WZB-Postprints Jianghong Li, Sarah E. Johnson, Wen-Jui Han, Sonia Andrews, Garth Kendall, Lyndall Strazdins, Alfred Dockery Parents‘ Nonstandard Work Schedules and Child Wellbeing A Critical Review of the Literature Suggested citation: Li, Jianghong/Johnson, Sarah E./Han, Wen-Jui/Andrews, Sonia/Kendall, Garth/Strazdins, Lyndall/Dockery, Alfred (2013): Parents‘ Nonstandard Work Schedules and Child Wellbeing. A Critical Review of the Literature, online: http://www.wzb.eu/sites/default/files/publikationen/postprints/li_parents_nonstandar d_work_schedules_and_child_wellbeing.pdf The article has been published online first in The Journal of Primary Prevention: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10935-013-0318-z. Parents' nonstandard work schedules and child wellbeing: A critical review of the literature Jianghong Liabc, Sarah E Johnsonab, Wen-Jui Hand, Sonia Andrewse, Garth Kendallbf, Lyndall Strazdinsg and Alfred Dockerl aCurtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Centre for Population Health Research, Curtin University, PO Box 855, West Perth, Western Australia, 6872, Australia. bTelethon Institute for Child Health Research, Centre for Child Health Research, The University ofWestern Australia, Perth, Australia. cWZB Berlin Social Research Center, Reichpietschufer 50, 10785 Berlin, Germany. dSilver School of Social Work, New York University, 1 Washington Square North, New York, NY 10003. eCurtin Business School, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987, Perth, Western Australia, 6845, Australia. fSchool of Nursing and Midwifery, Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987, Perth, Western Australia, 6845, Australia. gNational Centre for Epidemiology and Population Health, College of Medicine, Biology & Environment, Building 62, M Block, The Australian National University, ACT, Australia. Corresponding author: Jianghong Li Senior Researcher WZB Berlin Social Research Center (Wissenschaftszentmm Berlin fur Sozialforschung g GmbH: www.wzb.eu) Reichpietschufer 50 10785 Berlin, Germany Tel: +49 30 254 91 564 Fax: +49 30 254 91 562 Email: j ianghong.li@wzb. eu 1 Parents' nonstandard work schedules and child well-being: A critical review of the literature 2 ABSTRACT This paper provides a comprehensive review of empirical evidence linking parental nonstandard work schedules to four main child developmental outcomes: internalizing and externalizing problems, cognitive development, and body mass index. We evaluated the studies based on theory and methodological rigor (longitudinal data, representative samples, consideration of selection and information bias, confounders, moderators, and mediators). Of 23 studies published between 1980 and 2012 that met the selection criteria, 21 reported significant associations between nonstandard work schedules and an adverse child developmental outcome. The associations were partially mediated through parental depressive symptoms, low quality parenting, reduced child-parent interaction and closeness, and a less supportive home environment. These associations were more pronounced in disadvantaged families and when parents worked such schedules full time. We discuss the nuance, strengths, and limitations of the existing studies, and propose recommendations for future research. Keywords: Child mental health, Child obesity, Cognitive development, Nonstandard work schedules, Parental employment, Shift work 3 INTRODUCTION Around the world, many societies are transitioning from industrial and post-industrial economies to service economies, which Presser (2003, pp. 64-65) calls the "24/7 economy." Accompanying this economy is a demand for services around the clock, which has driven a rise in work schedules in evenings, nights, and weekends (so called "nonstandard schedules"). Research to date has documented a high prevalence of nonstandard (NS) work schedules in developed economies (ABS, 2009; McMenamin, 2007; Presser, 2003; Presser, Gornick, & Parashar, 2008; Williams, 2008), particularly among parents (ABS, 2009; Presser, 2003). This labor market trend has raised concerns about its potential impact on children's well-being. The influence ofNS work schedules on children's health and development is an important issue for social, economic and workplace policy. Future economic prosperity and social cohesion are contingent on all children having optimal physical and mental health and the capacity to participate fully in the workplace and society. If there were convincing evidence that children's health and development is adversely influenced by parents' work schedules, it would strengthen the case for improving work conditions and for family friendly workplace reform. Such evidence would also make a strong argument for appropriate income support and child care provision for families with children. The purpose of this paper is to provide a critical review and assessment of the evidence for the influence of parents' NS work schedules on their children's well-being. We confine the review to research on developed countries, discuss policy implications of the evidence, and offer directions for future research in this field. Definition and Prevalence ofNS Work Schedules 4 The definition ofNS work schedules varies across studies and countries, but essentially refers to schedules in which the majority of work hours fall outside a typical daytime Monday to Friday work week. In general, NS schedules include evenings, nights, rotating shifts (i.e., alternating between day, evening, or night shifts, but on a fixed schedule), split shifts, irregular hours, and regular weekend work. Based on United States (US) data from the Work Schedules and Work at Home Survey a supplement to the Current Population Survey in 2004, about 18% of all employed wage and salary workers ( 19% of men and 16% of women) reported a work shift for their primary job that fell outside of a usual daytime schedule (between 6am and 6pm) (McMenamin, 2007). The prevalence ofNS work schedules is much higher among African Americans (23%), part-time workers (29%), and workers employed in the service sector (36%) (McMenamin, 2007). NS work schedules are also prevalent in other developed economies but, due to different definitions, their prevalence may not be directly compared across countries. In 2005, about 28% of Canadian workers worked a NS schedule, the vast majority of whom were full-time shift workers (Williams, 2008). Between 2001 and 2004, about 43% of Australian workers regularly worked some form ofNS schedule, including weekends (Dockery, Li, & Kendall, 2009). Within Europe, the prevalence of weekday shift work varied widely across countries during 2005, from 15% in Luxembourg to 30% in the United Kingdom. The prevalence of usual weekend shift work ranged from 10% in Sweden to 34% in Italy (Presser et al., 2008). Parents with young children tend to be more likely to work NS schedules due to child care needs or costs, or because parents wish to maximize their time with children while undertaking the employment by 'shift' or 'tag-team' parenting (Barnett & Gareis, 2007; Garey, 1999; Han, 2004; Hattery, 2001; Presser, 2003; Wight, Raley, & Bianchi, 2008). In 2004, approximately 30% of working American parents (both men and women) with children 5 under 18 usually worked on weekends rather than typical weekdays (McMenamin, 2007). Based on the 2004 US Current Population Survey (US Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census, US Department ofLabor, & Bureau ofLabor Statistics, 2011) about 40% of mothers working NS schedules reported child care as the main reason for working NS schedules during the week (authors' own calculation). Australian Census data reveal that in 2007, in almost 60% of couples with children, either one or both parents typically worked some hours between 7pm and 7am (ABS, 2009). CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK In this section we discuss theoretical perspectives that help explain the potential effect of parental work schedules on children's well-being. We consider these theories to address the following three critical questions: 1) Why NS work schedules might influence children's development; 2) the plausible mechanisms (mediators) of this relationship; and 3) whether this relationship is moderated by characteristics of the child and family. Figure 1 illustrates the broad conceptual framework we developed from the relevant theoretical and empirical literature to guide this review. Throughout the discussions about the influence ofNS work schedules on children, standard (weekday, daytime) schedules are used as the comparison group. See Figure 1 (Appendix) Ecological Theory Bronfenbrenner's ecological theory (1979) conceptualizes child development as occurring within nested settings. The microsystems (e.g., family, school, childcare center) are the immediate settings in which a child is active, and are influenced by the mesosystems, namely the interrelationships between microsystems. Children and their immediate settings 6 sit, in turn, within the exosystem that arguably includes the parental workplace, and they all are situated within the context of the wider society and culture, the "macrosystem." Renamed a bioecological theory (Bronfenbrenner, 2005), it has since been extended to highlight the importance of genetic and other physiological characteristics and the continuous reciprocal interaction that takes place between the person and environment over time. Conceptual Resource Framework Following Bronfenbrenner, Brooks-Gunn and her colleagues (Brooks-Gunn, Brown, Duncan, & Moore, 1995) have operationalized the bioecological model in terms of familial and extra-familial resources and have developed a conceptual resource framework that integrates multidisciplinary perspectives (e.g., economists, sociologists, social demographers, developmental and clinical psychologists, and pediatricians). In broad terms, four categories of familial resources are thought to be critical for parenting and early socialization. These include income, time, human capital (e.g., parental education, together with special skills, training, and other characteristics), and psychological capital (e.g., the mental health of the parents, the quality of their relationships, the psychological importance to them of factors such as education and work, and beliefs about the parental role in childrearing). Extrafamilial resources include child care settings, schools, peer groups, community, and wider social contexts (Kendall & Li, 2005). If the family engages with these community resources appropriately, they constitute social capital, another important resource for children's development. Brooks-Gunn and co-authors also focused on the issue of decision-making and the choices parents face about allocating limited resources (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1995). They later acknowledged that the conceptual framework did not account for the development of the human capital of the child, the continuous reciprocal interaction ofwhich Bronfenbrenner spoke that explains why children are often resilient within the context of a poor or 7 dysfunctional family (Brooks-Gunn, 1995). Optimal child health and development is, therefore, a function of the quantity, quality and mix of familial and extra familial resources, the decisions parents make regarding the allocation of their resources, and the characteristics of the children themselves. Based on bioecological theory and the conceptual resource framework, we have devised the following model to guide our review (Figure 1), showing the key concepts, their indicators, and linking paths. Structural factors that have contributed to the emergence of the 24/7 economy (Path A) are technological and demographic change, globalization, and labor market deregulation (Dockery et al., 2009; Presser, 1999, 2003; Strazdins, Korda, Lim, Broom, & D' Souza, 2004). Based on bioecological theory, we view the workplace as an important part of the exosystem within which children grow and develop. Hence, parents' NS work schedules, as a distal factor, are likely to influence children's development (Path B). From the point ofview of the conceptual resource framework, parents' NS work schedules may influence their children's development through their impact on familial resources, such as income, parental time available for children, parental physical and psychological wellbeing, and the quality of the marital relationship (Paths C and D). According to the framework, parents who choose to work NS schedules may decide to trade-off income and time with their children for the potentially negative consequences that working NS schedules may bring. Whereas working NS schedules, particularly night and evening shifts, may enable more parent-child time during the day, such schedules can lead to fatigue and stress and hence reduce parents' physical and psychological capacity for providing quality parenting (Heymann, 2000). Similarly, if parents choose to work NS schedules to increase their income, it may mediate a positive effect ofNS schedules on child outcomes; however, physical and psychological tolls associated with working such schedules may offset this effect. NS work schedules also likely influence child development through family processes, 8 such as parenting, parental-child relationship and home environment. The impact ofNS work schedules on children may vary by the developmental age and gender of the child, the gender of parents, and family characteristics (Path E). These moderators may also modify the indirect effect ofNS work schedules on child development via the mediators (Paths F and G). Below we discuss the mediators and moderators in more detail in light of relevant theoretical and empirical literature. Mediators (pathways linking parental NS work schedules to child development) Previous studies have documented associations between working NS schedules and the physical and mental health of workers, including working parents, although results are by no means consistent. NS schedules, especially regular night shifts and rotating shifts, disturb the body's circadian rhythms, alter physiological functions, and potentially lead to chronic health conditions, anxiety, neurotic disorders and depression, and chronic sleep deprivation and fatigue (Barnett, 2006; Kantermann, Juda, Vetter, & Roenneberg, 2010; Totterdell, 2005; Vogel, Braungardt, Meyer, & Schneider, 2012). Working evening or night shifts (but not rotating shifts) has been associated with greater depressive symptoms among mothers and fathers (Perry-Jenkins, Goldberg, Pierce, & Sayer, 2007). Mental and physical health is an important resource for parents because they influence child health and development through their impact on family processes. Fatigue due to sleep deprivation and mental stress associated with working NS schedules can reduce the quality of time spent with children in developmentally important activities, and it can also lower the quality of parenting and the home environment. The stress associated with NS work schedules may adversely affect family dynamics and increase work-family conflict (Barnett, Gareis, & Brennan, 2008; Davis, Goodman, Pirretti, & Almeida, 2008; Liu, Wang, Keesler, 9 & Schneider, 2011) and marital instability, especially in association with night shifts (Davis et al., 2008; Kalil, Ziol-Guest, & Epstein, 2010; Presser, 2003). Some studies have reported that parents who work NS schedules spend more time with their children and are more likely to be present when children return home from school (Wight et al., 2008), but other studies have found that working NS schedules is generally associated with less time spent with children (Connelly & Kimmel, 2011; Rapoport & Le Bourdais, 2008). Further, parents working NS schedules generally spend less time with children in developmentally important activities, such as helping with homework and attending parent-teacher meetings or school plays, than those working standard hours (Wight et al., 2008). Previous research has also shown that, compared to standard work hours, working NS schedules was associated with insensitive and harsh parenting practices (Grzywacz, Daniel, Tucker, Walls, & Leerkes, 2011) and a decrease in the quality of the home environment, especially in low-income families (Heymann & Earle, 2001). Moderators Most developmental perspectives (including bioecological theory and the conceptual resource framework) emphasize that the nature and strength of influences on children's outcomes will depend on the children's age, developmental status, and needs. Attachment, psychoanalytic, and family theorists have emphasized the importance of the parent-child relationship in developing children's trust and a sense of identity, and have drawn attention to the importance of age-related transitions in developmental capabilities (Sroufe &Waters, 1977; Thompson, 2006). Infants and toddlers require a large investment of time and effort from a primary caregiver to meet their physical needs and form a secure attachment. As toddlers, they require constant supervision and activities focused on language development, including reading time with their parents. Parents are invaluable in helping young children to 10 understand and express language, develop a variety of skills, and solve cognitive tasks (Bradley, 2002). Further, parents aid in the development of emotional capacities, such as regulating emotions, dealing positively with frustration, and delaying gratification (Eisenberg & Valiente, 2002). Thus, the early years constitute an important developmental stage for examining the impact ofNS work schedules on children's development due to schedulerelated parental stress and fatigue. During middle childhood and adolescence, parental NS schedules may exert an influence on different developmental domains and through different mechanisms, such as parent-child closeness and supervision. These later years mark a time of important changes related to school entry and transitions, as well as developmental advances that establish children's sense of identity and their relationships with parents and peers (Eccles, 1999). Adolescence is an important developmental stage in which young people begin engaging in risky behaviors. Thus, parental supervision and monitoring may be just as important during these late developmental stages as in early childhood. Given the different developmental needs of boys and girls (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000), the association between parental work schedules and child well-being may vary by child gender. Boys have higher levels of activity than girls but they are less able to regulate attention and control impulses (Else-Quest, Hyde, Goldsmith, & Van Hulle, 2006). They also manifest higher levels of direct aggression, associated with externalizing behavior, poorer peer relations, and lower pro-social behavior than girls (Card, Sawalini, Stucky, & Little, 2008). Thus, boys may be more affected by parental stress associated with NS work schedules than girls. Boys also appear to have more adverse cognitive outcomes than girls if their mothers were employed for more than 30 hours per week while they were infants (Brooks-Gunn, Han, & Waldfogel, 2002). Similarly, heightened sensitivity to increased maternal work hours has been observed in adolescent boys, relative to girls, in low-income 11 families (Gennetian, Lopoo, & London, 2008). Studies of dual-earner families with schoolaged children also suggest a stronger association between parental work demands and poor monitoring for boys than girls (Bumpus, Crouter, & McHale, 1999; Greenberger, O'Neil, & Nagel, 1994). Parental gender adds another dimension to the complex relationship between NS work schedules and child outcomes due to gendered pathways between parenting and child outcomes (Lamb, 2010; Laursen & Collins, 2009; Raley & Bianchi, 2006). Because parents tend to engage in more activities with same-gender children (e.g., fathers and sons), it is likely that fathers' absence due toNS work schedules has a larger detrimental effect on boys than girls. Fathers' long work hours (55 hours or more per week) have been associated with higher levels of externalizing behaviors in boys than girls (Johnson, Li, Kendall, Strazdins, & Jacoby, 2013). The association between NS work schedules and child outcomes may also differ by the gender of parent due to differential sharing of child care and household work responsibilities. Despite increases in the proportion of women entering the labor force, women remain largely responsible for family life (Maume, 2011; Maume & Sebastian, 2012). Women working NS schedules report higher levels of sleep deprivation and work-to-family conflict than their male counterparts (Maume & Sebastian, 2012; Tuttle & Garr, 2012), and work-to-home conflict has a negative effect on marital quality in women but not in men (Maume & Sebastian, 2012). This suggests that maternal NS work schedules may exert a larger effect on the family and children than paternal shift schedules. NS work schedules present parents with both advantages and challenges in balancing work and family demands. Whether or not a parent chooses (or can choose) to work NS schedules is likely to moderate the effect ofNS schedules on both family processes and child outcomes. We know that some parents choose to work such shifts in order to spend the day 12 with their young children (Garey, 1999), whereas for others working non-day shifts is a job requirement (Presser & Cox, 1997). In the former case, any physical or mental stress associated with working non-day shifts might be offset by parents' satisfaction with their ability to spend time with and take care of their children (Garey, 1999). In the latter case, stress, parental depression, and marital instability induced by working NS schedules, as well as the physiological tolls (e.g., fatigue and interrupted sleep patterns) of such schedules, could adversely affect the child's well-being (Heymann, 2000). Family structure and income constitute another potential moderator of the NS work effects on child outcomes. It is well established that socioeconomic disadvantage, such as living in a single-parent, low parental education and low-income family, is associated with poor child health and developmental outcomes (Hertzman, 1999; Keating & Hertzman, 1999). These factors may exacerbate any negative effect ofNS work schedules on child development. Families with more social and economic resources may be better able to cope with challenges presented by NS work schedules and may even benefit from working such schedules, especially when parents chose to work these schedules. In 376 dual-earner middle-class families, Davis, Crouter, and McHale (2006) found higher levels of adolescentreported intimacy with mothers when their mothers worked NS hours, compared to when they worked standard hours, perhaps because these shifts meant that they could spend more quality time with their children. Family characteristics other than income and family structure may also modify the impact ofNS work schedules on family processes and thus on child development. Such characteristics are diverse and are often better captured in qualitative research. Small-scale qualitative studies of nurses suggest that their families adapt toNS work schedules quite well and in some respects they even benefit from shift work. For example, in a study by Barnett and Gareis (2007), 8-14 year old children of mothers working an evening shift as nurses rated 13 their fathers as having greater awareness of their activities and better parenting skills, and they themselves were more likely to disclose information to their fathers. Thompson' s study of night working nurses and their families in the UK also reveals that mother's absence due to work allowed the father to take on more child care and other domestic responsibilities (Thompson, 2009). Thus maternal NS schedules may draw fathers into closer care relationships with children and generate less gender stereotyped family interactions (McHale, Crouter, & Whiteman, 2003). However, to fulfil the gendered expectations to be "good" wives and mothers, the women in the Thompson study attempted to mitigate any potential negative impact of working night shifts on their families, at the expense of their own wellbeing, in the form of significant reductions in their own sleep duration, worse mood, and reduced alertness (Thompson, 2009). This paper represents the first comprehensive and critical assessment of existing research evidence linking parents' NS work schedules to child development. The paper makes three key contributions to the field: development of a new conceptual model to guide the review; a synthesis and critical assessment (based on theory and on multiple methodological criteria) of the diverse research findings on the topic; and theory- and evidence-based recommendations for future research. METHODS This review focused on studies that directly examined the link between parents' NS work schedules and child mental, physical, and cognitive development. The search included peer-reviewed journal articles and books on this topic and was restricted to the literature from English-language sources in developed countries from 1980 to December 2012. We identified the majority of the studies through an electronic search in ProQuest, Web of Knowledge, Science Direct, PsyciNFO, and OVID Medline. 14 We searched for broad key words and their combinations in the title of the article (nonstandard work, nonstandard hours, work schedules, shift work, night work, evening work, and weekend work), together with terms identifying outcomes anywhere in the full text of the article (mental health, behavior, overweight, obesity, body mass index (BMI), cognition, sleep, well-being, and child or adolescent). The initial search produced 364 records (See Figure 2). Using the reference lists of these articles and the web pages of some authors and professional associations to extend the search, we identified an additional three studies. After removing duplicate records, 241 records remained that were potentially relevant. An assessment of the titles and abstracts of these records against the selection criteria (examining a direct link between parental NS schedules and child developmental outcomes) resulted in 24 articles that we read in full. We excluded just one article after assessing the full text of the 24 studies because it did not examine a direct link between work schedules and child outcomes (Barnett & Gareis, 2007), leaving 23 studies in the final review. Many of the excluded articles focused on adult health and well-being outcomes, including sleep disturbance (n = 94), or family outcomes such as time spent with children, parenting quality, parent-child closeness, marital stability, and the home environment (n = 32) in respect toNS work schedules; and some of the other excluded studies focused only on the prevalence and determinants ofNS work schedules. While not reviewed, we discussed the studies that examined the relationship between NS work schedules and family processes in both the introduction and discussion to facilitate our understanding of the pathways through which NS work schedules may influence child development. See Figure 2 (Appendix) We did not use a fixed definition ofNS schedules as a selection criterion as there is no one single definition in the literature and by doing so we would have omitted a significant 15 number of relevant studies. The studies based on US national datasets typically define NS schedules as hours worked outside 6am- 6pm on the main job, with evening shifts sometimes defined as 2pm- 9pm and night shifts as 9pm- 8am (e.g., Han, 2008; Han & Miller, 2009; Han, Miller, & Waldfogel, 2010). Studies based on the Canadian National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth (NLSCY) and the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia Survey (HILDA) defined self-reported worked schedules on the main job, including regular evening shifts, regular night shifts, irregular hours, and split or rotating shifts or weekends, as NS schedules (Dockery et al., 2009; Strazdins, et al., 2004; Strazdins, Clements, Korda, Broom, & D'Souza, 2006). Due to substantial diversity in the way both NS work schedules and child outcome variables were measured (e.g., mental health, behavioral difficulties), we undertook a comprehensive review of the extant published literature rather than conducting a meta-analysis. Evaluation Criteria We employed five methodological criteria to present the findings of the studies reviewed (see Appendix ): (1) sample representativeness, (2) study design (longitudinal vs. cross-sectional, (3) adequate control for a minimum set of socio-demographic characteristics as confounders and covariates, (4) use of analytical methods to address selection bias, and (5) examination of mediating and moderating factors. Study quality ratings, which ranged from 0 to 5, indicated the number of criteria met by the study as determined by the authors. The most important issue we considered in rating the studies was the extent to which studies have adjusted for potential selection bias. That is, we whether the observed associations between NS work schedules and child outcomes could be attributed to other unobserved or omitted factors associated with the likelihood of working NS schedules and having poor (or positive) child outcomes. Comprehensively dealing with selection bias entails adjusting for major 16 known confounders and covariates using longitudinal data and employing such analytical techniques as Ordinary Least Square (OLS) Regression, fixed effects modeling, or propensity score matching. We consider a minimum set of key socio-demographic confounders and covariates to be family structure, parental education and age, the number of parental work hours, child gender and age, and the number of children in the household. Ethnicity was also considered as key covariate to adjust for as appropriate. Some studies were based on a predominantly homogeneous population, making this adjustment unnecessary (Strazdins et al., 2004). In other studies using fixed effects models, all time-invariant covariates, such as ethnicity or race, would have to be omitted. See Table 1 (Appendix) There are other sources of information bias, including self-reported outcome measures, missing cases and loss to follow-up in longitudinal data collection. Because these issues are inherent in non-experimental studies, such as those covered in this review, we did not use these factors as a criterion to rate the studies. We have, however, paid particular attention to research methodology that may also appropriately and adequately address missing information and/or attrition issues. We have also addressed these common methodological issues in the discussion section of this review. RESULTS Overview of the Results The 23 studies that met the inclusion criteria (see Appendix) included 22 peerreviewed journal articles and one book, all based on non-experimental data. Seventeen studies were based on a US sample, two studies used an Australian sample (Champion et al. 2012; Dockery et al., 2009), two analyzed data from a Canadian sample (Strazdins et al., 17 2004, 2006), and there was one study each from the UK (Barton, Aldridge, & Smith, 1998) and Croatia (Radosevic-Vidacek & Koscec, 2004). Eleven studies were cross-sectional and 12 were longitudinal. Several studies were based on the National Longitudinal Study of Youth-Child Supplement (NLSY-CS), a data set that may overrepresent children who were born to young mothers with lower education and income (Chase-Lansdale, Mott, BrooksGunn, & Phillips, 1991). Three studies were based on data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD), which may underrepresent children from disadvantaged families. The age of the children across these studies ranged from birth to 20 years. Twelve studies examined both parents' NS work schedules, ten focused only on mothers' NS work schedules, and one study examined only fathers' work schedules (Barton et al., 1998). Mental health and behavioral problems were the most common type of child outcome examined (15 studies). Four studies focused on cognitive development (Han, 2005; Han & Fox, 2011; Heymann, 2000; Odom, Vernon-Feagans, & Crouters, 2013), three studies examined children's body weight as the outcome (Champion et al., 2012; Miller & Han, 2008; Morrissey, Dunifon, & Kalil, 2011), and one focused on children's sleep patterns (RadosevicVidacek & Koscec, 2004). Two of the 15 studies that examined mental health and behavioral problems also analyzed school engagement and after-school activities as additional outcomes (Han, 2006; Hsueh & Y oshikawa, 2007). The 23 studies provided a range of unstandardized and standardized effect sizes (ES). Wherever possible, a standardized ES was calculated from the published data to facilitate comparisons (see Appendix). Of those that used multiple linear regression techniques, six studies reported standardized beta coefficients(~), which can be interpreted as ES. In the nine studies the authors provided unstandardized beta coefficients (b) and we calculated ES using band standard deviations (SD) of the outcome variable (b/SD). Four studies using logistic 18 regression provided an odds ratio (OR). In two studies where the authors used structural equation models (Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al., 2010), standardized beta coefficients(~) were reported that were equivalent toES. In two studies (Barton et al., 1998; RadosevicVidacek & Koscec, 2004), analyzes of variance (F test) statistics were reported. On the whole, the effect sizes of parental NS schedules on children's behavioral and cognitive outcomes were small by conventional standards, mostly < .20 (Cohen, 1988). The significant effect sizes, however, were larger for preschool-age or younger children (ES = .20 - .35). In studies stratified by indicators of socioeconomic status, effect sizes were larger in low-SES (Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006), low-income (Han, 2008; Han et al., 2010), and single parent families (Dockery et al., 2009; Han & Waldfogel, 2007). Four studies analyzed only low-income or low-wealth samples: two studies examined preschool-age children (Joshi & Bogen, 2007; Odom et al., 2012), with small effect sizes for cognitive ability(< .20) and medium effect sizes (unmediated ES = .36- .55) for behavioral outcomes; and two analyzed school-age children (Dunifon, Kalil, & Bajracharya, 2005; Hsueh & Yoshikawa, 2007), with a small effect size (mostly ES < .1 0). In studies that included both parents, the ES was comparable for maternal and paternal work schedules, although the relative strength of association varied by child age, SES and type ofNS schedule. The ES for BMI was also mostly small,< .20 (Miller & Han, 2008; Morrissey et al., 2011). Miller and Han (2008) found a considerable effect size for the number of years mothers worked a NS shift on child BMI among families in the second income quartile (ES = 0.27). Child Mental Health and Behavioral Problems In this section, we summarize the findings with reference to the conceptual framework presented in Figure 1. Although child age is included as a moderator in our conceptual framework (Figure 1), because few studies had directly tested child age as a 19 moderator and because the majority of the studies analyzed the data separately by child age group, we begin the discussion of the results separately for preschool and school-age children and adolescents. Preschool Children Evidence from both cross-sectional (Gassman-Pines, 2011; Joshi & Bogen, 2007; Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006) and longitudinal studies (Daniel, Grzywacz, Leerkes, Tucker, & Han, 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009) was consistent and suggests that young children with at least one parent who worked NS schedules had more emotional and behavioral problems than those whose parents worked standard schedules. The magnitude of the association was similar for both mothers' and fathers' NS schedules (Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006). Evidence suggests that exposure to parental NS work schedules in the child's first few years of life is particularly detrimental. Mothers' or fathers' NS schedules in their child's infancy were associated with more behavioral problems at ages 2 and 3, as compared to parents with standard schedules (Daniel et al., 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009). Two studies found that evening or night shifts had the strongest and most consistent associations with child behavioral problems, such as excessive fussiness and distractibility, as well as internalizing and externalizing behaviors (Gassman-Pines, 2011; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009). School-Age Children and Adolescents The findings from two longitudinal studies that used representative samples and addressed selection bias reported a significant association between the child's cumulative exposure to parental NS work schedules and mental health and behavioral problems. Han (2008) found that behavioral problems among 4- to 10-year-old children increased with the number of years that mothers had worked a NS schedule. Similarly, Han and Miller (2009) reported that the number of years mothers worked night shifts and fathers worked evening shifts was 20 significantly associated with higher risks of depression in children aged 13 or 14. Han et al. (2010) found that the number of years mothers had worked a night shift was also linked to adolescent smoking, drinking, drug use, delinquency, and sexual activity. Based on a sample of low-income families (primarily single mothers), Hsueh and Yoshikawa (2007) found that 5- to 16-year-old children whose primary caregiver worked variable NS schedules had more teacher-reported externalizing behaviors but fewer parent-reported internalizing behaviors than children whose caregivers did not work such hours. In contrast, Dunifon et al. (2005) reported no association between parental NS schedules and behavioral problems in 372 children aged 5-15 whose mothers were receiving cash assistance in an urban Michigan county. Mediating Factors Studies that have examined pathways linking NS schedules with child behavioral problems reported that mediating factors include parental depressive symptoms (Daniel et al., 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009; Strazdins et al., 2006), poor parenting and parental supervision (Han et al., 2010; Strazdins et al., 2006), reduced parent-child closeness and less time spent with children (Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al., 2010; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009), and a less supportive home environment (Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al., 2010; Han & Waldfogel, 2007). Of note, Han et al. (20 10) found that irregular shifts were associated with greater parental knowledge of child's whereabouts, which, in turn, reduced risks for adolescent risky behavior. The authors speculated that families with parents who worked irregular shifts were of higher SES in this particular NLSY sample, and therefore may have chosen these schedules to meet their child care or family needs. Moderating Factors Whereas the majority of the studies adjusted for child gender in their analyses, few specifically examined child gender as a moderating factor. Whereas Han et al. (2010) found 21 that adolescent boys were more likely than girls to engage in risky behavior due to cumulative exposure to mothers' night shifts, two studies based on small local samples found that girls were more negatively affected by parents' NS work schedules (Barton et al., 1998; Joshi & Bogen, 2007). With regard to family structure, there is consistent evidence from three studies based on large samples that children of single-mothers tended to have more problems associated with NS work schedules than those living in two-parent families (Dockery et al., 2009; Han, 2008; Han & Waldfogel, 2007). Similarly, there is a consistently stronger association in low-SES families than in middle- or high-SES families (Han, 2008; Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006). Han (2008) found a stronger relationship between the number of years a mother had worked NS schedules and poorer behavioral outcomes in 4- to 10-yearold children who either lived in single-mother or low-income families, whose mothers worked in cashier or service occupations, or whose mothers worked non-day shifts full-time. Child Cognitive Development Four studies have assessed cognitive outcomes in respect to parental NS work schedules. In a US sample, Han (2005) found that children of mothers who worked NS schedules in their first year oflife had poorer cognitive outcomes two to three years later, although the results varied by dimensions of cognitive performance, timing and length of exposure to these schedules. Children had lower scores on the Mental Development Index (MDI) at 24 and 36 months, and significantly lower verbal comprehension and expressive language skills at 36 months, if their mother had worked a NS schedule in the first year of life but not in the second or third year. Using data from the NLSY-CS, Heymann (2000) found a higher proportion of school-aged children with poorer outcomes in mathematics, vocabulary and reading if parents worked evenings or nights. Based on the same dataset but using growth-curve modeling, Han and Fox (2011) found that the number of years a mother worked 22 a night shift was associated with lower reading scores, and the number of years she worked evening or night shifts was associated with lower progress in math skills between ages 6 and 14 . Having a father who worked more years at an evening shift was also associated with reduced mathematic scores. The authors' mediation analysis suggested that eating meals together, parent knowledge of children's whereabouts, and some after-school activities were plausible explanations for these associations (Han & Fox, 2011 ). In a US sample of 250 African American children (aged 24 to 36 months) born to mothers residing in low-income and rural counties in North Carolina and Pennsylvania, Odom and co-authors found that mothers' NS work schedules at 24 months were associated with lower expressive language ability at both 24 and 36 months (Odom et al., 2013). These associations were mediated through negative maternal interactions with their children and negative work-family spill over. Childhood BMI Analyzing data from the NLSY-CS, Miller and Han (2008) found that the BMI of 13to 14-year-old American children increased significantly if mothers worked either a few(< 4) or many (10 or more) years ofNS schedules. This relationship was stronger among the "near poor" (i.e., families in the second quartile of family income, a level of income where families could not qualify for a number of public assistance programs yet tend to have substandard living), compared to families in other income quartiles. In a largely representative sample of 434 children born to Caucasian women in Adelaide, South Australia, Champion et al. (2012) reported that fathers' NS work schedules (shift schedules, evenings, nights, and weekends) were associated with increased odds of child overweight and obesity. In contrast to other studies on the topic, mothers' NS work schedules were not significantly associated with the outcome variables examined. However, when both parents worked NS schedules the 23 investigators found an increased risk for child overweight and obesity. Due to the study's cross-sectional design and the fact that all types ofNS work schedules were combined into one category, the findings reported by Champion et al regarding mothers' NS work schedules need to be interpreted with caution. Using longitudinal data from the NICHD SECCYD and within-child fixed-effect models, Morrissey et al. (2011) found no significant association between maternal NS work schedules and child BMI among 990 American school children aged 8 to 12. However, the NICHD SECCYD sample is not nationally representative, with 80% of the children living in two-parent families and more than 75% in higher-income families. Other Outcomes Two of the 15 studies that examined child behavioral problems (already reviewed above) also examined children's school engagement (Han, 2006; Hsueh & Y oshikawa, 2007) and involvement in extracurricular activities (Han, 2006) as additional outcomes. One study focused on child sleep patterns (Radosevic-Vidacek & Koscec, 2004). These studies found that children tend to have lower levels of school engagement, attend fewer extracurricular activities, and sleep less when their parents work NS hours. The studies provided no information, however, about the mechanisms that might underpin this association. Whereas parents working NS hours (e.g., evenings or nighttime) are available during daytime when outside school activities take place (3pm-6pm), they may not have sufficient energy to take their children to such activities or may lack time to do so due to the competing demands of housework, such as preparing meals before they go to work in the evening. It is possible that disrupted family processes or child mental health and behavioral problems associated with NS work schedules may affect the child's sleep and school engagement. 24 DISCUSSION Guided by our conceptual framework, we examined studies that investigated the associations between parents' NS work schedules and four child developmental outcomes (internalizing and externalizing problems, cognitive development, and body mass index) and three other related outcomes (sleep pattern, school engagement, and extracurricular activities). Of 23 studies reviewed, 21 studies reported a statistically significant negative association between NS work schedules and at least one child developmental outcome. Thus, the majority of the studies support our general hypothesis that parental NS work schedules, as a distal factor or part of the "exosystem" in which children grow and develop, have negative consequences for the developing child with regards to mental health and behavioral problems, cognitive development, overweight and obesity, and other related outcomes. Two studies that did not find a significant association between NS work schedules and child outcomes were Dunifon (2005) and Morrisey (2011) and their respective colleagues, although both studies were based on longitudinal data and used child fixed effects models to address potential selection bias. Dunifon et al. (2005) examined behavioral problems in a small sample of children from low-income families (N = 372, ages 2 -15) and found no effect ofNS work schedules on child behavioral problems. In contrast, Han (2008) analyzed a large longitudinal national data set that over-represented less advantaged families (US NLSY-CS, N;::::; 7,000, ages 4- 10), and reported a significant association between the number of years mothers worked NS schedules and behavioral problems in children, particularly in low-SES families. A child fixed effects model was also used in this study. Morrisey et al. (2011) did not find a significant relationship between NS schedules and child BMI in a sample of US children (NICHD SECCYD N = 990, ages 8-12) in which lowincome families were underrepresented. In contrast, Miller and Han (2008) also analyzed longitudinal data and used child fixed effects models to address selection bias, but they 25 reported a significant association between NS schedules and child BMI in a large sample of teenage children (ages 13-14). These differing results may be in part attributed to the size and the characteristics of the population under investigation, and different constellations of factors adjusted in the studies. Findings regarding child gender differences in the effect ofNS work schedules on child behavioral outcomes differed by study quality. Highly rated studies (meeting all five criteria) reported that adolescent boys were more likely than girls to engage in risky behavior due to their cumulative exposure to mothers' night shifts (Han et al., 2010), but two crosssectional studies based on small local samples (meeting only two of the five criteria) found that girls were more negatively affected by their parents' NS work schedules (Barton et al., 1998; Joshi & Bogen, 2007). The most consistent associations were reported among preschool-age children for both cognitive and mental health/behavioral problems, and among adolescents for risky behaviors. These findings suggest that parental NS work schedules matter for both early and later developmental stages but in different developmental domains. Consistent with our broad conceptual framework, there is evidence that the negative associations between NS work schedules and child behavioral problems are partly mediated through family resources, such as parental psychological capital (e.g., depressive symptoms) (Daniel et al., 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009; Strazdins et al., 2006), family processes such as low quality parenting (Han et al., 2010; Strazdins et al., 2006), reduced child-parent interaction and closeness (Han & Miller, 2009; Han, et al., 2010; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009), and a less supportive home environment (Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al, 2010; Han & Waldfogel, 2007). We should be cautious, nevertheless, about concluding that maternal depressive symptoms constitute a mediating factor, as in almost all studies ofbehavioral problems that focused on young children (age 0-10) child mental health/behavioral problems were 26 measured with mother-reported ratings (e.g., CBCL, BPI), which are likely to be influenced by the mothers' mental health. As expected, some of the studies reviewed have shown that associations between NS work schedules and child developmental outcomes differ by family SES. For example, based on studies that addressed selection bias and controlling for key confounders and covariates, there is clear evidence that associations between parents' NS work schedules and child outcomes are more pronounced in low SES families (e.g., low-income, single-parenthood, and low occupational status). This evidence is demonstrated in studies that found significant interactions between SES and NS work schedules (Dockery et al., 2009; Han, 2008; Han & Waldfogel, 2007; Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006) when mental health and behavioral problems were examined. These findings suggest that families with more economic resources and human capital may better be able to meet the challenges ofNS work schedules than less advantaged families. The association between NS work schedules and child outcomes is also magnified when parents work NS schedules on a full-time basis, compared to working these schedules on a part-time basis. These findings suggest that evening and night shifts are particularly detrimental to child developmental outcomes (Daniel et al., 2009; Han & Fox, 2011; Han & Miller, 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009). Further, cumulative exposure to NS work schedules has a negative impact on child developmental outcomes (Han, 2008; Han & Fox, 2011; Han & Miller, 2009; Miller & Han, 2008), thus underscoring the importance of using longitudinal data in future inquiry. Strengths and Limitations of Reviewed Studies The robustness of the evidence provided by the studies reviewed depends on their methodological rigor. The majority were based on large and/or representative samples, controlled for key confounders, and examined moderating and mediating factors, thus 27 providing in-depth information about the link between NS work schedules and child developmental outcomes. There were, however, a number of limitations. Cross-Sectional Data Due to the nature of the topic, experimental data were not a possibility and thus a causal relationship between parental NS schedules and children's well-being is difficult to establish. Whereas it is encouraging that 12 out of 23 studies used a longitudinal design, ten studies were based on cross-sectional data, thus precluding inferences about NS work schedules as a causal factor for child well-being, and raising a concern about reverse or reciprocal causality. For example, it is possible that parents arrange their work schedules as a way of managing children with more behavioral problems. In addition, the measurement of work schedules at one point in time does not provide information about how long children have been exposed to these work patterns and the changes that may have occurred over time. Use of longitudinal data would reveal whether or not, or to what extent, the any disadvantages associated with NS schedules found at one time point persists over time. Longitudinal studies reviewed to date have begun to consider both the onset and duration of children's exposure to parents' NS work schedules (Han & fox, 2011; Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al., 2010). We call for many more studies in the future to take a longitudinal approach. Less Information about Father's Work Schedules We were pleased to see that 13 out of the 23 studies examined fathers' work schedules. However, the remaining ten studies did not do so, primarily due to lack of data. The association between NS work schedules and child outcomes may differ by the gender of the parent due to gender differences in sharing child care and household work responsibilities and also gender differences in occupations. With an increasing emphasis on paternal involvement in children's development, the field will benefit from giving equal attention to the work schedules of both mothers and fathers, and, in particular, joint work schedules in 28 dual-earner families. One cross-sectional study examined joint NS schedules worked by both parents in 434 nine-year-old children and found that these schedules were associated with child overweight and obesity, but with a weak statistical significance. The small sample size and the cross-sectional design limited the generalization of this study. Given that fathers tend to provide more child care than normal when mothers work NS schedules (Barnett & Gareis, 2007; Thompson, 2009), it is important to understand how both mothers' and fathers' work schedules may independently or jointly shape children's development. Indeed, the evidence from the studies that examined both mothers' and fathers' NS work schedules suggests that both parents' NS schedules matter (Champion et al., 2012; Han & Fox, 2011; Han & Miller, 2009; Rosenbaum & Morrett, 2009; Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006). Whereas maternal work schedules (particularly night shifts) appear to be more strongly linked to child well-being, the type ofNS schedule each parent works has differential but significant associations with child outcomes. Maternal night shifts and paternal evening shifts had the most consistent negative associations with child and adolescent mental health issues (Han & Fox, 2011; Han & Miller, 2009; Han et al., 2010). Unfortunately, most of the existing large datasets do not have as detailed information about fathers as on mothers. We call for future data collection efforts to overcome this common limitation. Lack ofData on Child Care and Choice ofNS Work Schedules Most studies lacked information about the availability and quality of child care available to parents working NS schedules. The studies examined also lack precision as to measurements of the timing of child care arrangements that can be matched to the timing of parental work schedules. The impact of parental NS schedules on children may depend on the availability, affordability and quality of care arrangements. For example, formal care for children is rarely available outside standard business hours and weekdays. Children whose mothers work NS schedules are more likely to be cared for by fathers in two-parent families or by other 29 relatives or non-relatives in a single-mother family (Han, 2004). When both formal and informal supports are absent, parents working NS schedules may have great difficulties in juggling work and family demands. This is a particularly important issue for single-parent families and a plausible explanation for the findings from this review that the adverse association between NS schedules and child outcomes is stronger in single-parent families. The quality of child care also matters. Previous research has shown that high quality child care has a long-term positive impact on children's development (Kohen, Hertzman, & Willms, 2002). With the passage of the US federal welfare reform law, many low-income and single mothers have no choice but to place their young children at low quality childcare facilities, which may impair their children's development (Chaudry, 2004). Socially disadvantaged families may be more likely to use poor quality child care when working NS schedules. Hence child care quality is a plausible mechanism linking NS schedules to poor child well-being and warrants future inquiry. Closely tied to child care is the issue of whether parents choose to work NS schedules or have job flexibility in order to meet family and child care needs. These issues were not considered in the majority of studies reviewed. NS work schedules may present advantages to both-parent families where parents are able to choose work schedules to meet their child care needs and to enable fathers' greater participation in parenting (Barnett & Gareis, 2007; Thompson, 2009). Indeed, some parents choose to work NS schedules as a way of spending more time with their children (Hattery, 2001). Working mothers with flexible schedules tend to spend more time in direct child care but less time in shared leisure activities (Rapoport & Bourdais, 2008). However, it is unclear if the choice of flexible NS schedules benefits children's mental health and cognitive development. Parents who choose to work flexible NS schedules may still be prone to stress and fatigue associated with NS schedules. Tuttle and Garr (2012) have shown that women working shift work have greater work-to-family conflict 30 than men, even when women have more control over their work schedule. It is important for future research to take this issue into consideration. Recent welfare reform in the US has seen a great number of low income single mothers move into poor quality jobs that require inflexible NS schedules (Jones-DeWeever, Peterson, & Song, 2003; Presser, 1999; Presser & Cox, 1997). Reliance on Parent-Reported Measures of Child Behavioral Outcomes There is considerable research on the concordance between parent- and child/adolescentreported measures ofbehavioral problems. However, there is no evidence that child-reported measures are more accurate than those reported by their parents. The accuracy of reported child behavioral problems is influenced by the saliency ofbehavioral problems to parents and children, and the willingness of both to report these problems (Karver, 2006). The accuracy of self-reports may also differ by the relevance of the problems to a specific setting (home versus school) and the parent's gender. The literature recommends employing multiple informants when collecting information on child and adolescent behavioral problems (Karver, 2006; Salbach-Andrae, Lenz, & Lehmkuhl, 2009; Seiffge-Krenke & Kollmar, 1998). Child-reported measures ofbehavioral problems should be collected in studies that involve older children who are able to answer the questionnaire. Out of all 15 studies of mental health and behavioral problems, 11 involved school-aged children and adolescents, but only five of these used child-reported measures ofbehavioral outcomes, and the other six studies analyzed samples combining young and older children (ages 2-16). While motherreported measures may be considered practical for young children (ages 0-5) in these studies, such measures can be complemented or enriched by a secondary carer (e.g., child care centre or kindergarten or preschool teachers, fathers, grandparents or nannies). Further, mothers may be biased either downward or upward in their assessment of their children's behavior, particularly when maternal mental health is a concern (Sawyer, Streiner, & Baghurst, 1998). 31 Hsueh and Y oshikawa (2007) have shown that parental NS schedules were associated with teacher-reported child behavioral problems but not with mother-reported child behaviors. Other Sources ofInformation Bias Self-reported measures, missing cases and loss to follow-up are also potential sources of information bias. None of the four studies that examined child cognitive outcomes, and none of the three that examined adolescent body weight, used self-reported outcome measures. Instead objective measures were used, including cognitive and language test scores and body weight and height. However, in all 15 studies of child behavioral problems, parent-, teacheror child-reported outcome measures were used. Self-reporting is unavoidable in both clinical and non-clinical studies that examine mental health and behavioral problems. For example, well-established instruments, such as the Kessler Psychological Distress Scales (Kessler et al., 2002), the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Steer, & Carbin, 1988), the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991), and the Behavior Problems Index (Zill, 1990) are all based on data collected from self-reports. As discussed above, one way of minimizing potential bias is to collect information from multiple informants (child/adolescent, mother, father, teacher, and secondary cares). Missing cases and loss to follow-up are also a source of potential informational bias and are common problems with survey and cohort data. The vast majority of the studies covered in this review utilized such data. To the extent that low SES groups are often over-represented in missing cases and in loss to follow-up (Li, Kendall, Henderson, Downie, Landsborough, & Oddy, 2008), the negative effects ofNS work schedules on child outcomes are likely to be underestimated. FUTURE DIRECTIONS Parental work is an important social determinant of child health and wellbeing, especially in the era of changing economic dynamics and an increasingly globalised economy. In 32 particular, occupations that require employees to work NS schedules, such as in the service sector, are expected to account for proportionally high job growth in the future (US Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2012). The findings from the studies reviewed have shown that NS work schedules exert a larger negative impact on children from low SES backgrounds than on children from families with more resources. This has important implications for understanding well-established social gradients in child health and development (Keating & Hertzman, 1999). Poor working conditions, including parental NS schedules, are a plausible mechanism mediating these social gradients and disparities in child developmental outcomes. Therefore, the impact of parental NS work schedules on children's developmental outcomes warrants more fine-grained research. This line of enquiry also needs greater guidance by a theoretical framework that recognises broader societal and community influences and considers the characteristics of parents and the child at different developmental stages. Below we discuss a number of issues for future investigators to consider. Links between NS Schedules and a Broader Range of Developmental Outcomes Most studies to date have focused on behavioral and mental health outcomes, only four have examined children's cognitive development, and only three have investigated obesity. Much more research is needed to enhance our knowledge about the relationship between NS work schedules and child cognitive outcomes, particularly academic achievement in school-age children. Further research is also needed not only to examine the link between NS work schedules and child BMI but also to investigate whether and how proximal factors, such as nutrition and physical activity, may also be influenced by NS work schedules. Based on the conceptual resource framework, we would expect parental NS work schedules to influence these developmental outcomes through the pathways of time available for the use of family and psychological capital (i.e., parental mental health and the quality of 33 the relationships between the parents themselves and with their children). It is also plausible that these various developmental outcomes are interrelated contemporaneously or longitudinally. With the use of more rigorous research design and advanced modelling, it will be possible to examine various developmental outcomes of children who are exposed to parental NS work schedules over time. This would help researchers determine ifbehavioral and cognitive development in early childhood leads to mental health problems and risk-taking behavior in teenagers, relative to children whose parents work standard daytime schedules. The field will also benefit from more research addressing the important issue of whether or not the association between parental NS work schedules and early child development will persist or dissipate over time. Better Specification ofNS Work Schedules Some of the studies reviewed have shown that night shifts were associated with poor cognitive and behavioral outcomes among young children, and with higher levels of depression and more risky behaviors among adolescents. On the other hand, two studies reported that irregular or variable shifts were associated with reduced adolescent risk-taking behaviors (e.g., smoking, drinking, and using drugs) via improved parental knowledge of their child's whereabouts (Han et al., 2010; Han & Waldfogel, 2007). We note, however, that the data (NLSY-CS) used in these studies suggested that parents who reported having irregular shifts tended to choose such schedules and/or have some control over the time when they worked. Rotating and irregular shifts would have less predictable effects on parental time at home, which might make it harder for families to plan and attend events together. These shifts, nevertheless, can be beneficial to children if the shifts are employee-initiated rather than required by employers (Henly, Shaefer, & Waxman, 2006). Such findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between the evening, night, rotating, irregular or 34 weekend work of mothers and fathers and of taking into account whether parents choose these shifts. Often researchers have collapsed different types ofNS schedules into one single category due to inadequate sample sizes in each group. As noted by other scholars in the field, such an analytic strategy limits our understanding about which schedules influence child development and family processes (Barnett, 2006; Presser, 2003). Further, no studies have considered the location ofNS work schedules (at home vs. outside home) and its potential benefit or detriment to child wellbeing. Parents working NS schedules at home may be able to adjust hours to suit their family needs. Rapoport and Bourdais (2008) have shown that working at home in general is associated with more time devoted to household chores for mothers and more time for social activities and family meals for fathers. Future research should investigate whether the effects ofNS schedules worked at home are different from those worked elsewhere. Better specification ofNS schedules also requires a focus on the family as the unit of analysis, considering joint work scheduling patterns in dual-earner families. The degree to which the work schedules of parents in dual-earner families overlap also has important implications for parental relationships, the division of household labour, and parental participation in children's activities (Barnett, 2006; Staines & Pleck, 1983), all of which may influence child outcomes. Attention to a Wider Range of Moderating and Mediating Factors Fourteen of the 23 studies reviewed examined a range of moderating or mediating factors that were likely to play a role in the association between NS work schedules and child development. There was, however, a general lack of information on the child's temperament, parental marital satisfaction, levels of actual and perceived social support, and parents' job quality. These factors have been shown to influence child development (Brooks-Gunn, Han, 35 & Waldfogel, 2010; Han & Waldfogel, 2007; Strazdins, Shipley, Clements, Obrien, & Broom, 2010). Strazdins and colleagues (2010) reveal that when parents hold poor-quality jobs their children show more emotional and behavioral difficulties, independent of income, parent education, family structure, and work hours. Similarly, job characteristics and job quality associated with certain types ofNS schedules may be an important confounder or moderator. For example, the effects ofNS work schedules may be exacerbated by stressful work conditions, such as long hours, lack of support from coworkers and supervisors, and pressures for meeting deadlines. It is critical that future research adequately examines the role these factors may play in mediating or moderating the relationship between NS schedules and various domains of child development. Further, whereas most of the reviewed studies adjusted for family structure and income as confounders, relatively few examined how the relationship between NS schedules and child outcomes may differ by these contextual factors. It is important to examine factors that may modify the effect of working NS schedules on child development, such as those based on SES (Repetti, 2005) and other characteristics of the family and the child. Families are complex and diverse with different capacities for responding to the challenges of combining work and family. Families with more resources (e.g., both-parent and high income families) are either less affected (Han, 2008; Han & Waldfogel, 2007; Strazdins et al., 2004, 2006) or unaffected by NS work schedules (Dockery et al., 2009; Morrissey et al., 2011). Some small scale studies linking NS work schedules with family processes (Barnett & Gareis, 2007; Davis et al., 2006) suggest that family with more resources can benefit from mothers' NS work schedules in terms of fathers' participation in parenting. It is thus important for future research and interventions to identify and target subgroups of children from less advantaged families, particularly those who have low levels of multiple developmental resources (e.g., parental SES, time, psychological and physical health). 36 More Sophisticated Analytical Approaches Causality and selection bias have always been a concern in social science research. Increasingly, studies use longitudinal datasets to handle temporal issues in linking parental work schedules with children's well-being. Longitudinal data, however, do not always enable researchers to conclusively answer the fundamental question of causality. In the absence of experimental data, some existing studies have used more sophisticated statistical approaches to address this issue. For example, Han (2008) used a child fixed effects model to tackle the issue of unobserved heterogeneity. Other studies have used propensity score matching (Han et al., 2010) to address selection bias and causality. These statistical tools allow researchers to compare outcomes for children of parents who worked NS and the children of parents who did not work such schedules, but had a similar predicted propensity to do so. In this way, these two groups are comparable so we can minimize the possibility that observed associations between NS work schedules and child outcomes are attributable to selection bias (see discussion in Hill, 2008). As more longitudinal data and sophisticated statistical techniques become available, future studies need to tackle the issue of causality. Implications for Practice and Policy We envision a number of ways in which the government and society as a whole can intervene to prevent or buffer the negative effects ofNS work schedules on children and families through policy initiatives, where such impact exists. None of the reviewed studies examined indicators of broader influence outside the home, such as the neighborhood, community resources (e.g., the accessibility and cost of child care facilities, school, beforeand after-school care for school age children, and public transportation), and work place policy initiatives. These factors can potentially mitigate the negative association between NS 37 work schedules and child development. For example, greater support at the workplace for fathers to increase their levels of involvement in child care, and greater quality of father involvement in household work generally, will come a long way to help families cope with their daily stress due toNS work schedules. This will, in turn, enhance family and child wellbeing. School also has an important role to play, such as in the provision of healthy breakfast and lunch at school cafeterias, and greater social and emotional support and intellectual stimulation targeting children whose parents work NS schedule and may have a reduced capacity to adequately provide their children with these healthy developmental inputs. Further, the availability of before- and after-school care and child care for young children during NS work hours can reduce the stress on parents who work NS schedules. As also noted by Barnett (2006), the availability of medical appointments on weekends and public transportation outside normal business hours can assist parents working NS schedules to cope with demands from work and family. In the absence of such community resources, parents with NS work schedules may resort to unreliable options (Barnett, 2006). Finally, given evidence that children living in low-income families are more vulnerable, ensuring adequate pay and/or supplements paid for NS schedules is another intervention option for industrial relations and regulatory efforts. The trend towards the 24/7 economy is unlikely to reverse in the future, and the evidence to date suggests that some aspects of children's development is shaped by the timing of their parents work. We are fully aware of the complexity of the ways in which parental market work affects children's health and development. In spite of the best efforts made by scholars to capture such complexity, existing research may still barely do justice to the influences (positive and negative) at play, and the challenges and difficulties confronting working parents and their children. Possibly, mixed methods may enable researchers to better understand the everyday experiences of today's families and how these experiences interact 38 with parentallabor market involvement to influence children's development. In many respects the field is yet to mature, and the next real task is for research and policy to do justice to the complex relationship between parental NS work schedules and children's health and development. 39 References ABS. (2009). Work, life and family balance, Australian social trends (No. 6361.0). Canberra, ACT: Australian Bureau of Statistics. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the ChildBehavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department ofPsychiatry. Barnett, R. C., (2006). Relationship of the number and distribution of work hours to health and quality-of-life (QOL) outcomes. In P.L. Perrewe & D.C. Ganster (Eds.), Research in Occupational Stress and Wellbeing (Vol. 5, pp. 99-138). New York: Elsevier Ltd. Barnett, R. C., & Gareis, K. C. (2007). Shift work, parenting behaviors, and children's socioemotional well-being: A within-family study. Journal of Family Issues, 28, 727- 748. Barnett, R. C., Gareis, K. C., & Brennan, R. T. (2008). Wives' shift work schedules and husbands' and wives' well-being in dual-earner couples with children: A withincouple analysis. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 396-422. Barton, J., Aldridge, J., & Smith, P. (1998). The emotional impact of shift work on the children ofNS workers. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 24(Suppl.3), 146-155. Beck, AT., Steer, RA, Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8,77-100. Bradley, R. H. (2002). Environment and parenting. In M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2: Biology and ecology ofparenting (pp. 281-314). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. 40 Bronfenbrenner, U. (Ed.). (2005). Making human beings human: Bioecological perspectives on human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). Children in families in communities: risk and intervention in the Bronfenbrenner tradition. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human development. (pp. 467-519). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Brooks-Gunn, J., Brown, B., Duncan, G. J., & Moore, A K. (1995). Child development in the context offamily and community resources: An agenda for national data collections. Washington, DC: The National Academy of Sciences. Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W.-J., & Waldfogel, J. (2002). Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: The NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 73, 1052-1072. Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W-J., & Waldfogel, J. (2010). First-year maternal employment and child development in the first seven years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), 75, 1-147. Bumpus, M. F., Crouter, A C., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Work demands of dual-earner couples: Implications for parents' knowledge about children's daily lives in middle childhood. Journal ofMarriage and the Family, 61, 465-475. Retrieved from http://www.j stor.org/stable/3 53 762 Card, N. A, Sawalini, G. M., Stucky, B. D., & Little, T. D. (2008). Direct and indirect aggression during childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review of gender differences, intercorrelations, and relations to maladjustment. Child Development, 79, 1185-1229. Champion, S. L., Rumbold, A R., Steele E. J., Giles, L. C., Davies, M. J., & Moore V. M. 41 (2012). Parental work schedules and child overweight and obesity. International Journal of Obesity, 36, 573-580. Chaudry, A (2004). PuttingchildrenfirstNew York: Russell Sage Foundation. Chase-Lansdale, P. L, Mott, F. L., Brooks-Gunn, J, & Phillips, D. A (1991). Children of the National Longitudinal Survey ofYouth: A unique research opportunity. Developmental Psychology, 2 7, 918-931. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed. ). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Connelly, R., & Kimmel, J. (2011). The role non-standard work status in parental caregiving for young children. Eastern Economic Journal, 37, 248-269. Daniel, S. S., Grzywacz, J. G., Leerkes, E., Tucker, J., & Han, W.-J. (2009). Nonstandard maternal work schedules during infancy: Implications for children's early behavior problems. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 195-207. Davis, K. D., Crouter, A C., & McHale, S. M. (2006). Implications of shift work for parentadolescent relationships in dual-earner families. Family Relations, 55, 450-460. Davis, K. D., Goodman, B., Pirretti, A E., & Almeida, D. M. (2008). Nonstandard work schedules, perceived family well-being, and daily stressors. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 70, 991-1003. Dockery, A, Li, J., & Kendall, G. (2009). Parents' work patterns and adolescent mental health. Social Science and Medicine, 68, 689-698. Dunifon, R., Kalil, A, & Bajracharya, A (2005). Maternal working conditions and child well-being in welfare-leaving families. Developmental Psychology, 41, 851-859. Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6 to 14. The Future of Children, 9, 30-44. 42 Eisenberg, N., & Valiente, C., (2002). Parenting and children's prosocial and moral development. In M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook ofparenting: Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A (2006). Gender differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 33-72. Garey, A I. (1999). Chapter 5: Motherhood on the night shift. In Weaving Work and Motherhood (pp. 108-139). Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Gassman-Pines, A (2011). Low-income mothers' nighttime and weekend work: Daily associations with child behavior, mother-child interactions, and mood. Family Relations, 60, 15-29. Gennetian, L. A, Lopoo, L. M., & London, A S. (2008). Maternal work hours and adolescents' school outcomes among low-income families in four urban counties. Demography, 45, 31-53. Greenberger, E., O'Neil, R., & Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking workplace and homeplace: Relations between the nature of adults' work and their parenting behaviours. Developmental Psychology, 30, 990-1002. Grzywacz, J. G., Daniel, S. S., Tucker, J., Walls, J., & Leerkes, E. (2011). Nonstandard work schedules and developmentally generative parenting practices: An application of propensity score techniques. Family Relations, 60, 45-59. Han, W.-J. (2004). Nonstandard work schedules and child care decisions: Evidence from the NICHD study of early child care. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 231-256. Han, W.-J. (2005). Maternal nonstandard work schedules and child cognitive outcomes. Child Development, 76, 13 7-154. 43 Han, W.-J. (2006). Maternal work schedules and child outcomes: Evidence from the National Survey of American Families. Children and Youth Services Review, 28, 1039-1059. Han, W.-J. (2008). Shift work and child behavioral outcomes. Work, Employment and Society, 22, 67-87. Han, W.-J., & Fox, L. E. (2011). Parental work schedules and children's cognitive trajectories. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 73, 962-980. Han, W.-J., & Miller, P. (2009). Parental work schedules and adolescent depression. Health Sociology Review, 18, 36-49. Han, W.-J., Miller, P., & Waldfogel, J. (2010). Parental work schedules and adolescents' risky behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1245-1267. Han, W.-J., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). Parental work schedules, family process, and early adolescents' risky behavior. Children and Youth Services Review, 29, 1249-1266. Hattery, A J. (2001). Tag-team parenting: Costs and benefits of utilizing nonoverlapping shift work in families with young children. Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 82, 419-427. Henly, J. R., Shaefer, H. L., & Waxman, E. (2006). Nonstandard work schedules: Employerand employee-driven flexibility in retail jobs. Social Service Review, 80, 609-634. Hertzman, C. (1999). Population health and human development. In D. Keating, & C. Hertzman (Eds.), Developmental health and the wealth of nations (pp. 153-184). New York: Guildford Press. Heymann, J. (2000). The widening gap: Why America's working families are in jeopardy and what we can do about it. New York: Basic Books. Heymann, S. J., & Earle, A (2001). The impact of parental working conditions on school-age children: The case of evening work. Community, Work & Family, 4, 305-325. Hill, J. (2008). Discussion of research using propensity-score matching: Comments on "A 44 critical appraisal of propensity-score matching in the medical literature between 1996 and 2003" by Peter Austin, Statistics in Medicine. Statistics in Medicine, 27, 2055- 2061. Hsueh, J., & Y oshikawa, H. (2007). Working nonstandard schedules and variable shifts in low-income families: Associations with parental psychological well-being, family functioning, and child well-being. Developmental Psychology, 43, 620-632. Jones-DeWeever, A, Peterson, J., & Song, X. (2003). Before and after welfare reform: The work and well-being of low-income single parent families. Washington, DC: Institute for Women's Policy Research. Johnson, S., Li, J., Kendall, G., Strazdins, L., & Jacoby, L. (2013). Mothers' and fathers' work hours, child gender and behavior in middle childhood. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 75, 56-74. Joshi, P., & Bogen, K. (2007). Nonstandard schedules and young children's behavioral outcomes among working low-income families. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 69, 139-156. Kalil, A, Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Epstein, J. L. (2010). Nonstandard work and marital instability: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72, 1289-1300. Karver, S. M. (2006). Determinants of multiple informant agreement on child and adolescent behavior. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 34, 251-262. Kantermann, T., Juda, M., Vetter, C., & Roenneberg, T. (2010). Shift-work research: Where do we stand, where should we go? Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 8(2), 83-159. doi: 10.1111/j .1479-8425.2010.00432.x Keating, D., & Hertzman, C. (1999). Developmental health and the wealth of nations, Guildford Press: New York. 45 Kendall, G. E., & Li, J. (2005). Early childhood socialization and social gradients in adult health: A commentary on Singh-Manoux and Marmot's "role of socialization in explaining social inequalities in health" (60: 9, 2005, 2129-2133). [comment]. Social Science and Medicine, 61, 2272-2276; discussion 2277-2279. Kessle,r R.C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L.J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D.K., Normand, S.L., Waiters, E.E., Zaslavsky, AM. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32, 959-76. Kohen, D., Hertzman, C., & Willms, J.D. (2002). The importance of quality child care. In J.D. Willms (Ed.), Vulnerable children: Findings from Canada's National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (pp. 261-276). Edmonton: The University of Alberta Press. Lamb, M. E. (2010). How do fathers influence child development? Let me count the ways. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (Fifth ed., pp. 1-26). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A (2009). Parent-child relationships during adolescence. In R. M. Lemer & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology. Vol 2. Contextual influences on adolescent development (Third ed., pp. 3-42). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. Li, J., Garth, K., Henderson, S., Downie, J., Landsborough, L., & Oddy, W. (2008). Maternal psychosocial wellbeing and breastfeeding duration. Acta Paediatrical, 97, 221-225. Liu, H., Wang, Q., Keesler, V., & Schneider, B. (2011). Non-standard work schedules, workfamily conflict and parental well-being: A comparison of married and cohabiting unions. Social Science Research, 40, 473-484. Maume, DJ. (2011). Reconsidering the temporal increase in fathers' time with children. 46 Journal ofFamily and Economic Issues, 32, 411-423. Maume, DJ., & Sebastian, RA (2012). Gender, nonstandard work schedules, and marital quality. Journal ofFamily and Economic Issues, 33, 477-490. McHale, S., Crouter, A, & Whiteman, S. (2003). The family contexts of gender development in childhood and adolescence. Social Development, 12, 125-148. McMenamin, T. M. (2007). A time to work: recent trends in shift work and flexible schedules. Monthly Labor Review, (December), 3-15. Miller, D. P., & Han, W.-J. (2008). Maternal nonstandard work schedule and adolescent overweight. American Journal ofPublic Health, 98, 1495-1502. Morrissey, T. W., Dunifon, R. E., & Kalil, A (2011). Maternal employment, work schedules, and children's body mass index. Child Development, 82, 66-81. Odom, E., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Crouter, AC. (2013). Nonstandard maternal work schedules: Implications for African American children's early language outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 379-387. Perry-Jenkins, M., Goldberg, A, Pierce, C. P., & Sayer, A J. (2007). Shift work, role overload, and the transition to parenthood. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 69, 123138. Presser, H.B. (1999). Toward a 24-Hour Economy. Science, 284, 1778-1779. Presser, H. B. (2003). Working in a 2417 economy: Challenges for American families. New York: Russ Sage Foundation. Presser, H. B., & Cox, A G. (1997). The work schedules of low-educated American women and the welfare reform. Monthly Labor Review, (April), 25-34. Presser, H. B., Gornick, J. C., & Parashar, S. (2008). Gender and nonstandard work hours in 12 European countries. Monthly Labor Review, (February), 83-103. 47 Radosevic-Vidacek, B., & Koscec, A (2004). Nonstandard working families: Parents' working schedule and sleep patterns of adolescents attending school in two shifts. Revista De Saude Publica, 38, 38-46. Raley, S., & Bianchi, S. (2006). Sons, daughters, and family processes: Does gender of children matter? Annual Review of Sociology, 32, 401-421. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.32.061604.123106 Rapoport, B., & Le Bourdais, C. (2008). Parental time and working schedules. Journal of Population Economics, 21, 903-932. Repetti, R. L. (2005). A psychological perspective on the health and well-being consequences of parental employment. In S. M. Bianchi, L. M. Casper, & R. B. King (Eds.), Work, family, health and well-being (pp. 21-41). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Incorporated. Rosenbaum, E., & Morett, C. R. (2009). The effect of parents' joint work schedules on infants' behavior over the first two years of life: Evidence from the ECSLB. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 13, 73 2-7 44. Salbach-Andrae, H., Lenz, K., & Lehmkuhl, U. (2009). Patterns of agreement among parent, teacher and youth ratings in a referred sample. European Psychiatry, 24, 345-351. Sawyer, M., Streiner, D., & Baghurst, P. (1998). The influence of distress on mothers' and fathers' reports of childhood emotional and behavioral problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 407-414. Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Kollmar, F. (1998).Discrepancies between mothers' and fathers' perceptions of Sons' and daughters' problem behaviour: A longitudinal analysis of parent-adolescent agreement on internalizing and externalizing problem behavior. (1998) Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatric, 39, 687-697. 48 Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A (2000). From neurons to neighbourhoods. Washington D.C: National Academy Press. Sroufe, L. A, & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development, 48, 1184-1199. Staines, G. L., & Pleck, J. H. (1983). The impact ofwork schedules on the family. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Strazdins, L., Clements, M. S., Korda, R. J., Broom, D. H., & D'Souza, R. M. (2006). Unsociable work? Nonstandard work schedules, family relationships, and children's well-being. Journal ofMarriage and Family, 68, 394-410. Strazdins, L., Korda, R. J., Lim, L. L. Y., Broom, D. H., & D'Souza, R. M. (2004). Aroundthe-clock: Parent work schedules and children's well-being in a 24-h economy. Social Science andMedicine, 59, 1517-1527. Strazdins, L., Shipley, M., Clements, M., Obrien, L. V., & Broom, D. H. (2010). Job quality and inequality: Parents' jobs and children's emotional and behavioral difficulties. Social Science & Medicine, 70, 2052-2060. Thompson, E. (2009). Understanding how night work influences the everyday family lives of nurses, their husbands and children. PhD Thesis, the Department of Sociology, University of Surrey. Thompson, R. A (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self In W. Damon (Series Ed.), R.M. Lerner (Series Ed.), N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.). Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 24-98). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, John & Sons. Totterdell, P. (2005) Work schedules. In J. B. Barling, E. K. Kelloway & M. R. Frone (Eds). Handbook ofwork stress, California: Sage Publications Inc. Tuttle, R., & Garr, M. (2012). Shift work and work to family fit: Does schedule control 49 matter? Journal ofFamily Economic Issues, 33, 261-271. US Bureau ofLabor Statistics. (2012). Employment Projection 2010-2020: News Release 1st February 2012 (USDL-12-0160). Retrieved January 22, 2013 at: http://bls.gov/news.release/pdf/ecopro.pdf US Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census, US Department of Lab or, and Bureau ofLabor Statistics.(2011). Current Population Survey, May 2004: Work Schedules and Work at Home Supplement. ICPSR04346-v2. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 2011-12-21. doi: 10.3886/ICPSR04346.v2. Vogel, M., Braungardt, T., Meyer, W., & Schneider, W. (2012). The effects of shift work on physical and mental health. Journal ofNeural Transmission, 119, 1121-1132. Wight, V. R., Raley, S. B., & Bianchi, S. M. (2008). Time for children, one's spouse and oneself among parents who work nonstandard hours. Social Forces, 87, 243-271. Williams, C. (2008). Work-life balance of shift workers. Perspectives. Statistics CanadaCatalogue no. 75-001-X. August, 5-16. Zill, N. (1990). Behavior problems index based on parent report. Washington, DC: Child Trends. Appendix FIGURE 1. Theoretical Background: Adapted from Bioecological Theory (Bronfenbrenner, 2005) and Conceptual Resource Framework (Brooks-Gunn et al.,1995) Mediators Structural Factors Technological change Globalization Labor market deregulation A Nonstandard (NS) Work Schedules Weekend Evenings Nights Irregular shifts Split shifts Family Resources Parent health Time for children Income Human capital Psychological capital Social capital Family Processes Parenting Parent-child relationship Home environment B Moderators Child age Child gender Parent gender Family structure Family income Reason for NS work schedules Child Development Mental health and behavioral problems Cognitive capability Health Appendix FIGURE 2. Summary of the Literature Search Process Records identified through database searching (n = 364) Additional records identified through other sources (n = 3) ! I ! Records after duplicates removed (n = 241) I Records excluded for not meeting inclusion criteria (n = 217) 0 0 0 Full-text articles assessed for eligibility (n = 24) Studies included in comprehensive review (n = 23) r------+ adult health and wellbeing (n =94) family process outcomes (n =32) other (n =91) Full-text articles excluded after initial assessment (n = 1) Appendix Table 1. Summary ofReviewed Studies (N= 22) Study Sample and Age Design Definition of Child Outcome Confounders and Nonstandard (NS) Measures Covariates (C)a I Results/Effect Size Quality Ratingc Moderators (Mo)/ Work Mediators (Me)/ Analysis Techniques_(AT) Mental health an et al., 2010 US: NLSY-CS Birth Mother and father: Risky behaviors at age C- = MIN, family income, Full sample (all ages) Five cohorts of to age # of years parent 13--14: welfare, maternal -Indirect effect of mothers'# of years of night shift (via time children born 13/14 worked evening Ever smoked occupation, birthweight, spent with children) on smoking (r.. = -.096**), drinking (r.. =- 1982-1991 years (2pm-midnight), night Ever drunk alcohol smoking & alcohol .081 **) and drug use (r.. = -.095**) (N = 4,200)A (9pm--8am), or Ever used illicit drugs consumption during -Indirect effect of mothers'# of years of night shift (via home Longitudinal irregular shift (other Number of delinquent pregnancy environment) on drinking (r.. = -.114***), delinquency (r.. =- NS types) from birth behaviors Me = Child reported: time .082***), and sex (r.. = -.145***) to age 11/12. Ever had sex together, maternal & -Indirect effect of mothers'# of years and fathers'# of years Variables also created Child_reported paternal closeness, irregular shift (via parental knowledge) on smoking (r.. = - by developmental parental knowledge, .088***), drinking (r.. = - .114***), and delinquency (r.. = - .077***) stage (<5, 5-10, 11- HOME score - Direct effect of father# of years irregular shifts on drinking (r.. = 12) Mo = Child gender, .080**) income-to-needs ratio, Strength & nature of effects varied by developmental stage at maternal occupation, which NS work occurred family structure. Subgroup analysis showed effects of mothers' night shift AT= SEM/PSM stronger among boys, low-income families, when nonprofessional and when a sole parent for majority of time 5 Daniel et US: NICHD- 6-36 Mother: Behavioral problems C = MIN, poverty level, Effect of mothers' NS schedule (without mediators/ with al., 2009 SECC month Began NS work (CBCL) at 24 & 36 weeks of maternity leave, mediators) on Internalizing if (N = 1,364 (evening, night or months: job flexibility, location -Began NS in 1st yr (24 months): ES = 0.02/-0.04 children born in variable) in 1st year, Internalizing T-score Mo = Child temperament -Began NS after 1st yr (24 months): ES = --0.13/--0.12 1991 and or began after 1st Externalizing T-score Me = Maternal depression -Began NS in 1st yr (36 months): ES = 0.24**/ 0.20* mothers are FT year vs. only standard Mother_reported and sensitivity. -Began NS after 1st yr (36 months): ES = 0.18/0.16 AT= OLS Externalizing if by 6 months)}\ 4 -Began NS in 1st yr (24 months): ES = 0.31**/ 0.24* Longitudinal -Began NS after 1st yr (24 months): ES = 0.03/0.08 - Began NS in 1st yr (36 months): ES = 0.21 *I 0.19* -Began NS after 1st yr (36 months): ES = 0.15/0.13 Interactions significant at p < .10 indicating NS schedules had greater effects on internalizing (24 & 36 months) and externalizing (24 months)if mother began NS work in the 1st year of the child's life & child had difficult temperament Han, 2008 US: NLSY-CS 4-10 Mother & father: Behavioral problems C = MIN All families: children born years NS (6pm- 6am) vs. (BPI): Mo =Years lived with Effect of mothers'# of years NS shift on BP I: ES = 0.03* 1982-1991 of standard shift (# of Total score couple or sole parent, -Years NS & always single mother family: ES = 0.09** mothers who years) Mother reported average family income, -Years NS & bottom 3rd of income distribution: ES = 0.07** had ever worked mother's occupation-# of - Years NS & years cashier/service occupation: ES = 0.01* (N = 12,207)A years, average work hours - Years NS & average hour Longitudinal AT= Child FEM s are FT (>35hrs): ES = 0.1 0*** Two-parent families: - Mothers'/fathers' # of years NS shift: ES = 0.03/ ES = 0.01 4 -Years mother NS & average hours are FT: ES = 0.1 0*** - Years father NS & average hours are FT: ES = 0.1 0*** Rosenbaum US: ECLSBC 9-24 Couple: Behavioral problems C- MIN, preterm, Effect of parental NS shift (without/with mediators) on behavioral & Morett, (N = 1,650) month At least one parent (ITSC) at 24 months birthweight, childcare type, problems at 24 months: 1009 Children born in works NS shift; Mother_reported. job benefits, more than 1 - Either/ both parents work NS shift: ES = 0.19***/ ES = 0.14* 2001 & in DE 6-category variable - job, child ITSC at birth - Father day, mother evening/night: families at day (6am--6pm)/ Mo = Parent gender -Father day, mother irregular: ES = 0.23**/ ES = 0.18* baseline (9 evening (2pm- Me = Father-child -Father evening/night, mother day: ES = 0.32**/ ES = 0.15 months)}\ midnight)/night (9pm- interaction, marital quality, - Father irregular, mother day: ES = -0.15/ ES = --0.22* Longitudinal 8am )/rotating/split/ shared dinners, self-rated - Both evening/night/irregular: ES = 0.06/ ES = -0.21 other health, depression 4 ES = 0.35***/ ES = 0.34*** AT= OLS Dockery et Australia: HILDA 15--20 Either mother or SF36 mental C = MIN, family prosperity, Effect of either parent working NS hours (without/with mediators) al., 2009 waves 1-4, years father works component score (M= long-term disability. on SF36 mental health score: 2001-2004 NS hours in couple 50, SO= 10). Mo = Family structure, -All families: ES = -0.08*/ ES = --0.08* (unbalanced families; one parent in Child_reported work hours -Lone parent: ES = --0.19*/ ES = --0.19* panel: N = 3,429 lone parent families Me = Time with children, -Two-parent: ES = -0.06/ ES = --0.07 observations, (all types including parental mental health. 1 ,691 youth, weekend) vs. AT= OLS 1,197 houses)A standard 3 Cross-sectional Dunifon et US: WES, Ll 2-15 Mother mostly Behavioral problems C = MIN, average hourly Total sample (results from OLS) tl., 2005 women years NS i.e., evening or (BPI) at wave 4_(aged wage, irregular hours, Effect of mothers' NS shift on behavioral problems: mixed day/evening (at 5-15): lengthy commute, marital - NS shift at W1 & internalizing: ES = -0.07 (N = 372) from 3 cash assistance 1 wave only, or at 2 or Internalizing status; mother's self-rated - NS shift at W1 & externalizing: ES = --0.07 rolls, 1997-2002 more waves) vs. Externalizing health, mental health, - NS shift at W1 & positive behavior: ES = --0.06 (4waves, mostly standard shift Positive behavior learning disability, stress, - NS shift at W2+ & internalizing: ES = 0.12 Mother_reported domestic violence - NS shift at W2+ & externalizing: ES = 0.04 employed in at Mo = Child age/ gender, - NS shift at W2+ & positive: ES = -0.08 least one wave no of other adults in house Longitudinal AT= OLS/ FEM No significant interactions women Han & US: NSLY-CS Birth Mother & father: Adolescent C- MIN, welfare reliance, -Indirect effect of maternal# of years night shift and paternal# Miller, 2009 Five cohorts of to age Standard (6am-6pm), Depression Scale at family income, marital of years evening shift (via home environment) on depression (r.. children born 13/14 evening (2pm- age 13--14 status, birthweight, = -.036***) 1982-1991 years midnight), night Child_reported smoking or drinking in -Indirect effect of paternal# of years evening shift (via paternal (N = 4,200)1\ (9pm--8am), irregular. pregnancy, occupation closeness) on depression (r.. = -.096***) Longitudinal Measured as # of Me = Time with parents, -Indirect effect of maternal# of years irregular shift & paternal# years from birth to parent-adolescent of years irregular shifts (via mothers' knowledge of whereabouts age 11/12 relationship, monitoring, on depression (r.. = -.070***) 3 HOME score, frequency of meals, TV. AT= SEM Han & US: NLSY-CS, 8 1G-14 Mother & father: Risk-taking behavior: C = MIN, marital status, Two-parent families Waldfogel, waves 1988- years 6 category: Substance abuse birthweight, mother's Effect of mothers' evening shift (unmediated/mediated) on: 2007 2002 Standard (8am-6pm), Disobedience cognitive ability, income -Criminal behavior: OR= 1.48 (0.26)*/ OR= 1.38 (0.26) (N = 12,207)A Evening (2pm-12), Criminal behavior Mo = Family type. Single-mother families Cross-sectional night (9pm-8am), School-related trouble Me= Parental monitoring, Effect of mothers' rotating shift (unmediated/mediated) on: rotating, irregular Child_reported child-parent closeness. - Disobedience: OR= 1. 77 (0.41 )*I OR= 1.63 (0.38)* 3 AT= LOGR -Criminal behavior: OR= 1.57 (0.29)* I OR= 1.49 (0.28)* -School trouble: OR= 1.74 (0.33)** I OR= 1.59 (0.31)* Hsueh & US: Ll from 5-16 PCG (mother): 4- Behavioral problems C- MIN, parental gender, Effect of mothers' NS shift (without mediators) on: Yoshikawa, Milwaukee, years category variable - (BPI) at 2-year (age access to car, income, Parent-reported 2007 Wisconsin New Fixed NS shift (at 5-12) & 5-year (age receipt of AFDC -Internalizing (2-year): 13 = -.1 0/13 = -.13* (fixed /variable NS Hope Project least 50% hours 6-16) Me = Parental stress, shift) 1994-1995 outside 8am-4pm, Internalizing perceived time pressure, -Internalizing (5-year): 13 = .09/ 13 = -.05 (N = 486 incl. weekend); Externalizing regularity of family -Externalizing (2-year): 13 = .01/ 13 = .02 parents, 529 variable NS; variable School engagement mealtime. - Externalizing (5-year): 13 = -.03/ 13 = .02 children with standard vs. fixed School performance. AT= OLS Teacher-reported valid data) standard at 2-year Teacher and parent Longitudinal followup reported. -Internalizing (2-year): 13 = -.071 13 = .02 Adjusted for 2-year -Internalizing (5-year): 13 = -.02/ 13 = -.00 outcomes at 5--year - Externalizing (2-year): 13 = -.05/ 13 = .15* followup - Externalizing (5-year): 13 = -.05/ 13 = .01 Strazdins et Canada: NLSCY 2-11 Mother or father or Social & emotional C = MIN, child care use Effect of parental NS shift (without/with mediators) on social and al., 2006 1996--1997 years both NS (any incl. wellbeing derived from Mo =Child age (2-4/5- emotional wellbeing: (N = 4,306 DE weekends) vs. both CBCL (M= 0, SO= 1). 11), SES (derived - Father NS (all children): families, 6,156 standard PCG_reported composite) - Mother NS (all children): 13 = .14**/13 = .08* Me= Family functioning, - Both NS (all children): 13 = .14** /13 = .07* parental depressive - Father NS (1st SES quartile, all children): 13 = .16/13 = .07 symptoms, hostile or - Mother NS (1st SES quartile, all children): 13 = .18/13 = .09 ineffective parenting -Both NS (1st SES quartile, all children): 13 = .19*/13 = .07 AT= Linear mixed model - Father NS (pre-school children): 13 = .25**/13 = .18** with household random -Mother NS (pre-school children): 13 = .20**/13 = .12* children) 1\ Cross-sectional 3 13 = . 16**/13 = . 11 ** 3 effect -Both NS (pre-school children): 13 = .22**/13 = .14** Gassman- US: Children of Pre Mother: Child behavior C -MIN, teenage parent, Effect of each increasing hour of mothers' night work on: Pines, 2011 Ll working school Based on daily diaries Externalizing living with grandparent; -Externalizing: ES = 0.04 mothers from age -night (6pm-6am) or Internalizing other daily level covariates -Internalizing: ES = 0.04 preschool at weekend vs. daytime Positive behavior e.g., whether child was - Positive behaviors: ES = -0.06* four Head Start (8am-6pm), (#of Mother-child sick that day, care by Centres (N = 61 hours of each) interactions- 5 father. Interaction significant at p < .05 indicated that the effect of# of mothers, 724 subscales Mo =weekend hours worked at night on the weekend reduces positive behavior person-days) Maternal mood AT= MLM more so than # of hours worked at night on a weekday Cross-sectional Mother_reported I Han, 2006 US: NSAF, 6--17 Mother: Behavioral problems C- MIN, childcare type Effect of mothers' NS schedule on behavioral problems at: children of years NS (6am-6pm) vs. (BPI) Mo =Child age (6-11/12- - 6-11 yrs (1997/1999): ES = --0.06/0.01 standard Extra-curricular 17), marital status and -12-17 yrs (1997/1999): ES = -0.03/0.05 (N = 20,823 in activities work hours, family poverty 1997; School engagement and welfare status, N = 21,730 in MKA (mostly mother) parenting stress and 1999) reported mental health. working mothers 1\ 2 AT= OLS Cross-sectional US: 1999, 2-4 Mother: "regular" Behavioral problems, C- MIN, city, welfare, Effect of mothers' NS schedule (without/with mediating factors) Bog en, 206 Ll children years NS (all types including CBCL (M= 0, SO= 1): income, health insurance, on: 2007 from Welfare, weekend) vs. Internalizing depressive symptoms & -Internalizing: 13 = 0.47*1 13 = 0.32 Children & standard Externalizing social support, birthweight -Externalizing: 13 = 0.55**/ 13 = 0.37* Families: A Positive behavior or preterm -Positive behavior: ES = --0.36**/ ES = -0.27* Three City Mother_reported Mo = Child gender, oshi & 2 2 Study presence of biological Interactions significant at p <. 10 indicate the effect of mothers' Cross-sectional father, & other adults. NS schedules on internalizing was less if other adults were in the Me = Parenting stress. household; and effect on externalizing was less if child was a AT= OLS boy Strazdins et Canada: NLSCY 2-11 Mother/father/both At least one emotional C = MIN, child care use Effect of NS schedule on child emotional or behavioral difficulty: tl., 2004 1996-97 years NS (any incl. or behavioral difficulty Mo = Child age (2-4/5- - Father NS (all ages): OR= 1.29 (1.04-1.60)* (N = 4,433 DE weekends) vs. both (14%) 11),SES(derived - Mother NS (all ages): OR= 1.43 (1.13-1.81 )** families, 6361 standard - usually PCG_reported composite measure from - Both NS (all ages): children) worked in past 12 education, income and - Father NS (1st SES quartile, all ages): OR= 1.35 (0.83-2.19) months occupation) - Mother NS (1st SES quartile, all ages): OR= 1.67 (1.02-2. 75)* AT= LOGR - Both NS (1st SES quartile, all ages): OR= 1.62 (1.03-2.54)* 1\ Cross-sectional 2 OR= 1.40 (1.12-1. 73)** -Father NS (pre-school children): OR= 1.89 (1.30-2.74)*** - Mother NS (pre-school children): OR= 1.65 (1.09-2.48)* - Both NS (pre-school children): OR= 1.81 (1.24-2.66)** Barton et UK: (N= 190 8-11 Father: C =Age Effect of father working a regular shift on: al., 1998 children of years Shift or day CDI total score and Mo = Child gender -Perceived academic competence (girls only): F (1 ,80) = 4.40*; employed subscales AT= MANOVA and discrepancy between perceived and ideal levels of fathers- Child_reported (SPPC) subscales competence (girls only) F (1, 76) = 4.99 * manual/semi- Girls had more symptoms than boys when fathers worked shifts skilled workers) of depression (total score): F(1 ,76) = 4.93*; negative mood: F Cross-sectional 1,87)= 4.42*; interpersonal problems: F (1, 87) = 8.33**; and anhedonia: F (1, 87) = 4.30* Cognitive Ability (see also Han, 2006 and Hsueh & Yoshikawa, 2007 for outcomes related to school engagement, school performance and involvement in extracurricular activity) Odom et al., US: N- 231 2-3 yrs Mother: Child expressive C - Child age, income/ Effect of mother working a NS schedule without (with) mediators 0 2013 children of NS (fixed evening, language outcomes: needs ratio, work hours, on: employed fixed night, rotating NOW@ 24 months maternal education, formal -NOW at 24 months: 13 = -.16*/13 = -.12 African shift or irregular) vs PLS @ 36 months child care -PLS at 36 months: 13 = -.12*/13 = -.06 American standard (most hours Me= maternal positive mothers born in 8am-5pm) at 24 engagement, negative 2002 in low- months work-family spillover AT= OLS wealth rural households from The Family Life Project Longitudinal Han & Fox, US: NLSY-CS Birth Mother & father: PIAT Reading and C = MIN, marital status, Mothers 1011 (N = 7, 105). up to Standard (6am-6pm); Math (level and family income, welfare, Effect of# of years night shift (without/with mediators) on: Six cohorts of 13-14 Fixed evening (2pm- trajectory) from ages 5 occupation -Reading level: ES =- 0.02*/- 0.02* children born years 9pm); Fixed night to 14). Me = Child reported: time - Reading trajectory: ES = 0.01 1982-1993 who (9pm-6am); Variable together, maternal & - Math level: ES = 0.02/0.01 have been (other schedule). paternal closeness, parent - Math trajectory: ES = -0.05** missing events, parental Effect of# of years evening shift (without/with mediators) on: followed for 13-14 year # of years worked knowledge, HOME score, -Reading level: ES = -0.01/-0.01 period) evening, night or shared meals, after-school - Reading trajectory: ES = 0.00 variable shifts activities - Math level: ES = 0.03**/ 0.02 AT= Multilevel GCM - Math trajectory: ES = -0.04** A Longitudinal Effect of# of years variable shift (without/with mediators) on: - Reading level: ES = 0.02**/0.02** 4 - Reading trajectory: ES - Math level: ES =-0.02* =0.02**/0.01 - Math trajectory: ES =-0.02 Fathers Effect of# of years night shift (without/with mediators) on: -Reading level: ES =- 0.04**/0.01 - Math level: ES =0.03*/0.03* Effect of# of years evening shift (without/with mediators) on: - Reading level: ES - Math level: ES =-0.02/-0.02 =-0.02/-0.03* (Note that there was not association between fathers work schedule and trajectories) Han, 2005 US: NICHD- 0-3 Mother: BMDI at 15 & 24 C -MIN, maternal Effect of mother beginning NS schedule in the child's 1st year SECC (N= 900 years NS (combined months cognitive ability, family and continuing to 3rd year (without/with mediators) on: children whose evening 3pm- BSR at 36 months income, poverty, - Bayley MDI (15 months): ES =--0.20*/ ES =--0.13 mothers had midnight/ night Reynell Verbal depression at one month - Bayley MDI (24 months): ES =--0.21**/ ES =--0.12 worked in the (11 pm-7am)/ variable comprehension & Me first 3 years) hours) vs. standard. Expressive language employment, maternal - Reynell verbal (36 months): ES Children born in Measured seven at 36 months depression, home - Reynell expressive (36 months): ES 1991A combinations of onset Mother_reported. environment, mother's Longitudinal and duration =Amount of maternal sensitivity, childcare type and quality AT= OLS - Bracken school readiness (36 months): ES =--0.02/ ES =0.03 =--0.30***/ ES =-0.21 * =--0.20*/ ES =-0.15 (See paper for results for other schedule onset and duration) 3 eymann, US: NLSY-CS School Parents: Mathematical ability C =_Child gender, parental Effect of parent's NS work schedules on: 000 (pp 199G-1996 aged Evening (6-9pm) (PIAT); Vocabulary education, marital status, - Low maths achievement- bottom quartile on PIAT (#of hours Night shift Reading; Repeating a work hours, family income worked by parent in evening): OR= 1.17, p < 0.05 working year at school; AT= OLS -School suspension (night shift): OR= 2.72, p < .01 parents)A School suspension 55-56) (N = 4,689 Other OLS results N/A Cross-sectional Body Mass Index (BMI) Champion Australia: 9 Mother and father Overweight or obese C = MIN, time child Effect of mother's NS work schedule on: et al., 2012 Generation 1 years (partner living in the based on age and spends in front of a TV, - Overweight/obese: OR= 1.26, p > .05 Study (N = 434 home): gender standardised computer or game system Effect of father's NS work schedule on: children of Standard (9am - BMI (International AT= LOGR - Overweight/obese: OR= 1.97, p < .05 mothers living in 6pm); NS ('always' or Obesity Taskforce) Adelaide 2008- 'often' working shifts, - Overweight/obese: OR= 2.26, p > .05 2010) after 6pm or overnight (compared with standard work schedule) 1\ 2 Effect of both parents working a NS work schedule on: or weekend); Not Cross-sectional employed. Joint parental work schedules Morrissey et US: NICHD 8-12 Mother: Age and gender C- MIN, birthweight, child Effect (without mediators) of mothers' NS schedule on BMI (in al., 2011 SECC (N= 990 years NS (7pm-8am) vs. standardised BMI grade, income child FEM- authors preferred model): ES = 0.20 children in 3rd, standard at each Me = TV time, physical 5th and 6th grade/ number of data activity, HOME Effect (without mediators) of# of periods mother worked NS grades- points with NS environment, parental schedule on BMI (in child FEM): ES = 0.02 complete data schedules from 3 supervision &engagement, 4 for at least 2 months to 2nd grade mother depression grades). Born in - max 19) Mo = gender, grade, 1991/\ maternal education Longitudinal AT= REM I Child FEM No significant moderated effects Miller& US: NLSY-CS 13--14 # of years mother Continuous BMI C = MIN, birthweight, Effect of# of years mother worked NS shift on: Han, 2008 Five cohorts of years worked Risk of overweight mother's cognitive ability, BMI continuous if children born NS (evening 2pm- (cutoff >85th income at baseline, years -full sample: ES = 0.10* 1982-1991 midnight/night 9pm- percentile of BMI) in poverty, frequency of -ever I never a single parent: ES = 0.11/ ES = 0.08 (N = 2,353 8am/split/ other) vs. Child_reported TV, shared dinners; - income quartiles 1 to 4: ES = 0.22; 0.27*; 0.04; 0.07 children of standard (6am-6pm) mother's BMI BMI >85th percentile if mothers who Mo = Family income, -full sample: OR= 1.34(1.07-1.68)* ever worked)/\ whether child had ever -ever I never a single parent: OR= 1.25(0.91-1. 71 )/OR= Longitudinal lived with a single mother. 1.43(1.03-1.99)* AT= OLS/LOGR - income quartiles 1 to 4: OR= 1.66 (0.97-2.85); 1.97(1.20-3.26)**; 1.05(0.68-1.60); 1.18(0.77-1.83) Sleep Patterns Radosevic- Croatia: 11-18 Couple: Sleep patterns: C = child gender, type of Effect of having one shiftworking parent on: rrek& (N_=_2,363 years Both day, one Child reported school (elementary/high) - usual waketime of high school students when attending school students in DE nonstandard, both Usual bedtime AT= MANOVA in the morning (earlier): F (2, 1,360)= 4.97** families 2001- nonstandard Usual waketime MO = child gender Effect of having both shiftworking parents on: 02)/\ Child reported Calculated -sleep duration of high school students attending school in the Sleep duration morning (shorter): F (2, 1,360)= 5.24** Bedtime delay and - bedtime on the weekend (later): F (2, 1 ,360) = 7.85** sleep extension - See paper for more results on bedtime delay and sleep oscec, 2004 Cross-sectional 3 extension on weekends. +p< 0.10 *p < .05, **p < .01' ***p < .001 Note. b = unstandardized coefficient; !3 =standardized coefficient; BMDI = AFDC =Aid to Families with Dependent Children; Bayley Mental Development Index; BMI =Body Mass Index; BPI = Behavioral Problems Index; BSR = Bracken School Readiness; CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist; CDI = Children's Depression Inventory; DE = dual earner; ECLSBC = Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey Birth Cohort; ES =Effect Size calculated by authors as b/SOy; FEM = Fixed Effects Model; FT= full-time; GCM =Growth Curve Modelling; HI LOA= Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia; HOME= Home Observation Measurement of the Environment; ITSC =Infant Toddler Symptom Checklist; Ll =low income; LOGR =Logistic Regression; MANOVA = Multivariate Analysis of Variance; M = mean; MKA = Most Knowledgeable Adult; MLM = Multilevel Modelling; N/A = not available; NOW= Number of different words; NICHD-SECC = National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care; NLSCY = National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth; NLSY-CS = National Longitudinal Study of Youth- Child Supplement; NSAF = National Survey of American Families; OR= Odds Ratio; OLS =Ordinary Least Squares regression; PCG =Primary Caregiver; PIAT = Peabody Individual Achievement Test; PLS = Preschool Language Scale; PSM = Propensity Score Matching; SO= standard deviation; REM = Random Effects Model; SEM = Structural Equation Modelling; SES =socioeconomic status; SF36 =Short Form 36; SPPC =Self-perception profile for children; UK =United Kingdom; US= United States; WES =Women's Employment Study; W1 =one wave; W2+ =two or more waves; AStudy is representative of a population or subpopulation. aM IN= minimum set of sociodemographic confounders and covariates included in the analysis i.e., child gender, child age (or developmental stage), number of children in household (or presence of siblings/birth order), family structure (couple/lone, presence of a non-biological parent, marital status), parental age (at least of mother), parental work hours (at least of mother, and at least FT/PT status), parental education, race/ethnicity (of parent or child). bWhere possible all results for the study have been provided regardless of statistical significance. When there are too many results to report in the table, only those significant at p < .05 have been presented (e.g., for SEM models). cstudy quality rating is from 0-5 indicating the number of criterion met by the study as determined by the authors: (1) sample is representative of population or subpopulation, (2) study design is longitudinal, (3) a minimum set of sociodemographic confounders have been considered, (4) analytical methods have been used to address selection bias, and, (5) the study has considered at least one moderator and one mediator.
© Copyright 2024