J . Child P.y?r.hoi P.wchiat. Vol. 42. No. X, pp. 1065-1081, 2001 Cambridge University Press G 2001 Associatlon for Child Psychology and Psychlatry Printed m Great Brltaln. All rights reserved OOZI-~~~O/OI$I~~+~OO The Differential Assessment of Children’s Attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), Normative Sample and ADHD Performance Tom Manly Vicki Anderson Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, U.K. University of Melbourne, Australia Ian Nimmo-Smith Anna Turner Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, U.K. The Radcliffe Infirmary, Oxford, U.K Peter Watson Ian H. Robertson Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, U.K. Trinity College Dublin, Ireland “Attention” is not a unitary brain process.Evidence from adult studies indicates that distinct neuroanatomical networks perform specific attentional operations and that these are vulnerable to selective damage. Accordingly, characterising attentional disorders requires the use of a variety of tasks that differentially challenge these systems. Here we describe a novel battery, the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), comprising nine subtests adapted from the adult literature. The performance of 293 healthy children between the ages of 6 and 16 is described together with the relationships to IQ, existing measures of attention, and scholastic attainment. This large normative sample also allows us to test the fit of theadult model of functionally separable attention systems to the observed patterns of variance in children’s performance. A Structural Equation Modelling approach supports this view. A three-factor modelof sustained and selective attentionand higher-level “executive” control formed a good fit to the data, even in the youngest children. A single factor model was rejected. There are behavioural and anatomical grounds to believe that Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) is particularly associated with poor self-sustained attention andbehavioural control. The TEA-Ch performance of 24 boysdiagnosed with ADD presented here is consistent with this view. When performance levels on WISC-I11 subtests were taken into account, specific deficitsinsustained attention were apparent whileselective attention performance was within the normal range. Keywords; ADD/ADHD, assessment, attention, executive function, normal development. Abbreviations; ADD: Attention Deficit Disorder; ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; SEM: StructuralEquation Modelling; TEA: TestofEveryday TEA-Ch: Test of Everyday Attention for Children. Introduction Vast and increasing numbers of children are referred clinical services with suspected attentional problems. In some studies, referral rates for Attention Deficit Hyper6 % of all school activity Disorder (ADHD) have reached age boys and 1.5 % of girls(U.S. data: Swanson, Learner, & Williams, 1995). Deficits in attention processes have been attributed to many other developmental conditions (Lang, Athanasopoulous, & Anderson, 1988) including autism (Burack et al., 1997), Asperger syndrome (Klin, Sparrow, Volkmar, Cicchetti, & Rourke, 1995) Tourette syndrome (Georgiou, Bradshaw, Phillips,& Chiu, 1996), Requests for reprints to: Tom Manly, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF, U.K. (E-mail: [email protected]). Attention; leukaemia (Brouwers, Riccardi, Fedio, & Poplak, 1985) Turner syndrome (Skuse etal., 1997), and acquired brain to injuries (Anderson & Pentland, 1998; Kaufmann, Fletcher, Levin, Miner, & Ewing Cobbs, 1993). Although the ADHD construct has become increasingly cognitive in its emphasis (moving from the more behaviourally descriptive “hyperkinetic” terminology), the diagnosis continues to rest exclusively on reports of behaviourandinferencesaboutunderlyingprocesses. Although parents or teachers may be asked whether a child “has difficulty in sustaining attention”, no performance-basedmeasuresofsuchcapacitiesarerecommended in the latest DSM revision (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Although clearly problematic, this reliance on report and inferencereflects bothourapparentlyacute sensitivity tothedirection,intensity,andlimitationsin 1065 1066 T. MANLY et al. 1990;Stametal.,1993;Tranel & Hyman, 1990), sustained attention (Brouwer & Van Wolffelaar, 1985; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Rueckert, Sorensen,& Levy, 1994; Wilkins, Shallice,& McCarthy, 1987), the capacity toswitch(Owenetal.,1993)anddivideattention (Baddeley, Bressi, Della Salla, Logie, & Spinnler, 1991) and, of course,arangeofhigher-level“executive” measures(Burgess & Shallice,1996;Duncan,1986; Shallice & Burgess, 1991 ; Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, 1997). In line with this premise of separability, Robertson and colleagues developedtheTestofEverydayAttention Separation of Attentional Functions in Adults (TEA; Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994, 1996), asingle battery for adults with eight subtests Experimentalpsychologyhaspredominantlyoperationalised attention as the systematic variation in perdesignedtomakedifferentialdemandsonsustained formance of a single task under different “attentional attention,selectiveattention,dividedattention,and conditions”. For example,if people are asked to respond attentional switching capacities. to visual targets presented on a computer screen, then Standardisingneuropsychologicalmeasuresrequires theirresponsetimestoleft-orright-sidedtargetsare that large numbers of the healthy population are assessed broadly equivalent. If, however, they receive a useful hint in order that a given individual’s performance can be about where the targetis most likely to appear, then their meaningfully compared with the population mean. The performance improves. The absence of eye movements standardisation of theTEA also provided an opportunity means that the visual stimulation under each condition is totestPosnerandPetersen’s(1990)postulationofa identical,as is thetaskinstructionandgoal.The separationbetweenselectiveandsustainedattention, difference in reaction times can therefore be interpreted as which had been developed on the basis of limited numbers reflecting a hidden, “ top-down’’ process that modulates of subjects in functional imaging and lesion studies, on a detection efficiency in a particular location-attention much larger sample of the neurologically healthy popu(Posner, 1980). In a similar way, controlling for reading lation.Ifacommon,unitaryprocess is thoughtto speed and colour naming allows the conflict condition in contribute heavily to the performance on one group of of the colour-word Stroop task to be interpreted in terms measures but not to another-and there are individual selective processing and response suppression. differences in the efficiency ofthat process-then the The application of such approaches to studies of adult variance in those measures should group together and patients with acquired brain lesions, and more recently, diverge from others. within functional imaging studies, has enhanced underTheresultsofafactoranalysisconductedonthe standing of the neural basis of attention and separations normalisation sample of the TEA indeed lent statistical between different attentional systems. Reviewing across support to Posner and Petersen’s proposal. For example, these areas in 1990, Posner and Petersen argued for three twosustainedattentiontaskswithverydifferent osbasiccharacteristicsofattentionfunctionswithinthe tensible demands (one counting tones over relatively brief brain. The first was that specific attention systems existed.periods and the other monitoring for rare targets within a The second was that these were separable from more 10-minstream)loadedontoacommonfactorand “basic” perceptual, cognitive, and output systems. The divergedfromselectiveattentionmeasures.Similarly, third was that within the attention system, specific regions two visual search tasks had a common loading with the and networks performed different types of operation. On Stroop colour-word task (Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe, & the basis of current evidence these distinct systems were Leber, 1989), supporting the characterisation of a selecprovisionally characterised as: (a) a capacity to move tive attention factor. attention within space (spatial attention); (b) a capacity to enhance the processing of particular target characterisChildren’s Attention ticsregardlessofspatiallocation(selectiveattention); and (c) a capacity to maintain a particular processing set The main question that we pose in this article-and over time (sustained attention). whichunderpinnedthedevelopment of theTest of TheseconclusionshaveimportantclinicalimplicaEveryday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch) battery-is tions. The functional/anatomical separation of attention whetherthebroad views of attentiondevelopedpresystemsfrombasicperceptionmeansthat-through dominantlywithadultshavevalueinthinkingabout acquired brain damage or developmental difficulty-it is attention and disorders of attention in childhood. Alpossible to have deficits that are exclusively or predomin- though it cannot be assumed that the processes of the antly attentional in nature. The functional/anatomical developingbraincorrespondcloselytothoseseenin separation within the attention system means that-again maturity, there are potential advantages if such links can depending on the locus of any damage-quite different be made.If adult models form reasonable approximation profilesofattentionaldeficitscanbeseen,eachwith (andpresumablyanincreasinglyreasonableapproxidifferent implications for problems in everyday life. mation as children get older) then both assessment and For cliniciansworkingwithadultsthishasledto rehabilitation of attention disorders in children could increasinglyspecific and differentialassessments,for benefit from the findings of adult studies. example of spatial attention (Driver & Halligan, 1991 ; IndevelopingtheTEA-Chouraimwastoadapt Posner & Petersen, 1990; Wilson, Cockburn,& Halligan, measures that had proven effective in adult attention into 1987), selective or focused attention (Bench et al., 1993; game-like assessment tools for use with children between Duncan et al., in press; Pardo, Pardo, Janer, & Raichle, the ages of 6 and 16. As with the TEA, in standardising another’s attention, and the difficulties experienced by psychometristsincapturingthisconstructwithany precision. The fundamental problem in measuring attention is that,asapostulatedcentralprocess,it is atonce everywhereandnowhere.Theinfluenceofattention cannot be measuredunlessaperson is askedtodo something. That something will inevitably involve many other perceptual, cognitive and output systems that may be as-or more-influential onperformancethanattention. DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION 1067 computerised tests of attention and general ability using and collecting norms for a new battery we also had the opportunity to statistically test the patterns of converSEM. In line with the predictions of this study, among gence and divergenceinperformanceagainstabroad 106second-gradestudentstheyfoundthataunitary model developed within the adult literature. model of attention formed a poor fit to the observed However, as we can only measure attention indirectly variance,whileathree-factormodel of sustainedattention, selective attention, and“ central capacity” formthis is a somewhat ambitious aim. As children will vary in manyabilities(motorskill,taskcomprehension,laned a significant and parsimonious fit. The final approach that we describe here is to examine guage,and so on)thatmaybeshared betweentests theperformanceofaclinicalgroupontheTEA-Ch hypothesised to make different attention demands, our battery and other measures. Although the etiology and postulated attention factors would have to be expressed very stronglyto be seen amid this “noise”. Such problems unitary nature of ADHD remains controversial, there is some convergence on frontal abnormalities that may be of “task impurity” have been held to account for the more pronounced within the right hemisphere (Barkley, generally IOW correlations observed between executive Grodzinsky, & DuPaul,1992b;Brumback measures in adulthood (Burgess, 1997; Miyake et al., in & Staton, 1982;Castellanosetal.,1996;Filipeketal.,1997; press). As developing children may be expected to show & greater variability along these nonattentional dimensions, Heilman,Voller, & Nadeau, 1991; Lou, Henriksen, Bruhn, 1984; Swanson, Castellanos, Murias, LaHoste,& the challenge may be much greater than with adults. & Heilman,1988).Inadults, The first approach to meeting this challenge was in the Kennedy,1998;Voeller rightprefrontalfunctionhasbeenassociatedwitha design of the tests. We aimed to minimise the demands on number of functions including sustained attention memory’, reasoning, task comprehension, motor speed, (Cohen & Semple, 1988; Cohen, Semple, Gross, King, & verbal ability, and perceptual acuity while maintaining Nordahl, 1992; Lewinet al., 1996; Pardo, Fox, & Raichle, the demands on the targeted attentional system. T o this end, for example, we controlled for motor speed on a 1991; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Wilkins et al., 1987) visual search task by comparing performance under high and response inhibition (Garavan, Ross, & Stein, 1999). Consistentwiththis view-and withlinksbetween andlowattentionaldemands (see Methodssection). children’s and adults’ attention-previous A D H D stuWhere possible, language was avoided in the stimuli or dieshaveemphasisedpoorperformanceintasksof the required responses and the perceptual demands were ; Douglas, 1972 ; reduced to detection rather than complex discrimination. sustainedattention(Barkley,1997 Hooks, Milich, & Lorch, 1994; Shue & Douglas, 1992) Demonstrations and practice trials with correction were and response inhibition (Barkley, 1997; Barkley, Grodused to reduce the impact of task comprehension differzinsky, & DuPaul, 1992a; Logan, Schachar,& Tannock, ences andtotryandimprovethe reliabilityofper1997; Swanson et al., 1998). formance. In the first study presented we here describe the patterns Such attempts can only ever have limited success. The ofperformanceseenin293healthychildrenonnine secondapproachtominimisingtheimpactof“task subtests of the novel TEA-Ch battery. We report the impurity”wastouseStructuralEquationModelling reliability of the assessments, the relationships between (SEM) in our analysis. This technique is related to the the TEA-Ch and existing tests ofattention (in addition to more conventional exploratory factor analysis method IQ and scholastic attainment tests), and the relative fits of buthasimportant differences. Inconventionalfactor aunitary vs. theoreticallyderivedthree-factorSEM analysis,themodelisdeterminedina“bottom-up” fashion from the data.SEM allows one to test an a priori model of separable attention processes. In the second study we will examine the performance of 24 children specified model against the observed variance. with a diagnosis of A D H D . If one takes results from three very different tests that are theoretically designed to tap a common underlying process, then that process should be better represented by the covariance between the tests than the variance in any Study l : Normative Data and Structural Equation Model specificmeasure-particularly if the “peripheral” featuresofthetestsdiffer.Itisthereforepossibleto Method differentiate between the hypothesised “latent variables” of the model and the “manifest variables” of each of the Participants. A total o f 293 children between the ages of 6 and 16wererecruited fromstate schools in Melbourne, measures.ElegantSEMapproachestoexecutiveand Australia. Equal numbers of boys and girls were tested ineach attentionalprocessesinadultsaredescribedin,for of six age-bands, band 1 = 6-7 years, 2 = 7-9 years, 3 = 9-1 1 example, Burgess, Veitch, Costello, and Shallice (2000) years, 4 = 11-13 years, 5 = 13-15 years, and 6 = 15-16 years and in Miyake et al. (in press). In a study that has only (the second digit in each range reflecting higher cutoffpoint for recentlybeen drawntoourattention,Shapiroand the band). Exclusion criteria were: previousheadinjury or colleagues (Shapiro, Morris, Morris, Flowers, & Jones, neurologicalillness;developmentaldelay or sensory loss; referral forattentional or learning problems; assessment as 1998)examinedthepatternsofchildrenperforming having special educational needs. The sex and age distribution of the sample are presented in Table 1 below. Socioeconomic status (SES) was determined for By reducing memory demands we mean, for example, limiting each child using the Daniel’s Scale of Occupational Prestige the number of target items that need to be remembered, using (Daniel, 1983) on which parental occupation is rated between 1 demonstrationand practice to make instructions easier to and 7, with a score of1 reflecting highand a score of7 indicating remember, and making minimal call on previous knowledge. low SES. The mean SES coding for the group was 4.04 (SD = There is,however, considerable blurring betweenprocesses 1.23; range = 1.7-6.6). ascribed to “workingmemory”, particularly in the central A subgroup of 160 children selected at random from the full executive component,andattention (Baddeley,1993).As sample (meanage = 10.80, SD = 2.83, range = 6.14-15.96) described below, a number of the TEA-Ch measures clearly were also assessed on four subtests of the WISC-111 and the make demands on this form o f on-line information processing Wide Ranging Achievement Test (Revised) (Jastak & Wilkinand manipulation capacities. 1068 MANLY Table 1 Distribution of Boys andGirls byAgeBand Normative Sample of the TEA-Ch Battery Age band Total (1) 6-7” 20 (2) 7-9 29(3) 9-1 1 30(4) 11-13 25(5) 13-15 13(6) 15-16 147 Total 26 38 56 54 58 58 29 293 T. et al. in the Girls Boys 18 30 Upper cutoff of each band (e.g. only children under 7 years would appear in band 1). son, 1984). This group did not differ from theremainder of the sample in terms of age (1 = 0.67.p = .51), sexdistribution ( x z = 1.51, p = .22) or SES ( 1 = 0.20, p = .84). A subgroup of 96 children also completed the colour-word Stroop, the Trails Test, and the Matching Familiar Figures Test (see measures below).The mean age ofthis subsample was 11.04 years ( S D = 2.92, range = 6.14-15.96). Again this group was representativein terms ofage ( t = -0.06, p = .95) and sex distribution (x’= 0.17, p = .68). Although the subsample tended to be from slightly higher socioeconomic groups, the difference did not reach statistical significance (mean SES for subsample = 4.26, S D = 1.29; mean SES for remaining sample = 3.94, S D = 1.20; 1 = - 1 . 8 2 , = ~ .07; see below for influence of SES on TEA-Ch performance). Measures The Test of Everyday Attention.for Children (TEA-Ch): Sustained attention subtests. (1) Score! Sustained attentionrequires the active maintenance ofa particular responseset underconditions oflow environmental support (e.g.when there are few triggers to the relevant behaviour, when the task lacks interest or reward). The Score! subtestis a 10-item tone-counting measure based on a task originally described by Wilkins et al. (1987). In each item, between 9 and 15 identical tones of 345 ms arepresented, separated by silent interstimulus intervals of variable duration (between 500 and 5000ms). Children were asked to silently count the tones (without assistance from fingers) and togive the total at the end-as if they are “keeping the score by counting the scoring sounds in a computer game”. If a child wasunable to count to15 or was unable to pass two practice trials (with relatively few tones) the test was not given. The requirement to pass practice items as a way of ensuring task comprehension, checking on possiblesensoryproblems and improving the reliability of the measures, was a feature of each of the tasks. The duration of the test was approximately 5 min 40 S (some variability occurring dueto the need to repeat instructions and so forth). In their 1987 study, Wilkins and colleagues reportedthe sensitivity of an analagous task to focal right frontal lesions. Subsequent work with adults using the clinical version in the Test ofEveryday Attention (Elevator Counting subtestRobertson et al., 1994) has shown that right-hemisphere strokes disproportionatelyimpair performance (Robertson, Manly, Beschin, et al., 1997).Similarmeasureshave also produced predominantly right frontal activation in healthy adult participants infunctional imaging studies (Lewin et al., 1996; Pardo et al., 1990). (2) Score D T . Stuss, Shallice, Alexander, and Picton (1995) have argued that in sustained attention tests, the low level of exogenous activation for the goalrelevant schema (listening to the test, counting etc.) made it vulnerable to possible competitors forprocessing resources. In conventional tone-counting or vigilance tasks, the natureof the competitorswill vary with the individual andenvironment.The Score DT measure was designed to increase the sensitivity of the basic Score! task by including a “built-in” distractor. The tone-counting aspect of thetask is identical tothat ofScore!described above.In addition, meaningful, auditory speech-in the form ofnews bulletins-was simultaneously presented. Children were askedto keep acount of the toneswhilst at the same time keeping “an ear out”for the mention of an animal during the news broadcast. They were asked to “concentrate most on the counting. Ifyotr concentrate toomuch on the news, the counting i s very dificult”. Two practice items were given beforethe I O items of thetest. Test duration was approximately 5 min 40 S . (3) Code Transmission. Vigilance tasks, in which people are asked to monitor a stream of information for the occurrence of a rare target, have been predominantly used in the study of sustained attentioninboth normal and clinical populations (Koelega,1996; N. H. Mackworth, 1948: J. F. Mackworth, 1970; Parasuraman,Warm, & See,1998;Rosvold,Mirsky, Sarason, Bransome, & Beck,1956;See,1995). Although a disproportionate decrement in performance over time is often considered the hallmark ofasustained attention deficit, the value of this measure in clinical groups has beencalled into question (Manly & Robertson, 1997;Stuss et al., 1989;Van et al., 1987).In Zomeren & Ven den Burg,1985;Wilkins particular, ithas been argued that suchanalysis may miss fluctuations in attention over much briefer time-scales (Stuss et al., 1989), and that, as performance on simple detection tasks becomes more routine orautomated with practice, the demands on anterior attentional circuits can actually be reduced (Fisk & Schneider, 1981 ; Paus et al., 1997; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, et al., 1997). Findings of impaired “vigilance level” performance in adult brain-injured patients at any stage within such tasks have. however, been ubiquitous (Brouwer & Van Wolffelaar, 1985; Godefroy, Cabaret, & Rousseaux, 1994; Loken, Thornton, Otto, & Long, 1995;Ponsford & Kinsella,1992; Rueckert & Grafman, 1996; Spikman, Van Zomeren, & Deelman, 1996; Whyte, Polansky, Fleming, Branch-Coslett, & Cavallucci, 1995). The CodeTransmission subtest is an auditory vigilance-level measure. The childrenwereasked to monitor astreamof monotonous digits (presented at a rate of one every 2 S) for the occurrence of aparticular target sequence (e.g. 5 5)and then to report the digit that occurred immediately before. The target sequence was constant throughout the test. Following a practice sequence to ensure comprehension, 40 targets were presented over the 12 min of the task. (4) Walk Don’t Walk. This measure was adapted from the Sustained Attention to ResponseTest (SART; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, et al., 1997). The SARTwas designed to place emphasis on maintaining attention over one’s own actions and to avoid some of the problems of more automatic responding described above. The SART has proved sensitive to traumatic brain injury (Robertson, Manly, Andrade, et al., 1997) and to predict the reports of everyday “ action lapses” in clinical and normal populations (Manly, Robertson, Galloway, & Hawkins, 1999; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, et al., 1997). Although the task requires the periodic and unpredictable withholding ofthe routine response, behaviouraland electrophysiological evidence suggest that theactive maintenance ofattention across the task is a strong determinant of the success of response suppression (Manly et al., 1999, 2000). In the Walk Don’t Walk subtest, children are given an A4 sheet showing “paths” each made up of 14 squares. They are asked to listen to a tape thatwill play one sound (go tone)if the move to the next square should be made and another (no-go tone) if not. The moves were made by “dotting” each square witha marker pen, the penbeingheldapproximately2cm above the page between each tone. The go and no-go tones were identical for the first 208 ms (329.6 Hz sine tone), the no-go tone being marked bya concluding vocal exclamation (” D’oh ! ”). The task therefore ideallyrequiredchildren to listen to the entire soundbefore making their response. The go tones were presented in aregular, DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION 1069 Figure I. Stimuli for the Sky Search and Sky Search DTsubtests of the TEA-Ch with EWO targetscircled. Children are asked to search for pairs where two spacecraft are the same. rhythmic fashion with the no-go tone occurring unpredictably within the sequence {between the 2nd and 12th steps). Intertone intervals began at 1500 ms for item 1. Although held constant within each item, the intervals weresystematically reduced with each new item, reaching a minimum of SO0 ms at item 20. Two demonstration trials and two practice trials were given before the test items. The dependent variable wasthe numberof items correct outof 20. The total durationof the test items was approximately 6 min 16 S. TEA-Ch:Selccliw arremtion measures. The colour-word Stroop task has frequently been used as a paradigmatic definition of (nonspatial) selective attention, for example, in adult functional imaging studies (Bench et al., 1993; George et al., 1994; Pardo et al., 1990). In children as young as 6, where differenoes in readingability may be considerable, this task may be less appropriate. Following the common loading with the Stroop task intheadult TEA factor analysis, we therefore adopted twononlinguisticvisualsearch tasksto represent this factor. ( I ) Sky Search. In this measure the children were given a laminated A3 sheet depicting rowsof paired spacecraft (see Fig. I). Four distinctive types of craFt were presented, with most pairs being of mixed type. They were instructed to try and find aIl of the target items, defined by a pair of identical craft, as quickly as possible. Twenty targets were distributed among 108 distractors. Termination of the task was self-determined with the child marking a box in the lower left corner when they had finished. Both speed and accuracy were emphasised. Prior to completing the main test,children first completed a practice A4 sheet to ensure Comprehension of target identity and the selfcompletion procedure. In order to control for differences that are attributable to motor speed ratherthan visual selection, the childrenthen completed a motor controlversion of the task.The A3 stimulus sheet was identical to that of the SkySearch test with the exception that all ofthe distractor items were removed (see Fig. 2). The task therefore consisted of circling all 20 target items as quickly as possible and then indicating completion. Time taken to comptetion and accuracy were recorded for each part of the test. A time-per-target score was calculated of the "motor control" timeItimeJtargets found). Subtraction per-target from the more attentionally demanding Sky Search time-per-item produced an "attention score that was relatively free from the influence of motor slowness or clumsiness. The time required for this test clearly depends upon thespeed of an individual child. The mean time spent on the test item for this sample was 84.7 S (SO = 39.25) (see results For changes with 'I W). (2) Map Mission. A potential confound withinthe Sky Search task is the influence of strategic differences due to the requirement to self-determinewhen the task is complete. A further"timed-out" visualsearchmeasure was therefore incorporated into thebattery. In the Map Mission, the children weregiven a printed A3 laminated city map. Eighty targets (small restaurant knife-and-fork symbols measuring 4 m m x 3 mm) were randomly distributed across the map. Distracting symbok of a similar size (depicting Supermarkettrolleys, cups, and cars)were also present. The children were instructed to find and circle with a pen as many target symbols as possible within 1 m i n u t e a period in which the detection/marking of all 80 targets would be extremely unlikely. The score was the nurnkr of targets correctly marked. TEA-Ch: Attmtional confro/. (1) Crearurc Counting. Switching from one task or mental set to another is almost invariably associated with a temporary a transitory loss of efficiency (Rogers slowing, which may reflect & Monsell, 1995). Clinically, the capacity to switch attention has often been tested using complextasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton, Chelune, Talleg, Kay, & Curtiss, T. MANLY et al. I070 Figwe 2. Sky Search motor control stimulus sheet. The stimulus sheet is identical to the visual search version, save for the removal of distracfors. Figwe 3. Item from the Creature Counting subtest. 1981, 1993), which require many other capacities. The Visual Elevator subtestof the TEA simplified these demands by using explicit cues as towhen to switch setand which set to switch to. This was the model for the Creature Counting subtest in the current battery. On each page of the Creature Counting stimulusbooklet, a variable number of “creatures” were depicted in their burrow (see Fig. 3). Interspersedbetween the creatures were arrows either pointing up or down. The children were asked to begin counting the creatures from the top down butto use the arrows as a cue to switch the direction of theircount. A correct response to the item shown in Fig. 3 below would therefore be, “ I , 2 (up arrow) 3,4,5,6(down arrow) 5 4 (up arrow) S 6 7 8”. The children’s ability to count up to and down from E5 was assessed prior to beginning the test.Followingtwopractice items on whichfeedback was given, the childrencompleted seven test items. The accuracy of the response and the time taken to compbete the page were recorded. If children scored 3 or more items correct, a timing score was calculated (seconds- per-switch) by dividingthe time taken to complete correct items by the number of switcheswithin those items.Again, the duration orthis test varies with thespeed of the child, but would usually be approximately S min. (2) Opposite Worlds. Deficits in the inhibition of a verbal disorders (Georgiou response have been noted in developmental et al., 1996) and in adults with acquired brain lesions (Burgess & Shallice, 1996, 1997). I n investigating verbal inhibition in children, Passler, Isaac, and Hynd (1985) used a set of dark and light stimulus cards. Children were asked to respond “night” to the light card and DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION F!gur.c 4. 1071 Item from the Opposite Worlds subtest. “day” to the dark card, this being viewed as a reversal of the most automatic or congruent response. The incongruent condition indeed took longer to complete than a congruent version. Using more explicit stimulus-verbalresponseassociations, Gerstadt, Hong, and Diamond(19941, adapted this task using pictures of the moon and the sun.In the Opposite Worlds task. the aim was to make the association as explicit as possible by using the digits 1 and 2 as the stimuli and the words “one” and two” as the response options. In the task the children were presented with a stimulus sheet showing a mixed, quasi-random array of the digits 1 and 2 (see Fig. 4). In the “Sameworld“ conditionthey were asked to read out the digits aloud as quickly as possible in the conventional manner. The purpose of theSameworld condition was to reinforce the “prepotent” set of naming the numbers in the conventional manner in the context of the test materials, and also to identify any unexpecteddifficulties a child may experience with the task. In the “Oppositeworld” condition they were asked to say the opposite foreach digit (“one” for 2 and ‘*two” for 1) as quickly as possible, inhibiting the prepotent verbal response. In the task, the examiner pointed to each digit in turn, only moving onto the next when a correct response was given, thus turning errors into a rime penalty. Following practice in each condition, four test pages were run in the order; Sameworld. Oppositeworld, Qppositeworld, Sameworld. The time taken to completeeach condition was recorded. Total time for the Oppositeworld condition was taken as the dependent variable. The average time spent on the test items in this study was 24.2 S (SD = 8.99) for the Sameworld and 30.67 (SD = 1 1.86) for the Oppositeworld. TEA-Ch:D u d task memure. (1) Sky Search DT. Performance decrements under dual task conditions tend toform sensitive measures o f neuroIogical impairment (e.g. Baddeley et al., 1991; Stuss et al., 1989). The TEA combined two of its subtests to form a dual task measure and this model was adopted here. En the Sky Search DT test children were asked to complete a parallel version of the Sky Search Task (see Fig. l), which differed only in the location of the targets. As they performed the visual search they were asked to simultaneously and silently count the number of tones presented within each item of an auditory counting task, giving the total at the conclusion of each item. Although the countingtask used the same stimuli as the Score! subtest, aregular pacing of one tone persecond was used. Following practice, the task and ltiming were initiated by a countdown played on the tape. The test was ended and timing stopped when the child indicatedcompletion of the visual search component. As it is possible that a child could completely neglect one of the tasks, scores from both measures were incorporated into a total score. Specifically the time taken to find each visual target was calculated (total tirne/correctly identified targetswa). The proportion of the counting items with correct totals was then calculated(total itemscorrect/total itemsattemptedj ( b ) . Poor counting performance was then used to inflate the timeper-targetscores by dividing (a) by (b). Finally, in order to assess the decrement from single task visual search performance, the raw time-per-target score from the SkySearchtaskwas subtracted from this value. To take an example, a child took 89 S to complete the task during which he found 19 targets, His time-per-target score was therefore 89/19 = 4.68. During this time he gave correct totals to threeof the six counting items he wa5 exposed to. His proportion correct score was therefore 3 / 6 = 0.5. Dividing the time-per-target score by this proportion inflates his time-per-targetscore to 4.68/0.5 = 9.36. In the original Sky Search test, his time-per-target score was 3.2 S. Subtracting this from the dual weighted time-per-target gives the decrement value, 9.36 - 3.2 = 6.16. The time taken to complete this measure varies according to the child. I n this sample the mean timeto completion was 99.3 S ( S D = 59.453. In the factor analysis of the TEA, the analogous measure loaded onto the sustained attention Factor.Consequently, it was predicted that a similar relationship would be observed in the children. E.risting nleasurcs. ( 1 ) The colour-word S/roop. (Trenerry et al., 1989). In this paradigmatic measure of nonspatialselective attention,the children were first asked to name the colour of simple colour 1072 MANLY T. et al. Table 2 Raw Scores on Each Subtest by Age Group Age band Score! (accuracy) Score DT (accuracy) Code Transmission (accuracy) Walk Don’t Walk (accuracy) Sky Search (time-per-target) Map Mission (total time) Creature Counting (accuracy) Creature Counting (time) Opposite Worlds (time) Sky Search DT (decrement) 1 ( N = 38) Mean ( S D ) 6.05 (2.32) 11.03 (3.50) 25.39 (9.81) 12.47 (4.03) 6.73 (3.03) 20.1 1 (6.38) 3.03 (2.43) 5.36 (3.56) 50.62 (15.80) 14.71 (15.37) 2 ( N = 56) Mean (SO) 7.79 (2.02) 13.70 (3.05) 31.16 (9.82) 12.98 (5.10) 5.07 (4.21) 29.95 (9.65) 4.86 (136) 5.36 (2.02) 34.49 (10.37) 5.25 (6.40) patches. They were then asked to read a list ofcolour words. In the condition of interest they were asked to name the colour of ink in which words are written, with the two always being in conflict. The measurewas the number correct within a 40-S period. (2) Trails Test (Spreen & Strauss, 1991). This task is thought totap selective attention/visual search and the capacity to switch attention. This measure has two parts. In Trails A, children were asked to draw linesbetweencirclesrandomly arranged on a page. Each circle contained a number and the connection of circles should follow a rule of ascending number sequence. In partB of the test, children were asked to join circles based on analternating rule ofascending numbers and letters in alphabetical sequence. The time taken to complete each measure and the number of errors made were recorded. (3) Matching Fandiar Figures Test ( M F F T ) . (Arizmendi, Paulsen, & Domino, 1981). In thistest, considered a measure of impulsivity, the children were asked to match a single stimulus with one of six similarlooking pictures. A speed-accuracy tradeoff is generally observed whereby fast, “impulsive” responding tends to produce more errors. (4) Wechsler Intelligence Scale f o r Children,3rdedition. (WISC-111;Wechsler,1991). Four subtests from thiswidely used measure of general intellectual function were used; two were measures ofverbal knowledge and reasoning (Vocabulary and Similarities) and two were measures of speeded visuospatial and motor function (Block Design and Object Assembly). ( 5 ) WideRangeAchievementTest-Revised (WRAT-R). (Jastak & Wilkinson, 1984). A measure of scholastic attainment in the areas of reading, spelling, and arithmetic. Procedure The 293 children were seenfor onesession during which allof the TEA-Ch subtests were completed. Tests were completed in the fixed order; Sky Search, Score!, Creature Counting, Sky Search DT,Map Mission, Score DT, Walk Don’t Walk, Opposite Worlds, and Code Transmission. The total duration of testing was approximately 1 hour, although this would vary with the amount of demonstration and practice required by each child. There were brief opportunities to rest between tasks as the examiner set upthe next test. Those children who completed further measures did so at a subsequent session. Children from the TEA-Ch retest sample were seen between 5 and 20 days after their first session. The aim in the testing sessions was to recreate the conditions in which most clinical testing occurs. That is, the rooms were protected from distracting noise and visual stimulation insofar as was possible. The auditory materials were presented using conventional portable taperecorders (without headphones) and 3 ( N = 54) Mean ( S D ) 8.78 (1.24) 16.37 (2.29) 37.52 (2.70) 15.48 (4.55) 3.71 (1.57) 42.57 (9.92) 5.37 (1.53) 4.02 (1.30) 28.81 (6.87) 1.18 (2.27) 4 ( N = 58) Mean ( S D ) 8.86 (1.34) 17.31 (1.98) 36.72 (5.88) 15.57 (4.43) 3.05 (1.17) 44.50 (10.33) 5.76 (1.13) 3.52 (0.88) 24.65 (6.44) 1.58 (3.90) 5 ( N = 58) Mean ( S D ) 9.48 (0.82) 18.19 (1.86) 38.16 (2.45) 16.41 (4.44) 2.80 (0.94) 50.05 (1 0.78) 5.17 (1.44) 3.24 (0.80) 22.95 (3.92) 0.98 (1.93) 6 ( N = 29) Mean ( S D ) 9.48 (1.02) 18.34 (1.61) 38.31 (2.29) 17.66 (1.88) 2.34 (0.69) 56.83 (8.92) 5.52 (1.55) 2.79 (0.79) 21.75 (5.89) 1.03 (2.01) the level was set so as to be comfortable for the child given the particular environment. Results Age and sex eflects within the TEA-Ch measures. The influence of age on performance of each of the TEA-Ch variables was initially analysed using correlation across the group as a whole (boys and girls). Unsurprisingly, age exerted a significant effect for each measure (Pearson’s r in each case being; Sky Search .58, Score! .54, Creature Counting (accuracy) .27, Creature Counting (timing) .63, Sky SearchD T .39, Map Mission.75, Score D T .69, Walk Don’t Walk .50, Opposite Worlds .73, and Code Transmission S S ) , t h e p value in each case being below < .001 needed for corrected statistical significance. As can be seen in Table 2, the absolute difference in performance between each age band tends to diminish in the older groups, suggestive of a developmental plateau and/or, for some measures, the emergence of ceiling effects (see below). The TEA-Ch was designed to be appropriate for both 6-year-olds and 16-year-olds. As can be seen in Table 2, thefirstaim of avoiding floor effects in the youngest group was achieved to a considerable degree. N o child achieved a zero score on the Score!, Score DT, Code Transmission, or Walk Don’t Walk subtests. Similarly, no child failed to find any of the targets in the Sky Search that thedemorMapTasks.In itself,thissuggests onstration and practice items were successful in explaining the tasks and that the perceptual demands of the tasks were relatively insensitive to normal variation in vision and hearing. Only on the Creature Counting subtest were complete failures to score observed (seven children). Whereas the time-based measures clearly show variationthroughouttheagerange,ceiling levelsofperformance in older children were observed on some of the item-based measures. Of the adolescents over the age of 15, forexample, 72 scoredatceilingontheScore! Subtest-a measure that similarly attracts ceiling performance in most neurologically healthy adults (Robertson et al., 1996). Ceiling levels of performance were less frequently observed in adolescents over the age of 15 on the Score DT, Code Transmission, Walk Don’t Walk, Creature Counting, and Map Mission tests (24Y0,37 YO, 10 YO,34 %, and 0 % respectively). The effects of sex for the entire sample were examined for each measure (raw scores) using ANOVA. There was no significant difference between the performance of the DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION 1073 Table 3 inTable3.Wherestrong ceilingeffects undermined correlationalanalysis,percentageagreement in raw scores between time l and 2 are shown. Validity. As discussed above, the first indication that children were able to understand and to perform the basic tasks of the TEA-Ch subtests comes from the accuracy data. With the exception of the Creature Counting subtest (where seven of the children failed to score at all), the children were able to perform a t least one item of all of the measures correctly. Further confidence regarding the insensitivity of the auditory material to subtle differences in hearing emerges from the Score D T subtest. In this measure, the children were asked to count tones while listening to a “news broadcast” for the mention of an animal. Although, as expected, performancein the tonecounting aspect of the task varied widely, over 9 0 % of the sample correctly identified at least8 of the 10 animal 0) on this,themost names(andnochildscored demanding auditory discrimination task in the battery. Relationshipto IQ. One hundred and sixty children completed four subtests of the WISC-111 in addition to the nine subtests of the TEA-Ch. The mean pro-rated IQ * * p < ,001. of the group was 107.78 ( S D = 14.2). The correlations between WISC-111 and TEA-Ch scaled scores are presentedin Table 4 (the use of scale scores partials out the effects of age). 147 girls and the 146 boys on any task with the exception Itmightbeexpectedthat“cognitivecongruence” of the Creature Counting test (timing score), where the would lead to significant positive correlations between boys performed moderately better than the girls (boys even two rather disparate measures in such a large sample. mean switch time = 3.95 S, S D = 1.78; girls mean switch However,only4ofthe10TEA-Chscoresshowed time = 4.48 S, S D = 1.89; F = 5.91, p .OS). significant relationships to pro-ratedIQ scores (Creature To considerdifferencesbetweenboysand girls at Countingaccuracy,MapMission,WalkDon’tWalk, different ages, raw scores were compared for each age andCodeTransmission; r = .31, .25, .21, and.l7 band separately. Only one task, the Sky Search visual respectively)-values that lose statistical significance searchmeasure,producedsignificantdifferences,with when correction for multiple correlations is performed girls outperforming boys in the age bands 9-1 1 and 13-1 5 (only p values of < .001 would reach corrected signifi( F = 6.8, p < .05 and F = 5.6, p < .05, respectively). cance). Reliability. Test-retestreliabilitywasassessed ona Theresultssuggest,therefore,thattheTEA-Ch is random subgroup of 55childrenfromacrosstheage assessing abilities that are not well tapped by (at least ranges, seen between 5 and 20 days following their first these) measures of general ability-and that additional assessment. Pearson’s correlations between raw performassessment with such measures is far from redundant. ance scoresat test 1 and test 2 are shown in Table 3 below. Convergent validity: Relationship to other measures of attention. Ninety-sixchildrenfromthesamplecomThe verywideagerangeoftheTEA-Chsample,of course, contributes greatly to the very strong correlations pleted additional measures of attention. As standardised shown. As a more conservative test, correlations with agescoresforchildrenwerenotavailableonallofthese measures,comparisonwiththeTEA-Chsubtestswas partialled out were performed. These are also presented Test Reliability: Test-Retest Correlations between the TEA-Ch Subtests Repeated at Between5 and 20 Days (Raw Correlations, Correlations with Age Partialled Out, and Percentage Agreement in Scoresfor Measures with Ceiling Eflects are Shown) Pearson’s Correlation with correlation age partialled Subtest coefficient out/% agreement Score! .64*** 76.2 Yo Score DT .74*** 71.4 yo .82*** .78 Code Transmission Walk Don’t Walk .73*** 71.0% .7s Sky Search .go*** Map Mission .M*** .65 Creature Counting .69*** .7 1 Accuracy Timing .73*** .57 .92*** Opposite Worlds .S5 Dual Task .81*** .8 1 Decrement Table 4 The Relationships Between TEA-Ch Subtest Performanceand Subtests of the WISC-111 (Age-scaled Scores) TEA-Ch subtest IQ Vocabulary Similarities Block Design Object Assembly Sky Search“ Score! Sky Search DTb Creature Counting Accuracy Speed Map Mission Score DT Walk Don’t Walk Opposite Worlds Code Transmission .l4 .l4 .05 .IO .l4 .05 .l4 .l2 .07 .l5 .l2 .l2 .31** .23** .20* .30** .27** .l2 .l5 .IO .l4 .07 .o1 .16* .l4 ,002 .03 .08 .24** .09 .24* .09 .18* - .01 .25** .l5 .2 1* .07 .17* - .16* .os .19* .09 .16* -.os .27** .l6 .24* .09 .l 1 Controlled score. Dual task decrement. * p < .05; **p < .01 (uncorrected significance levels:p < ,001 required for statistical significance when full correction for multiple comparisons is used). a T. MANLY et al. 1074 Table 5 Partial correlation CoefJients (Age Partialled Out) Between TEA-Ch Measures and Other Measures of Attention’ Other attentionalmeasures .29** TEA-Ch measures Stroop Trails Score! Score DT .40*** Code Transmission .30** Walk Don’t Walk Sky Search DT .31** Sky Search Map Mission Creature Counting Accuracy Time Opposite Worlds .08 .l7 .23* .l1 - .05 .40*** .31** .31** .31** -.l5 .24* A (time) .04 .09 - .O1 .l4 .l4 .l0 .69*** Trails B (time) .30** .l7 .45*** .37*** .O1 .l9 .26** .l3 .21* .l9 MFFT errors .28** .40*** .20* .22* -.l6 .35*** .08 .25* ”For ease of interpretation low (good) time scores are scaled positively to be consistent with high (good) accuracy scores. h Matching Familiar Figures Test. * p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < ,001 uncorrected significance levels; all others nonsignificant. combined with a linear transform. The ordered residuals made using partial correlations (age partialled out) on were then plotted against the corresponding quantile of a raw scores. The results are presented in Table 5. standardnormaldistribution.The besttransformfor Although correction for multiple correlations means that onlyp values of lessthan .OO 1 should be strictly taken each subtest was selected by the criterion of minimising as statistically significant, a number of patterns emerge thenonlinearityoftheregression.Theappropriate quantileswerethenestimatedbyapplicationofthe that nevertheless offer some support to our characterisationoftheprimaryattentionaldemandsofeach inverse power transform to the corresponding normal subtest.TheSkySearchandMapMissiontasks,for distributions. This transformation therefore reflects the example,werecharacterisedasprimarilyselectiveatrelationship of an individual’s raw score to the mean and tention tests. Both correlated with the superficially very distribution of their age band (see Yandell, 1997, Chapter different Stroop measure ( r = .40, p < .001 and r = .31, 13). In terms of modelling the data, this transformation p < .01 respectively)-often takenasaparadigmatic definition of selective attention processes. This relationoffers further advantages in normalising the scores and ship is unlikely to be primarily mediated by speed alone, effectively removing the influence of age. as the speed component in the Sky Search score was If the TEA-Ch measures make demands on attention, greatly reduced by the motor control manipulation (see and attention is best thought of as a unitary factor, then Measures). In addition, the Stroop was not significantly themostparsimoniousmodelinaccountingforthe related to a number of other speeded measures in the variancewould be asinglelatentvariable.Thiswas TEA-Ch (Sky Search DT, Creature Counting, Opposite investigated and found not to provide an adequatefit to Worlds) nor, indeed, to any sustained attention measure. the data, as indicated by the significant x2 value, x2 (27) = 95.05, p < .001. As might be predicted given the common requirement for speeded visual search, both Trails A and B showed We therefore investigated whether our broad model based on the adult literature formed a useful fit to the significantcorrelationswiththeSkySearchandMap patterns of test performance seen in the children. Each of Mission tests. These relationships were somewhat weakthe variables from the TEA-Ch battery was ascribed to a ened in the more cognitively complex Trails B, where relationships with other measures, including sustained latentattentionalvariablebasedontheirtheoretical attention, began to emerge. origins (see Measures). Score! Score DT, Code TransIt is of note that the Matching Familiar Figures Test, mission, and Walk Don’t Walk were ascribed to “susoften taken as a measure of behavioural inhibition, had tained attention” (with the Dual Task Decrement mearather low relationships with the Walk Don’t Walk and suresalsobeingassociatedwiththisfactorgiventhe Opposite Worlds subtests, which feature a requirement to results of Robertson et al., 1996). Map Mission and Sky withholdaprepotentmotororverbalresponse.The Searchwereassociatedwiththe“selectiveattention” relationships were certainly no greater than with other factorandCreatureCountingandOppositeWorlds measures without such an obvious requirement, such as associatedwithabroad“executive”or“attentional the Score! or Score DT tests. control” factor. Fit of the adult model of attentional separability to the The scaledscores fromthevariablesandthethree data: Structural Equation Models. In order to produce “latent” factors were entered into the EQS structural a clinical measure, the raw scores of the TEA-Ch subtests equationmodellingsoftwarepackage(Bentler & Wu, were transformed to age-scaled scores (following con1995). The patterns observed were consistent with this vention, e.g. Wechsler measures), these were scaled to a model as indicated by a nonsignificant statistic, (224) mean of 10 and a SD of 3; range1-19. This was achieved = 33.431, p = .lo. In addition, and particularly for large by first analysing each subtest using a one-way ANOVA samples, Bentler recommends the use of three incremental (forboysandgirlsseparately)withtheagebandsas fit measures;theComparativeFitIndex(CFI),the factorsafterapplyingaBox-Cox(power)transform Normed Fit Index(NFI) and the Non-Normed Fit Index x’ x‘ 1076 MANLY T. et al. Table 7 Relationship between TEA-Ch Age-scaled Scores and Age-scaled Scores,fionz the Wide Ranging Achievement Test ( N = 160) WRAT TEA-CH subset .26* Score! Score DT Transmission Code .18* .19* Don’t Walk Walk Sky Search DT .17* Sky Search” Map Mission Creature Counting Accuracy.l6 .22* Speed Opposite Worlds Reading Spelling Arithmetic .18* .17* .17* .19* .26* .22* .09 .l3 .l4 .l6 .27* .28* .33** .l0 .l9 .l4 .17* .IO .l3 .06 .40* .l8 .l4 .08 Controlled score. * p < .05; ** p < .01 uncorrected significance levels. Table 8 Perfornzance of 24 Boys Diagnosed with A D H D Relative to the Nornzative (male) Sanzpk TEA-Ch subtest F value P Score! Raw score Age-scaled score Score DT score Raw Age-scaled score Walk Don’t Walk 158)(1, score Raw Age-scaled score Sky Search DT Raw decrement score Age-scaled score Sky Search score Raw Age-scaled score Opposite Worlds score Raw Age-scaled score poor predictor of TEA-Ch performance.No relationship reached a statistical significance corrected for multiple correlations and only one (Creature Counting) reached uncorrected significance ( r = - .16, p < 5.0). The correlations between TEA-Ch measures and the Wide Ranging Achievement Test-Revised Reading, Spelling and Arithmetic scales (Jastak & Wilkinson, 1984) are presented in Table 7. Again, no relationship meets statistical significance once correction for multiple correof note, however, that the lations is applied. It is perhaps four measures of sustained attention show the strongest relationships with the attainment scores. Theresultsfromthisstudyarediscussedafterthe clinical study described below. Study 2 : The Performance of Boys with an ADHD Diagnosis Method Participants. Twenty-four boysmeetingDSM-IV criteria for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder weretested on subtests of the TEA-Ch and WISC-I11 measures. The mean age of the sample was 9.95 ( S D = 2.23). The boys were recruited from referrals made to anOutpatient Child and Family Centre. Diagnoses, according to DSM-IV criteria, weremade by a Consultant Psychiatrist and ClinicalPsychologistbased on Conners rating scales (Conners, Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998) supplied by the schools and two 1.5-hour assessment sessions. Exclusions from the current study were made if there was (a) a diagnosis of comorbid clinical or psychiatric disorder; (b) peripheral sensory loss, physical disability, epilepsy, or unequivocal braindamage; (c)severe adversity and/or involvement with Child Protection Agencies. The children were seen prior to the prescription of any medication. All but one of the children attended mainstream school (the exception attended a Special Needs School due to behavioural, ratherthan cognitive, difficulties). None of the group was identified as having specific learning needs, or indeed was seen as warranting a Statement of Educational Needs. Measures. The boyswere administered the Score! Score DT, WalkDon’tWalk, Sky Search, Sky Search DT, and Opposite Worlds subtestsof the TEA-Ch (use of the remaining measures Code Transmission, Map Mission, andCreature Counting was not practicable within the time available). A twotest short-form of the WISC-I11 (Sattler, 1988; Wechsler, 1991), comprising Vocabulary and BlockDesign subtests, was also administered. (1, 158) = 17.5 ( I , 158) = 14.8 < ,001 ( I , 157) = 88.0 (1, 157) = 45.9 < ,001 < ,001 = 66.0 ( I , 158) = 50.3 < ,001 < .001 (1, 157) = 14.8 (1, 158) = 5.65 < .OO 1 < .05 (I. 158) = 0.39 ( I , 158) =.907 1.68 (1, 158) = 23.7 (1, 158) = 21.8 < ,001 .983 ns. < ,001 < ,001 p values < ,001 required to reach statistical significance with correction for multiple comparisons. Procedure. Neuropsychological assessmentwascompleted 2 weeksbeforeanymedical treatment wascommenced.The same experimenter saw each participant alone. Testing took place at home or at the clinic, and was completed at one sitting. Test administration was completed according to the standardised procedures described above. Results Basic comparison with t h e normative sample. Analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs), comparing the A D H D boys’ results with those of boys from the normative sample (with age covaried), were performed on raw scores and the age-adjusted scaled scores. TheF statistics are shown with the corresponding p-value in Table 8 below. The results clearly show that the A D H D boys were significantly poorer than control boys at performing all of the TEA-Ch measures with the exception of the Sky Search subtest (although if formal correction for multiple comparisons is adopted, the Sky Search D T measure would also fail to meet corrected significance levels). Although this comparison indicates that the ADHD boys performed the measures more poorly than controls, us littleaboutwhetherattentionskillswere ittells particularly poor in this group. The boys, for example, alsoperformedpoorlyonothermeasureswith less obvious demands on attention, such as the Vocabulary subtest of the WISC-111. To examine whether attention represents adisproportionate problem in ADHD, children wereselectedfromtheTEA-Chsamplewhowere matchedtotheADHDgrouponage and level of performance on the WISC-111 Vocabulary subtest. It is important to note that only age and Vocabulary scores were used in the selection of controls-indeed, given the poor performance of the ADHD boys on this measure, relatively few children from the sample met these conditions ( N = 15) (see Table 9). As can be seen in Table 9, although the groups had almost identical performance on the Vocabulary measure, the ADHD children continued to showsignificantly poorer attention skills on many of the TEA-Ch measures. The Sky SearchD T test, which had previously reacheduncorrectedsignificance,nolongerhadthis DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION Table 9 Conzparison of 24 ADHD-Diagnosed Boys with 15 Age and WISC-III Vocabulary Matched Controls Vocabulary controls ADHD group P n.s. Age WISC-I11 n.s. Vocabulary (2.78) score 5.42 (2.13) 5.53 .96 (2.80) 9.53 Design Block TEA-Ch Score (3.66) ! 7.08 (3.09) 10.47 Score DT (2.79) 4.9610.13 (3.68) (2.86) 4.79 Walk (3.31)Don’t 9.33 Walk Search Sky DT (3.85) 6.04 (5.36) 7.87 (3.22) 8.93 Search Sky Opposite Worlds (3.65) 9.73 1.54) 1077 10.46 < .05 < .01 < ,001 < ,001 9.67 (4.00) 6.75 (4.21) n.s. n.s. < .os p values < ,001 requiredto reach statistical significance withcorrection for multiple comparisons. Table 10 Performance of a Group of 24 ADHD Boys Compared with Control Children Matched on WISC-III Block Design Subtest Scores and Age Design Block controls ADHD group P 9.99 n.s. Age WISC-I11 7.09 Design Block (4.09) 9.41 Vocabulary TEA-Ch Score! Score DT Walk Don’t Walk Sky Search DT Sky Search Opposite Worlds (2.78) 5.42 < .001 9.05 (2.77) .05 (3.66) 7.08 (2.79) 4.96 < ,001 9.36 (3.19) < ,001 (2.86) 4.79 8.76 (3.35) 7.33 (3.45) (4.27) 5.46 ,116 10.09 (2.76) .68 (4.00) 9.67 < ,027 (4.21) 6.75 9.19 (2.66) p values < ,001 required to reach statistical significance withcorrection for multiple comparisons, capacity.Thedifferencebetweenthegroupsofthe Opposite Worlds measure would now also disappear if full correction for multiple comparisons is adopted. As a final, and more conservative test of the specificity of attentional dysfunction within the group, the A D H D boys were matched with controls on the basis of age and WISC-111 BlockDesignscores.Thisisaconservative comparison because, as with a numberof measures from theTEA-Ch,the BlockDesigntestrequires a fast performanceand,incommon withsomeaspectsof attention, is thought reflect to efficiency ofpredominantly right hemisphere functioning (Lezak, 1995). Twenty-two children from the normative sample met the age and Block Design performancelevel criteria. The comparisons on TEA-Ch subtests, presented in Table 10, show significant and disproportionate deficits on three of the sustained attention factor measures (Score!, Score DT,andWalkDon’tWalk)andontheattentional controlmeasure,OppositeWorlds.Ifcorrectedfor multiple comparisons, however, only the Score DT and WalkDon’tWalkmeasures,bothfromthesustained attentionfactor,wouldcontinuetoreachstatistical significance. Discussion ThesubtestsoftheTEA-Charenotmeasuresof attention.Theyaremeasuresofauditoryandvisual detection, of counting, of response speed, and so forth. Separableattentionprocessesareinferredconstructs believed to contribute significantly to differences in the efficiency of performance on these tasks. By simplifying instructions,usingpracticesessions,andreducingthe contribution of perception, memory, and reasoning, our aim was to minimise variability due to nonattentional factors. The results from293 children from the normal schoolage population give some grounds to believe that these aims were met to a reasonable degree. For all but one of the tasks, all of the children-even as young as 6-were able to perform least at one item correctly. Taken together with the mean levelsof performance (shown in Table 2), thisindicatesthatthebasiccomprehensionand perceptual demands were met. In the case of the Score! subtest, for example, the children’s task was to maintain a count of the number of identical sounds that they heard within the period of each item. Over 90% of the sample gave correct totals for 5 or more of the 10 items. As a basic capacity to count up to15 was assessed prior to the task, and the sounds must have been detectable, errors wouldseemtostemfromadifficultyinvoluntarily maintaining“one’smind”onthis verytediousactivity-in other words, sustained attention’ (cf. Wilkins et al., 1987). The nonsignificant relationships between the TEA-Ch It is a difficult conceptual and clinical question to disambiguate poor attention from poor motivation-perhaps most particularly in dull sustained attention tasks. The reliabilityofthe measures rather argues in favour of a “can’t do” account-in that motivational (“won’t do”) factors may be more volatile from one day to another. However, in practice a difficulty in maintaining attention to an unrewarding task,from either source, may have similar functional consequences. 1078 T. MANLY et al. functions, the conceptual gulf between tests and the real measures and conventional WISC-111 IQ tasks (at least within the range we assessed) provide additional support worldcan be substantial.Inthestandardassessment for the relatively focused nature of the tasks and for the setting, the examiner acts to focus the child’s attention on the material, provides clear instructions as to what is value of separate assessment of attentive functions. Tests of executive function in adults (particularly those expected, is suitably encouraging, and attempts to protect that stress novelty) can be inherently unreliable (Burgess, theenvironmentfromexternaldistractors.Thesefeatures, together with the relative novelty of the materials, 1997; Wilson et al., 1997)-indeed, it has been argued are important in making a reasonable comparison bethat frontal/dysexecutive difficulties can themselves be tween different children. It is important to ask, however, thought of as a deficitin the reliable application of whether a child’s performance under these conditions has strategy,attention,orbehaviouralcontrol(Rabbitt, anybearingontheirbehaviourinacrowded,noisy 1997). In this sense, the poorest level of performance seen classroom,inthecacophonichome,orwhentheininachild-whether ornotitcanberepeatedlyobserved-may be informative. However, given the influformation that is relevant and the nature of the goal are ence of factors with day-to-day variation (noise, mood, not clearly specified? Clearly the best way of assessing other preoccupations and so forth), clinically it is useful whether such difficulties occuris to ask teachers, parents, to have a sense of the stability of a measure. In this respect orthechildrenthemselves.In accounfing forsuch the predictive value of TEA-Ch scores from one session problems, however, consideration of particular cognitive toanotheraresurprisinglygoodalthough,giventhe deficits,inconjunctionwithemotional,social,and rapiddevelopmentofabilitiesseenintheyounger environmentalfactors,may be crucial in selectingthe most appropriate form of support. children, such reliability can only provide a short-range One group of children who,by definition, are reported forecast. to experience difficulties in these everyday situations are Given that underlying attentional processes do conthose diagnosed with ADHD. In the second study, the tribute to performance on the measures, itis possible to performance of such children on the TEA-Ch measures ask whether this looks like the footprint a single of system under standard testing conditions was examined. (say, efficiency of “top-down’’ control) or, as suggested diagnosis ofA D H D Twenty-four boys with DSM-IV a by Posner and Petersen (1990) among others, like the (who were not yet prescribed medication) showed signifiinfluence of different systems performing different types cant deficits across sustained attention and attentional of attention process. control subtests of the TEA-Ch but, notably, no deficit Using the SEM approach, thesingle system model did onthespeeded-visualsearchtask.Giventhepoor not form a good fit to the data. Usinga broad three-way division into sustained and selective attention and execuperformanceofthegrouponarangeofmeasures, tive orattentionalcontrolbasedonadultmodels, includingvocabulary,thiscomparisondoeslittleto indicate the specificity of attentional dysfunction within however, did. The results showed that the fit of this model was not the group. More conservative comparisons with children significantlydifferent fortheolderandtheyounger who were matched on both age andprrformance levels on WISC-111 subtests revealed persistent deficits. The results children. This suggests that despite the relative immaare consistent with previous findings that have emphaturity of these capacities, the differential nature of the processesisstillsufficientlyexpressed to be detected sised both a sustained attention deficit and a difficulty in across the tasks. the suppression of prepotent responses (Barkley, 1997; It remains important to see whether this fit emerged Barkley et al., 1992b; Douglas, 1972; Hooks et al., 1994; from “peripheral” aspects of the tasks that are of little Logan et al., 1997; Shue & Douglas, 1992; Swanson et relevance tothepostulatedattentionalfactors.It is also consistentwith viewsof a al.,1998).Theyare certainly true, for example, that the sustained attention neurological basis to the ADHD disorder-which have tests tended to be based on summed item correct scores identifiedabnormalitiesincludingwithinrightfrontal and (consequently) are more vulnerable to ceiling effects systems. than the selective attention tasks. The use of the ageThere are important questions about the representascaling technique (on which the model is based), however,tiveness of our ADHD sample. In particular, although considerably reduced the impact of this factor and the comorbid diagnoses are very common in A D H D , chilskew and kurtosis of the sustained attention scores were dren with comorbid diagnosis were excluded from our study. Our aim, however, without wishing to over-reify unremarkable. thediagnosticcategory,wastoexaminethe“pure” Otherfactorssuchastherequirementforspeeded responses, or linguistic responses, can alsobe excluded to effects of A D H D a s a basis for subsequent evaluation of a degree. The use of the “motor control” task in the Sky the effect of comorbid and other factors. Whether such Search test, for example, resulted in the speed contribuchildrenwouldshowsimilarprofilesontheTEA-Ch tions to the two selective attention tasks being rather remains an open question. A second important issue in different. Verbal responses were required in the sustained this respect concerns the rather poor performance of the attentionandattentionalcontrolmeasures.Further group on WISC-111 measures, compared with a number supportive evidence emerges from the relationships beof other reported ADHD samples. Although our comtween the TEA-Ch and other measures of attention. As parison indicates that specific deficits in attention still are with healthy adults (Robertson et al., 1996), the colourapparentwhen we control for WISC-111 performance word Stroop showed the strongest relationship to mealevels, furtherwork is againrequiredtoexaminethe sures of selective attention with very different “surface” pattern in ADHD children who enjoy higher levels of characteristics-and relatively poor relationships to susgeneral performance. tained attention measures. Anderson and colleagues (Anderson, Fenwick, Manly, The central aim of clinical assessment is to predict and & Robertson, 1998)havepreviouslyreportedonthe account for difficulties that may arise within everyday TEA-Chperformance of childrenwhohadsustained life. Perhaps most particularly for attention executive and closed head injuries an average of 6 years prior to the DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT OF ATTENTION assessment.Althoughclosedheadinjuriesdisproportionately compromise frontal/temporaI functions, damage to a wide range of cortical and subcortical structures is possible (Mattson & Levin, 1990). As a group, these childrenshoweddeficitsacrosstherangeofTEA-Ch measures including the selective attention factor relative to a control group (matched on both age and WISC-111 BlockDesignsubtestperformance).Althoughfurther work is clearly required to look at a range of disorders, of differentially theseearlystudiesindicatethevalue assessingattentionalfunctions.Perhapsmostimportantly, in adults the assessments of separable attentional function has led to improved targeting and effectiveness inrehabilitativeinterventions(e.g.Robertson,Tegner, Tham, Lo, &L Nimmo-Smith, 1995; Sturm, Willmes, Orgass, & Hartje, 1997). It is tobehopedthatthe extension of suchmeasuresforchildrencouldprove similarly effective-both in rehabilitation and in assessing the specific effectsof pharmacological interventions. Acknowledgements-We are grateful to the Universityof who Melbourne andthe U.K. MedicalResearchCouncil supported this research. Our thanks also to Mick Wilson,Sarah Jones, and theThames Valley TestCompany fortheir assistance in developingthe measure and forpermission to use examples of stimuli from the test in Figures 1 4 . We are indebted to Rani Jacobs, Tim Godber, and John Gora for their help with the project, and toJulia Darling forher careful preparation of this manuscript. Copiesof the TEA-Ch may be obtained from www.tvtc.com References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manualofnzental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Anderson, V.,Fenwick, T., Manly, T., & Robertson, I. H. (1998). Attentional skills following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 12, 937-949. Anderson, V., & Pentland, L. (1998). Residualattention deficits following childhood head injury: Implications for ongoing development. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8, 283-300. Arizmendi, T., Paulsen, K., & Domino, G. (1981). The Matching Familiar Figures Test-A primary, secondary and tertiary evaluation. Journal of Clinical Ps-vchology, 37, 81 2818. Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Working memory or working attention. In A.D. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 152-1 70). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A.D., Bressi, S., Della Salla, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s Disease. Brain, 114, 2521-2542. Barkley, R. A. (1997).Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention,and executive functions:Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin. 121, 65-94. Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. J. (1992a). Are tests of frontallobe functionusefulin the diagnosisof attention deficit disorders? The Clinical Neuropsychologist,8, 121-140. Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. J. (1992b). Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review and research report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 163-188. Bench, C . J., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M., Friston,K. J., Paulesu, E., Frackowiak, R. S. J.. & Dolan,R. J. (1993). Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the Stroop test. Neuropsychologia, 31, 907-922. Bentler, P. M,, & Wu, E. J. C. (1995). E Q S j o r Windows;User’s guide. Encino, CA : Multivariate Software. 1079 Brouwer, W. H., & Van Wolffelaar, P. C. (1985).Sustained attentionand sustainedeffort after closedhead injury: Detection and 0.10 Hz heart rate variability in a low event rate vigilance task. Cortex, 21, 11 1-1 19. Brouwers, P,, Riccardi, R., Fedio, R., & Poplak, D. (1985). Long-term neuropsychological sequelae childhood of leukemia:Correlation with CT brain scanabnormalities. Journal of Paediatrics, 106, 723-728. Brumback, R.A., & Staton,R. D. (1982).Anhypothesis regarding the commonality of right hemisphere involvement in learningdisability attentional disorder, andchildhood major depressive disorder. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 1091-1097. Burack, J. A., Enns, J. T., Johannes, E. A., Stauder, E. A., Mottron, L., & Randolph, B. (1997). Attention and autism: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar (Eds.) Handbook of autismandpervasive developnzental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 226-247). New York: Wilson & Sons. Burgess, P. W. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function research. Jn P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology offrontal and executive function(pp. 81-1 11). Hove, U.K. : Psychology Press. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1996).Bizarreresponses,rule detection and frontal lobe lesions. Cortex, 32. 241-259. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton tests. Bury St. Edmunds, U.K.: Thames Valley Test Company. Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., Costello, A. D., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38, 848-863. Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D., Vaituzis, M. S. A., Dickstein, D. P., Sarfatti, S. E., Vauss. Y . C.,Snell, J. W., Rajapakse, J. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1996). Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives ofGeneral Psychiatry, 53, 607-61 6. Cohen, R.M,, & Semple, W. E. (1988). Functional localization ofsustained attention. Neuropsychiatry,Neuropsvchology and Behavioural Neurology, l , 3-20. Cohen, R. M,, Semple, W. E., Gross, M,, King, A.C., & Nordahl, T. E. (1992). Metabolic brain pattern of sustained auditory discrimination. Experimental BrainResearch,92, 165-172. Comers, C. K, Sitarenios, G, Parker, J. D., & Epstein, J. N. (1998).Revision andrestandardization of the Comers’ Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R) : factor structure reliability and criterion validity. Journal ofAbnorma1 Child Psychology, 26, 279-291. Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege andprestige: Occupationsin Australia. Melbourne, Australia: Longman-Chesire. Douglas, V. I. (1972). Stop, look and listen: The problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Canadian Journal ojBehavioura1 Science,4 , 259-281. Driver, J., & Halligan, P. W. (1991). Can visual neglect operate in object-centred co-ordinates?An affirmativesingle-case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8 , 475496. Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 27 1-290. Duncan, J.,Bundesen, C., Olson, A., Humphreys, G., Chavda, S., & Shibuya, H. (in press). Systematic analysis of deficits in visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Filipek, P. A., Semrud-Clikeman, M,, Staingard, R. J., Renshaw, P. F., Kennedy, D. N., & Biederman, J. (1997). Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficithyperactivity disorder with normal controls. Neurology, 48, 589-601. Fisk, A. D., & Schneider, W.(1981). Control and automatic processing during tasksrequiring sustained attention: A new approach to vigilance. Human Factors, 23, 737-750. Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., & Stein, A. (1999). Right hemispheric 1080 MANLY T. dominance of inhibitory control : An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings o f t h e National Academy of Science, 96, 8301-8306. George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P.I., Rosinsky, N., Ring, H., Casey, B. J., Trimble, M. R., Horwitz, B., Herscovitch, P., & Post, R. M. (1994). Regional brain activitywhen selecting response despite interference : An H2 150 PET study Human Brain of theStroopandanemotionalStroop. Mapping, 1, 194-209. Georgiou, N.,Bradshaw, J. L., Phillips, J. G., & Chiu,E. (1996). The effect of Huntington’s disease and Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome on the ability to hold and shift attention. Neuropsychologia, 34,843-851. Gerstadt, C . L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action : Performance of children 2-7 yearsold on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129-153. Godefroy, O., Cabaret, M.,& Rousseaux, M. (1994). Vigilance and effects of fatigability practice and motivation on simple reaction time tests in patients with lesion of the frontal lobe. Neuropsychologia, 32, 938-990. Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C . (1995). Multivariate datuanalysis (4th ed.). Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Curtiss, G. (1 98 1,1993).Wisconsin Card SortingTest Manual. Odessa, FL : Psychological Assessment Resources. Heilman, K. M,, Voller, K. K., & Nadeau, S. E. (1991).A possible pathophysiologic substrate of attention deficit 6 (Suppl), hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Neurology, S76-81. Hooks, K., Milich, R., & Lorch, E. (1994). Sustained and selective attention in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal o j Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 69-77. Jastak, J. F., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Wide Ranging Achievement Test-Revised. Washington, DC: Jastak Assessment Systems. Kaufmann, P. M,, Fletcher, J. M,, Levin, H. S., Miner, M. E., & Ewing Cobbs, L. (1993). Attentionaldisturbanceafter pediatric closed head injury. Journal of Child Neurology, 8, 348-353. Klin, A., Sparrow, S., Volkmar, F., Cicchetti, D., & Rourke, B. P.(1995). Asperger syndrome. In B.P. Rourke (Ed.), Syndrome o j non-verbal learning disabilities (pp. 93-1 18). New York : Guilford Press. Koelega, H. S. (1996). Sustained attention.In 0. Neumann (Ed.), Handbook ofperception and action, Vol. 3. London: Academic Press. Lang, W., Athanasopoulous, O., & Anderson, V. (1988). Do attentional profilesdiffer across developmental disorders? Journal o j t h e International Neurological Society, 4,221, Lewin, J. S., Friedman, L., Wu, D., Miller, D. A., Thompson, L.A., Klein, S. K., Wise, A. L.,Hedera, P,, Buckley, P,, Meltzer, H., Friedland, R.P,, & Duerk, J. L. (1996). Cortical localization of human sustained attention: Detection with functional MR using a visual vigilance paradigm. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 20, 695-701. Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Logan, G. D.,Schachar,R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8, 60-64. Loken, W. J., Thornton, A. E., Otto, R. L., & Long, C . J. (1995). Sustained attention after severe closed head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 592-598. Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., & Bruhn, P. (1984). Focal cerebral dysfunction in developmental learning disabilities. Lancet, 335, 8-1 1. Mackworth, J. F. (1970). Vigilanceand attention:A signal detection approach. Harmondsworth, U.K. : Penguin. Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. TheQuarterly Journal of’ Experimental Psychology, I , 6-21. et al. Manly, T., Datta, A., Heutink, J., Hawkins, K., Cusack, R., Rorden, C., & Robertson, I. H. (2000). Anelectrophyiological predictor of imminent action error inhumans. Journal o f Cognitive Neuroscience, 21E (Suppl), 111. Manly, T., & Robertson, I. H. (1997). Sustained attention and the frontal lobes. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology qffiontal and executivefunction (pp. 135-150). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press. Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M,, & Hawkins, K . (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37, 66 1-670. Mattson, A. J., & Levin, H. S. (1990). Frontal lobe dysfunction literature. followingclosedhead injury: A reviewofthe Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 282-29 1. Miyake, A,, Friedman, N. P,, Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D. (inpress).Theunityand diversityofexecutive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobes tasks: A latent variableanalysis. Cognitive Psychology. Owen, A. M,, Roberts, A. C., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damageor Parkinson’s disease. Bruin, 116, 1 159-1 175. Parasuraman, R., Warm, J. S., & See, J. E. (1998).Brain systems of vigilance. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1991). Localization of a humansystem for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature, 349, 61-64. Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P., Janer, K., & Raichle. M. E. (1990). The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroopattentional conflict paradigm. Pvoccwlings of’ the Nutional Acudemy of Science U S A , 87, 256-259. Passler, M. A., Isaac, W., & Hynd, G. W. (1985). Neuropsychologicaldevelopmentof behaviour attributed to frontal lobe functioning in children. Developmento1 Neuropsycholog.~,I , 349-370. Paus, T., Zatorre, R. J., Hofle, N., Caramanos, Z., Gotman, J.. Petrides, M., & Evans, A. C. (1997). Time-related changes in neural systems underlying attention and arousal during the performance on an auditory vigilance task. Journal of’ Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 392408. Ponsford, J., & Kinsella, G. (1992). Attentional deficitsfollowingclosed-head injury. JournalofClinical and E.uperimental Neuropsychology, 14, 822-838. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orientatingofattention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25. Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review ofllieurosciencc, 13, 2542. Rabbitt, P.(1997).Review chapter. InP. Rabbitt(Ed.), Methodology oj:fiontal and executive function. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press. Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J . , Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentionalfailures intraumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35,741-758. Robertson, I . H., Manly, T., Beschin, N., Haeske-Dewick. H., Homberg, V., Jehkonen, M,, Pizzamiglio,L.,Shiel,A.. Weber, E., & Zimmerman, P.(1997). Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1527-1532. Robertson, I. H., Tegner, R., Tham, K., Lo, A., & NimmoSmith, I. (1995). Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: Theoretical and rehabilitation implications. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 4 16-430. Robertson, 1. H., Ward, A., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Test of’Everyday Attention.Bury St Edmunds, U.K.: Thames Valley Test Company. Robertson, I. H., Ward, A., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1996). Thestructure of normal human attention : The Test of Everyday Attention. Journal of the Internationnl Neuropsychological Society, 2, 523-534. DIFFERENTIAL ASSESSMENT O F ATTENTION Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switchbetweensimplecognitive tasks. Journal of E.xperimental Psychology-General, 124, 207-231. Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Samson, I., Bransome, E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20,343-350. Rueckert, L., & Grafman, J. (1996). Sustained attention deficits in patients with right frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 34, 953-963. Rueckert, L., Sorensen, L., &Levy, J. (1994). Callosal efficiency is related to sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 32, 159-173. Sattler, J. (1 988). Assessment of children’s intelligence andspecial abilities. Boston, MA : Allyn & Bacon. See, J. (1995). Meta-analysis ofthesensitivitydecrementin vigilance. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 230-249. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Deficit in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727-741. Shapiro, M,, Morris, R., Morris, M,, Flowers, C., & Jones, R. (1998). A neuropsychologically based assessment model of the structure of attention in children. Devc~lopmentalNeuropsychologJ1, 14, 657-677. Shue, K. L., & Douglas, V. I. (1992). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and the frontal lobe syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 20, 104-124. Skuse, D. H., James, R. S., Bishop, D. V. M,, Coppin, B., Dalton, P,, Aamodt-Leeper, G., Bacarese-Hamilton, M., Creswell, C., McGurk, R., & Jacobs, P. A. (1997). Evidence from Turner’s syndromeof an imprinted X-linkedlocus affecting cognitive function. Nature, 387, 705-708. Spikman, J. M., Van Zomeren,A. H., & Deelman, B. G. (1996). Deficits of attention after closed head injury : Slowness only. Journal o j Clinical and E-xperimentul Neuropsychology, 18, 1-13. Spreen, O., & Straws, E. (1991). Acompendium of’neuropsychological tests. Oxford : Oxford University Press. Stam, C. J., Visser, S. L., Op de Cod, A. A. W., De Sonneville, L. M. J., Schellens, R. L. L. A., Brunia, C. H. M., De Smet, J. S., & Gielen, G. (1993). Disturbed frontal regulation of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 116, 1139-1 158. Sturm, W., Willmes, K., Orgass, B., & Hartje, W. (1997). Do specific attention deficitsneedspecific training? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 7, 81-103. Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., & Picton, T. W. (1995). A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional 1081 functions. Annals of the New York Academyof Sciences, 769, 191-209. Stuss, D. T., Stethem, L. L., Hugenholtz, H., Picton, T. W., Pivik, J., & Richard, M. T. (1 989). Reaction time after head injury:Fatigue, dividedandfocused attentionandconsistency of performance. Journal of Neurology?Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 79, 81-90. Swanson, J., Castellanos, F. X., Murias, M,, LaHoste, G., & Kennedy, J. (1998).Cognitiveneuroscienceof attention deficithyperactivity disorder andhyperkinetic disorder. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 236-271. Swanson, J. M,, Learner, M., & Williams, L. (1995). More frequent diagnosis of ADHD. New EnglandJournal oj Medicine, 333, 944. Tranel, D., & Hyman, B. T. (1990). Neuropsychological correlates of bilateral amygdala damage. Archives of Neurology, 47, 349-355. Trenerry, M.R., Crosson, B.,DeBoe, J., & Leber, W. R. (1989). Stroop NeuropsychologicalScreening Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Van Zomeren, A. H., & VandenBurg,W.(1985).Residual complaints of patients twoyears after severeclosedhead injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 48, 21-28. Voeller, K. K. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1988). Attention deficit disorder in children: Aneglectsyndrome? Neurology,38, 806-808. Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Whyte, J., Polansky, M,, Fleming, M,, Branch-Coslett, H., & Cavallucci, C. (1995). Sustained arousal and attention after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 33, 797-8 13. Wilkins, A. J., Shallice, T., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Frontal lesions and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 359365. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. (1997). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. BurySt Edmunds, U.K.: ThamesValleyTest Company. Wilson, B., Cockburn, J., & Halligan, P. (1987).The Behavioural InattentionTest. BurySt Edmunds, U.K.: ThamesValley Test Company. ,for designed Yandell, B. S. (1997). Practicaldataanalysis experiments. London: Chapman & Hall. Manuscript accepted 7 August 2001
© Copyright 2024