Chen, C.-H., & She, H.-C. (2012). The Impact of Recurrent On-line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation on Students' Argumentation and Conceptual Change. Educational Technology & Society, 15 (1), 197–210. The Impact of Recurrent On-line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation on Students’ Argumentation and Conceptual Change Chien-Hsien Chen and Hsiao-Ching She Institute of Education, National Chiao-Tung University, Taiwan // [email protected] // [email protected] ABSTRACT This study reports the impact of Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning on 8th grade students’ scientific argumentation ability and conceptual change involving physical science. The control group (N=76) were recruited to receive conventional instruction whereas the experimental group (N=74) received the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation program for about 25 physical science class periods of 45 minutes each, which is about one third of the physical science class periods in a semester. Results indicate that the experimental group significantly outperformed the conventional group on the post-Physical Science Conception Test and the Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test. The quantity and quality of scientific arguments that the experimental group’s students generated, in a series of pre- and post-argumentation questions, all improved across the seven topics. In addition, the experimental group’s students successfully constructed more correct conceptions from pre- to post-argumentation questions across the seven topics. This clearly demonstrates that the experimental group’s students’ argumentation ability and conceptual change were both facilitated through receiving the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation program. Keywords Scientific Argumentation, Conceptual Change, On-line Synchronous argumentation, Physical science, Recurrent online learning, 8th grade students Introduction The need to educate our students and citizens about how we know and why we believe in the scientific worldview has become increasingly important. It is no longer sufficient to merely deal with what we know (Driver et al., 1996; Millar & Osborne, 1998). Osborne et al. (2004) further pointed out that such a shift requires a new focus on how evidence is used in science for the construction of explanations, that is, on the arguments that form the links between data and the theories that science has constructed. More specifically, the construction of arguments is a core discursive activity of science (Osborne et al., 2004). Scientific discursive practices such as assessing alternatives, weighing evidence, interpreting texts, and evaluating the potential validity of scientific claims are all seen as essential components in constructing scientific arguments, which also are fundamental in the progress of scientific knowledge (Latour, 1987). In short, argumentation is a collective cognitive development process which involves using evidence to support or refute a particular claim, coordinating the claims with evidence to make an argument, forming a judgment of scientific knowledge claims, and identifying reliable and consensual scientific knowledge. Several studies show that educational support of argumentation may foster students’ argumentation ability (JiménezAleixandre, & Rodriguez, 2000; Kuhn et al., 1997) and improve scientific knowledge (Zohar & Nemet, 2002). Most of the argumentation studies were conducted in the classroom for a very short period of time and were not able to improve students’ argumentation efficiently. The authors feel that it is necessary to provide students with the opportunity to argue effectively with recurrent opportunities and for a longer period of time in order to improve the quality of their argumentation. Osborne et al. (2004) suggested that developing argumentation in a scientific context is far more difficult than enabling argumentation in a socio-scientific context. Students generally considered physical science to be difficult to learn. Though it is rather difficult to improve argumentation in a science context, we believe it is important to provide students with the recurrent opportunity to learn and use argumentation in the context of physical science. The constructivist view of learning highlights the significance of the individual learner’s prior knowledge in subsequent learning (Driver & Bell, 1986). Cobern (1993) shares the similar idea of learning as a process wherein an individual is actively involved in linking new ideas with current ideas and experience. Learning by construction and involving changes is similar to the idea that the construction of new knowledge takes place at a construction site consisting of existing structures built on a foundation (Cobern, 1993). The notion of conceptual change involves the restructuring of relationships among existing concepts and often requires the acquisition of entirely new concepts. The students who learn something are the ones who understand a new idea, judge its truth value, judge its ISSN 1436-4522 (online) and 1176-3647 (print). © International Forum of Educational Technology & Society (IFETS). The authors and the forum jointly retain the copyright of the articles. Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than IFETS must be honoured. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from the editors at [email protected]. 197 consistency with other ideas, and are willing to change their minds to accept it. It is recognized that learning does not take place in a social vacuum (Driver, 1995). Driver (1995) indicated that whether or not an individual’s ideas are affirmed and shared by others in classroom exchanges affects how the knowledge construction process is shaped. The nature of argumentation has the potential to contribute to the collective development and judgment of scientific knowledge claims and the identification of reliable and consensual descriptions of nature (Kolsto & Ratcliffe, 2008, p.117). Much effort and many studies have focused on fostering students’ conceptual change from the constructivist viewpoint (She, 2004; She & Lee, 2008; She & Liao, 2010; Hewson & Hewson, 1988; Venville & Treagust, 1998). However, none of them have tried to include argumentation in fostering conceptual change. Obviously, argumentation has great potential for fostering students’ communication skills in order to interchange perspectives and meanings. Assessing alternatives, weighing evidence, interpreting texts, and evaluating the potential validity of scientific claims are all seen as essential components in constructing scientific arguments. But how can argumentation successfully foster students’ conceptual change? According to the previous conceptual change studies, the following major characteristics are important for successful conceptual change: (1) Creating dissonance, which can raise students’ awareness about their own conceptions and provide an opportunity for them to experience the dissonance and become further dissatisfied with their own conceptions (She, 2004; Posner et al., 1982). (2) Challenging students’ beliefs about science conceptions (She, 2004; Vosniadou & Brewer, 1987). (3) Providing plausible mental structures for students to reconstruct more scientific conceptions (She, 2004). (4) Actively engaging students in the process of conceptual change (Hewson & Hewson, 1983; She, 2004). (5) Actively involving students in group discussions to shape their knowledge construction process and changing conceptions (Driver, 1995; Venville & Treagust, 1998). Therefore, the ideas of successful conceptual change described above were taken into consideration during our design of argumentation activity in order to optimize scientific learning. Though there is a substantial body of research showing that instructors tend to adopt their conventional instruction into online courses, however, Scagnoli, et al. (2009) suggested that simply changing face-to-face courses to an online environment can’t confirm the same success. Additionally, consensus has not been reached regarding whether online learning is more effective than conventional instruction on students’ academic achievement. Many studies suggest no difference in academic achievement scores following on-line learning and conventional courses (Delfino & Persico, 2007; Russell, et al. 2009). Larson and Sung (2009) further demonstrated that there are no significant differences on exam scores when comparing online, blended and face-to-face instruction. On the other hand, the majority of articles reported that online learning is better than traditional learning with the focus on the perspectives of engagement or social situations, pedagogical characteristics, and satisfaction (Larson & Sung, 2009; Menchaca, 2008; Wuensch et al., 2009) instead of focusing on students’ learning outcome. One study demonstrated that, for a wellness course, the online learning group’s levels of achievement were significantly higher than those of the traditional face-to-face learning group (Lim et al., 2008). Kirtman (2009) reported a contrary result that students who received traditional instruction performed significantly better than the students who received online instruction, in both mid-term and final exams. Salcedo (2010) further demonstrated that students who received traditional instruction for foreign language classes performed better on three out of four assessments than did the students who received online instruction. As the consensus still remains unclear, we are interested in exploring whether or not students receiving the on-line scientific argumentation course perform better than the conventional group. From the point of view of science educators, we claim that it is very difficult to bring about conceptual change and argumentation unless the instructional design is based on well-developed conceptual change and argumentation theories and models (Yeh & She, 2010). Though a few studies have proposed their on-line argumentative learning environment for promoting students conceptual development and conceptual change (Ravenscroft, 2000, 2007), they lack empirical evidences to prove their effectiveness. Thus, this study attempts to explore whether or not students who received the On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning would outperform a conventionally educated group of students in their conceptual change and scientific argumentation. Sandoval and Reiser (2004) suggest that online learning environments can provide excellent support for students constructing their scientific explanations and knowledge negotiation process in argumentative writing. Synchronous communication can deliver a higher degree of elaboration and construction of arguments as students work on a common shared artifact (De Vries et al., 2002; Janssen et al., 2006). Our study specifically designed a synchronous argumentation Web-based learning environment to provide students with the opportunity to argue with their group in real time and to create a higher degree of elaboration and construction of arguments. 198 Constructing a good argument is not a simple task and we believe that guidance and support would help students to scaffold and build their sense of an effective argument. On-line argumentation provides the advantage of allowing students to see arguments and counterarguments on the screen, which supports them in refining their argumentation (Kirschner et al., 2003). Wray and Lewis (1997) have indicated that the use of “writing frames” would support the process of writing and provide vital clues as to what is needed. Osborne et al. (2004) indicated that stems provide students with prompts to construct their argument in a coherent manner and within a writing frame, which then can be used as a structure for producing a written argument. Therefore, we specifically programmed our learning environment to provide students with writing frames of five argumentation components to scaffold their arguments in science learning. The current study specifically designed On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning to provide a recurrent opportunity for middle school students to engage in argumentation for about one third of the physical science class periods in a semester. The strategy is to provide students with writing frames of five argumentation components to scaffold their arguments in the On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning. We believe that it is a promising direction, taking consideration of conceptual change aspects into the design of a series of pre-, experiment-related, and post-argumentation activities. Research Questions Three major research questions were examined in the study in order to measure the effectiveness of On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning. The first question explored whether On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning was more effective than conventional instruction in facilitating students’ conceptual change as well as scientific argumentation in physical science. Second, examine the quantity and quality of scientific arguments that experimental group’s students generated in a series of pre- and post-argumentation questions across a semester. Third, explore the nature and extent of conceptual change from pre- to postargumentation question that the experimental group’s students made across a semester. In addition, the relationship between scientific conceptual change and argumentation ability was examined. Designs and Characteristics of the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning is designed to provide recurrent argumentation opportunities for students learning physical science, replacing the regular physical science in middle school. Therefore, the five units of seven topics were chosen from the current middle school physical science mandatory content and standards. The current study reported the effects of implementing seven topics for physical science: chemical reaction (1 and 2), acid and base (1 and 2), oxidation and reduction, organic substances, and friction. Seven topics of physical science were used in this study. Each unit generally covers two or three main topics, for instance unit 1 on chemical reaction covers the influence of the contacting area on the rate of chemical reaction, and the influence of concentration on the rate of chemical reaction. Each topic is specifically designed a pre-argumentation question and an experiment-related argumentation question was focused on the core concepts of the preargumentation question that they argued (figure 1). Students were asked to provide reasons for the argument and went to an actual laboratory to carry out their experiments based upon their hypothesis and experimental design. The same post-argumentation question was given for students to argue again after finishing the laboratory work. Facilitate students’ conceptual change To facilitate students’ conceptual change, each topic is specifically designed to initiate a pre-argumentation question, followed by an experiment-related argumentation question, the activity of carrying out the experiment in the laboratory, and finally a post-argumentation question. Students would be exposed to different ideas which may be different from their own during the pre-argumentation and experiment-related argumentation question. After they carry out the experiment and receive the result from the experiment, dissonance is created and they build a plausible mental structure if the result is different from their prediction. The same post-argumentation question is given for students to argue again after finishing the laboratory work. Post-argumentation provides them an opportunity to reconstruct their mental structure according to the experiments they have visualized, arguing with peers, exchanging 199 conceptions, justifying their belief, and further modifying their original conceptions throughout the process. This process is intended to encourage students to reconstruct their scientific conceptions through a series of preargumentation, question-testing argumentation questions, laboratory activities, and post-argumentation. Facilitate students’ argumentation ability In order to promote students’ argumentation ability, the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning environment has tools specifically designed for students to use while they are participating in argumentation. In order to facilitate students’ ability to produce a good written argument, our interface specifically designed two layers of templates for them to use. The first layer provides the definition and choices of five components of argumentation: data, claim, warrant, backing, and rebuttal; the second layer provides three or four writing frames for each component of argumentation (figure 2). Figure 1. On-line pre-argumentation discourse and first layer of template 200 The writing frame is intended to provide guidance and support that will help students in constructing a good argument. The following stems were provided: “I think/believe…, because…; The reason why I agree with…argument, is because the evidence of……; I do not agree with…my reason is…” Students need to choose one of the components of argumentation first and then choose one from three or four writing frames that they feel appropriate to share their argument. The learning environment provides the advantage of real time argumentation, so students can receive prompt rebuttals to their arguments which can better retain their interest and thus make learning more effective. Figure 2. On-line post-argumentation discourses and second layer of template Method Participants and procedures A total of 150 eighth grade students, recruited from four classes of a middle school, participated in this study. Two classes of students (74) received the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning (experimental group) and the other two classes of students (76) received conventional instruction (control group). The experimental group’s students were further divided into 12 groups, with an average of six students assigned to 201 each group. The experimental group received the seven topics of the physical science Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning for one semester, with 25 class periods, each class lasting about 45 minutes. This was about one third of the physical science class periods over a semester. The teacher introduced the five components of argumentation to the students and used it in the classroom for one class before the students received the Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning and assessment. The conventional group students went through the same content of physical science in traditional instruction and traditional laboratory work. All students were administered the two-tier Physical Science Conception test (PSCT) and the Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test (PSDAT) before and one week after learning. In addition, the experimental group students’ on-line scientific argumentation process also was collected to determine the quality and quantity of students’ argumentation and scientific conceptions they held before and after learning from the OLSA. Instruments Physical Science Conception test (PSCT) The PSCT is a two-tier multiple choice diagnostic instrument that was developed to measure the degree of students’ conceptual change in physical science conceptions. The content validity was established by the same panel of six evaluators, ensuring that the items were properly constructed and relevant to the seven topics of physical science Web-learning materials that we developed. There are five items for each topic, and each item contains two tiers. In the first tier, students are required to choose the correct scientific concepts, while in the second tier they choose the correct reason for choosing these specific concepts. There are 35 items and each item has two tiers. Students need to answer both tiers of each question correctly in order to receive one point, so the highest possible score is 35. The Cronbach α of ADRT was 0.86 for the pre-test and 0.92 for the post-test. Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test (PSDAT) The PSDAT is a two-tier multiple choice diagnostic instrument that was developed to measure the degree of students’ argumentation ability involving physical science conceptions. There are five scenarios, covering five units of seven topics. Each scenario includes the contextual background and argumentation discourses. There are five questions under each scenario, for a total of 25 questions. Each question contains two tiers. The first tier of each question requires the student to identify a specific statement from the argumentation discourses at scenario as a correct data, claim, warrant, backing, or rebuttal, respectively, and justify why they chose that specific statement as a correct data, claim, warrant, backing or rebuttal. The content validity was established by the same panel of six evaluators, ensuring that the items were properly constructed and relevant to the five units of the OLSA physical science learning program. There are 25 items covering five units. Students need to answer both tiers correctly in order to receive one point, so the highest possible score is 25. The Cronbach α of PSDAT was 0.91 for the pre-test and 0.92 for the post-test. Qualitative Analysis of On-line scientific argumentation The qualitative data collected from students’ on-line scientific argumentation was analyzed from two perspectives. Each statement generated by an individual was classified into two different levels of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal, respectively. Data is considered to be non-argumentative statements. A level 1 claim is an argument consisting of a claim without any data or fact. A level 2 claim is an argument consisting of a claim with data or fact. A level 1 warrant is an argument consisting of a theory or principle without connection to the claim, or one which does not clearly describe the theory. A level 2 warrant is an argument consisting of a claim with a clearly described theory or principle. A level 1 backing is an argument only consisting of a backing without any connection to claim/warrant, or one which does not clearly describe the connection among them. A level 2 backing is an argument consisting of a claim with backing, and or with data or warrant. A level 1 rebuttal is an argument consisting of a weak rebuttal without clear explanation. A level 2 rebuttal is an argument consisting of a claim with a clearly identifiable rebuttal (Table 1). The cross-coder reliability is 0.91. 202 In addition, students’ on-line scientific argumentation discourses were analyzed from a conceptual change perspective. Each argumentation statement was determined to be correct, partially correct, or incorrect; and the comparison between the correctness of pre-argumentation and post-argumentation is further performed by the t test. The quality of conceptual change also was presented to see how students change their conceptions from pre- to postargumentation questions across seven topics. The cross coder reliability is 0.95. Components Claim Warrant Backing Rebuttal Table 1. Analytical Framework used for determining the quality of argumentation Levels Definition Examples An argument only consists with a The greater the concentration, the faster the Level 1 claim without any data or fact. reaction is. An argument consists of a claim with I saw that the greater the concentration of HCl, Level 2 data or fact. the faster the reaction with marble is. Thus I think that the greater the concentration, the faster the reaction is. An argument only consists with a The more molecules there are, the greater the Level 1 theory or principle without opportunity for collision. connection to the claim, or not clearly describes the theory. An argument consists of a claim with The greater the concentration is, the faster the Level 2 theory or principle. reaction is. It is because the more molecules there are, the greater the opportunity for collision. An argument only consists with a I agree with David’s idea, because I had a Level 1 backing without any connection to similar experience that producing oxygen claim/warrant, or not clearly describe experiment with high concentration of hydrogen the connection among them. peroxide. An argument consists of a claim with I support Ann’s idea, because I have done the Level 2 backing, and or with data or warrant. concentration experiment (HCl react with marble), which proves that the greater the concentration, the faster the reaction is. So there is greater intensity of the molecular collisions. An argument only consists of a weak I do not agree with Thomas’s idea, because that Level 1 rebuttal and without clearly some person who drink high concentration wine explanation. would not get drunk at all. An argument consists of a claim with I disagree with Jim’s idea that the lower the Level 2 a clearly identifiable rebuttal. concentration is, the faster the reaction is. The lower the concentration, the smaller the amount of molecules, thus the lower the opportunity for collision. Results ANCOVA analysis of the Physical Science Conception test (PSCT) The two-tier PSCT was developed to measure the degree of students’ conceptual change in physical science conceptions. One-factor ANCOVA was conducted to examine the effects of instructional approaches using postPSCT scores as the dependent measures, and students’ pre-PSCT scores as the covariate. The results of the onefactor ANCOVA: specifically, instructional approaches (F=4.86, p= 0.029) reach a statistically significant effect on the performance of post- PSCT. In summary, the OLSA group outperformed the traditional group on postperformance of Physical Science Conception test. Multivariate analysis of the Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test (PSDAT) One-factor ANCOVA was conducted to examine the effects of instructional approaches using post-PSDAT scores as the dependent measures, and students’ pre-PSDAT scores as the covariate. The results of the one-factor ANCOVA: 203 specifically, instructional approaches (F=7.28, p= 0.008) reach a statistically significant effect on the performance of post- and retention-PSDAT. In summary, the OLSA learning group outperformed the traditional group on postperformance of Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test. Multiple regression analysis This section examines the relationship between students’ degree of conceptual change and their scientific argumentation ability. Therefore, the stepwise regression method was used to explore whether the pre- PSDAT or pre-PSCT test would be most important for predicting the post-PSDAT scores. Results indicated that the best single predictor for post-PSDAT sores was the pre-PSCT, followed by pre-PSDAT scores. The standardized regression coefficient for pre-PSCT, and pre-PSDAT were 0.41 and 0.31. Together pre-PSDAT and pre-PSCT accounted for 38.0% of the variance in post-PSDAT scores. The Quantity and Quality of On-Line Scientific Argumentation The experimental group’s student on-line scientific argumentation learning process was analyzed in two aspects: nature and extent of argumentation ability and of conceptual change. The quality and quantity of students’ argumentation and conceptual change were presented in the following in order to manifest the nature and extent of experimental group’s on-line scientific argumentation process. Argumentation ability All argumentation questions were designed to require 10-15 minutes for students to argue. With an average of six students in a group, the mean frequency of arguments generated by each group in each question increased progressively from 7.38 to 18.77 arguments during the 10-15 minutes from topic 1 to 7 (Figure 3). 20 18 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 Unit1 Unit2 Unit3 Unit4 Unit5 Unit6 Unit7 Figure 3. Distribution of mean frequency of arguments generated by each groups’ students across seven units Repeated measures of ANOVA were used to examine any increases in mean frequency of arguments from topic 1 to topic 7. The mean frequency of arguments generated by each student in each question significantly increased from 204 1.17 to 3.04 from topic 1 to 7 (F=30.74, p<0.0001) (Table 2). Clearly, the group argumentation and individual students’ argumentation pattern are similar. The post-hoc comparisons indicated that the number of arguments generated by each student is statistically significantly greater when comparing later topics with earlier topics. This clearly demonstrates that students’ ability to generate arguments indeed increased from topic 1 to topic 7 across the semester. Table 2. Repeated measures of ANOVA of arguments generated by each student across seven topics F value of repeated measures M Topic 1 Topic 2 Topic 3 Topic 4 Topic 5 Topic 6 Topic 7 * p<.05, **p<.01, SD 1.17 2.20 2.68 2.05 1.91 2.38 3.04 *** p<.001; N=74. .61 2.20 2.05 1.16 1.05 1.22 1.38 ANOVA 30.74*** (p=.000) Post hoc comparisons 2>1*** 3>1***,3>4**,3>5** 4>1*** 5>1*** 6>1***,6>4*,6>5*** 7>1***,7>2**,7>4***,7>5***,7>6*** Each statement was categorized into two levels of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal arguments. With an average of six students in a group, the mean frequency of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal arguments generated by each group in each question from topic 1 to 7 ranged 2.29-13.3, 0.63-4.58, 0.75-4.27, and 0.18-1.77 (Figure 4). 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Claim 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Warrant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Backing Rebuttal Figure 4. Distribution of mean frequency of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal arguments generated by each groups’ students across seven units 205 Table 3 shows that the mean frequency of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal arguments generated by each student for each question from topic 1 to 7 ranged from 0.37-2.16, 0.10-0.97, 0.12-0.69, and 0.03-0.29. The increase of arguments was found to be statistically significant when comparing earlier topics with later topics through the use of repeated measure of ANOVA in all aspects, regardless of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal (F(claim)=32.69, p <0.0001; F(warrant)=29.95, p <0.0001; F(backing)=11.63, p <0.0001; F(rebuttal)=6.06, p <0.0001). The post-hoc comparisons indicated that the frequency of arguments is statistically significantly greater when comparing later topics with earlier topics, regardless of claim, warrant, backing, and rebuttal in general. Table 3. Multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) of Claim, Warrant, Backing, and Rebuttal Arguments Generated by Each Student across seven topics Level 1 Level 2 Total Units M SD M SD M SD F* Post hoc comparisons CLAIM 32.69*** Topic 1 .31 .33 .05 .13 .37 .40 Topic 2 .47 .74 .08 .19 .55 .75 2>1* Topic 3 3>1***,3>2***,3>4***,3>5***,3>6***,3>7*** 1.81 1.40 .35 .81 2.16 1.62 Topic 4 .98 4>1***,4>2***,4>5** 1.07 .85 .33 .48 1.41 Topic 5 .58 5>1***,5>2*** .86 .53 .23 .31 1.09 Topic 6 .82 6>1***,6>2***,6>5** 1.14 .76 .26 .29 1.40 Topic 7 .76 7>1***,7>2***,7>5*** 1.23 .75 .22 .24 1.44 WARRANT 29.95*** Topic 1 .31 1>3***,1>4*** .41 .30 .08 .14 .48 Topic 2 1.13 2>1***,2>3***,2>4***,2>5**,2>6*,2>7* .68 1.04 .30 .36 .97 Topic 3 .07 .21 .03 .11 .10 .27 Topic 4 .09 .27 .08 .22 .17 .32 Topic 5 .46 5>3***,5>4*** .25 .33 .25 .33 .50 Topic 6 .43 6>1*,6>3***,6>4***,6>5* .50 .40 .11 .18 .61 Topic 7 .42 7>1**,7>3***,7>4***,7>5* .47 .35 .15 .20 .62 *** BACKING 11.63 Topic 1 1>5***,1>6** .23 .33 .06 .11 .29 .34 Topic 2 2>1***,2>3***,2>4**,2>5***,2>6*** .49 .70 .14 .29 .64 .80 Topic 3 3>5**,3>6** .11 .33 .16 .36 .27 .45 Topic 4 4>5**,4>6** .11 .24 .18 .36 .28 .45 Topic 5 .02 .08 .10 .19 .12 .23 Topic 6 .05 .11 .09 .16 .14 .19 Topic 7 7>1***,7>3***,7>4***,7>5***,7>6*** .38 .39 .32 .41 .69 .70 *** REBUTTAL 6.06 Topic 1 .01 .06 .02 .06 .03 .08 Topic 2 .03 .15 .01 .06 .04 .18 Topic 3 .03 .13 .11 .44 .15 .52 Topic 4 .03 .11 .16 .32 .19 .36 4>1***,4>2*** Topic 5 .05 .11 .15 .26 .20 .32 5>1***,5>2** Topic 6 .10 .22 .13 .22 .23 .39 6>1***,6>2*** Topic 7 .18 .29 .11 .19 .29 .42 7>1***,7>2***,7>3***,7>5* * ** *** Note: p<0.1, p<0.01, p<0.001; N=74. Each statement generated by students was further categorized into two levels of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal arguments in order to reveal its quality. The mean frequency of two levels of arguments generated by each group in each question shows a growing pattern overall, regardless of levels of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal argument (Table 3). Repeated measures of ANOVA showed that an increase of level 1 arguments was found to be statistically significant when comparing earlier topics with later topics in all aspects, regardless of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal (F(claim)=53.50, p <0.0001; F(warrant)=23.88, p <0.0001; F(backing)=14.59, p <0.0001; F(rebuttal)=3.03, p <0.005). The increase of level 2 arguments was also found to be statistically significant when comparing earlier topics with 206 later topics in all aspects, regardless of claim, warrant, backing and rebuttal (F(claim)=9.38, p <0.0001; F(warrant)=11.26, p <0.0001; F(backing)=5.62, p <0.0001; F(rebuttal)=7.89, p <0.005). Conceptual Change The nature of each argument was judged and classified into three categories as correct, partially correct, and incorrect. The results show that the mean score of correct conceptions for each argument generated by each student increased from pre- to post- argumentation questions across all 7 topics, and 6 topics reached a statistically significant difference level (Ttopic 1=2.25, p=0.027; Ttopic 3=3.31, p=0.001; Ttopic 4=4.16, p=0.000; Ttopic 5=7.18, p=0.000; Ttopic 6=5.05, p=0.000; Ttopic 7=3.96, p=0.000). The mean score of partially correct conceptions decreased from pre- to post-argumentation for three topics and only one of the topics reached a statistically significant difference level (Ttopic 7=2.82, p=0.006), and slightly increased for three topics. The mean frequency of incorrect conceptions decreased from pre- to post-argumentation for about six topics, and only one of the topics reached a statistically significant difference level (Ttopic 5=5.15, p=0.000) (Table 4). Table 4. Analysis of the correctness of conceptions for each argument generated at pre- and post-argumentation question by each student across seven topics Pre-argumentation Post-argumentation Mean T Sig difference M SD M SD Topic 1 C .66 .66 .89 .72 .23 2.25* .027 PC .45 .50 .32 .40 -.13 -1.62 .110 IC .07 .18 .05 .20 -.02 -.62 .535 Topic 2 C .74 .74 .97 .99 .23 1.71 .091 PC .64 .75 .81 .82 .18 1.42 .160 IC .04 .20 .01 .12 -.03 -1.00 .321 Topic 3 C 1.50 1.49 2.72 3.07 1.22 3.31** .001 PC .36 .73 .45 1.17 .08 .47 .641 IC .20 .55 .20 .60 .00 .00 1.000 Topic 4 C 1.32 1.27 2.00 1.39 .68 4.16*** .000 PC .58 .91 .57 .76 -.01 -.12 .908 IC .12 .44 .05 .23 -.07 -.140 .167 Topic 5 C .79 .80 1.56 1.09 .77 7.18*** .000 PC .47 .60 .33 .46 -.14 -1.70 .094 IC .69 .81 .19 .40 -.50 -5.15*** .000 Topic 6 C 1.49 .93 2.28 1.52 .79 5.05*** .000 PC .36 .47 .37 .48 .01 .21 .838 IC .12 .23 .08 .22 -.04 -1.39 .169 Topic 7 C 1.59 .96 2.17 1.23 .57 3.96*** .000 PC .88 .60 1.13 .85 -.77 2.82* .006 IC .23 .29 .16 .26 -.07 -1.61 .111 Note: C: correct conceptions; PC: partial correct conceptions; IC: incorrect conceptions; N=74. Conclusions and Discussions This study reports a Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning program that was developed based on the conceptual change and scientific argumentation theories in order to promote 8th grade students’ conceptual change and scientific argumentation ability in a physical science context. This study is a major step from 207 previous Web-based instructional learning programs, as it brings well-developed conceptual change and scientific argumentation pedagogy theories and models into Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning. In addition, our learning environment contains two layers of template to provide students’ guidance and support in constructing a good argument. It also provides the advantage of real time argumentation, so students can receive prompt rebuttals to their arguments. This helps to make learning more effective. The results of this study are quite positive as they demonstrate that On-Line Argumentation learning is far more effective than conventional instruction for promoting students’ conceptual change and scientific argumentation. Our results add positive documentation to the current research that students who receive a Web-based learning course can perform better than a conventional group’s students in their physical science concept construction. In addition, we argue that most computer-assisted learning studies cannot effectively change students’ alternative conceptions or science learning because their instructional materials are not developed based on solid theories or models of conceptual change or science learning(She & Lee, 2008; Liao & She, 2009; Yeh & She, 2010). This study supports the idea that including argumentation and conceptual change theories into the design of Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning are important for success. In addition, the results of the on-line scientific argumentation process indicated that the amount of arguments generated by student is significantly greater when comparing later topics with earlier topics, regardless of level 1 or level 2. It clearly demonstrates that students’ ability to generate arguments indeed increased from topic 1 to topic 7 across the semester. Moreover, the mean frequency of arguments increased significantly from earlier topics to later topics, regardless of claim, warrant, backing, and rebuttal. These results demonstrated that our design indeed improves the quantity and quality of argumentation in the physical science context, regardless of the data collected from tests or on-line scientific argumentation process. Our results demonstrate that students’ argumentation significantly improves across a semester-long intervention of argumentation in a physical science context. The breakthrough we made fully supports Osborne et al.’s suggestions that a recurrent opportunity for students to be involved in argumentation may make difference (Osborne et al., 2004). Our results also confirmed that constructing a good argument is not a simple task and that students need guidance and support to help them scaffold and build their sense of an effective argument. With the supports of writing frames, which indeed cultivate their ability to generate claim, warrant and backing arguments fairly quick except rebuttal. This clearly supports the claim that the on-line synchronous argumentation learning environment, with the support of writing frames, indeed speeds up students’ ability to generate better and higher level arguments within very short period and continuously grow till the end. Moreover, our data found that students’ ability to generate argumentation is not quite stable across a semester. It is rather difficult to generate rebuttal arguments and takes a much longer period to cultivate compared to the claim and warrant arguments. Our data indicated that the quality and quantity of claim and warrant arguments become stable after topic 5, and rebuttal arguments increased gradually all the way from topic 1 through topic 7. It indicated the need to provide students with recurrent opportunities to use argumentation in science in order to stabilize their ability of using argumentation and increase their level of argumentation. The mean frequency of correct conceptions for arguments generated by each student increased from pre- to postargumentation questions across all 7 topics, and 6 topics reached a statistically significant difference level. The finding is quite positive and promising with regards to changing students’ alternative conceptions from both the assessment and on-line argumentation process. It is clear that the success in conceptual change is due to the design of Recurrent On-Line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation learning, which organizes conceptual change into a series of pre-, experiment-related, and post-argumentation activities. Our results demonstrate that our design of taking conceptual change ideas is critical for successfully changing students’ conceptions through a series of argumentation activities, specifically the ideas of creating dissonance, which provide an opportunity for them to experience the dissonance and become further dissatisfied with their prior conceptions (She, 2004; Hewson & Hewson, 1988;); providing plausible mental structures for students to reconstruct more scientific conceptions (She, 2004); and actively engaging students in the group discussions to shape their knowledge construction process and changing conceptions (Driver, 1995; Venville & Treagust, 1998). Finally, the regression results show that the best single predictor for post-PSDAT sores was the pre-PSCT, followed by pre-PSDAT scores. It demonstrated that constructing arguments of quality will be restricted and hampered 208 without the sources of specific knowledge or relevant scientific evidence. It implies that students who hold less alternative conceptions are more likely to perform better in the post-Physical Science Dependent Argumentation Test. Acknowledgements This research is based in work supported and funded by the National Science Council (NSC 95-2522-S-009-001MY3), Taiwan, R.O.C. References Cobern, W. W. (1993). Contextual constructivism: The impact of culture on the learning and teaching of science. In K. Tobin (Ed.), The practice of constructivism in science education (pp. 51-69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Delfino, M., & Persico, D. (2007). Online or face-to-face? Experimenting with different techniques in teacher training. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 23(5), 351-365. De Vries, E., Lund, K., & Baker, M. (2002). Computer-mediated epistemic dialogue: Explanation and argumentation as vehicles for understanding scientific notions. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11(1), 63-103. Driver, R. (1995). Constructivist approaches to science learning. In L. P. Steffe & J. Gale (Eds.), Constructivism in education (pp. 385-400). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Driver, R., & Bell, B. (1986). Students thinking and the learning of science: A constructivist view. School Science Review, 67(6), 443-456. Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., & Scott, P. (1996). Young people’s images of science. Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press. Hewson, M., & Hewson, P. W. (1983). Effect of instruction using students' prior knowledge and conceptual change strategies on science learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(8), 731-743. Hewson, P. W., & Hewson, M. G. (1988). An appropriate conception of teaching science: A view from studies of science learning. Science Education, 72(5), 597-614. Janssen, J., Erkens, G., & Kanselaar, G. (2006). Visualization participation to facilitate argumentation. Paper presented at the 7th International Conference of the Learning Sciences June/July, Bloomington, IN. Jiménez-Aleixandre, M. P., & Rodriguez, A. B. (2000). Doing the lesson or doing science: argument in high school genetics. Science Education, 84(6), 757-792. Kirschner, P. A., Buckingham Shum, S. J., & Carr, C. S. (2003). Visualizing Argumentatio: Software Tools for Collaborative and Educational Sense-Making. London: Springer Verlag. Kirtman, L. (2009). Online versus in-class courses: An examination of differences in learning outcomes. Issues in Teacher Education, 18(2), 103-116. Kolsto, S. D., & Ratcliffe, M. (2008). Social aspects of argumentation. In S. Erduran & M.P. Jiménez-Aleixandre (Eds.), Argumentation in science education: perspectives from classroom-based research (pp.114-133). Berlin, Germany: Springer. Kuhn, D., Shaw, V., & Felton, M. (1997). Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentative reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 15(3), 287-315. Larson, D. K., & Sung, C. H. (2009) Comparing Student Performance: Online versus Blended versus Face-to-Face. Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, 13(1), 31-42. Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: how to follow scientists and engineers through society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Liao, Y.W. & She, H.C. (2009). Enhancing Eight Grade Students’ Scientific Conceptual Change and Scientific Reasoning through a Web-based Learning Program. Educational Technology & Society, 12 (4), 228-240. Lim, J., Kim, M., Chen, S. S., & Ryder, C. E. (2008). An empirical investigation of student achievement and satisfaction in different learning environments. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 35(2), 113-119. Menchaca, M. (2008). Learner and instructor identified success factors in distance education. Distance Education, 29(3), 231-252. 209 Millar, R., & Osborne, J.F. (1998). Beyond 2000: Science Education for the Future. London: King's College London. Osborne, J., Erduran, S., & Simon, S. (2004). Enhancing the quality of argumentation in school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(10), 994-1020. Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W., & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66(2), 211-227. Ravenscroft, A. (2000). Designing Argumentation for Conceptual Development. Computers & Education, 34(3-4), 241-255. Ravenscroft, A. (2007). Promoting Thinking and Conceptual Change with Digital Dialogue Games. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning (JCAL), 23(6), 453-465. Russell, M., Carey, R., Kleiman, G., & Venable, J. D. (2009). Face-to-face and Online Professional Development for Mathematics Teachers: A Comparative Study. Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, 13(2), 71-87. Salcedo, C. S. (2010). Comparative Analysis Of Learning Outcomes In Face-To-Face Foreign Language Classes Vs. Language Lab And Online. Journal of College Teaching & Learning, 7(2), 43-54. Sandoval, W. A. & Reiser, B. (2004). Explanation-Driven Inquiry: Integrating Conceptual and Epistemic Scaffolds for Scientific Inquiry. Science Education, 88(3), 345-372. Scagnoli, N. I., Buki, L. P., & Johnson, S. D. (2009). The influence of online teaching on face-to-face teaching practices. Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, 13(2), 115-128. She, H.C. (2004). Fostering “Radical” conceptual change through Dual Situated Learning Model. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(2), 142-164. She, H.C. & Lee, C.Q. (2008). SCCR Digital Learning System for Scientific Conceptual change and Scientific Reasoning. Computers & Education, 51(2), 724-742. She, H.C. & Liao, Y.W. (2010). Bridging Scientific Reasoning and Conceptual Change through Adaptive Web-based Learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47(1), 91-119. Venville, G., & Treagust, D. F. (1998). Exploring conceptual change in genetics using a multidimensional interpretive framework. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35(9), 1031-1055. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1987). Theories of knowledge restructuring in development. Review of Educational Research, 57(1), 51-67. Wray, D., & Lewis, M. (1997). Extending literacy: Children reading and writing non-fiction. London: Routledge. Wuensch, K. W., Aziz, S., Ozan, E., Kishore, M., & Tabrizi, M. H. N. (2009). Technology and pedagogy: The association between students’ perceptions of the quality of online courses and the technologies employed. Journal of Online Learning and Teaching, 5(2), 253-262. Yeh, K.H. & She, H.C. (2010). On-line Synchronous Scientific Argumentation Learning: Nurturing Students’ Argumentation Ability and Conceptual Change in Science Context. Computers & Education, 55(2), 586-602. Zohar, A., & Nemet, F. (2002). Fostering students' knowledge and argumentation skills through dilemmas in human genetics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(1), 35-62. 210
© Copyright 2024