglophone and transparent
orthography nations.
It can be easy to learn to read & write all words?
How quickly could you learn to read & write using my
‘Fleksispel’: A 44 rule English orthography
Wuns upon u tiem thair wer three litul pigz
hou livd in a smorl hows with thair muthu.
Wun dae muthu pig sed tou her noysee childrun,
“It’s tiem for you tou bild yor own howsuz.”
Soe of thae went.
Thu ferst litul pig met u man with a loed of stror.
“Pleez cood I hav sum ov yor stror,”
he arskt poelietlee.
“Sertunlee you fien yung pig,” sed thu man,
and hee gave thu litul pig az much hae az he wontud.
- Self-teaching: The key gift of regular orthographies:
Wuns you no the GPCs, you can reed & riet orl werds.
-
- While our students are caught in lengthy early literacy,
other nations’ students are flying in sophisticated literacy.
- English orthographic complexity is extremely high
(? too high).
So is ‘the high cost of orthographic disadvantage!
The importance of Orthographic Advantage Theory is revealed when
English is compared with transparent orthographies.
Slow progress by all readers
Readers at end of Year 1 (& 2):
Seymour, Aro and Erskine (2003)

Transparent orthographies
>90% accuracy:
 Norwegian Dutch Icelandic
Swedish Spanish Italian
Finnish Turkish German
Greek

Less transparent
orthographies >70%
accuracy:
 French Danish Portuguese

Relatively opaque English
34% accuracy
 34% accuracy in Year 1
 76% accuracy a year later
Greater weakness in delayed readers
Landerl, Wimmer, & Frith (1997)

English & German weak readers:
 English 16 times > vowel errors.
 German readers hardest words
(3 syllable pseudowords) better than
English readers easiest words
(1 syllable real words)
Huge difference in effectiveness of
remediation & no. of „Treatment
Resisters‟
(See bibliography: Torgesen, Vellutino, vs.
Cossu, Schneider, Oloffson)
 Virtually no Treatment Resisters in
transparent orthography nations: even
severely disabled children develop
fluent spelling & reading accuracy.
 Many (?most) Anglophone students
with literacy weakness don‟t reach
effortless fluency (esp in spelling).
 A large number make little progress
despite extensive intervention
Nations differ in their valuing of
orthographic awareness



Korea: High valuing & awareness
Finland: Low valuing & awareness
Australia: Low valuing & awareness
SOPHISTICATED LITERACY INSTRUCTION
100%
Lev5
Lev5
English
Lev4
Korean
Lev3
80%
Lev4
Korean
60%
Lev3
40%
EARLY
Lev2
LIT’CY
INSTR’N
20%
Lev2
Lev1
Lev1
ermany
10 yrs
Taiwan
tzerland
<Lev1
elgium
Estonia
?8+/-0.5yrs
ermany
Poland
Taiwan
Sweden
5 yrs
tzerland
erlands
Estonia
tenstein
Poland
NZ
Sweden
Ireland
erlands
Canada
ustralia
g Kong
tenstein
Finland
NZ
Korea
elgium
ustralia
0 yrs
0%
Ireland
English
15 yrs
<Lev1
Korea shows high awareness of the impacts of orthographic
complexity, having resolved orthographic complexity,
with massive improvements at minimal expense.


Hangul Day celebrates the bringing of literacy to the people.
Centuries ago, King Sejong the Great demanded a simple system for
writing spoken Korean so everyone could express their thoughts in
writing. Finding nothing satisfactory in other nations, they made up
their own set of symbols (Initially 11 Vs & 17 Cs, simplified in 1933
to 10 Vs & 14 Cs). Since the end of Japanese occupation, Hangul is
the nation‟s sole writing system.
“South Korea illustrates the pace of progress that is possible…. Two
generations ago, the country had the economic output of
Afghanistan today and ranked 24th in education output among the
current 30 OECD countries. Today, South Korea is the world's top
performer in secondary school graduation rates (93%).
• Schleicher and Stewart (2008, p.45), in Galletly & Knight (In press)
Korea was ‘Australia’ but with far less funds. Look at it now!
Perhaps AUSTRALIA should focus > on Korea & < on Finland.
Canada is the highest achieving Anglophone nation.
NB Multilingualism seems to increase Orthographic Advantage.
FINLAND shows low awareness. This is not a problem for them,
though we get naive comments about ‘The Finnish Advantage’


Orthographic advantage is not currently recognised widely, e.g.,
it is not yet used as a variable by PISA analysts, who tout the
Finnish advantage as
• PISA 2006: FREEDOM of curriculum, assessment, and
enquiry; EQUITY of learning opportunities; SAFETY; and
TEACHER EDUCATION (Arinen, 2008).
• PISA 2011: The Finnish Advantage: GOOD TEACHERS!
From Orthographic Advantage perspectives, this is naive:
• Finnish teachers have it easy:
 All students fluent readers and writers from early Grade 2.
 Homogenous high school classes without disengaged
struggling readers.
• In contrast, Australian teachers are excellent teachers:
despite classes loaded with disengaged, reluctant readers, our
teachers do so well that our high achievers pull our average
up to 9th place in PISA reading, despite our >30% of
students scoring poorly.
AUSTRALIA shows low awareness, which hugely affects us via:
1. Suboptimal resourcing.
2. Suboptimal instructional emphases.
A: English readers have multifaceted
‘Orthographic Disadvantage’ (Galletly & Knight, 2004)
Compared to transparent orthography nations, we have

Slower development of reading & writing.

Delayed language development due to delayed equalisation of print:
oral vocabularies.

Delayed maturation of cognitive processing (phonemic & orthographic
awareness).

Delayed „full‟ emphasis on Sophisticated Literacy

High heterogeneity of middle & upper school classes, due to many
older students having reading & writing difficulties.

The secondary impacts of enduring literacy low achievement, including
disengagement, social-behaviour difficulties, and anxiety/depression.

Work-place illiteracy, with its dangers and expense.

Generational disadvantage: parents unable to read to their children.

Extensive, expensive, ineffective remediation.

Polarised achievement in international comparisons (lots of high
achievers but also lots of low achievers.

Low equity: strong linkage of SES & literacy achievement.
Orthographic advantage seems equally strong in
nations using transparent transitional orthographies!
Nations which first teach a transparent orthography
before moving to a more complex one, also get
Orthographic Advantage, e.g.,

Taiwanese students have ceiling level phonemic
awareness after 10 weeks of instruction. This is likely to
expedite learning of logographic Kangi.
Should we be using a transparent transitional
orthography?


Certainly our indigenous communities should be
learning to read & write their fully transparent
orthographies, rather than working with English first.
Orthographic Advantage Theory
(Galletly & Knight, 2004, 2011a, 2011b, In press)

The regularity of the Orthography which a nation chooses to use
strongly influences key aspects of nationwide literacy including
• The Ease and Expense of successful Early Literacy instruction.
• The richness of home literacy pre- and during school years.
• The complexity of teaching circumstances across school years:
 Including the proportion of students with
• Weak literacy
• Weak language skills.
• Disengagement & behaviour difficulties.
• High needs for adult support and low adult: child ratios.
• Rates of adult & workplace literacy difficulties.
• Likelihood of success of reforms to improve reading.
Q. What’s ‘Transition from Early to Sophisticated Literacy’ (TESL) paradigm?
(Galletly & Knight, 2011a, 2011b, In press)

‘First we learn to read & write then we read & write to learn’’
(Chall, 1989)



Stage 1: Early Literacy Development
• Learning to Read & Write –strong emphasises on Text Decoder skills.
Stage 2: Sophisticated Literacy Development
• Reading and Writing to Learn (and communicate fluently)
The Transition from Early to Sophisticated Literacy
• Is strongly impacted by Orthographic Advantage/Awareness.
• Has VIP implications for ease of instruction.
SOPHISTICATED LITERACY INSTRUCTION
English
Korean
Korean
English
EARLY
LIT’CY
INSTR’N
0 yrs
5 yrs
?8+/-0.5yrs
10 yrs
15 yrs
3 TESL (Transition from Early to Sophisticated Literacy) groups
Ease of
Transition
1. RAPID TRANSITION
a. Resolved
b. Facilitated
Rapid
Rapid
Easy
Easy
2. COMPLEX
TRANSITION
Slow
Complex
Orthography
type
Highly regular
orthography
A transitional highly
regular orthography
Less regular GPC
orthographies.
No. of GPCs
Usually <50 GPCs
~1: 1 correspondence
Usually <50 GPCs.
1: 1 correspondence.
>500GPCs
Many: many GPCs
Nations using Korea. Most European China, Japan, Taiwan. Anglophone nations.
this paradigm
nations.
Thailand.
Complexity of
Low
Low
V. High
instruction
Transitional used with
Only a very small
Words read
All texts read.
logographs, so:
proportion of
By endGr1
All words written.
All texts read.
all words.
All words written.
Ease for selfHigh.
High.
Low
teaching
Length of
Brief: <<1yr for most Brief, <<1yr for most. Lengthy: 3 yrs to all
Early Literacy
school years.
3 TESL (Transition from Early to Sophisticated Literacy) groups
1. RAPID TRANSITION
a. Resolved
b. Facilitated
Ease of
v. successful
Early Literacy
Very easy
Very easy
2. COMPLEX
TRANSITION
Very hard
Very high due to
Expense of
Very low due to
limited self-teaching
Very low due to
v. successful
self- teaching
hence needs for low
self- teaching
Early Literacy
adult: child ratios
LOW: >Homogenous LOW:>Homogenous HIGH:>Helerogenous
Complexity of
literacy needs, &
literacy needs, &
literacy needs, &
Teaching
probably less
probably less
probably less
Circumstances
disengagement &
disengagement &
disengagement &
behaviour problems
behaviour problems behaviour problems
Likelihood of
Low:
High: Simpler
educational
High: Simpler teaching
Teaching
teaching
reforms being
circumstances.
circumstances v.
circumstances.
successful
Literacy needs
Complex.
Literacy needs
homogenous & focused
Literacy needs v.
homogenous &
on sophisticated literacy
Hetergenous, & for
focused on
sophisticated literacy Early & Sophis’d lit’y
Q. Did we misinterpret Freire? A: Yes!





Freire expertly used Effective Principles of Instruction on all Dimensions of Learning to
take Brazilian peasants from print illiteracy to being literate.
He built Attitudes & Perceptions (DoL1. He also built mature orthographic awareness of
all the orthographic content needed for Brazilian literacy (Letter sounds, & sounding-out)
- he taught „Learning to Read & Write‟ using drills with decontextualised words (Dol2-3).
This took an extremely short time, because the orthography is transparent.
He then used „Reading & Writing to Learn‟ to continue Social Emancipation.
We only noticed the empowerment. We didn‟t notice the careful explicit instruction using
decontextualised words and word parts, until students were sufficiently competent to
move on to independent reading & writing.
If applying Freire to Australia, we should focus on Orthographic Awareness as well as
empowerment.
D1 Attitudes
& Perceptions
Acquire &
Integrate
New
Knowledge
D2
D5 Habits of Mind
Extending &
Refining
Knowledge
D3
Knowledge Development
Using
Knowledge
Meaningfully
D4
Q. What are the key aspects of change & instruction
building Orthographic Awareness?
At systemic & national levels:

Gather more data: Go and See! Send teachers to observe transparent
orthography classrooms in Korea, Finland, Wales, Estonia, Italy, etc. It is
teachers, not academics, who are likely to notice pivotal instructional &
achievement differences.




Study the literacy development of bilingual groups using a highly
transparent orthography, e.g. NZ Mauri, Australian indigenous languages.
Study the changes in literacy achievement in nations which have changed
their orthography in order to have orthographic advantage, e.g., Korea,
some Russian nations.
Consider the high costs of orthographic advantage & whether we can afford
this cost, given it is a sociocultural choice our government has made (albeit
unwittingly.
In indigenous communities, change to bilingual education teaching reading &
spelling of their first language first (Transparent orthographies from Wycliffe Bible
Translators)
PART 2: Ideas for Practice
Key instructional implication: Build them together in a co-ordinated manner!
Orthographic
Awareness
(Knowledge
& Skills)
Why be metacognitive about Orthographic Awareness?




All 5 Dimensions of Learning (Marzano & Pickering) are VIP.
For complex learning areas, basic learning (DoLs 2 & 3) is not sufficient.
Metacognition (DoLs 4 & 5: higher-order learning) builds student ownership.
Being metacognitive (using a framework of why, when, how) re Orthographic
Awareness expands your teaching/learning zone (Vygotsky‟s ZPG)
D1
D5
Attitudes &
Perceptions
Acquire &
Integrate
New
Knowledge
D2
Habits of Mind
Extending &
Refining
Knowledge
D3
Knowledge Development
Using
Knowledge
Meaningfully
D4
Are we emphasising Orthographic Awareness enough? What’s
the fall-out of insufficient emphasis?


Current curricula tend to assume Orthographic
Awareness is needed only wrt Spelling, and
little attention is focussed on optimising
reading-accuracy.
It‟s likely Spelling, Reading & Orthographic
Awareness would be best improved by
• A concentrated co-ordinated school-wide
focus simultaneously building
 Reading-accuracy,
 Spelling and
 Orthographic awareness
It’s time to embrace reading of decontextualised words!




additives additionally activities additional actually addition
When we paid for the dog, we additionally bought a bed, food & toys.
We‟ve avoided reading decontextualised words. It‟s time to stop!
Reading decontextualised words build orthographic awareness and
spelling far more effectively than reading in context, as it‟s fully
dependent on orthographic awareness.
Message 1: Keep major foci on reading contextualised texts.
Message 2: Add reading of decontextualised words.
Why isn‟t context enough to support reading progress?
• Orthographic awareness empowers reading of decontextualised
words.
• Both empower reading of contextualised text.
• Whereas just over 100 'heavy duty' words (e.g., the, in, was, etc.)
account for around half of all the letter strings appearing in printed
school English, a very large number of words exist which appear very
rarely in print (Carroll, Davies & Richman, 1971; Nagy & Anderson,
1984).
• In fact fully eighty percent of English words occur less than once
in a million words of running text (Carroll et al, 1971).
(Share & Stanovich, 1995, p.15)
Principles for building Orthographic Awareness
Close the Print-verbal Vocabulary Gap:


De-emphasise correct spelling in first-draft writing. Focus on
„Getting your fine mind down on paper.‟
Build fluent writing of spelling approximations (“Brave
Spelling”= Phonemic Equivalents), e.g. use Guestimating
(Galletly, 2001) as an easy way to write unfamiliar words).

Use scaffolded & unscaffolded reading texts for reading
development.

Use scaffolded reading texts for KLA work.
(Guestimating Fun, pp.107-109 of Galletly, S. 2001 Two Vowels Talking)
Steps for writing big words

Teach the 3 rules
• If you can say it on your fingers, you can write it.
• Say every syllable as you write it.
• Every syllable has a vowel. (Which one: I don‟t care. Step 1 is to
get vowel spacers into multisyllabic words – dinuso animil
Q. Principles for building Orthographic Awareness
•
Be aware that orthographic skill is evidenced & developed in
1.Spelling & spelling errors. 2. Word reading. 3.Discussing
orthography, e.g., letters, sounds, GPCs.
•
Build skill in integrated instruction building reading accuracy,
spelling & orthographic awareness in tandem, e.g. „Words their
Way‟ seems a useful tool towards this end.
•
Consider teaching reading of a transitional orthography to bring
phonemic awareness to a ceiling level, early in literacy
development.


E.g., Teach & practice Flexispel, then use it in scaffolding
vocabulary in texts:
photosynthesis (fo-to-sin-thu-sus), antique (an-teek),
E.g., let‟s build national pride in use of an Indigenous language
(perhaps Koori) used to label artifacts, etc. in the same manor
that NZ uses English & Mauri (a transparent orthography)
Q. Principles for building Orthographic Awareness
•
•
Decontextualised words help: No context so must rely on orthography
Will building „deeper‟ reading accuracy build orthographic awareness and
therefore spelling?

Rapid Reads

Repeated reading of texts with errors on changed words.

Build orthographic awareness using Spelling Grids
1C Sausages
A lady went into a butcher shop
complaining abowt the sausages
she had just bought.
‘The middal is meat,” she
exclaimed, ‘but the ends are
sawdust!’
‘Well,’ said the butcher, ‘theze days
it’s hard to make ends meat.’
excellently
interesting
confident
perfectly
sure
important
1
2
1D Sausages
A lady went into a butcher shop
complaining about the sossages
she had jest bought.
‘The middle is meat,” she
exclaimed, ‘but the ends are
saudust!’
‘Well,’ said the bootcher, ‘these
days it’s hard to make ends meat.’
3
4
5
6
7
8
explaining
believed
think
agree
convincing
show
A
finnally
thrid
wy
furst
because
another
secand
reezin
B
finally
thurd
wie
first
becors
anuther
seccond
reasen
need
example
amazingly
asking
final
very
C
finilly
therd
why
fearst
becorse
anothor
second
reeson
extreme
firmly
disagree
definitely
opinions
should
D
finelly
third
hwy
ferst
becuase
annother
seccand
reason
firstly
because
why
second
reasonable
third
Ones that tricked
Will building metacognition about reading accuracy, spelling, &
orthographic awareness boost all 3 areas? Yes!
Orthographic
Awareness
(Knowledge
& Skills)
Emphasise 20 English Vowel Sounds
5 Vowel sounds
ă
bat
ĕ
bet
ĭ
bit
ŏ
bot
ŭ
but
5 Vowel NAMES
ā
mate
ē
Pete
5
ar
tar
or
for
ī
bite
R
ō
hope
ū
tube
Vowels
er
her
air
hair
ear
dear
5 other Vowels
Would you boys show/er?
oo1
good
oo2
food
oi
boil
ow
cow
schwa (ə)
again tiger
Building Early Years’ Orthographic Awareness


Teach letter name categories:
•
Sound at start of name (B K Z),
•
Sound at end (F M X),
•
Sound not in name (H Y W).
Teach that all letters have 5 concepts

1. A Name,

2. Sound/s,

3. A Capital,

4. A Lowercase,

5. You find it in words.
Emphasise three grain sizes in
words & syllables:
1. Regular words (phonemes):
• Letter-sound knowledge - know 5 to 7 letters (name, sound, capital, lowercase,
find/hear it in words)..
• Early phonological awareness - blending 3 sound words, onset-rime listing,
isolating initial/final/vowel sounds.
2. Pattern words (rimes):
• Build fluent rhyming (rapid rhyming of real and nonsense words sam-bam-lamtam-wam)
• Match written pattern words (rhyming words)
3. Whole words
•
•
•
•
Word constancy - know 5 sight words (Family name snap, preschool pets, etc)
Notice them in books and games.
Use the vocabulary during reading activities and book reading (listed above).
These skill are developed through play activities, with no need for drill.

Teach words having 3 grain-size categories:
•
Explore how words move from being Tricky to being Pattern/Regular
words as Orthographic Awareness builds.
Build Metacognitive Orthographic Awareness
•
•
•
•
•
•
Find the ways (GPCs) we write our commonest vowel sound (the
schwa: Ə, as in tiger virus began moutain).
We have 6 signatures of Shakespeare – each spelled differently.
Today when I mark the roll, reply by spelling your name a new way.
Find how many ways we write /or/ in different words.( >>10).
Explore the ≥ 20 common vowel sounds of Australian English
Learn Dr G‟s 2 rulz of Inglish speling:

Inglish spelling iz syllee kumpaired to sensubul speling nashuns!
• = I‟m not stupid. English spelling is!

Inglish speling iz fassinayting!

= You‟ll never master English spelling if you‟re not
interested in it. Be fascinated. Keep finding more exciting
patterns.

Find spelling rules vs exception words using a convention
of „If a pattern is in ≥ words, it‟s a rule.‟
Run a Middle School (Y5-9) unit building from Dr G‟s rulz, using
Flexispel‟s stages, exploring the history of English writing &
orthography, and discovering and mapping rules & exceptions.
Bibliography











Aro, M. (2004). Learning to read: The effect of orthography. Jyvaskyla, Finland:
University of Jyvaskyla.
Bryson, B. (1990). Mother tongue. London: Penguin Books.
Chall, J. S. (1989). Learning to read: The great debate 20 years later - a
response to "Debunking the great phonics myth". Phi Delta Kappa, 70, 521-538.
Cossu, G. (1993). When reading is acquired but phonemic awareness is not: A
study of literacy in Down's syndrome. Cognition, 46, 129-138.
Cossu, G. (1999). The acquisition of Italian orthography. In M. Harris & G.
Hatano (Eds.), Learning to reading and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp.
10-34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Galletly, S. A. (2004). Reading accuracy and phonological recoding: Poor
relations no longer. In B. Knight & W. Scott (Eds.), Learning Disabilities:
Multiple Perspectives. Melbourne: Pearson Education Australia.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2004). The high cost of orthographic
disadvantage. Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 9, 4-11.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2011a). Transition from Early to
Sophisticated Literacy (TESL) as a factor in cross-national achievement
differences. Australian Educational Researcher.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (2011b). A theory of differential
disadvantage of Anglophone weak readers with language and cognitive
processing weakness. Australasian Journal of Special Education.
Galletly, S. A., & Knight, B. A. (In press b). Because trucks aren’t
bicycles: Orthographic complexity as an important variable in reading
research. Australian Educational Researcher.
Galletly, S. A., Knight, B. A., Dekkers, J., & Galletly, T. A. (In press).
Indicators of late emerging reading-accuracy difficulties in Australian
schools. Australian Journal of Teacher Education.
Bibliography (continued)










Galletly, S. A. (1999). Sounds & Vowels: Keys to literacy progress. Mackay, Qld,
Australia: Literacy Plus.
Galletly, S. A. (2001). Two vowels talking: Keys to literacy progress. Mackay,
Australia: Literacy Plus.
Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2000). Working memory deficits in children
with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. The British
Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 177-194.
Goswami, U. C. (2002). Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: A crosslinguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 141-163.
Hanley, J. R., Masterson, J., Spencer, L. H., & Evans, D. (2004). How long do the
advantages of learning to read a transparent orthography last? An investigation
of the reading skills and incidence of dyslexia in Welsh children at 10-years of
age. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 1393-1410.
Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading
achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
34, 401-413.
Jimenez, J. E., Siegel, L. S., & Lopez, M. R. (2003). The relationship between IQ
and reading disabilities in English-speaking Canadian and Spanish children.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 15-23.
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H. C. A., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic
consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315-334.
Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading
disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211-224.
Luke, A., & Freebody, P. (2000). Further Notes on the Four Resources Model.
Reading Online. Downloaded on 27 Feb 2009 from
www.readingonline.org/research/lukefreebody.html.
Bibliography (continued)









Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Erskine, J. M., Guttorm, T., Laakso, M.-L., et al. (2004).
The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Annals
of Dyslexia, 54, 184-220.
Olofsson, A., & Niedersoe, J. (1999). Early language development and kindergarten
phonological awareness as predictors of reading problems: From 3 to 11 years of age.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 464-472.
Poskiparta, E., Neimi, P., & Vauras, M. (1999). Who benefits from training in linguistic
awareness in the first grade, and what components show training effects? Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 32, 437-447.
Schneider, W., Ennemoser, M., Roth, E., & Kuspert, P. (1999). Kindergarten prevention of
dyslexia: Does training in phonological awareness work for everybody? Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 32, 429-442.
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in
European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174.
Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading:
The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15,
55 - 65.
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., Sipay, E. R., Small, S. G., Pratt, A., Chen, R., et al.
(1996). Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers:
Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits
as basic causes of specific reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 601638.
Venezky, R. (2004). In search of the perfect orthography. Written Language & Literacy, 7,
39–63.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. C. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and
skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological
Bulletin, 131, 3-29.