Research in Nursing & Health, 2007, 30, 333–346 Correlates of Mother–Premature Infant Interactions Diane Holditch-Davis,1{ Todd Schwartz,2z Beth Black,2§ Mark Scher3k 1 School of Nursing, Duke University, Durham, NC School of Nursing, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 3 Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, OH Accepted 25 November 2006 2 Abstract: This study’s purpose was to examine whether child characteristics, child illness severity, maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, and paternal support influenced interactions between 108 premature infants and their mothers. Mothers with singletons or more infant illness stress showed more positive involvement. Mothers with less infant illness stress, less education, or less participation in caregiving by fathers showed more negative control. First-time mothers and mothers of singletons provided more developmental stimulation. Children of younger and White mothers showed more social behaviors. Less maternal education and shorter period of mechanical ventilation were associated with greater developmental maturity. Greater maternal worry was related to more child irritability. These findings are consistent with the developmental science view that the mother–premature relationship is a complex, reciprocal process. ß 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Res Nurs Health 30:333–346, 2007 Keywords: premature infants; mother–infant interactions; parenting; infant behavior Interactions between prematurely born children and their mothers are of interest to nurses because these interactions are known to affect child developmental outcome (Forcada-Guex, Pierrehumbert, Borghini, Moessinger, & MullerNix, 2006; Smith, Landry, & Swank, 2006). More maternal positive involvement, lower use of negative control strategies, and more developmental stimulation (talking, teaching) are related to better outcomes for premature infants (Berlin, Brooks-Gunn, Spiker, & Zaslow, 1995; ForcadaGuex et al.; Olsen, Bates, & Kaskie, 1992; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). However, interactions of mothers with prematures are less mutually satisfying and developmentally appropriate than those with fullterms (Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Schmucker et al., 2005). Mothers of premature infants work harder to initiate and maintain Contract grant sponsor: National Institute for Nursing Research, National Institutes of Health; Contract grant number: NR01894. Correspondence to Diane Holditch-Davis, Duke University School of Nursing, Trent Drive, DUMC 3322 Durham, NC 27710. { Professor. z Research Assistant Professor. § Assistant Professor. k Professor and Director of Division of Pediatric Neurology. We thank Sola Park, Lisa Moorehead, Donna Harris, Mary Barkey, Leslie Miller, Paula Anderson, Jason Dickenson, William Wooten, Chithra Jeyaram, Tzu-Ying Lee, Laura Pence, Samia Shreitah, David Sterka, Daisy Wilson, Tanya Kewson, and Mark Johnson for technical assistance. Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/nur.20190 ß 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 334 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH interactions but receive fewer positive responses from their infants than mothers of fullterms (Singer et al., 2003). Many factors—both related and unrelated to prematurity—influence these interaction difficulties. According to the developmental science perspective (Cairns, Elder, & Costello, 1996; Thoman, Acebo, & Becker, 1983), children develop in a continuous, reciprocal interaction with the environment and are both influenced by and influence it. The child and environment form a complex system, made up of elements that are also systems, such as the mother and child (Thoman et al.). Interactions between mothers and children are affected by factors within the child, such as child characteristics and illness severity; factors within the mother, such as maternal characteristics and psychological well-being; and factors in the larger family environment, such as paternal support for the mother. Understanding how these factors relate to mother–premature interactions may help nurses plan interventions to improve interactions. The literature is inconsistent on the effects of these factors, and few investigators have studied them from a systems perspective. The purpose of this study, therefore, was to examine the degree to which selected child characteristics and illness severity, maternal characteristics, and psychological well-being, and a family characteristic—paternal support—are related to interactions between mothers and their premature infants. A child characteristic that has been reported to affect mother–premature infant interactions is sex. From 3 to 36 months corrected age, mothers have been reported alternatively to be less responsive to high-risk infant girls than to boys (Engelke & Engelke, 1992) or to respond more verbally and express more positive affect to highrisk premature girls than boys (Cho, HolditchDavis, & Belyea, 2004; Feldman & Eidelman, 2004). Three-year-old prematurely born girls looked at their mothers more and expressed more positive affect than did boys (Cho et al.). Feldman and Eidelman found that mothers provided more proprioceptive touch and less functional touch to boys than to girls from 3 to 24 months, but Weiss, Wilson, Hertenstein, and Campos (2000) found no differences in touching based on sex at 3 months. All of these studies included ethnically and socioeconomically diverse mothers; most infants were premature and had high-risk medical courses, although Feldman and Eidelman and Weiss et al. studied fullterms and healthier prematures. Multiple birth also affects interactions between mothers and premature infants, and a greater Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur percent of prematures than fullterms are multiple births (Blondel & Kaminski, 2002). Ethnically and socio-economically diverse mothers of multiples had less time for play and were less sensitive than mothers of singletons from birth to 24 months (Feldman & Eidelman, 2004; Feldman, Eidelman, & Rotenberg, 2004; Ostfeld, Smith, Hiatt, & Hegyi, 2000). In the first month after term, multiple birth infants were left alone more and looked at, talked to, and held less often by middle-class mothers (Holditch-Davis, Roberts, & Sandelowski, 1999; Ostfeld et al.). Triplets looked at and vocalized less often to their mothers than twins or singletons from birth to 24 months (Feldman & Eidelman). Caring for multiple birth infants is more difficult and stressful for mothers than caring for a single infant (Feldman et al). Despite this, in one study, interactions between socio-economically diverse mothers and their very-low-birthweight multiple birth infants did not differ from those of similar mothers and singletons from 1 to 9 months corrected age (Goldberg, Perrotta, Minde, & Corter, 1986). Sick premature infants were included in few of these studies; thus, more research is needed on the effects of high-risk multiple birth infants on mother–child interactions. Infant illness severity also affects the development of positive mother–infant interactions. Lowincome mothers of 12–24-month-old prematurely born toddlers with larger birthweights interacted more positively with their children than did mothers of toddlers with smaller birthweights (Feingold, 1994), and middle-class mothers interacted more positively with multiple birth and premature infants with fewer medical complications than with infants with more complications between birth and 24 months (Feldman et al., 2004; Muller-Nix et al., 2004). At 4 and 8 months corrected age, socio-economically diverse mothers of premature infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD) were less sensitive to infant cues and distress than similar mothers of fullterms or prematures without BPD (Jarvis, Myers, & Creasey, 1989), and infants with BPD were less responsive to maternal interactive behaviors at 8 and 12 months than low-risk infants (Singer et al., 2003). At 6 and 12 months corrected age, premature infants with severe neonatal medical complications, including BPD and intraventricular hemorrhage, were at greater risk for interactive difficulties than infants with milder illnesses (Landry, Smith, Miller-Loncar, & Swank, 1997). However, other investigators found that between 6 months and 4 years, mothers of sicker children showed more social behaviors to CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. compensate for their infant’s immaturity and lack of responsiveness (Holditch-Davis, Cox, Miles, & Belyea, 2003; McGrath, Sullivan, & Seifer, 1998). Maternal characteristics such as ethnicity, education, age, and parity also may affect mother–premature infant interactions. From 1 to 3 years corrected age, African-American mothers of prematures showed less warmth, talked less, touched less, provided fewer learning activities, and use more negative control strategies than White mothers even when SES was controlled (Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov, & Duncan, 1996; Cho et al., 2004; Tesh & Holditch-Davis, 1997). From birth to 36 months, ethnically diverse mothers with more education made less use of negative control and provided more stimulation for learning and more nurturing touch, looking, talking, and interactions than mothers with lower education levels (Feeley, Gottlieb, & Zelkowitz, 2005; Gordon, Chase-Lansdale, & Brooks-Gunn, 2004; Tesh & Holditch-Davis; Weiss et al., 2000). Older and first-time mothers of prematures provided more nurturing touch at 3 months corrected age (Weiss et al.) and were more emotionally responsive at 6–36 months than younger or multiparous mothers (Engelke & Engelke, 1992). However, Gerner (1999) found that parity had no affect on mother–premature infant interactions at 3 and 6 months even though it did affect interactions with fullterms. Sick premature infants were included in these studies; thus, maternal characteristics of ethnicity, education, age, and parity affected interactions even with high-risk prematures. Maternal psychological well-being also may affect mother–premature infant interactions. The experience of delivering a low-birthweight infant and the possibility of infant death place mothers at increased risk for psychological distress during the neonatal period (Singer, Davillier, Bruening, Hawkins, & Yamashita, 1996). Eight months after term, middle class, White mothers of prematures reported more parenting stress than mothers of fullterms, even when infant behaviors did not differ (Moran & Pederson, 1998). Mothers with better overall mental health had more responsive premature infants at 3 months corrected age (Schmucker et al., 2005; Weiss & Chen, 2002). Depressive symptoms in mothers of high-risk premature infants had a negative effect on maternal feeding competency and caregiving evaluations over the first year of life (Pridham et al., 2005; Pridham, Lin, & Brown, 2001), and depressed mothers were less responsive to their prematures, showed less positive interactive Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 335 behaviors, provided less cognitive growth promoting stimulation, and reported less attachment to them than non-depressed mothers (Feldman, Weldman, Leckman, Kuint, & Eidelman, 1999; Singer et al., 2003). With the exception of Moran and Pederson, these studies included ethnically and socio-economically diverse mothers and premature infants with high-risk medical courses. Finally, paternal support for the mother probably affects mother–premature infant interactions. Fathers of newborns have a uniquely important role in supporting mothers (Mercer, 2004). Although ethnically and socio-economically diverse mothers of prematures reported that fathers were their major source of support (Miles, Carlson, & Funk, 1996) and, for most of them, their closest relationship (Coffman, Levitt, & Guacci-Franco, 1993), how this factor affects maternal interactions with premature infants has been studied only rarely. Mothers of disabled children described fathers as being supportive when they provided financial support and helped with child care (Traustadottir, 1991). Fathers of premature infants provided more infant caregiving than fathers of fullterms at 1 month after discharge (Brown, Rustia, & Schappert, 1991) and had more positive interactions with their infants at 3 months corrected age (Harrison, 1990). AfricanAmerican, but not White, fathers who were more involved with their prematures had children with better developmental outcomes (Yogman, Kindlon, & Earls, 1995). How these effects relate to the interactions of mothers with their premature infants needs to be explored. Over the infant’s first year, paternal support was related to better mental health in mothers of prematures (Weiss & Chen, 2002), and unmarried mothers of medically fragile infants did not differ from married mothers in reporting the helpfulness of paternal support although they were less satisfied with it (Lee, Miles, & Holditch-Davis, 2006). Although factors that affect mother–premature infant interactions have been examined in previous studies, the direction of the effects, in many cases, have been contradictory. Researchers rarely have examined more than one or two maternal and infant factors. One exception is Feeley et al. (2005), who examined variables in all of these factors except child characteristics. However, they only used a global measure of maternal interactive behaviors, the total score from the Nursing-Child Assessment Teaching Scale (Barnard, 1983). Multiple maternal and infant factors have not been studied along with specific maternal and child interactive dimensions, as required by the developmental science perspective. Therefore, 336 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH the aim of the current study was to examine the degree to which selected child characteristics, child illness severity, selected maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, and paternal support were related to interactions between mothers and their premature infants at 6 and 18 months corrected for prematurity. Child characteristics examined were sex and singleton versus multiple birth. Child illness severity variables were birthweight, days of mechanical ventilation, and number of rehospitalizations. Maternal characteristics were ethnicity, education, age, and parity. Maternal psychological wellbeing was measured as perceived stress in the NICU due to unit and baby factors, perceived stress in the NICU due to parental role alteration, worry about child health, and depressive symptoms. Paternal support was determined by maternal marital status and maternal report of the amount of father participation in child caregiving. METHODS Participants Participants were 108 infants and their mothers who were enrolled in a larger study of biological and social risks of prematurity conducted from 1997 to 2003 (Holditch-Davis, Scher, Schwartz, & Hudson-Barr, 2004). Infants were required to be less than 35 weeks gestational age and either have a birthweight less than 1,500 g or require mechanical ventilation or continuous positive airway pressure. Seventy-three (68%) had both problems. Infants with congenital problems affecting development (such as Down Syndrome) were excluded, but infants with postnatal neurological insults were eligible. Infants were recruited from two tertiary hospitals in the United States: 58 from a southeastern perinatal center and 50 from a midwestern children’s hospital. All mother–child dyads with interaction data at 6 or 18 months were included in this report. These contacts occurred at times of different levels of stress in the mother–child relationship. Six months is a relatively stable time, when the transition home is complete and initial interactive differences between fullterms and prematures have decreased (Crawford, 1982; Wille, 1991). At 18 months, toddlers’ developing motor and language skills present new challenges in mother– child interactions. The infants had a mean gestational age of 28.9 weeks (SD, 2.7; range Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 23–34) and a mean birthweight of 1,232 (SD, 430). All infants, except two, one of whom was large for gestational age, were under 2,500 g at birth. They were 52% male, 69% singletons, and 31% multiples (25% twins, 6% triplets). They had a mean of 10.3 days of mechanical ventilation (SD, 17.6) with 87 infants requiring ventilation; 17% had a Grade I or II intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH), and 5% had a Grade III or IV IVH. Their mothers averaged 28.6 years of age (SD, 6.6) and 13.7 years of education (SD, 2.3). Fifty-nine percent were married; 43% were AfricanAmerican, 56% White, and 1% Asian. No mother was married to or lived with a partner other than the child’s father during the study. About 56% were first-time mothers. Four delivered another baby between the 6- and 18-month contacts. The only differences between infants and mothers from the two hospitals were that infants from the southeastern hospital were more likely to be multiple births (41.4% vs. 18.4%) and first births (67.2% vs. 42.0%). Calculating a power analysis for the mixed model is not feasible. An approximation based on a regression model showed that with a sample size of 108, 7 independent variables in the model, and an R2 of at least 0.125 (or 3 independent variables and an R2 of at least .095), the power to reject R2 ¼ 0 would exceed 0.80 at the .05 significance level (Gatsonis & Sampson, 1989). Variables Mother–child interactions. Videotapes of mother–infant interactions in the home were made at 6 and 18 months corrected age. They were scored several months later with the mother– child coding system used in previous studies (Holditch-Davis, Cox, et al., 2003; HolditchDavis, Miles, et al., 2001). The occurrence of child and mother behaviors were scored in each 10-second interval with each behavior only counted once in each interval. For scoring, the codes were divided into five subsets of behaviors, each of which was scored by two or three coders. The first author trained the coders until they achieved acceptable inter-rater reliability (kappas greater than .70) on their subset of behaviors. Ongoing inter-rater reliability was checked every couple of months by having two coders score the same subset of behaviors on the same tape. Cohen’s kappas for the maternal behaviors used in this report ranged from .76 to .91 with a mean of .82; for child behaviors, kappas ranged from .84 to .96 with a mean of .90. CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. 337 Table 1. Interactive Behaviors Scored During the Videotapes Mother Behaviors Interact Proximity Uninvolved Play with Child Positive Negative Child Behaviors — — — — Positive Negative Touch Talk Teach Talk — Together — — — — — Vocalize Object play Locomotion Walk — — — Look Gesture Fuss — Definitions Talks to, touches, gestures toward, or plays with child Close enough to achieve eye contact with the child Not interacting with, playing with, or looking at the child Initiates or takes part in child’s play activity or games Directs positive affect to mother or child (e.g., smile or kiss) Directs negative affect towards mother or child (e.g., frown or scold) Touches child in a non-negative way (e.g., pat or caress) Speaks words to mother or child Instructs child (e.g., names an object, demonstrates an activity) Interacts with the child but is not caregiving. Makes a non-fussy sound but does not say words Plays with an object but not with a person Moves body but without walking (e.g., belly creep or scoot) Walks independently or by supporting self on objects or people Focuses eyes on face of mother Makes a gesture, such as showing a toy or shaking head Makes a negative sound (e.g., crying or whining) These behaviors were combined into maternal and child variables, as described in Table 1. To adjust for variation in the lengths of videotapes, most variables were measured as a percentage of the scoreable videotape. Play with objects was measured as a percentage of the time that the child was not playing with the mother so that it measured the child’s ability to play independently. HOME. The HOME inventory (0–3 version) was designed to identify children under 3 years who are at risk for developmental delay due to a home environment lacking appropriate stimulation (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984). The HOME consists of 45 items arranged in 6 subscales. Each item is scored as present or absent, and the score equals the number of present items. The HOME is administered using both maternal interview and observation. The first author trained research assistants at both sites until they achieved 90% inter-rater reliability. Test-retest reliability for the total scale over 6-month intervals was .76, and internal consistency was .89 (Holditch-Davis, Tesh, Goldman, Miles, & D’Auria, 2000). Two HOME subscales were used in this report— subscale II, Acceptance of the Child’s Behavior and subscale V, Maternal Involvement. Two items from subscale II that were not directly related to maternal negative control—the presence of pets and more than 10 books in the house—were omitted. Similar to other studies (Bradley, Rock, Caldwell, & Brisby, 1989; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984; Holditch-Davis, Tesh, et al.), internal Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur consistency of the modified subscale II for this sample combining 6 and 18 months was .73; subscale V was .60. Maternal-child interactive dimensions. Ten maternal behaviors from the observation and two HOME subscales were grouped into three interactive dimensions: positive involvement, developmental stimulation, and negative control. Maternal dimensions were derived theoretically to capture the major types of maternal interactive behaviors shown to affect child development and to be affected by maternal depression (Berlin et al., 1995; Field, 1995; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Singer et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2006). Ten child behaviors were grouped into three child interactive dimensions: child social, developmental maturity, and irritability. These child dimensions were derived theoretically from the major types of child behaviors shown to be related to child developmental outcomes and affected by maternal depression (Field, 1995; Holditch-Davis, Bartlett, & Belyea, 2000; Holditch-Davis, Docherty, Miles, & Burchinal, 2001). Dimension scores were calculated by converting each variable to Z-scores (calculated from the data from both 6 and 18 months) and then averaging the Z-scores for each dimension’s variables. Because the mean and SD were based on both ages, these Z-scores preserved any existing differences between the two ages. These dimensions were developed using data from two different samples: 3-year-old prematurely born children and 12–24-month-old 338 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH toddlers of HIV-infected mothers. Internal consistency of these dimensions ranged from .57 to .98 in both samples, and correlations over ages were significant (Holditch-Davis, 2002). Table 2 gives each dimension’s component variables, alphas for internal consistency, means, and SDs. Child characteristics and illness severity. Sex, multiple birth, birthweight, and days of mechanical ventilation were obtained from the neonatal medical record. Because the length of mechanical ventilation was skewed (M 10.3; SD, 17.6), infants with a day or less of ventilation were scored as 1 day, and each subject’s score was transformed by using its logarithm. The number of rehospitalizations was obtained from a child health history completed by the mother at 2, 6, 9, 13, and 18 months. The number of rehospitalizations between neonatal hospital discharge and 6 months and between 6 and 18 months were used in analyses. Maternal characteristics. Maternal characteristics of ethnicity, education, and age were recorded on the Demographic Information Questionnaire that mothers completed at enrollment and at 2, 6, 9, 13, and 18 months. Ethnicity was scored as White or minority. One minority mother was Asian; the rest were African-American. Parity was obtained from the infant’s medical record. Maternal psychological well-being. Maternal psychological well-being was measured with the Parental Stressor Scale: NICU, the Worry Index, and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. The mother’s perception of the stress Table 2. Internal Consistency, Means, and SDs of the Mother–Child Interactive Dimensions for 99 Mother– Premature Infant Dyads at 6 Months and 92 Dyads at 18 Months 6 Months 18 Months M (SD) M (SD) Interaction Proximity Uninvolved with childc Play with child Mother positive Mother touch HOME subscale V Total Mother negative HOME subscale IIc Total Mother talk 71.63 (19.26) 28.38 (20.98) 20.09 (17.94) 15.70 (11.19) 10.08 (8.82) 23.56 (13.24) 5.43 (0.85) 0.06 (0.58) 0.23 (0.96) 5.77 (0.92) 0.23 (0.73) 40.94 (18.61) 57.38 (21.90) 84.47 (14.12) 34.21 (21.72) 14.08 (12.41) 8.56 (7.27) 9.28 (6.37) 5.55 (0.78) 0.07 (0.60) 0.73 (1.28) 5.20 (1.15) 0.24 (0.87) 48.29 (22.59) Teach Total Child positive Child look Child gesture Total Vocalize and talk 1.81 (3.50) 0.33 (0.53) 6.25 (4.62) 22.34 (10.70) 13.57 (7.85) 0.28 (0.65) 25.21 (12.72) 12.31 (12.62) 0.35 (1.04) 7.91 (5.24) 29.72 (12.98) 21.01 (8.72) 0.30 (0.83) 36.70 (16.26) Talk as % of vocalize þ talk Play with objects Locomote and walk Walk as % of locomote þ walk Total Child negative Child fuss as % of together Total 0.00 (0.00) 27.23 (14.86) 3.99 (6.58) 8.73 (24.71) 0.63 (0.27) 6.76 (6.90) 7.75 (9.37) 0.19 (1.09) 30.07 (21.25) 48.28 (18.70) 37.66 (15.88) 84.20 (24.75) 0.68 (0.50) 4.49 (4.77) 4.38 (6.62) 0.20 (0.74) Dimensiona Positive involvement, alpha ¼ .70 Negative control, alpha ¼ .56 Developmental stimulation, alpha ¼ .71 Child Social, alpha ¼ .71 Child developmental maturity, alpha ¼ .82 Child irritability, alpha ¼ .91 Variablesb a Dimensions totals are means of Z-scores for the component variables. Videotape variables are percentages of the total videotape time, except for variables with their divisors in their titles and Play with Objects, which was measured as a percentage of the time that the child was not playing with the mother. c These variables were reverse scored when combined with the other variables in the dimension. b Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. due to parenting her infant during hospitalization was measured with the Parental Stressor Scale: NICU (PSS:NICU; Miles, Funk, & Carlson, 1993). This tool has two subscales: the unit environment and infant illness (19 items) and parental role alterations (7 items). Mothers rate the items on a 5-point scale ranging from ‘‘not at all stressful’’ to ‘‘extremely stressful.’’ At enrollment and the 2-month contact, mothers were asked to retrospectively rate their experiences during hospitalization. PSS:NICU scores were correlated with anxiety scores and have good test-retest reliability (Miles et al., 1993; Shields-Poe & Pinelli, 1997). Internal consistency for the current study at enrollment and 2 months was .92 and .94 for the NICU environment and infant illness subscale and .93 and .92 for the parental alterations subscale. Because scores at enrollment and 2 months were correlated at .69 for the unit and baby subscale and .62 for the parental alterations subscale, the mean of item scores for each subscale over the two administrations was used in analyses. The value for the single administration was used for 14 mothers with only the enrollment data and 10 with only the 2-month data. The Worry Index (Miles & Holditch-Davis, 1995) was administered at all data collection points. On the Worry Index, mothers rate how much they worry about their child’s medical problems, normality, length of illness and possible death, re-hospitalization, caring for the child at home, the infant getting enough to eat, and sleeping enough. The mother rates the seven items using a 5-point scale from ‘‘not at all’’ to ‘‘very much.’’ The mean of the item scores at 6 and 18 months were used in analyses. Worry scores of mothers of premature and medically fragile infants were related to depressive and posttraumatic stress symptoms and to maternal personal growth (Holditch-Davis, Bartlett, Blickman, & Miles, 2003; Miles, Holditch-Davis, Scher, & Schwartz, in press). Internal consistency ranged from .71–.90 in previous studies (Holditch-Davis, Bartlett, et al., 2003; Miles & Holditch-Davis) and was .86 at 6 months and .78 at 18 months in the current study. The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD), used to measure depressive symptoms, focuses on depressed mood, particularly guilt, worthlessness, helplessness, and hopelessness (Radloff, 1977). The frequency of occurrence for each of 20 items is rated on a 4-point scale indicating how often in the last week they were experienced, ranging from rarely or none of the time (less than 1 day) to most or all of Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 339 the time (5–7 days). Scores on the CESD range from 0 to 60; higher scores indicate more depressive symptoms. The CESD is correlated with other measures of depression (Radloff; Weissman, Sholomskas, Pottenger, Prusoff, & Locke, 1977) and with other measures of psychological well-being in mothers of premature infants, including hospital environmental stress and worry about the child’s health (Miles et al., in press). Internal consistency for the current study ranged from .81 at 6 months to .86 at enrollment. CESD scores at enrollment, 6 months, and 18 months were used in analyses. Paternal support. Paternal support was measured using maternal marital status and the number of child caregiving activities performed by the child’s biological father. Marital status was obtained from the Demographic Information Questionnaire. At 6 and 18 months, the mother completed a questionnaire asking which of 18 caregiving activities were performed by the infant’s father, whether or not the mother was married to him and whether or not he lived in the child’s household. Internal consistency (KR-20) was .96 at 6 months and .97 at 18 months. Procedures The committees for protection of human subjects for the two study sites approved the study. Infants were enrolled when they were no longer critically ill (not receiving mechanically ventilation or with medical conditions that were immediately life-threatening) as long as an additional hospital stay of at least 1 week was anticipated and the mother provided informed consent. After enrollment, mothers filled out the demographic and psychological well-being questionnaires. Follow-up at home was conducted at 6 and 18 months corrected for prematurity. The 6-month videotapings were scheduled at a time when the infant was awake and due for a feeding. Given the increasing independence of older infants, the 18-month observation was scheduled at a time when the child was awake and not eating and when the mother was at home. The mother was told that the purpose of the videotape was to record the child’s spontaneous behaviors and play in the home. The mother was asked to do what she would typically do if at home with the child and to act as though the videotaper were not present. The videotaper remained with the infant even if the mother left the room. At the conclusion of a family’s involvement with the study, the mother was given a copy of the infant’s and her previous 340 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH videotapes. Although we planned to visit all families at 6 and 18 months, 9 families missed the 6-month contact, and 16 missed the 18-month contact. The HOME Inventory was administered after the videotaped interaction, and the scorer used information about the social environment gained during the videotaping as well as during the rest of the visit. Scoring the HOME took about 15 minutes. Although the HOME was partially scored using the same situation as the mother– child videotapes, the two measures were not redundant, as the interaction codes could be used to score only 8 of 45 items on the HOME. The mothers also completed child health history questionnaires at 2, 6, 9, 13, and 18 months. Mothers were paid $10 each time they completed questionnaires. The child was given a small gift at each home visit. Data Analyses Alpha was set at .05 for two-tailed tests. The number of covariates in the model was based on the general rule of thumb of having at least 5–10 observations per model term, which for 108 mother–infant dyads (and 191 observations) allowed us to fit 10–20 explanatory variables (Muller & Fetterman, 2002). Maternal and child interactive dimensions at 6 and 18 months were regressed over age using the general linear mixed model (GLMM, hereafter referred to as mixed model), a flexible statistical procedure for analyzing continuous longitudinal data that accommodates missing values and mistimed data (HolditchDavis, Edwards, & Helms, 1998). The mixed model, a generalization of the standard linear regression, allows analysis of data with several sources of variation, rather than just one. Like the standard model, it assesses the relationship between continuous outcome measures and explanatory variables. Unlike a standard regression, R2 cannot be determined in an unambiguous manner for the general linear mixed model because the model has correlated errors and multiple variance terms, not just a single one. Parameters of the mixed model include population (fixed) effects and individual (random) effects. Because the ages of the children at the 6- and 18-month contacts varied slightly, the actual age of the child in weeks past term (40 weeks post-menstrual age) at the time of 6- and 18-month contacts was used in analyses. Age was centered so that the intercept equaled the value at 26 weeks (6 months). Only the intercept and age were included in the random Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur effects component, providing mother–child dyadspecific intercepts and slopes across age. Covariates were child characteristics, child illness severity, maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, paternal support, and their interactions with child age. Four of these variables were measured at both 6 and 18 months: number of rehospitalizations, worry about child health, depressive symptoms, and number of caregiving activities performed by the father. For the remaining covariates (sex, multiple birth, birthweight, days of mechanical ventilation, ethnicity, education, age, parity, NICU stress due to unit and baby factors, NICU stress due to parental role alteration), a single value was used for each dyad. Diagnostics performed on the model residuals to determine whether model assumptions were satisfied indicated a departure from the assumptions for all interactive dimensions except positive involvement. Therefore, all dimensions except positive involvement were transformed using the natural logarithm. An elimination process was used to simplify the model. First, each group of variables was examined separately. Variables with p < .15 were included in an overall mixed model. Interactions of each main effect with age were added. A groupwise test of the interactions was performed at p < .15, and individual interactions tested only if the group met this criterion. Remaining variables were reduced until all variables in a final mixed model were p < .05. This procedure simplified the model and led to inferences that some effects were either zero or not large enough to be detected. RESULTS Examining dyads with data at both 6 and 18 months, all maternal dimensions, but not child dimensions, were significantly correlated over age, ranging from r(83) ¼ .28 for negative control to r(83) ¼ .41 for developmental stimulation. As Table 3 shows, some dimensions were intercorrelated within age. Maternal dimensions of positive involvement and developmental stimulation were correlated at both ages. Negative control was unrelated to other maternal dimensions. The child dimensions were unrelated except for a positive relation between child social behavior and irritability at 6 months and between child social behavior and developmental maturity at 18 months. Child social behavior was positively related to all maternal dimensions at both 6 and 18 months. Developmental maturity was related to maternal negative control at 6 months. Child CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. 341 Table 3. Correlations Among the Mother–Child Interactive Dimensions at 6 Months (n ¼ 99, Above the Diagonal) and at 18 Months (n ¼ 92, Bolded and Below the Diagonal) Dimension 1. Positive involvement 2. Negative control 3. Develop. stimulation 4. Child social 5. Develop. maturity 6. Irritability 1. Pos. 2. Neg. .02 .05 .80*** .40*** .02 .12 .12 .22* .12 .24* 3. Stim. .53*** .16 .22* .08 .14 4. Social 5. Maturity 6. Irritab. .36*** .23* .41*** .01 .35*** .19 .11 .21* .19 .04 .05 .03 .27** .10 .03 Note: Pos., positive; Develop., developmental; Neg., negative; Stim., stimulation; Irritab., irritability. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. irritability was related only to maternal negative control at 18 months. Correlates of Interactive Dimensions Table 4 presents the final mixed models for each interactive dimension. As shown by the direction of the corrected age effect, maternal positive involvement decreased with age; maternal developmental stimulation, negative control, child social behavior, and developmental maturity increased. Maternal positive involvement was higher for mothers of singletons and mothers with more stress due to the NICU environment and the infant’s illness. Less maternal stress due to the NICU environment and infant illness, less maternal education, and less paternal caregiving participation were related to more maternal negative control. Mothers of singletons and firsttime mothers provided more developmental stimulation than other mothers. Children of younger and White mothers showed more social behaviors. Shorter mechanical ventilation and less maternal education and were related to greater child developmental maturity. More maternal worry about child health was related to more child irritability. There were no significant interactions with age. Table 4. Relationship of Maternal and Child Interactive Dimensions at 6 and 18 Months to Child Characteristics, Illness Severity, Maternal Characteristics, Maternal Psychological Well-Being, and Paternal Support in the 108 Mother–Premature Infant Dyads Covariates Intercepta Estimate (SE) Corrected Age Estimate (SE) .14 (.16) .003* (.001) .41 (.32) .01*** (.001) Developmental stimul. .42*** (.05) .01*** (.001) Child social behaviors .82*** (.15) .01*** (.001) Developmental maturity .58*** (.09) .01*** (.001) .76*** (.20) .004 (.003) Dimension Positive involvement Negative control Child irritability Variable Multiple birth NICU stress NICU stress Mat. education Father careg. Multiple birth Parity Mother age Ethnicity Mech. ventilat. Mat. education Worry Estimate (SE) .28** (.10) .11* (.05) .10* (.04) .04* (.02) .01* (.01) .14* (.07) .15* (.04) .01* (.005) .18* (.06) .06* (.02) .02** (.01) .24** (.08) Note: Stimul., stimulation; Mat. education, maternal education; Father Careg., number of paternal caregiving activities; NICU Stress, stress due to baby’s illness and the NICU environment; Mech. Ventilat., log of days of mechanical ventilation a The intercept is tested for significant difference from zero. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 342 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH Correlates of Negative Control Variables Because the internal consistency of the maternal negative control dimension was relatively low, separate predictive models were calculated for its two component variables. Predictors of the modified HOME subscale II were ethnicity (F[1,103] ¼ 6.87, p < .05), maternal education (F[1,103] ¼ 6.17, p < .05), and maternal age (F[1,103] ¼ 6.01, p < .05). Greater mother negative (from the videotape) was related to more maternal stress due to the NICU environment and infant illness, F(1,104) ¼ 8.55, p < .01. Thus, predictors of the maternal negative control dimension combined predictors of its two component variables but with father caregiving participation instead of maternal ethnicity and age. DISCUSSION The interactions of mothers and their prematurely born children were related to child characteristics, child illness severity, maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, and paternal support. Different variables, and only a few of the possible covariates, were related to each interactive dimension. Although other investigators have examined the effects of most of these factors on mother–premature interactions, this is the first study in which the effects of representative measures of all of these factors on specific maternal and child interactive dimensions were examined. Most of the dimensions were affected by child age. Maternal positive involvement decreased with age; maternal developmental stimulation, and negative control and child social behaviors increased. As expected, child developmental maturity also increased. No interactions between age and child or maternal factors were significant, indicating that similar factors are related to mother–premature infant interactions at both 6 and 18 months corrected age. However, had we also studied these mothers and infants at an earlier age when the NICU experiences were more recent, we might have found more interactions between age and the other covariates. Both maternal and child variables were related to three interactive dimensions—maternal positive involvement, maternal developmental stimulation, and child developmental maturity. The lower amounts of positive involvement and developmental stimulation of mothers with multiples probably were due to the time demands of caring for two or three infants (Holditch-Davis Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur et al., 1999; Ostfeld et al., 2000). The finding that first-time mothers provided more developmental stimulation is similar to the findings of Weiss et al. (2000) and Engelke and Engelke (1992) but different from those of Gerner (1999). However, these investigators did not examine maternal behaviors that promote child development, but rather examined global maternal responsivity or touch. The finding that greater maternal stress due to the NICU environment and infant illness was related to more positive involvement contradicts previous findings that mothers had more positive interactions with healthier prematures (Feingold, 1994; Landry et al., 1997; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2003). Our findings support the suggestions of other investigators that mothers provide more interactions to compensate for the behaviors of sicker infants (Holditch-Davis, Cox, et al., 2003; McGrath et al., 1998) but also suggest that the mother’s perception of how sick her infant was may be more important for mothering than the actual severity of the infant’s illness. Maternal education and length of mechanical ventilation were related to child developmental maturity. These findings are consistent with other studies showing longer mechanical ventilation is related to poorer child development (Vohr et al., 2003). This effect may be due to medical complications that are correlated with mechanical ventilation, such as intraventricular hemorrhage, rather than the direct effects of ventilation. On the other hand, our findings that greater developmental maturity was related to lower maternal education contradict previous findings that poorer child development is related to lower maternal educational attainment (Gordon et al., 2004; Zahr, 1999). Yet this finding is not universal: Zahr found that higher maternal education was related to better motor skills at 8 months but not better cognitive skills and then only for AfricanAmerican prematures. These investigators used standardized assessments of multiple behaviors and assessed infant capabilities. Our measure of developmental maturity only examined the amounts of three behaviors—talking, walking, and playing independently with toys—that children actually exhibited during interactions, regardless of their capabilities. Thus, the differences in our findings from the literature probably are due to methodological differences. The other maternal and child dimensions were related only to maternal variables. The findings for maternal negative control were generally similar to those of previous studies. Findings that mothers with less education used more negative control are consistent with previous findings that lower CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. socioeconomic status is related to greater maternal use of negative control strategies (Gordon et al., 2004; Tesh & Holditch-Davis, 1997). Likewise, the finding that more maternal stress due to the NICU environment and infant illness was related to less negative control is consistent with our findings on positive involvement (a dimension that is roughly opposite to negative control) that mothers who were more stressed provided more positive interactions. As suggested by others (Holditch-Davis, Cox, et al., 2003; McGrath et al., 1998), this may be due to mothers providing more positive interactions to compensate for the immature behaviors of sicker infants. Few researchers have examined the effect of paternal caregiving activities on mother–infant interactions. Our finding that when fathers engaged in more caregiving (whether or not they lived in the household), mothers used less negative control shows that paternal support can affect mothering. Thus, there is a need for additional research on the activities of fathers of premature infants and the ways they have an impact on the maternal–child relationship and child outcomes. The child social dimension also was related only to maternal variables and was correlated with the maternal dimensions at both 6 and 18 months. Children of younger and White mothers showed more social behaviors. Although other investigators have found that White mothers of prematures showed more warmth, used fewer negative control strategies, and interacted more than African-American mothers (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1996; Cho et al., 2004; Tesh & Holditch-Davis, 1997), the current results are the first to show that this parenting style may affect the social behaviors of prematurely born children. These findings also might be due to either our sample having very few adolescent mothers, or the younger mothers requiring more independence of 6–18-montholds. Previous researchers have found that younger mothers had higher expectations for the behaviors of their infants than older mothers (Vukelich & Kliman, 1985). Other investigators have not examined the relationships between premature infant irritability and either multiple birth or maternal worry about child health. Mothers of multiple birth prematures have been shown to be less likely to respond to crying than mothers of singleton prematures (Ostfeld et al., 2000). Thus, multiple birth infants may be forced to cry longer to attract maternal attention. Generalized maternal anxiety has been related to decreased responsiveness of premature infants but not to irritability (Feeley et al., 2005; Schmucker et al., 2005). It is possible that either Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 343 greater maternal worry may lead premature infants to become more irritable, or greater infant irritability may lead mothers to worry more. Papousek and von Hofacker (1998) found that maternal anxiety was high when 1–6-month-old fullterm infants were highly irritable, but in another study of fullterm infants, maternal anxiety and depression at 4 and 8 months did not predict infant irritability at 12 months (Pauli-Pott, Mertesacker, & Beckman, 2004). A number of variables, including child birthweight, number of rehospitalizations, and sex, and maternal marital status, did not reach criteria for inclusion in any final analysis, indicating that they were not directly related to mother–premature interactions. Stress due to parental role alteration and depressive symptoms also had minimal effect on interactions. This is surprising, as a number of investigators have shown that maternal depression was related to the quality of mother–premature infant interactions (e.g., Feldman et al., 1999; Singer et al., 2003). However, the effects of stress and worry about child health have not been examined along with depression in previous studies. Investigators have found that maternal depressive symptoms are increased by worry about child health and the stress experienced in the NICU (Miles et al., in press). Thus, these variables, which we found affected mother– premature infant interactions, might explain the effects of maternal depression on mother– premature infant interaction found in other studies. Additional research is needed to clarify this issue. Several factors may limit the generalizability of this study. We only examined mother–premature infant interactions at two time points, 6 and 18 months after term. Studying interactive behaviors at earlier ages and more frequently might result in different findings. Longitudinal studies that examine the relationship between the maternal and child factors and mother–premature interactions over longer time periods are needed. Another limitation was the relatively low-internal consistency of the negative control dimension, probably due to the low rate at which mothers displayed this behavior. Items on the HOME, for example, include physical punishment occurring more often than once a week, and rates of maternal expression of negative affect typically make up less than 2% of mother–child interactions (Holditch-Davis, Bartlett, et al., 2000) as we found in the current study. Despite its infrequency, negative control has a major impact on children and their developmental outcomes (HolditchDavis, Miles, et al., 2001; Olsen et al., 1992). 344 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH Our negative control dimension also showed changes over age and was related to maternal and child variables. Finally, we did not examine other environmental factors that influence mother–child interactions, such as social support from family members other than the father. Future research is needed to examine how relationships with other caregivers affect mother–premature infant interactions. Overall, our findings are consistent with complex processes affecting maternal–premature infant interactions, as expected by the developmental science perspective. Three of six dimensions were related to both mother and infant variables, providing evidence of processes involving both mother and infant affecting interactions. Maternal dimensions also were correlated with child social behaviors, and to a lesser extent with other child dimensions. These findings are different than those of investigators who found that differences in the interactions of prematures and fullterms were due primarily to the mothers (Jarvis et al., 1989; Muller-Nix et al., 2004). The complexity of maternal–premature interactions was shown by the fact that different variables were related to each interactive dimension. Previous investigators have examined how many of these variables affected mother–premature interactions, but the direction of the effects were contradictory in many cases. For example, mothers have been described as both being less and more attentive to sicker prematures (Feingold, 1994; Holditch-Davis, Cox, et al., 2003; Landry et al., 1997; Muller-Nix et al.), possibly because these studies included only one or two of these variables. However, we did not examine the reciprocal effects of mother and child variables; this needs to be done in future studies. To develop the knowledge needed for nurses working with these vulnerable infants and mothers, studies of mother–premature infant interactions must be designed to capture the complex factors affecting their interactions and to examine how maternal and infant characteristics work together to influence interactions. As shown in our findings and those of other researchers (e.g., Landry et al., 1997; Ostfeld et al., 2000; Tesh & Holditch-Davis, 1997), multiple birth infants, infants with longer periods of mechanical ventilation, minority mothers, mothers with high levels of worry about the child’s health or low stress due to infant illness in the NICU, and mothers lacking support from the infant’s father may be at particular risk for having less-positive interactions than other premature infants and, thus, need to be a focus of nursing Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur interventions. In accordance with the developmental science framework, however, having a single risk may not be as important to infant outcomes as mothers and infants who exhibit multiple risk factors. Given that the interactive risks involved both mothers and infants and the interactive dimensions were inter-correlated, nursing interventions that focus on the entire mother– infant system are most likely to be effective in improving interactions and ameliorating the developmental handicaps associated with prematurity. REFERENCES Barnard, K.E. (1983). Nursing Child Assessment Teaching Scale. Seattle, WA: University of Washington. Berlin, L.J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Spiker, D., & Zaslow, M.J. (1995). Examining observational measures of emotional support and cognitive stimulation in black and white mothers of preschoolers. Journal of Family Issues, 16, 664–686. Blondel, B., & Kaminski, M. (2002). Trends in the occurrence, determinants, and consequences of multiple births. Seminars in Perinatology, 26, 239– 249. Bradley, R.H., Rock, S.L., Caldwell, B.M., & Brisby, J.A. (1989). Use of the HOME Inventory for families with handicapped children. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 313–330. Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P.K., & Duncan, G.J. (1996). Ethnic differences in children’s intelligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal characteristics. Child Development, 67, 396–408. Brown, P., Rustia, J., & Schappert, P. (1991). A comparison of fathers of high-risk newborns and fathers of healthy newborns. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 6, 269–273. Cairns, R.B., Elder, G.H., & Costello, E.J. (1996 Developmental science. Cambridge, Britain: Cambridge University Press. Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Administration manual for the home observation for measurement of the environment (revised ed.). Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock. Cho, J., Holditch-Davis, D., & Belyea, M. (2004). Gender and ethnicity and the interactions of prematurely born children and their mothers. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 19, 163–175. Coffman, S., Levitt, M.J., & Guacci-Franco, N. (1993). Mothers’ stress and close relationships: Correlates with infant health status. Pediatric Nursing, 19, 135– 140. Crawford, J.W. (1982). Mother-infant interaction in premature and full-term infants. Child Development, 53, 957–962. CORRELATES OF MOTHER–PREMATURE INTERACTIONS / HOLDITCH-DAVIS ET AL. Engelke, M.K., & Engelke, S.C. (1992). Predictors of the home environment of high-risk infants. Journal of Community Health Nursing, 9, 171–181. Feeley, N., Gottlieb, L., & Zelkowitz, P. (2005). Infant, mother, and contextual predictors of mother-very low birth weight infant interaction at 9 months of age. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 24–33. Feingold, C. (1994). Correlates of cognitive development in low-birth-weight infants from low-income families. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 9, 91–97. Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A.I. (2004). Parent-infant synchrony and the social-emotional development of triplets. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1133–1147. Feldman, R., Eidelman, A.I., & Rotenberg, N. (2004). Parenting stress, infant emotion regulation, maternal sensitivity, and the cognitive development of triplets: A model for parent and child influences in a unique ecology. Child Development, 75, 1774–1791. Feldman, R., Weldman, J., Leckman, J.F., Kuint, J., & Eidelman, A.I. (1999). The nature of the mother’s tie to her infant: Maternal bonding under conditions of proximity, separation, and potential loss. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 929–939. Field, T. (1995). Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 1–13. Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Borghini, A., Moessinger, A., & Muller-Nix, C. (2006). Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatrics, 118, e107–e114. Retrieved 8/2/2006 from http:// www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/118/1/e107. Gatsonis, C., & Sampson, A.R. (1989). Multiple correlation: Exact power and sample size calculations. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 516–524. Gerner, E.M. (1999). Emotional interaction in a group of preterm infants at 3 and 6 months of corrected age. Infant and Child Development, 8, 117–128. Goldberg, S., Perrotta, M., Minde, K., & Corter, C. (1986). Maternal behavior and attachment in lowbirth-weight twins and singletons. Child Development, 57, 34–46. Gordon, R.A., Chase-Lansdale, P.L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). Extended households and the life course of young mothers: Understanding the associations using a sample of mothers with premature, low birth weight babies. Child Development, 75, 1013–1038. Harrison, M.J. (1990). A comparison of parental interactions with term and preterm infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 13, 173–179. Holditch-Davis, D. (2002, February). Methodological issues in systematic observation. In D. HolditchDavis (Chair), Systematic observation in maternalchild research: Methods and issues. Symposium presented at the 16th annual meeting of the Southern Nursing Research Society, San Antonio, TX. Holditch-Davis, D., Bartlett, T.R., & Belyea, M. (2000). Developmental problems and the interactions between mothers and their three-year-old prematurely born children. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 15, 157–167. Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur 345 Holditch-Davis, D., Bartlett, T.R., Blickman, A., & Miles, M.S. (2003). Post-traumatic stress symptoms in mothers of premature infants. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing, 32, 161–171. Holditch-Davis, D., Cox, M.F., Miles, M.S., & Belyea, M. (2003). Mother-infant interactions of medically fragile infants and non-chronically ill premature infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 26, 300– 311. Holditch-Davis, D., Docherty, S., Miles, M.S., & Burchinal, M. (2001). Developmental outcomes of infants with bronchopulmonary dysplasia: Comparison with other medically fragile infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 24, 181–193. Holditch-Davis, D., Edwards, L., & Helms, R. (1998). Modeling development of sleep-wake behaviors: I. Using the mixed general linear model. Physiology and Behavior, 63, 311–318. Holditch-Davis, D., Miles, M.S., Burchinal, M., O’Donnell, K., McKinney, R., & Lim, W. (2001). Parental caregiving and developmental outcomes in infants of mothers with HIV. Nursing Research, 50, 5–14. Holditch-Davis, D., Roberts, D., & Sandelowski, M. (1999). Early parental interactions with and perceptions of multiple birth infants. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 30, 200–210. Holditch-Davis, D., Scher, M., Schwartz, T., & HudsonBarr, D. (2004). Sleeping and waking state development in preterm infants. Early Human Development, 80, 43–64. Holditch-Davis, D., Tesh, E.M., Goldman, B.D., Miles, M.S., & D’Auria, J. (2000). Use of the HOME Inventory with medically fragile infants. Children’s Health Care, 29, 257–277. Jarvis, P., Myers, B., & Creasey, G. (1989). The effects of infants’ illness on mothers’ interactions with prematures at 4 and 8 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 25–35. Landry, S.H., Smith, K.E., Miller-Loncar, C.L., & Swank, P.R. (1997). Responsiveness and initiative: Two aspects of social competence. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 259–262. Lee, T., Miles, M.S., & Holditch-Davis, D. (2006). Father’s support to mothers of medically fragile infants. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing, 35, 46–55. McGrath, M.M., Sullivan, M.C., & Seifer, R. (1998). Maternal interaction patterns and preschool competence in high-risk children. Nursing Research, 47, 309–317. Mercer, R.T. (2004). Becoming a mother versus maternal role attainment. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 36, 226–232. Miles, M.S., Carlson, J., & Funk, S. (1996.) Sources of support reported by mothers and fathers of infants hospitalized in a neonatal intensive care unit. Neonatal Network, 15, 45–54. Miles, M.S., Funk, S.G., & Carlson, J. (1993). Parental Stressor Scale: Neonatal intensive care unit. Nursing Research, 42, 148–152. 346 RESEARCH IN NURSING & HEALTH Miles, M.S., & Holditch-Davis, D. (1995). Compensatory parenting: How mothers describe parenting their 3-year-old prematurely born children. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 10, 243–253. Miles, M.S., Holditch-Davis, D., Scher, M., & Schwartz, T. (In press). Depressive symptoms in mothers of prematurely-born infants. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. Moran, G., & Pederson, D.R. (1998). Proneness to distress and ambivalent relationships. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 493–503. Muller, K.E., & Fetterman, B.A. (2002). Regression and ANOVA: A unified approach using SAS software (p. 225). Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child interactions. Early Human Development, 79, 145–158. Olsen, S.L., Bates, J.E., & Kaskie, B. (1992). Caregiverinfant interaction antecedents of children’s schoolage cognitive ability. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 309–330. Ostfeld, B.M., Smith, R.H., Hiatt, M., & Hegyi, T. (2000). Maternal behavior toward premature twins: Implications for development. Twin Research, 3, 234–241. Papousek, M., & von Hofacker, N. (1998). Persistent crying in early infancy: A non-trivial condition of risk for the developing mother-infant relationship. Child: Care, Health, and Development, 24, 395–424. Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., & Beckman, D. (2004). Predicting the development of infant emotionality from maternal characteristics. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 19–42. Poehlmann, J., & Fiese, B.H. (2001). Parent-infant interaction as a mediator of the relation between neonatal risk status and 12-month cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 171– 188. Pridham, K., Brown, R., Clark, R., Limbo, R.K., Schroeder, M., Henriques, J., et al. (2005). Effect of guided participation on feeding competencies of mothers and their premature infants. Research in Nursing and Health, 28, 252–267. Pridham, K., Lin, C.-Y., & Brown, R. (2001). Mothers’ evaluation of their caregiving for premature and fullterm infants through the first year: Contributing factors. Research in Nursing and Health, 24, 157– 169. Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385– 401. Schmucker, G., Brisch, K.-H., Kohntop, B., Betzler, S., Osterle, M., Pohlandt, F., et al. (2005). The influence of prematurity, maternal anxiety, and infants’ neurobiological risk on mother-infant interactions. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26, 423–441. Shields-Poe, D., & Pinelli, J. (1997). Variables associated with parental stress in neonatal intensive care units. Neonatal Network, 16, 29–37. Research in Nursing & Health DOI 10.1002/nur Singer, L., Davillier, M., Bruening, P., Hawkins, S., & Yamashita, T. (1996). Social support, psychological distress, and parenting strains in mothers of very low birthweight infants. Family Relations, 45, 343–350. Singer, L.T., Fulton, S., Davillier, M., Koshy, D., Salvator, A., & Baley, J.E. (2003). Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 233–241. Smith, K.E., Landry, S.H., & Swank, P.R. (2006). The role of early maternal responsiveness in supporting school-aged cognitive development for children who vary in birth status. Pediatrics, 117, 1608–1617. Tesh, E.M., & Holditch-Davis, D. (1997). HOME Inventory and NCATS: Relation to mother and child behaviors during naturalistic observations. Research in Nursing and Health, 20, 295–307. Thoman, E.B., Acebo, C., & Becker, P.T. (1983). Infant crying and stability in the mother-infant relationship: A systems analysis. Child Development, 54, 653– 659. Traustadottir, R. (1991). Mothers who care: Gender, disability, and family life. Journal of Family Issues, 12, 211–228. Vohr, B.R., Allan, W.C., Westerveld, M., Schneider, K.C., Katz, K.H., Makuch, R.W., et al. (2003). School-age outcomes of very low birth weight infants in the indomethacin intraventricular hemorrhage prevention trial. Pediatrics, 111, e340–346. Retrieved 8/29/2004 from http://www.pediatrics. org/cgi/content/full/111/4/e340. Vukelich, C., & Kliman, D.S. (1985). Mature and teenage mothers’ infant growth expectations and use of child development information sources. Family Relations, 34, 189–196. Weiss, S.J., & Chen, J.-L. (2002). Factors influencing maternal mental health and family functioning during the low birthweight infant’s first year of life. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 17, 114–125. Weiss, S., Wilson, P., Hertenstein, M., & Campos, R. (2000). The tactile context of a mother’s caregiving: Implications for attachment of low birth weight infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 91– 111. Weissman, M.M., Sholomskas, D., Pottenger, M., Prusoff, B.Q.A., & Locke, B.Z. (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: A validation study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106, 203–214. Wille, D.E. (1991). Relation of preterm birth with quality of infant-mother attachment at one year. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 227–240. Yogman, M., Kindlon, D., & Earls, F. (1995). Father involvement and cognitive/behavioral outcomes in preterm infants. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 58–66. Zahr, L.K. (1999). Predictors of development in premature infants from low-income families: African Americans and Hispanics. Journal of Perinatology, 19, 284–289.
© Copyright 2024