1. CHOMSKY'S OBSERVATIONS 2. EXPERIMENTS WITH "PRE-LANGUAGE" CHILDREN 3. STAGES OF ACQUISITION

LINGUISTICS 1, WEB QUIZ #6
1. CHOMSKY'S OBSERVATIONS
2. EXPERIMENTS WITH "PRE-LANGUAGE" CHILDREN
3. STAGES OF ACQUISITION
4. ACQUISITION OF PHONOLOGY (PRONUNCIATION)
5. ACQUSITION OF WORDS AND MEANINGS
6. DO ADULTS TEACH CHILDREN LANGUAGE?
7. HOW DO CHILDREN ACQUIRE LINGUISTIC PATTERNS?
8. DO CHILDREN MAKE LINGUISTIC ERRORS?
9. WHAT DO CHILDREN LISTEN FOR?
10. ACQUISTION OF A SECOND LANGUAGE BY ADULTS
1. CHOMSKY'S OBSERVATIONS
[NOTE: I'm not sure where "Chomsky's observations" first appeared in print, but they are now part of the
standard linguistics "lore".]
Noam Chomsky has made the following observations about children acquiring language
(course reader, page 95):
• Rapidity of acquisition
• Acquisition without special instruction
• Acquisition based on exposure to randomly encountered input
WHICH ONE OF THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS BEST CHARACTIZES THE
RELEVANCE OF THESE OBSERVATIONS FOR THE CLAIM THAT LANGUAGE
IS AN "INSTINCT"?
a. Children, if exposed to a language in the normal way, will automatically acquire it in
the same way that they automatically develop physical attributes that are genetically
specific to humans.
b. Children's brains are more "plastic" than the brains of adults, and they therefore have
the capacity to acquire all sorts of abilities that become difficult for adults.
c. Children are born with "grammar genes", but these shut down after a few years as the
brain must turn its energies to other cognitive functions.
d. Language is such an important ability that children are genetically predisposed to
acquiring this ability before other abilities, such as doing math or playing musical
instruments.
e. Chomsky's observations don't stand up to close scrutiny: 4-5 years isn't all that "rapid",
and children's caretakers provide much more "special instruction" and "specially
structured input" than is generally acknowledged.
1
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
2
2. EXPERIMENTS WITH "PRE-LANGUAGE" CHILDREN
Here is a diagram of a set up for a Conditioned Head Turn procedure used to test speech
perception of infants. The diagram comes from this paper:
http://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca/uploads/forms/1252960095WerkerPolkaPegg_1997.pdf
Reinforcer = animated and colorful toy
E1 = Experimenter 1
(holds infant's attention with "Toys")
E2 = Experimenter 2
(runs experiment, hidden from others)
Toys = stuffed animals, etc.
(used to hold infant's attention)
In question #4 below, we see that Sarah says, "Da," for both duck and dog. Suppose that
we want to test whether she can PERCEIVE the difference between the vowels in these
words by using the stimuli duck [duhk] and dock [dak] (because of font problems on web
pages, "uh" is substituted for the "upside down v" in 'but' in the table on page 60 of the
course reader).
USING THE SET-UP SHOWN IN THE DIAGRAM, WHICH OF THE FOLLOWING
WOULD DEMONSTRATE THAT SARAH COULD DISTINGUISH BETWEEN
THESE VOWELS?
a. After training, Sarah would hear duck-duck-duck-duck-dock and after the fourth
repetition of duck, she would turn in the direction of the reinforcer in anticipation of
hearing dock.
b. After training, Sarah would hear duck-duck-duck-duck-dock and turn in the direction
of the reinforcer before the toy lit up.
c. After training, Sarah would hear duck-duck-duck-duck-dock, and after hearing dock,
she would wait for the reinforcer to light up, then turn toward it.
d. After training, Sarah would hear duck-duck-duck-duck-dock. After each repetition of
duck, she would turn toward the reinforcer, then turn back, but upon hearing dock, she
would fix her gaze on the reinforcer and not turn back.
e. After training, Sarah would be attracted by the reinforcer, then hear dock-duck-duckduck-duck. As soon as she heard duck for the first time, she would turn away from the
reinforcer.
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
3
3. STAGES OF ACQUISITION
Video clips that can be seen here show Melissa at roughly three stages of language
development. In each clip she uses the word "see" as part or all of her utterance.
21 months (one word)
23.4 months (two word)
25.2 months (telegraphic)
(computer facing away so
that she can't see the
screen)
Melissa: See...see, see.
(turns computer around;
music stops)
Melissa: Oh boy!
(tries to restart it)
(on bed, stack of pillows
covering Bubbles, a stuffed
animal)
Melissa: Ah! Bubbles! I
see.
Grandma: Oh! Bubbles
was there! You were sitting
on him.
(in bedroom with Mom)
Mom: Look, Melissa. You
used to wear this.
(doorbell rings)
Melissa: Oh! That's
Grandma.
Grandpa: Shall we go say...
shall we go let Grandma in?
Melissa: OK. Let's see
Grandma's here. (runs to
living room) Let's see
Grandma's here. ...
(pointing to door) You ARE
there!
WHICH ONE STATEMENT BEST CHARACTERIZES STAGES IN LANGUAGE
DEVELOPMENT AS PART OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT?
a. Complexity in language structure matches complexity of conceptual ability in general.
b. A child moves from linguistically "labeling" salient concepts to applying a larger and
larger repetoire of rules that combine those "labels" into more complex linguistic
structures.
c. Children become better and better at imitating the speech patterns of adults as they
grow older.
d. Children progressively move from speaking ungrammatically to speaking
grammatically.
e. At the earlier stages, words like 'see' in these examples are more or less "categoryless",
but firming words up into true categories like VERB and NOUN allows the child to
create complex structures like VP and NP.
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
4
4. ACQUISITION OF PHONOLOGY (PRONUNCIATION)
A video clip that can be seen here shows Sarah at the one-word stage (17.1 months). The
main interest here is Sarah's linguistic abilities that are revealed by the items in red.
Mom: Sarah, what's this?
Sarah: Da!
M: (showing picture) That's right! It's a duck!
S: Da!
M: That's right! What's this?
S: Da!
M: (showing picture) That's right! It's a dog!
(gives book to Sarah, who giggles)
Grandpa: Can you find the dog? (Sarah
giggles) Where's the dog? (Sarah points to
picture of a dog) Yep!
G: Can you find the fish? (Sarah points to
picture of a fish) Yep!
G: Can you find the duck? Where's the duck?
(Sarah points to picture of a duck) That's right!
That's a duck! Uh-huh!
Which of the following statements BEST ACCOUNTS FOR THE FACTS
HIGHLIGHTED IN RED?
a. Sarah has a "word" [da] that is a general term for any living creature.
b. Sarah has looked at this book so many times that she has memorized how adults have
labeled the pictures.
c. Sarah is being coached by adults as she is asked to name pictures or to point to specific
pictures.
d. Sarah has the mental ability to apply duality of patterning, but her speech consists of
meaning elements that cannot be broken down into meaningless sounds.
e. Sarah has the concept of duality of patterning, but her ability to apply it in her mental
repertoire exceeds her current level of application in her speech.
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
5
5. ACQUSITION OF WORDS AND MEANINGS
A video clip that can be seen here shows Melissa at the beginning of the one-word stage
(16.2 months).
(Melissa playing with small plastic animals)
Grandpa: That's a sheep and that's a cow. (Melissa
plays with sheep) Baa! He says, "Baa!" (Melissa look
quizzically) Sheep says, "Baa!"
Melissa: (holding sheep and thinking) Ba. ... Ba.
(reaches into container and pulls out a toy horse)
Melissa: Ba, ba.
Grandpa: That's a horse. ... Horse.
WHICH ONE OF THE FOLLOWING STATEMENTS BEST ACCOUNTS FOR
MELISSA'S USES OF THE ba IN RED?
a. Melissa has not yet acquired the Whole Object Principle--instead of calling the whole
animal a "sheep", she names one of its "parts", in this case, the sound it makes.
b. Melissa has not yet acquired the Lexical Contrast Principle, which says that when
one hears a new word, the assumption is that it must refer to something for which we do
not yet have a name.
c. Melissa has not yet acquired the Mutual Exclusivity Principle, which says that
different objects should have different names.
d. As Melissa hears a new word (like Grandpa's word "Baa"), she is making hypotheses
about what the word might mean, for example, "small toy animal", then applying this
hypothesis.
e. Melissa has a new word that sounds nice, so she is taking the opportunity to use it.
6. DO ADULTS TEACH CHILDREN LANGUAGE?
The video on child language acquisition gave an example of how children interpret
certain sentences using a story about a boy who fell out of a tree, then discovered his
injury later when taking a bath. (Click here to see a clip from the video.)
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
6
CLAIM: This experiment provides good evidence that children learn complex grammar
without being taught.
FOUR of the answers in (a-e) seem like good support for this claim. Which ONE would
NOT furnish much, if any support for the claim?
a. The children did not all use the same wording in their responses.
b. Adults seemed puzzled as to why one question had two possible answers and the other
only one possible answer. Since they were puzzled themselves, they couldn't have taught
the children.
c. A number of children, clearly of varying backgrounds, were completely consistent in
their responses.
d. These questions involve grammatical complexity far beyond what children this age
would learn in school, much less learn from parents who are not trained in grammar
instruction.
e. The children had no trouble understanding what was being asked and were able to give
sensible repsonses.
7. HOW DO CHILDREN ACQUIRE LINGUISTIC PATTERNS?
We can line up sentences as following certain patterns. Though the sentences on the left
and right share patterns (shown by the labels in smaller type), the sentences on the left are
fine whereas those marked with the * on the right are not heard.
(1)
(2a)
(2b)
THE PATTERN IS OK
I
painted a barn red.
Subject Verb
He
NP
Adjective
seems happy.
Subject Verb
Does
he
DO
Subject
“Modifier”
seem happy?
Verb
“Modifier”
SIMILAR PATTERN IS NOT OK
*I
Subject
He
saw
is
Verb
Subject Verb
*Does he
DO
Subject
a barn red.
NP
Adjective
smiling.
“Modifier”
be
smiling?
Verb
“Modifier”
The video on child language (course reader, page 81--see also top of page 91) and Pinker
(page 276) claim that children, during acquisition, would never make errors of the type
shown by the “starred” examples (marked by the *). What is the basis of their claim?
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
7
a. Children are born with innate rules that they can use to form the sentences on the left,
but not the starred sentences on the right.
b. Children acquire language patterns by formulating rules that generate those patterns,
not by copying patterns through analogical reasoning.
c. Children would have heard the sentences on the left from adults, but they would not
have heard the starred sentences on the right.
d. Statistics: transcripts of child language show many sentences of the type on the left,
but not of the starred types on the right.
e. The sentences on the left would have a functional value in communication, whereas the
starred sentences on the right would not.
8. DO CHILDREN MAKE LINGUISTIC ERRORS?
Here are a couple of utterances by children from the video. (Click here to see a clip from
the video.)
“They drived to school.”
“What do you think
whatʼs in there?”
The video asks the question, "Are they really errors?" Clearly they do not conform to
adult English. WHY DOES THE VIDEO SUGGEST THAT THEY ARE NOT
"ERRORS"?
a. Though they do not conform to the rules of English, they do conform to the rules of
some other languages.
b. These sentences and words are probably repetitions of things that they have heard
other people (adults or children) say and are thus "correct" in the sense that they are
faithful to some "model", even though it is not standard English.
c. These sentences and words conform to mental rules that the child has formulated at the
current stage of acquisition. They are thus not "errors" in terms of the child's current
mental grammar.
d. Like all languages, English shows considerable dialect variability. When children
make these "errors", they are just speaking a non-standard variety of English.
e. These "errors" are just slips of the tongue. The children probably know better.
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
8
9. WHAT DO CHILDREN LISTEN FOR?
In the video that we saw in class, one experiment showed that a baby in the pre-speech
period could correctly identify who was washing whom? In another film on language
acquisition, Out of the Mouths of Babes, a 3.5 year old girl is asked to show, "The car is
bumped by the truck." She takes the car and bumps the truck with it. (Click here to see a
clip from the video.)
“Whereʼs Cookie Monster washing Big Bird.”
Could you show us, “The car is bumped by the truck.”
WHAT INFORMATION DO THESE CHILDREN SEEM TO BE RELYING ON AS
THEY MENTALLY ANALYZE THE LINGUISTIC INPUT?
a. The most common order for English sentences is SVO.
b. It is a linguistic universal that Subjects come before Objects. This fact must therefore
be part of the Language Instinct that children are born with.
c. The children are watching the adults for non-linguistic cues such as eye gaze, head or
hand movements, and the like.
d. Children select the most likely agent of the action based on likelihood of which event
would take place. For example, in the second example, the car toy is bigger than the truck
toy and thus is more likely to be the "bumper", and in the other video, Big Bird is more
likely to wash Cookie Monster than vice versa.
e. Children tend to be good guessers, but once in awhile they guess wrong, as in the case
of the little girl, who reversed who bumps who.
10. ACQUISITION OF A SECOND LANGUAGE BY ADULTS
Here are two selections from an interview in English with an adult native speaker of
Japanese, who was probably in his mid-30's at the time. (Click here to hear the two clips.)
INTERVIEWEE
In response to a question about whether zoos
perform a valuable service of preserving
IDIOMATIC ENGLISH VERSION
Linguistics 1, Web Quiz 6
endangered species even though a zoo is a sort of
"jail for animals".
Yeah, of course, I agree that today's
endangered species have to take care.
But why today some ... many species
wild animal are endangered? It's our
fault, so I don't know.
In response to a question about what drives US
relations with other countries
Oh for example some dictatorship
country even rooks like friendry, I don't
know really friendly country or not. So,
American foreign policy rooks ...
doesn't rook very oriented by friendly
country.
9
Yeah, of course I agree that today's
endangered species have to be taken care
of. But why, today, are some ... many
species of wild animals endangered? It's
our fault, so I don't know.
Oh, for example, some country that is a
dictatorship even looks like it is friendly,
(but) I don't know whether it is really a
friendly country or not. So American
foreign policy looks ... doesn't look to be
very oriented toward (truly) friendly
countries.
Some of the errors in this speaker's English are marked by various colors. One assumes
that these are interference errors, that is errors brought about because of differences
between Japanese and English.
WHICH ONE OF THE FOLLOWING SUGGESTIONS ABOUT DIFFERENCES
BETWEEN ENGLISH AND JAPANESE SEEMS LEAST LIKELY?
a. The phrase in blue suggests that if Japanese has a passive construction at all, it does not
use the pattern of the English passive. (The English passive makes the OBJECT of an
action into the SUBJECT and adds a helping verb "be", for example someone read
the book --> PASSIVE the book was read.)
b. The phrase in red suggests that Japanese does NOT add a plural-marking affix to
nouns, even when they are understood as plural.
c. The phrase in red suggests that the Japanese language lacks the ability to distinguish
"one" from "more than one".
d. The phrase in purple suggests that Japanese doesn't distinguish between r and l.
e. The phrase in purple suggests that Japanese either doesn't have a verb 'be' or uses 'be'
in ways that are different from the way it is used in English.