Young Children with Developmental Disabilities and Their Families: Needs, Policies, and Services During the past rwo decades we have witnessed an increase in the proportion of young children who are identified as having developmental disabilities and related special needs. Current figures indicate that approximatelY' 13% of all children, and 5% of preschool-aged children, receive special educa tion services (U.S. Department of Education, 1990; 2001). The largest increase in service provi sion over the past decade has occurred in the birth-to-kindergarten popularion (National Center for Educarion Statistics, 200 l). The in crease is attribucable to several facrors. Firsr, al though more children wirh very low birch weighr are surviving due to medical rechnological ad vances, they often experience a range of complica tions, including hypoxic (inadequate oxygen) ischemic encephalopathy (inadequate blood sup ply) (Shonkoff & Marshall, 2000). Such depriva tion can result in neuropathology and eventual developmental delays or disabilities (Hack, Wright, Shankaran, & Tyson, 1995). Stamon Chapman, Chapman, and SCOtt (2001), for exam ple, reported that the rare of learning problems among boys born with very low birth weight is 2.4 times greater than that among boys born full term. Second, children are being identified with po tential disabilities, such as learning disabilities, at increasingly younger ages (Lyon, L996). This may be a result of early screening programs at or befote school entry (e.g., Child Find), which have led to identifying children ar a young age with sus pected developmental problems (Meisels, 1991). Further, our understanding of early indicators of developmental difficulties has grown, and chil dren with certain disorders, such as autism, are now diagnosed at younger ages (e.g., Baron Cohen et aL, 1996; Frith, 2003). Finally, rhe growth of early imervention programs, as a result of federal legislation (Parr C of rhe Individuals with Disabilities Education Acc) (IDEA), has made such services more visible and accessible (Kochanek & Buka, 1998). With increasing evi dence of the value of early assistance (Guralnick, 1997; Shonkoff & Hauser-Cram, 1987), pediatri cians and other health care providers are replacing former practices that involved seeing if children "grow out of" their delayed or unusual develop ment with new practices that incorporate referrals to early intervention services. Healrh care providers serve a central role in identifying children who have a developmental 287 288 CHAPTER I4: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILlTrES AND THEIR FAMILIES •• disability or delay. In the National Institute of Child rt'ealth and Development (N(CHD) Study of Early Child Care, La Paro, Olsen, and Pianta (2002) found that the majority of vety young chil dren with special needs were identified by a med ical professional. Increasingly, physicians and other health care professionals have become in volved in the screening and referral processes for early intervention services (Soloman, 1995), yet many health care providers report they have a lack of familiarity with federal laws and the early in tervention system (Soloman, Clougherty, Shaffer, Hofkosh, & Edwards, 1994). The purpose of this chapter is to provide an overview of policies re lated to young children with developmental dis abilities and a review of research on the needs of their families and the effectiveness of early inter vention services. Federal Policy About Young Children with Special Needs Enormous policy changes related to services for young children with disabilities and their families have occurred over the past few decades in the United Srates. Indeed, as a nation we have moved from an implicit policy of institutionalizing chil dren with special needs, especially children with Down syndrome and other forms of intellectual disabilities, ro an explicit one of supporting fami lies as they raise children ar horne (Meisels & Shonkoff, 2000). Legislation developed over the past three decades has required publicly sup ported services be provided to younger and younger children. not require states ro provide services for children under school age, it did endorse the importance of such services. Over 10 years later, in 1986, Congress enacted Public Law 99-457, which en couraged (but did not mandate) states to provide services for infants and toddlers with disabilities and also strengthened incentives for states to pro vide services for preschoolers with disabilities. This was soon followed by legislation in 1988 (P.L. 102-119, the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act) (IDEA), which required states to both 1) develop a system of early intervention services for children from 0 to 3 years old (referred to as Part C) and 2) provide free and appropriate public education and related services to children with disabilities beginning at age 3 (referred to as Part B). Although the public schools were to be responsible for services for children of preschool age, each state had discretion as to the state agency responsible for administering and imple menting the system of early intervention (EI) services. Therefore. the lead agency for El varies by state. About one-third of the states initially se lected education as the lead agency, whereas slightly fewer chose public health, and fewer still selected mental health or human services (Garwood. Fewell, & Neisworrh, 1988). Current trends indicate that more departments of health and fewer departments of education are taking the lead (Meisels & Shonkoff. 2000). The legislation requires thar regardless of lead agency selected, the EI system should emerge from the coordina tion of health and education agencies at the state and local leveL Such collaboration between the medical and educational communities is a unique fearure of the early intervention system. Legislation (n' the 1970s Congress passed landmark legisla tion for children with disabilities, Public Law 94 142 (the Education for All Handicapped Children Acr of 1975), which established the right ro a free and appropriate public education for school-aged children with disabilities. Although the law did Eligibility One critical issue in providing El services is in de rermining who is eligible to receive them. The law states that such services should be provided (0 children wbo are experiencing developmental de lays and to tbose who have a diagnosed condition The Needs of Parents of Young Children with Developmental Disabilities that results in a high probability of subsequent delays. Stares.have the option of also providing services to children who are "at risk of having sub stantial delays if early intervention services are not provided" (20 U.S.c. # 1432). States have diffi culty determining which children are "at risk" and vary substantially in the extent to which ser vices are provided to these children (Hebbeler et aI., 200 l). A recent survey indicates that 58% of children enter EI due to developmental delay, 29% because of a diagnosed condition (e.g., Down's syndrome), and 13% because they are "at risk" for developing delays (Hebbeler et al., 2001). The most common reasons for receipt of EI services are listed in Table 14.1, and, as indicated in that table, most children who exhibit develop mental delays do so because of speech or commu nication problems. An imporrant requirement of IDEA is the de velopment of an individualized education plan (IEP) for children aged 3 or older and of an indi vidualized family service plan (IFSP) for children in early intervention. Like the IEP, the IFSP de lineates the needs and goals for the child, but un like the IE P, the IFSP also specifies family strengths and needs. The importance of rhe fam ily as the primary niche in which young children Table I 289 are nurtured and learn is dearly recognized in the legislative requirements relating to the IFSP. Although some concern has been expressed about the construction of the IFSP as a potential intru sion into family life (Krauss, 1990), the family focus inherent in the legislation reflects contem porary perspectives about the importance of the family in enhancing the optimal development of young children with disabilities. The Needs of Parents of Young Children with Developmental Disabilities All families need to adjust to the birth of a child, but adjustments can be more pronounced when the infant experiences health or developmental problems. Much has been written about the grief and "chronic sorrow" that parents experience in learning that their infant has a developmental dis ability (e.g., Solnit & Stark, 1961). It is now widely recognized, however, that individuals vary considerably in their responses to this event, and that expectations that all parents experience "chronic sorrow" may be misguided. Some variation in parental responses relates to parents' Common Reasons for Intervention Servicesa Reason Percentage b Speech/communication impairmem or delay Prenatal/perinatal abnormaliries (e.g., low birch weight) MotOr impaitment or delay Delayed development (global) Cognitive disorders (e.g., Down syndrome) Intellectual/cognitive impairment or delay Central nervous system disorders (e.g., cerebral palsy) Social environment fisk factors Social/behavioral irrtpairmem or delay Sensory impairment (e.g., vision, hearing impairmem) 41 19 ·Soum.' Nati"na/ Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (US. Department of EdJiCation, 2000), bpercentages ,um to more than 100 because children rnay have more than Ontl r!!Elion for reaipt of ,ervices. L7 L2 9 7 7 4 4 3 290 CHAPTER I4: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES II' I II I~ sociocultural beliefs about the etiology of the dis ailility (e.g., the child is a gift to parems who can master the challenge, or the child's disability is a punishmem for parems' past behaviors) (Garcia Coil & Magnuson, 2000). For most parents, the knowledge that their infam may exhibit unusual or delayed development requires an adjustment, but the assumption that all parents will endure "chronic sorrow" lacks empirical evidence. In the 1960s and 1970s, a stage theory was proposed to health professionals as a way to pre dict parems' reactions to the birth or diagnosis of a child with disabilities (Blacher, 1984). The three proposed stages followed those reponed for individuals coping with the illness or death of a dose family member (Kubler-Ross, 1997). In the first stage, parents experience disbelief and "shop" for physicians and treatmems. The second stage is characterized by guilt, anger, and disappoint ment. The third stage occurs when parents reori em themselves roward adjustment and acceptance of their child and take on an advocacy role. Although this stage theory may be appealing, the empirical evidence for rhese stages has been found to be weak (Blacher, 1984). Therefore, research has turned away from delineating stages toward understanding parents' adaptive functioning. Studies of Maternal Behaz.:ior Much research has been conducted on' caregivers, especially mothers, of young children with dis abilities. Many studies on the mother-child dyad have been guided by the transactional model (Sameroff & Chandler, 1975), which emphasizes the bi-directional imeractions between a mother and a child. This theoretical model does not as sume thar influences occur only from mother to child bur instead focuses on rhe responses that each has to the other in complex changing pat terns .•Barnard and colleagues (1989) refer co these imeractive patcerns as a "mutually adaptive dance." Investigations of the mother-child dyad of typically developing children indicate that te sponsive reactions by mothers rhat are contingent on children's behaviors promote positive cogni tive and social-emotional development in chil dren (Osofsky & Thompson, 2000). In comrast, mothers who repeatedly ignore children's re sponses or are highly controlling and imrusive imo children's activities reduce children's oppor tunities for self-efficacy (Heckhausen, 1993). Research on the mocher-child dyad when the child has a developmemal disability demonstrates that maternal contingent responsiveness during children's early years is an imporcant predictor of development of positive cognitive and communi cation skills ovet time (Hauser-Cram, Warfield, Shonkoff, & Krauss, 2001). Such productive mother-child interaction appears to be more challenging in dyads where the child has a dis ability than in other dyads, however. Children with certain disabilities, such as those with Down's syndrome, may provide fewer, more de layed, or less appropriate signals to parents dur ing interactions (e.g., Beeghly, Perry, & Cicchetti, 1989; Landry & Chapieski, 1990). Children with autism and related disabilities may rarely make eye contact with the mocher or caregiver (Hoppes & Harris, 1990), which results in reduced oppor tunities for mutually responsive interaction and joint attention. Thus, the child with a develop mental disability may display fewer or unusual cues, making ir difficult tor a caregiver to respond contingent to the child's actions. Possibly as a result of children's unusual or un expected cues, mothers of children with disabili· ties, in comparison to other mothers, appear to be more directive to children with disabilities (Marfo, 1990). For example, Mahoney, Fors, and Wood (1990) found that during free play activi· ties, mochers of children with Down syndrome, unlike morhers of typically developing children, directed rheir child's attention away from the ob jects [he child was using and toward more chal lenging casks, Crawley and Spiker (983) found that parents were more directive of children with Down syndrome who showed less interest in play and initiated fewer imeranions with objectS. The Needs of IJarents of Young Children with Developmental Disabilities ~uch directiveness could result in either pro • motive or deleterious outcomes for children. Directiveness could diminish children's attempts at self-initiation as has been found in research on dyads where the child is developing typically (Lepper, 1981). Alternatively, directiveness could assist children in their interactions by providing necessary scaffolding (i.e., incremental support and guidance as the child works on a task), for ex ample, by making materials easier for the child ro reach or asking questions that will aid in problem solution. Tannock (1988) found that maternal di rectives assisted children with Down syndrome in more fully participating in the interaction. Roach, Barratt, Miller, and Leavitt (1998) re ported that mothers of children with Down syn drome were both more directive and more supportive than were other mothers of both mental-age-matched and chronological-age matched typically developing children. They found that the combination of maternal directive ness and suppOrt elicited more object play and vo calizations by the children with disabilities. Thus, it appears that although mothers tend to interact using a more directive style with young children with disabilities, benefits accrue co rhose children when that style is complemented by maternal supporr of children's actions. Parenting Stress Maternal-child interacdon occurs within a family system that may be functioning well or poorly (Minuchin, 1988). If individuals experience high levels of sttess, their parental functioning is di minished. According ro tesearchers who focus on the family system (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983), the adaptation of the family to the birth Ot diag nosis of a child with disabilities can be explained by several facrors, including the meaning ascribed to the disability, and the internal and external te Sources of the family. Of the many factors thar comprise parental well-being, parenting stress appears to be the one 291 most frequently studied in relation to parenting a child with a disability. Although many have claimed that parents of infants with disabilities have exceedingly high levels of stress, empirical work indicates otherwise. Studies indicate that, on average, parents report normative stress during their child's infant and tOddler years (Shonkoff, Hauser-Cram, Krauss, & Upshur, 1992). In creasing stress levels occur during early childhood (Innocenti, Huh, & Boyce, 1992), with high stress levels during the middle childhood years (Hauser Cram et aI., 200l; Orr, Cameron, Dobson, & Day, 1993). These findings are based on families who have received early intervention services, however. It is possible that such services provide sufficient sup port CO parents to temper the levels of stress par ents feel during the infanr and toddler years. When the family support services of EI are with drawn and children make the transition from EI to preschool and school services, parental stress levels increase. The child-focused services of rhe school years may offer some families respite from intense involvement in services and the concomi tant demands made by such involvement bur may also lack the means for providing support for the family system as a whole. Mothers and fathers evince different patterns of parenting stress. Although mothers, in compari son co fathers, often provide more caregiving funcrions for children during the infant and tod dler years, fathers in general report more stress at that time (Hauser-Cram et a!., 200l). Fathers' in creasing levels of stress appeat co be moderated by problem-focused coping skills. Fathers who ap proach their srressful problems by selecting problem-solving strategies report less stress over time than fathers who lack such strategies. The stress levels of mothers, on the other hand, are moderated by the construction of satisfying social support netwotks (Hauser-Ctam er aI., 200 L). Mothers who find their support networks, what ever the size, co be helpful show less increase in stress. These different patterns of stress and its 292 CHAPTER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILlTIES AND THEIR FAMILlES ." moderators fot mothers and fathers suggest that different types of assistance would be valuable to different family members. The Role of Fathers We have little information about the service needs of fathers, however, because fathers have been rel atively neglected in studies of children with dis abilities (Lamb & Billings, 1927). Indeed, many studies have used the mother asa spokesperson for the family or viewed fathers only as adjuncts to mothers. In summarizing studies on fathers of children with disabilities, Hon;tby (1995) con cludes that they paint "a negative picture" (p. 105). For example, Wikler, Wasow, and Hatfield (1981) suggest that fathers are more af fected (than mothers) by the physical aspects of a child's disability. Schillinge, Schinke, and Kirkham (1985) contend that fathers have more difficulty accepting their sons wirh disabilities than accepting their daughters. Many of the prior studies, however, are based on autobiographical accounts and clinical impres sions, and most are dated in terms of contempo rary family arrangements and roles. Because many mothers of young children with disabilities are now in the workforce (Landis, 1992), the roles of fathers in families have changed. Therefore, we need carefully constructed research on fathers' perspectives in today's family. A few investigations have focused on rhe posi tive changes that have occurred in individuals' lives as a result of parenting a child wirh a dis ability. Abbot and Meredith (1986) interviewed parents of children with mental retardation and found thar 88% reporred positive ourcomes, such as having greater compassion and developing stronger families. Based on survey research, Scorgie and Sobsey (2000) found that parents of children with disabilities reported personal gtowth, improved relations with others, and changes in their philosophical or spiritual values as a result of their parenting experiences. Such studies indicate the complex emotions associated with parenting a child with a disability, which have yet to be fully investigated. As the core of the family system, parents set the emotional tone for the family and determine the opportunities available to children. Most families are composed of other members as well, and re search attention is increasingly focused on the en during importance of sibling relationships. Siblings of Children with Developmental Disabilities Siblings share a powerful and complex emotional bond that is often the most abiding of familial re lationships (Seligman, 1999). Early interactions between siblings provide a context for the devel opment of social competencies and a significant influence on emotional, behavioral, and cognitive development (Dunn, 1999). When one sibling has a disability, the relationship may take on ad ditional complexity. For the child with a disabil ity, who may experience limited peer interaCtion and few friendships (Gresham & MacMillan, 1997), the sibling relationship may provide the primary context for positive socialization experi ences with peers. Furthermore, the nondisabled sibling may assume the role of caretaker and com panion in adulthood (Seltzer, Greenberg, Krauss, & Gordon, 1997; Seltzer & Krauss, 2001). Clearly, a child's disability has an impacr on all members of the family, including siblings. Just as early studies of parents of children with disabilities focused on negative outcomes, early investigations of siblings also assumed maladjust ment. Traditionally, investigarors hypothesized that the presence of a child with a disability is a source of developmental risk for other children in the family (e.g., Farber, 1959). Much early work indicated thar sisters of children with develop mental disabilities often assumed much greater caregiving and household responsibility than their peers. This disproportionate responsibility Siblings of Children with Developmental Disabilities was considered (0 be related (0 increased levels of d(j){Jression and anger (Farber, 1959), a higher in cidence of anrisocial behavior (Gath, 1973), and decreased coping effectiveness (Grossman, 1972). Researchers now suggest that these early studies may noc reveal an accurate picture. For example, these studies were conducted be fore the passing of IDEA, when few, if any, special services or SUpPo[(S were available (0 families of young children with disabilities. The presence of a child with a disability may well have been a source of stress for the enrire family, which had to face the challenges of raising the child with little or no support from the community. Thus, these early studies may reflect an anachronistic under standing of the meaning of disability (Lamorey, 1999). Furthermore, several methodological criticisms of this work exist. First, studies were largely tet tospective reports based on patienrs referred for clinical assistance (Cuskelly, 1999). Thus, they failed (Q recognize the many siblings who did nor suffer from psychological adjustment problems. Second, these repons were based largely on anec dotal evidence and self-report. They did nor em pirically investigate actual differences In responsibility between siblings of children with disabilities and siblings of typically developing children, nor did they demonstrate any correlation between responsibility and poor psychological outcomes through systematic measurement (Damiani, 1999). The Role of Siblings Mote recem work has also focused on the roles that siblings of children with disabilities may play within the family. Research largely supports earlier findings that siblings, particularly sisters, of children with disabiliries spend more time in volved in caregiving activities, such as babysicting or helping with feeding, dressing, or bathing (McHale & Gamble, 1989), even when the typi cally developing sibling is younger (Stoneman, Brody, Davis, Crapps, & Malone, 1991). However, 293 generally speaking, female children appear (0 shoulder greater responsibility in the home, re gardless of the presence of a sibling with a dis ability (McHale & Gamble, 1989; Sroneman, Brody, Davis, & Crapps, 1987). Gender aside, when ocher home responsibilities,. such as house hold chores and self-care activities were consid ered, Cuskelly and Gunn (1993) found no difference in levels of responsibility between sib lings of children with disabilities and those of nondisabled children. Ie appears that, regardless of gender, siblings of children with disabilities do assume more childcare responsibility in the family, bur they are not expected to simultane ously take on more household chores or self-care responsibilities. Although it is largely accepted that when there is a child with a disability in the family, siblings bear greater childcare responsibility in the home, it is not clear that this heightened responsibility leads to global adjustment problems. Although McHale and Gamble (989) found a positive rela tion between amount of time spent in childcare activities and anxiety, they also reponed that de pression, self-esteem, and conduct problems were not related to home responsibilities. Others report a positive telation between responsibility and so cioemotional functioning of siblings. For exam ple, Cuskelly and Gunn (1993) found that sisters of children with disabilities exhibited fewer con duct problems when they had more responsibili ties in the home. Finally, there is some indication that when typically developing siblings of chil dren with disabilities have greater caregiving re sponsibility, these sibling relationships are charactetized by low levels of conflict (Stoneman et aI., 1991). There is a belief that siblings of children with disabilities may be at increased risk for adjust ment problems because they receive less attention from parents than their brothers and sisters (Stoneman, 2001). This is supported by the liter ature on typically developing siblings in which differential parental attention is often associated 294 CHAPTER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES tith deleterious sibling outcomes (Brody, .. Stoneman, & Burke, 1987). Nonetheless, evidence is mixed about whether siblings of children with disabilities actually receIve less actention (Kaminsky & Dewey, 2001; McHale & Gamble, 1989; Stoneman et aI., 1987). Furthermore, stud ies on siblings of children with disabilities do nor clearly indicate that the effects of differemial at tention are detrimental. Corter and colleagues (Corter, Pepler, Stanhope, & Abromovitch, 1992) found no relation between maternal partiality and sibling interactions. Pit-ten Care and Loots (2000) reported that although siblings perceived a difference in parental attention, they were accept ing of it and largely recognized and appreciated their parems' attempts at jusrness. Sibling Interactions Interactions between siblings when one has a de velopmental disability may differ appreciably from those in which both siblings are typically developing. Although McHale and Gamble (l989) found no difference in the amoum of time sibling pairs spent in imeraction with each other, the nature of the imeractions and the type of ac tivities were very different when one sibling had a disability. In addition to more caregiving, siblings of children with disabilities often display more managerial behavior and engage in less coopera tive interaction with their brothers and sisters when compared to siblings of typically develop ing children (Dallas, Stevenson, & McGurk, 1993; Stoneman et aI., 1987). Additionally, when compared ro siblings of typically developing chil dren, siblings of children with Down syndrome have been observed to be more nurturing towards their brothers or sisters, regardless of birth order (Abramovitch, Stanhope, Pepler, & Coner, 1987). There is substamial evidence that the sibling relationship develops differemly when one mem ber has a disability. In dyads in which (he rypi cally developing sibling is oldet, there is a high degree of role asymmetry in the relationship as the older sibling assumes a position of dominance (Abramovitch et aI., 1987; Dallas er aI., 1993; StOneman et aI., 1987). Although this is typical of any sibling relationship, when one sibling has a disability, the imbalance of power is not mini mized over time bur rather may become more pro nounced (Stoneman, 2001). When the child with a disability is the older sibling, the developmem of the sibling relation ship may be even more atypical. Over time, the pair may experience a reversal of roles as the younger sibling catches up to, and bypasses, the older sibling in terms of cognitive ability and/or funCtional skills, eventually assuming the domi nam role (Abramovirch et aI., 1987; Brody, Stoneman, Davis, & Crapps, 1991; Dallas et aI., 1993; Stoneman et aI., 1991), Although the de velopmem of the relationship becween children with disabilities and their siblings may not be normative, there is no evidence that chis asymme try is in any way detrimental to [he developmem of the individuals or the relationship. Stoneman (2001) emphasizes (hat relationships [hat are atypical may be considered adaptive, rather than necessarily pathological or problematic. Many current investigations regarding the sib lings of children with disabilities consider the complexity of both the sibling relationship and [he family concext and recognize a range of possi ble psychological outcomes. It is imponant co un derstand the differenc processes and mechanisms by which these potencial outcomes occur (McHale & Gamble, 1989). for example, Dyson (L989) compared older siblings of children with disabili ties to older siblings of typically developing chil dren and found no significant difterences between the cwo groups on measures of self-concept, social competence, and behavior problems. She reponed, however, significant within-group variation re lated to borh child and family characteristics. [0 other words, Dyson found that for bmh groupS, those with and without siblings with disabilicies, Siblings of Children with Developmental Disabilities there ~as great variabiliry in adjustmem and be havior. For all siblings, some demonstrated high levels of self-concept and social competence with few behavior problems, whereas some exhibited (he opposite. This suggests that such distinctions have more to do with individual differences and differences in family functioning than with whether or not a child with a disabiliry is present in the home. In a follow-up srudy, Dyson, Edgar, and ernic (989) indicated that family context variables, such as parental stress, family relation ship, social support, and the family's emphasis on personal growth, were significant predictors of self-concept, social competence, and behavior problems in siblings of children with a variety of disabilities. Sibling Adjustment Sibling adjustmem is related to many factors, in cluding (he rype and severity of the sibling's dis ability and rhe temperamem of both siblings; however, evidence increasingly poims (0 the im portance of the family comext. Parents' psycho logical well-being, imeractions, and responses (0 stress affect the well-being of the individual chil dren within the family. In a 3-year longitudinal study, siblings of children with pervasive develop mental delay (POD) exhibited more behavior problems than either the siblings of children with Down syndrome or those of typically developing children (Fisman, Wolf, Ellison, & Freeman, 2000). Sibling behavior problems in all groups, however, were related ro parental stress, and par ents of children with POD were found co main tain the highest stress levels ovet time. McHale and Gamble (1989) found no direct connections between the characteristics of children with dis abilities and th@ well-being of theit older brothers and si~ters, bur they did find that siblings who ex perienced more negative imeractions with their mothers exhibired more depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem. Thus, family environmem is a 295 critical factot in understanding sibling adjust ment to childhood disability. Some recent studies have found that the pres ence of a child with a disability has a positive im pact on family climate and sibling adjustment. In one study, siblings of children with Down syn drome were collectively found co have above aver age scores on a measure of self-concept (Van Riper, 2000). Then again, higher self-concept scotes were related ro greater family resources and fewer srressful events experienced by the family. These family characteristics, as well as coping strategies and effective problem-solving communication techniques, also predicted the sibling's social competence. Although family context is important in un derstanding the relationships that exist between siblings when one has a disability, focus on this tOpic is just emerging in the research literature (Stoneman, 2001). There is evidence that children regard theit siblings with an intellectual disabil ity more positively when they perceive their fam ilies as communicative and emotionally responsive (Weinger, 1999). This indicates that in families in which members are safely able to ex press a range of feelings, children express a greater acceptance of their siblings with disabilities. More investigation is needed, however, to determine the process by which the family climate influences the relationship between siblings when one has a disability. In general, many children reflect positively on the experience of having a sibling wirh a disabil ity (Eisenberg, Baker, & Blacher, 1998; Grossman, 1972; Kaminsky & Dewey, 2001; Pit ten Care & Loots, 2000; Roeyers & Mycke, 1995; Van Riper, 2000). These children rend to rate their sibling relarionships more positively than do comparison children (Roeyers & M ycke, 1995), report fewer conflicts, and express greater admita tion for their siblings (Kaminsky & Dewey, 2001). Many children acknowledge that they have benefited from having a sibling with a disability. 296 CHAPTER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES 4fhey credit their siblings with helping them gain virtue~ such as patience, tolerance, benevolence, and appteciation of health and family (Eisenberg et aI., 1998; Van Riper, 2000). THE ROLE OF SUPPORT GROUPS When children talk about the negative aspects of having a sibling with a disability, they often cire worry about health concerns and the futute (Eisenberg et aI., 1998). In one study, 75% of sib lings teported that they sometimes worried about their sibling's health, or future, or both (Pit-ten Cate & Loots, 2000). Damiani (1999) suggests that worry is quire prevalent among the siblings of children with disability and that this might present one of the greatest risk factors for these children. Given the worries of these siblings, such chil dren may benefit from support groups (hat ad dress (he concerns and uncerrainty (hat (hey feel about the futute lives of their brothers and sisters and their place in it. Research also indicates that siblings have a need for information. Roeyers and Mycke (1995) found thar brothers and sisters of children with autism rated their sibling relation ships more positively when they had more knowl edge about the nature of autism. In a sample of Dutch children, aged 10 to 19 years, with siblings wirh physical disabilities, many of the parrici pants were unable to provide details regarding their siblings' disabilities, but when given the op portunity asked many questions about medical matters (Pit-ten Cate & Loots, 2000). This is a further indication that siblings may need a reli able and accessible source of information about disabilities outside of the family; health care pro fessionals could be a source of such information. Siblings may also benefit from services targeted a(. supporting their psychological well-being, par ticularly in the presence of multiple risk factors. A group of low-income children, all having a sibling with a developmental disability, demonstrated de creased levels of anxiety, depression, and stress, and improved self-esteem after participating in a 15-week after-school program (Phillips, 1999). The program consisted of recreational activities, assistance with homework, and discussion groups focused on issues of developmental disabilities. A comparison group of children, who teceived no in tervention, showed no similar gains over the same time period. Clearly, siblings of children with disabilities have much to gain from interventions focused on their unique needs. The importance of the family context, however, carries additional implications for intervention. Given the existing empirical ev idence, interventions focused on reducing parental stress, anxiety, and depression and increasing fam ily communication and coping strategies may provide benefits for all members of the family. Many EI programs aim [0 support families as they raise children with disabilities and, as discussed in the next section, do so by providing a range of highly individualized services. Early Intervention Services Early intervention services are multidisciplinary services provided [0 children with developmental disabilities, delays, or risks during the first few years of life. The goal of these programs is [0 pro mote the health and optimal development of the children as well as to support adaptive parenting and positive functioning of their families (Shonkoff & Meisels, 2000). A wide range of dis ciplines are involved in providing such services, including public health, medicine, education, psychology, social work, child care, speech and language services, and occupational and physical therapy, and, therefore, a broad range of services are provided (Table 14.2). The specific services are usually highly individualized based on child and family needs and strengths. Early intervention programs are serving almOS( 60% more children and families now than they were a decade ago when states were first mandated to provide such services (U .S. Deparrmeot of Education, 2001). In addition to improved early Early Intervention Services 297 Table q.2 Types of Early Intervention Services Commonly Provided to Children Familiesa, b Type Percentage" Service coordination Speechllanguage therapy Special instruction Occupational therapy Physical therapy Developmental monitoring Health-related services Family training Other family support (e.g., counseling) Audiology Social work services 80 53 44 39 38 38 25 20 14 14 12 'ServlceI fT1(jst commonly prl)llided within the first 6 months 0/ enrollment. 'Soum: National Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (U.S. Department 0/ Education, 2000). 'Percentages sum to more than 100 became children and /amilier often receive multiple services. diagnostic methods, rhe increased demand for services seems to be dtiven by a strong advocacy movement for and by individuals with disabilities and a heightened public awareness of the impor tance of the firsr 3 years of life in shaping devel opmental outcomes (Gutalnick, 1998). Notwithstanding the growing emphasis on, and awareness of, EI as an important factor in pro moting optimal development for young children with disabilities, several challenges to research on the EI system persist. Fitst, researchers disagree about how to determine the effectiveness of EI. Debates exist about which child outcomes deserve to be studied; whether family outcomes also should be evaluated; how to measure and reliably record the types, intensity, and individualized na ture of services themselves; and the extent to which both quantitative and qualitative ap proaches are needed ro understand family processes (Hauser-Cram, Warfield, Upshur, & Weisner, 200e). Hisrorically, evaluation tesearch on El focused almost exclusively on cognitive out comes with a neglect of socioemotional function ing of children and families (Shonkoff & Hauser-Cram, 1987). There appears to be consid erable agreement that childten with established disabilities who receive EI demonstrate less dete riotation in theit scores on standardized tests of intellectual ability than their peers who do not re ceive comprehensive services (Guralnick, 1998; Guralnick & Bricker, 1987; Hines & Bennett, 1996; Spiker & Hopmann, 1997). These advanta geous effects, however, have generally been found only during the first 5 years of life; long-term gains remain to be examined (Guralnick, 1998). Despite the focus of EI services on family strengths and needs as well as on the individual child, few evaluation studies have considered parental benefits of participation In EL Examining parental benefits of EI services is im portant in its own right because positive adjust ment to parenting a child with a disability has potential advantages for all family members. Furthermore, because rhe family context is central to the optimal development of children, including children with disabilities (Hauser-Cram er aI., 2001), assisting families with maintaining a sup portive context for parenting is an aim of most EI 298 .. CHAPTER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABIUTlES AND THElR FAMILIES • programs. In one of the few studies on parental outcomes associated with EI participation, Warfield and colleagues (Warfield, Hauser-Cram, Krauss, Shonkoff, & Upshur, 2000) found that at (he end of the EI experience, mothers who had re ceived more hours of EI services reported im proved family cohesion and more helpful social support nerworks in comparison to other mothers in EL Evaluating Early Intervention Programs Most studies of E[ effectiveness have focused on the program as a whole. [n an attempt to deter mine which features of EI programs were most ef fective, researchers at the Early Intervention Research Insritute conducted a series of longitudi nal studies employing randomized experimental designs. These studies yielded little information about the relative effectiveness of various program components, however. For example, White et aL (1994) reported no outcome differences for chil dren who received intensive, family-centered in tervention serv.ices in comparison to those who received center-based services once per week. One reason why so few program variation effects were fDund may be that not all children and families benefit equally from equal amounts of interven tion (Dunst & Trivette, 1997). Indeed, given the diversity of children enrolled in the system and the wide range of services provided, it is difficult, and perhaps not even particularly meaningful, to consider the effectiveness of EI at a macro level (McCollum, 2002). Additionally, Marfo and colleagues (1992) found that factors such as a child's developmental c9mpetency at entry into EI and the home envi ronment were stronger predictors of developmen ,tal outcomes than were specific program variables. In other words, children who entered El with the greatest delays tended to show the least improve ment in developmental scores, but the quality of the home environment and parental expectations also comributed significantly to post-intervemion developmental sratus. These are important find ings for several reasons. First, that entry-level child characteristics were the most influential pre diCtor of developmental outcomes highlights the need for parents and professionals to be realistic in their expectations of what EI can accomplish for )'oung children with significant deficits. Second, the importance of family ecology in determining developmental outcomes underscores the neces sity for EI services to focus not only on the child, but also on the family as a whole. Guralnick (1997) makes a useful distinction between first-generation and second-generation research in EL First-generation research focuses on investigating the general effectiveness of com prehensive EI programs. [n contrast, second generation research addresses more specific issues that are aimed at optimizing individual outcomes, infDrming program design, and increasing our un derstanding of rhe influence of individual child and family characteristics on intervention efficacy. Some researchers maintain that global efficacy of EI has been established by first-generation re search, and second-generation studies are now needed to inform the development of services that are responsive to individual needs and will evoke the most advantageous outcomes (Guralnick, 1997; McCollum, 2002). Such studies are diffi cult to conduct, however, given the individualized natute of EI and the heterogeneous population of children and families served. Challenge of Inconsistency and Fragmentation A related challenge to the evaluation of EI services is due to the inconsistency and fragmentation within the EI system. For example. EI programs var)' widely from state to state. Not only does fed eral legislation allow the states latitude in choos ing a lead agency, it also gives them considerable discretion in the determinarion of who is eligible for services. Alrhough all states must serve chil Early Intervention Services dren with established disabilities and developmen tdt delays, these criteria are not clearly defined by the federal government for children under 3 years of age, and states are allowed to establish their own criteria. State definitions of "developmental delay" vary widely (Hebbeler et aI., 1999). For example, Massachusetts has specific guide lines abour the minimal extent of delay required for a child to be eligible for EI based on the child's age (e.g., 1.5 months delay for children aged 6 months or less, 6 months delay for children aged 19-36 months) (Massachusetts Department of Public Health, 1998). In Utah, a significant delay is defined as 1.5 standard deviations at or below the mean or below the 7th percentile in one or more areas of development based on a standard as sessment (Utah Department of Health, 1999). In Alabama, infants or toddlers must be delayed by at least 25% in their cognitive, communicative, so cial, emotional, or adaptive development to be eli gible for EI services (Alabama Department of Rehabilitation Services, 2003). Additionally, states may also decide whether or not to provide services [Q children "at risk" for exhibiting delays. This has resulted in great inconsistency; many children who are eligible for services in one state are not in an other. Hence, although there exists a federal man date to serve infants and toddlers with disabilities, (here is little equity with regard to who may par ticipate in EI ptograms (Bailey, 2000). Additionally, there is considerable variability among states along several other dimensions. Specifically, states differ with regard to (he num ber and type of agencies that are involved in the provision of services and in the ways that services are coordinated among agencies. There is even significant disparity in the degree to which states manifest within-state differences in local systems (Hebbeler et al", 1999). For example, in Illinois each local area has an entity called "Child and Family Connecrioos" un der contract from the state lead agency, which is responsible for intake and for connecting families with the local service provider. This system pro 299 vides some uniformity across the state in terms of intake, referral, and services. In contrast, in Ohio each county has a collaborative gtoup that is re sponsible for establishing an EI system. That sys tem varies widely from county to county; one county may have a single entry point to EI ser vices, and the adjacent county may offer several points of entry. Any general discussion of EI and its effective ness is made more difficult by the heterogeneous nature of the children and families who partici pate in EI services. State differences aside, chil dren may be eligible for EI services for a variety of reasons. The heterogeneity of children entering EI is illustrated by early reports from the National Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (NEILS). Commissioned by the Office of Special Education Programs, NEILS involves a nationally represen tative sample of more than 5,000 children and their families (Hebbeler et at, 2001; U.S. Department of Education, 2000). Preliminary dam include a total of 305 specific descriptors of reasons for participams' initial eligibility for EI services, such as visual impairmem, Down syn drome, spina bifida, or homelessness, to name bur four. These descriptors can be classified into di verse categories, such as sensory impairmems, congenital disorders, cemral nervous system dis orders, and social environment risk factors (Hebbeler et al., 200 l; U.S. Departmem of Education, 2000). Because E[ serves children with a broad range of abilities and needs, systems nationwide offer a wide array of services to both children and their families. These services are provided by many dif ferem types of practitioners and professionals, fre quently through multiple agencies. Services may be child focused, such as occupational therapy, physical thempy, or speech and language services; medical care or special education services may also be included. Parents may receive mental health counseling, social services, or attend parenting classes, information sessions, or suppOrt groups. EI services may also be provided in a range of 300 CHAI)TER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES ·environments, from the home, to community set • tings such as childcate centers, to segregated, self contained programs. Goals may focus on improving cognitive outcomes, communication skills, physical functioning, or social and emo tional competencies. A systemwide objective of EI is that children and their families receive a pack age of services that is individualized to meet their needs. Developmental Systems Model Given the diversity of the participants, the range of goals, and the breadth of services provided, combined with the inconsistencies and fragmen tation that characterize the system at the state and national levels, it is small wonder that questions about effectiveness have been difficult ro address with scientific rigor, and a coherent framework in which ro view and discuss the EI system has been elusive. However, despite this diversity, there are some unifYing themes that emerge when EI is considered overalL Although service delivery models may differ from state to state, the federal legislation (Parr C of IDEA) requires an interdis ciplinary collaborarive system of services and agencies that serve children and families with a wide range of abilities and needs. When we look ar the various models of EI that currendy proliferate, tWO overarching principles emerge (McCollum, 2002). First, EI practices are guided by 'an ecological perspective of human de velopment that recognizes that rhe child develops within multiple intersecting environments (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Second, service delivery models adopt a "systems of service" framework that stresses coordination and collaboration among various agencies and professionals. Taken together, these guiding themes underlie the de velopmental systems model of EI (Guralnick, 2001)~ FAMILY The developmenral systems model recognizes thar the family is the primary context of develop ment for the young child (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). Guralnick (1997) notes that within this context, families influence child development in three ways: through the quality of parent-child interac tions, through the types of experiences that are made available to the child, and by ensuring the child's health and safety. These three patterns are largely determined by both personal characteris tics of the family, such as psychological well-being and intellectual ab!lity, as well as environmental characterisrics, such as the availability of social suppOrt and the family's financial resources. Furthermore, this model adopts a transactional perspective (Sameroff & Fiese, 2000) by recogniz ing rhat these factors interact with child charac teristics, such as the nature and severity of a child's disability and the child's individual tem perament, ro shape the family ecology and creare a unique developmental COntext for every family. Currem configurations of E[ emphasize family by providing a system of services aimed ar sup poning a family ecology that optimizes child de velopment. EI programs roUtinely include services focused on providing support and informarion to family members, including memal healrh ser vices, counseling, and educational programs fo cused on patenting a child with a disability. Furthermore, many child-focused therapemic services take place in the home environment andlor involve family members actively. SCHOOL AND COMMUNITY Present EI praCtices also recognize the impor tance of Other, more distal, contexts of develop ment beyond the family, most specifically, school and community. There is considerable emphasis in both EI theory and practice on encouraging participants in E[ programs to engage in inclusive community settings and activities. Such practices promote peer relationships and enhanced social competence (Guralnick, 2000). It is underslOo d that one role of E[ is (Q enable young children with disabilities (Q participate in the same devel opmental contexts as theIr typically developing 301 References • ~ peers (McCollum, 2002), including daycare and preschool settings. Toward this end, practitioners make every attempt to provide EI services in in clusive natural environments (Walsh, ROllS, & Lutzer, 2000). Thus? to the maximum extent pos sible, all services and therapies are provided in home, school, or community settings, as opposed to segregated settings. Indeed, Guralnick (2001) considers community inclusion to be one of the three core principles of a developmental systems model, along with a focus on families and inte gration at the systems leveL Ers focus on family and community contexts draws heavily on current theories of human devel opment, recognizing that the factors that influ ence optimal development fot childten with and without disabilities are many and diverse, and of ten interact in complex ways. Clearly, no single provider Ot agency can address all of these factors or the wide array of developmental needs that may be presented by the heterogeneous community of children and families who participate in EI pro grams. To provide the comprehensive, individual ized package of services that has become the hallmark of the EI system, an integrated, multi disciplinary system of services consisting of mul tiple practitioners and agencies is essentiaL For such a system to operate smoothly, a high level of collaboration and cooperation among the various components is warranted. Addicionally, a service coordinator is an important component to help successfully navigate the complexities of the system and smooth transitions for families. Family members also may be viewed as integral components of the system of service. The forma tion of parent-professional partnerships that rec ognize parents as cocoordinators and co-providers of services for their children is key to the success of EI sei:vices (Turnbull, Turbiville, & Turnbull, 2000). The formation of such pactnerships, however, requires EI service providers to be knowledgeable of and sensitive to the various ethno-theories that parents hold about intervention practices and about the nature of developmental disabilities. Parents' belief systems, and rhe cultural contexts in which such beliefs develop, are an often over looked but important part of the provision of ser vices (Garcia Coli & Maguson, 2000; Super & Harkness, 1997). The future of the EI system will depend on irs ability to attend to the development of cultutal competence of service providers (Hanson, 1998). Conclusion In conclusion, research on the effeCtiveness of EI faces many challenges. Nevertheless, this system of services has much to offer young children with disabilities and their families. It is an evolving system that requires responsiveness ro a heteroge neous range of children and families. Health care providers are importam sources of information about such services, and they will serve families well by becoming knowledgeable not only about the particular needs of children with disabilities and their families but also about the various EI services provided in their local communities. References Abbott, D., & Meredith, W. (986). Strengths of parenrs with retarded children. Family Relatiom, 35, 371-375 Abramovitch, R., Stanhope, L., Pepler, D., & Correr, C. (1987). The influence of Down's syndrome on sibling interaction. Journal of Child Prychology and Psychiatry, 28, 865-879 Alabama Departmem of Rehabilitation Services. (2003). A/abama'f Early fnten'entiotJ SYftem: General informa tion, Retrieved Augusr 6,2003 from http://www.rehab. state. al. uslhfJ11le/ferviml AEfSIGeneral + /nform. Bailey, D. B. (2000). The federal role in early intervenrion: Prospects for the future. Topi{f in Early Childhood Spe cial Education, 20(2), [-78 Barnard, K. E., Hammond, M. A., Booth" C. 1, [lee, H. L., Mitchell, S. K., & SpIeker, S. J. ([989) t-.L',,,clr(,01enr and meaning of parent-child interaCtion. L J Mor rison, C. Lord, & D. P. Keating (Eds.), '\P/'!Jed develop mental pJychology, Volume 3 (pp. 39-80). New York: Academic Press. 302 CHAPTER I4: YOUNG CHILDREN WITH DEVElOPMENTAL D[SABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES BarOlt,-Cjlhen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Swe(cenham,j., Nighringale, N., Morgan, K., Drew, A., & Charman, T. (1996). Psychological markers in rhe detecrion of aucism in infancy in a large population. Britiih jour nalofPsychiatry, 168,158-163. Beeghly. M., Perry, B. M .. & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Structural and affective dimensions of play developmenr in young children with Down syndrome. International journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 257-277. Blacher,]. (1984). Sequential stages of paremal adjustment to rhe birrh of a child wirh handicaps: Fact or arti fact' Mental Retardation, 22, 55-6S. Brody, G. H .. Stoneman, Z., & Burke, M. (1987). Child temperaments, maternal differential behavior, and sibling relationships. Developmental Psychology, 23, 354-362. Brody, G. H., Sroneman, Z .• Davis. C H .• & Crapps,]' M. (1991). Observations of rhe role relations and behav ior between older children with mental retardation and their younger siblings. Americanjournal ofMental Retardation, 95, 527-536. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experimlmts by nature and design. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press. Bronfenbtenner, U, (1986). Ecology of the family as a con rext for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental PIychology, 22, 723-742. Corter, C, Pepler, D .. Stanhope, L.. & Abramovitch, R. (1992), Home observations of mothers and sibling dyads comprised of Down's syndrome and nonhandi capped children. Canadian journal ofBehavioural Sci ence, 24. 1-13. Crawley, S .• & Spiker. D. (983). Morher-child interactions involving rwo-year-olds with Down syndrome: A look at individual differences, Child De1/elopment. 54. 1312-1323. Cuskelly, M. (1999) Adjustment of siblings of children with a disability: Methodological issues, International journal for the l'.dvancement ofCoumelling. 21. 111-124. Cuskelly, M.. & Gunn, P. (1993). Marernal repom of behav ior of siblings of children with Down syndrome. Americanjournal ofM.ental Retardation, 97. 521-529. Dallas. E., Stevenson. j., & McGurk. H. (993). Cerebral palsied children's interactions with siblings II. Interactional structure.journal ofChild Psychology and Psy,hiatry, 34, 649-671. Damiani, V. B. (1999). Responsibility and adjustment in sibling;; of children with disabilities: Update and review. Families in SOliety. 80, 34-40. Dunn. J. (1999). Siblings, friends, and the development of social understanding. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.). Relationships as dC1Jelopmental contexts: The Min fmota symposia on child plychology (Volume 30, pp. 263-279). Mahwah. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Dunst, C j.. & Triverte, C M. (1997). Early intervention with young at-risk childten and their families. (n R. T. Ammerman & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook ofpre vention and treatment with children and adolescents: Inter ventiom in the real world context (pl" 157-180). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Dyson. L L. (1989). Adjustment of siblings of handicapped children: A comparison. journal of Pediatric Psychology, 14.215-229 Dyson. L L. Edgar, E.. & Crulc, !C, (1989). Psychological predictors of adjustment of siblings of developmen tally disabled children. Americanjournal ofMental Retardation, 94. 292-302. Eisenberg, L, Baker, B. L., & Blacher,]. (1998). Siblings of children with mental retardation living at home or in residential placemenr.journal ofChild Psychology and PJychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39, 355-363. Farber. B. (1959). The effects of severely retarded children on rhe family sysrem. Monographs 0/ the Society for ReJearch in Child Development, 24 (2, Serial No. 71), Fisman. S., Wolf, L. Ellison. D .. & freeman. T. (2000). A longitudinal smdy of siblings of children with chronic disabilities. Canadian journal of Plychiatry, 45,369-375 Frith, U, (2003). Auti.frtt: Explaining the enigma. Malden. MA: Blackwell. Garcia Coli, C, & Magnuson, K. (2000). Cultural differ ences as sources of developmental vulnerabiliries and resources. (n J, P. Shonkoff & S j. Meisels (Eds.). Handbook ofearly childhood intervention (2nd ed .. pp. 94·-114). New YOtk: Cambridge UniverSltr Press Garwood. S, G .. fewell, R. R" & Nc,sworth, J. T. (1988). Public Law 94-142: You can get there from here l Top ICI In Early Childhood Special Education, 8, 1-·1 1. Gath, A, (1973). The school age siblings of mongol chil dren. BritishjoILrnalofPrychiiltry, 123, 161--167 Gresham, EM.• & MacMillan, D. L (1997), Social compe tence and affective characreristics of students with mild disabilities. Review of Educational Research, 76, 377-415. Grossman. F K. (1972). Brothet'l and lister,. of retarded (hil dren: An exploratory study. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse Uni· verslty Press. Guralnick, M. J. (1997 J. Second-generation tesearch .n rbe field of early intervention. In M. j. Guralnick (Ed.), The effeail.enm ofearly inten.enlwll (PI', 3-20). Balti more: Brookes. Guralnick. M. J. (1998). Effectiveness ofearly intervention for vulnerable children: A developmental perspecrive. [lmericall journal 011 Mental Retardation. 102. 3I9-.)4'j Gutalnick, M. J (2000) Early childhood tntervenrion: Evo lution of a sysrem, Focltf on AutiJm arJd Other D,~.eloP maual Disabilitie;, 15(2),68-79. Guralnick, M. J (2001). A developmemal systems model for early intervention I nfantl It"d ),lImg Children. 14(2). I-IS. - References <Guralnick,"M. J., & Bricket, D. (1987). The effectiveness of early intervention for children with cognitive and general developmental delays. In M. J Guralnick & E C Bennen (Eds.), The effectiveness ofearly tntervention for at-risk and handicapped children (pp. 115-17 3). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 303 Kaminsky, L, & Dewey, D. (200l). Sibling relationships of children with aurism.Journal of Autism and Develop mental Dil0rders, 31,399--410. Krauss, M. W. (1990). A new precedent in family policy: The individualized family service plan. Exceptional Children, 56, 388-395. Kochanek,T 1'., & Bub, S. L (1998). Influential factors in the urilization of early inrervenrion services.Journal of Early Interventioll, 2 I, 3'23-338. Hack, M. Wright, L L, Shankaran, S., & Tyson, J. E. (1995). Very low birth weight outcomes of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Network, November 1989 to Kubler-Ross, E. (1997). On death and dying. New York: October, 1990. AmericanJournal of Obstetrics and Gyne Scribner. cology, 172, 457--464. Lamb, M. E., & Billings, L. A. (199 7). Fathers of children Hanson, M. J (1998). Echnic, culrutal, and language diver with special needs. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role ofthe sity in intervention settings. In E. W. Lynch & M. J. father in child development (pp. 179-190). New York: Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural competence: A Wiley. guide for working with young children and their families Lamorey, S. (1999). Parentificarion of siblings of children (2nd ed., pp. 3-22). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. with disabiliry or chronic disease. In N. D. Chase Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., & Krauss, (Ed.), Burdened children: Theory, research, and treatment M. W (2001). Children with disabilities: A longitu ofparentification (pp. 75-9D. Thousand Oaks, CA: dinal study of child development and parent well Sage Publicarions. being, Monographs ofthe Society for Research in Child Landis, L J. (1992). Marital, employmenr, and childcare sta Development, 66 (3, serial No. 266). rus of mothers wirh infanrs and coddlers wirh disabil Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Upshur, C C, & Weis iries. Topi,J in Early Childhood Spaial Education, 12, ner, 1'. S. (2000). An expanded view of progtam eval 496-·507. uation in early childhood intervention. In J. P. Landry, S. H., & Chapieski, M. L (1990). Joint atcenrion of Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook ofearly six-month-old Down syndrome and pcererm infants: childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. '187-509). New I. Arrcntion to roys and morher. AmericanJournal on York: Cambridge University Press. Mental Retardation. 91. 488--498. Hebbeler, K., Spiker, D., Wagner. M .. Cameto. R., LaParo, K. M., Olsen, K., & Pianra, R. C. (2002). Special McKenna, P., & SRI International. (1999). State-to education eligibility: Developmenral precursors over state variations irJ early intm'ention lY'tems. Menlo Park, rhe firsr thteeyears of life, Exceptional Children, 69, CA: SRI Imernarional. 55-66. Hebbeler, K., Wagner, M., Spiker, D .. Scarborough, A., Lepper, M. R. (1981). Intrinsic and exrrinsic morivarion in Simeonson, R., & Collier, M. (200 I). A first look at the children: Detrimental efiecrs of superfluous social characterirlies a/children and families erJtering early inter controls. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposium vention services (NEILS Data Report I). Menlo Park, on child psychology, Volume 14. Minneapolis, MN: Uni CA: SRI InternationaL versiry of Minnesota Press. Heckhausen, J. (1993). The developmenr of mastery and irs Lyon, G. R. (1996). Learning disabilities. The Future ofChil perception within caretaker-child dyads. In D. J. dren, 6, 54-76, Messet (Ed.), Maifery mutiv,ltwn ill early childhood: Marfo. K. (1990). Marernal direcriveness in inreractions Development, mealuremtm and Joti,tl processes (pp. wich mentally handicapped children: An analytical 55~-79). London: Routledge. commentary. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Hines, S., & Bennett, F. (1996) Etfecriveness of early inter 31,5.31-549. vention for children With Down syndrome. AIerttal Marfo, K., Dinero, 1'., Browne, N., Gallanr, D., Smyrh, R., Retardation and Developmental Dhabilitm ReJearch & Corbett, A. (1992). Child, program, and family Reviews, 2, 96·-10 I ecological variables in early inrervenrion. Early Edu Hoppes, K., & Harris, S. L (1990). Perceptions of child cation and Development. 3, 27--44. arrachmenr and marernal gtatification in mothers of Mahoney, G, Fors, S., & Wood, S. (1990). Maternal direc childten wirh aucism and Down syndtomejournal of rive behavior revisited. AmericanJournal on Menial ClinicalchildPsy,hology. 19. )65-370. Retardation, 94, 398--406. Hornby, G. (1995). Fathets' views of the effects on their Massachusetcs Deparrmenr of Public Health. (1998). Massa families of children with Down srndtome.Journalof Child atJd Family Sludief. 4( I), 103-117. chusettJ Early Intervention OperalionalStandards. Retrieved on Augusr 6, 2003 from www.stalema.US/ Innocemi, M. S., Huh, K., & Boyce, G. (1992). Families of dph/tch!eiopltnd.pdf children with disabilities: Notmative dara and other considerations on parenting stress. Topics in Early McCollum, J. A. (2002). Influencing che developmenr of Childhood Special Education, 12. 403--407, yOUrtg children wirh disabilities: Current themes in 304 • .. CHAPTER 14: YOUNG CHILDREN WIlTI DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES AND THEIR FAMILIES earLy imervention. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7,4-9. McCubbin, H. t, & Patrerson,}. M. (1983). The famiLy srress process: The double ABCX modeL of adjust mem and adaptation. Marriage and Family Review, 6, 7-37. McHale, S. M., & Gamble, W. C. (1989) Sibling relation ships of children with disabled and nondisabled brothers and sisters. Developmental Psychology, 25, 421-429. Meisels, S.). (1991). Dimensions of early identification. journal of Early Intervention, 15, 26-35. Meisels, S.)., & Shonkoff,). P. (2000). Early childhood intervemion: A continuing evolmlon. In j. E Shon koff & S. J Meisels (Eds.), Handbook ofearly childhood intervention (2nd ed., PI'. 3-31). New York: Cam bridge Univetsity Press. Minuchin, P. P. (1988). Relationships within the family: A systems perspective on development. In R. A. Hinde &). Srevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within fam ilies: Mutual influences (PI'. 7-26). New York: Oxford Univetsity Press. NadonaL Cemer for Education Statistics. (2001). Digest of educational Jta/iJtief, 2001 (NCES number 2002130). Retrieved Ocrobe;r 30, 2002 from http://nees.edlgovl pubJ20021digcst200 I. Orr, R. R., Cameron~ S.j., Dobson, L. A., & Day, D. M. (1993). Age-related changes in stress experienced by families wirh a child who has developmemal delays. Mental Retardation, 31,171-176. Osofsky,j. D., & Thompson, M. D. (2000). Adaprive and maladaptive parenting: Perspectives on risk and pro tective factors. In). P. Shonkoff & S. J Meisels (Eds), Handbook ofearly childhood intervention (2nd ed., PI'. 54-75). New York: Cambridge University Press. Phillips, R. S. C. (l999). Intervention with siblings of chil dren with developmental disabilities from economi cally disadvantaged families. Families in Sodety, 80, 569-577. Pic-ten Care, L M., & Loors, G. M. P. (2000). Experiences of siblings of children with physical disabilities: An empirical invescigarion. Difability and Rehabilitation, 22. 399-408. Roach, M. A., Barran, M., Miller,). E, & Leavitt, L. A. (l998). The strucrure of mmher-child play: Young children wirh Down syndrome and rypically develop ing children. Developmental Psychology, 34, 77-S7. Rbeyers, H., & M ycke, K. (995). Siblings of a child with autism, wirh mental retardation and with normal development. Child: Care, Health mid Development, 21, 305-319. Sameroff, A. J, & Chandler, M. J (1975) Reproductive risk and the cominuum of careraking casualiry. In F D. Horowitz, M. Herhetingron, S. Scarr-Salapark, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review ofchild development research: Volume 4 (pp. 187-244). Chicago: Universiry of Chicago Press. Sameroff, A. j., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Transactional regula tion: The developmentaL ecology of early interven tion. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. j. Meise Ls (Eds.), Handbook ofearly childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. l35-159). New York: Cambridge University Press. Schilling, R. E, Schinke. S. P., & Kirkham, M. A. (1985). Coping wirh a handicapped child: Differences between mothers and farhers. Social Science and Medi cine, 21, 857-S63 Scorgie, K, & Sobsey, D. (2000). Transformational outcomes associared wirh parenting children who have disabili ties. Mental Retardation. 38 (3), 195-206. Seligman, M. (l999). Childhood disability and the family. In V L. Schwean & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), Handbook ojpsychosocial charactertstiu ofexceptional children. (pp. Ill-II3). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers Selrzer, M. M., Greenberg, J S., Krauss, M. W., & Gordon, R. M. (1997). Siblings of adults with mental retatda cion or mental illness: Eifects of lifestyLe and psycho logical well.being. Family Relations; Interdisciplinary jourlJal of ApplIed Family Studies, 46, 395-405. Seltzer, M. M, & Krauss, M. W. (2001). Quality oflife of adults with meneal retardation developmemal dis abilities who live with family. Mental Retardation and Det-·elopmental Duabilllw Research Review), 7, 105-·114 ShonkotT, J P., & Hauser-Cram, P. (1987). Early interven tion for disabled tntants and cheir families: A quanti tative analysis. P<ditttFt"" 80. 650-658. Shonkoff,j. P., Hauser·Cram. P, Krauss, M. W., & Upshur, C. C (992) Developmenr of infants with disabili~ ties and their I:lmiltes. Monograph! ofthe Society fOY ReJean-h in Child Df!1Jelopment, 57 (6), (Serial No. 230). Shonkoff, j. P., & Marshall, P C 0(00). The biology of developmental vulnerabIlity. [n j. P. Shonkoff & S. j. Meisels (Eds), H"ndbo,)" ofearly fhildhood intervention (2nd ed, pp. ) '). 'i,) New York: Cambtidge Univer sity Press. Shonkoff,j. E, & Meisel,;. S.). (2000) Preface. In). P. Shonkoff & S.). Mei,eis (Eds), Handbook ofearly child hood inten'entiol'l (2nd ed, pp. xvii-xviii). New York: Cambridge University Ptess. Solnit, A j., & Stark. M. H. (961) Mourning and the birth of a defective chIld. P!Yfhoanalytic Study 0/ the Child. /6. 'i2.Vi)7 Solomon, R (1995) PedIatriCIans and early intervention: Everyrhing you !Ieed to know but are tOO busy to ask. in/anti and FlJin\; Cluldren. 7(j), 38-5 L Solomon, R., Cloughen}', S. L. Shaffer, D., Hofkosh, f)., & Edwards, M. (l99 j) Communiry-based developmen References 305 U.S. Department of Education. (1.990). Twelfth annual report to Congrm on the implementation of The Education ofthe Handicapped Act. Washingron, DC: Author. Spiker, D., & Hopmann, M. R, (1997). The effectiveness of U.S. Departmenr of Education. (2000). Twenty-second annual early intervencion for children wirh Down syndrome. report to congreH on the implementation ofthe I ndividllals In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), The effictivenm ofearly inter vention (pp. 27 l-305). Baltimore: Brookes. with Disabilities Education Act. Washingmn, DC: Author. Stanton-Chapman, T. L, Chapman, D, A" & Scott, K. G, U.S. Department of Educatiop. (2001). Twenty-third annual (2001), Idemification of eady tisk factors for learning rep()rt to congreH on the implementation ofthe Individuals disabilities. Journal of Early Intervention, 24, 193-206. with Disabilities Education Act. Washingmn, DC: U.S. Sroneman, Z. (2001). Supporting positive sibling relation Department of Education. ships during childhood. Mental Retardation and Devel • .' tal assessmenc sites: A new model for pediatric "child find" activities. Infants and Young Children, 7, 67-7 L opmental Disabilities Ruearch Reviews, 7, 134-142. Utah Department of Health (1999). Utah State Plan: The Individual! with Disabilities Act Part C: Early Interven Sroneman, Z., Brody, G. H., Davis, C H., & Crapps,]. M. tion for Infants and Toddlen with Disabilities. Retrieved (1987). Mentally retarded children and their same August 6, 2003 from www.utahbabywatch.()rgl sex: siblings: Naturalistic in-home observations. agencyinfoleligibility. AmericanJournal of Mental Retardation, 92, 290-298. Van Riper, M. (2000). Family variables associated with well Stoneman, Z., Brody, G. H., Davis, C H., Crapps,]. M., & being in siblings of children with Down syndrome, Malone, D. M (1991). Ascribed role relations Journal of Family Nursing, 6, 267-286. between children with memal retardation and their Walsh, S., Rous, B., & bItZer, C. (2000). The federal IDEA younger siblings, AmericanJoumal of Mental Retarda Natural Environments Provisions: Making it work. tion, 95, 537-550. Yollng Exceptional Children, Monograph Series No.2, Super, C M., & Harkness, S, (1997). The cultural structur 3-15 ing of child developmem. In]. W. Berry, Y P. Warfield, M. E" Hauser-Cram, P., Krauss, M. W., Shonkoff, Poorringa, J. Pandey, P. R. Dason, & T. S. Saraswathi J. P., & Upshur, C C (2000). The effect of early (Vol. Eds,), Handbook ofcrOJJ-cultural psychology: Volume intervention services on maternal well-being. Early 2. Baric procesrcs and hllman development (pp. 1-39). Education and Development, I I, 499-517. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Weinger, S, (1999), Views of rhe child wirh retardation: lannock, R. (1988). Control and recipwcity imeracrions Relationship to family functioning. Family Therapy, with Down syndrome and normal children. In 26(2), 63-79. K. Marko (Ed.), Parent-child interaction and dn<elopmen tal disabilities: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. White, K R., Boyce, G. c., Casto, G., Innocenti, M. S., 162-180). New York: Praeger. Taylor, M.]., Goetze, L, & 8ehl, D. (1994). Compar ative evaluations of early inrervention alrernatives: A Turnbull, A P., Turbiville, V, & Turnbull, H. R. (2000). response to commentaries by Guralnick and Telzrow. Evolution of family.professional partnerships: Collec Early Education and Development. 5, 56-68. tive empowetmenc as the model for the eady twenty first cenmry. In J. P. Shonkoff & S J. Meisels (Eds.), Wikler, L., Wasow, M., & Harfield, E. (981). Chronic Handbook ofearly childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. scresses of families of mentally retarded children. 630-6'50). New York: Cambridge University Press. Family Relations, 30, 281-288. Handbook of Human Development for Health Care Professionals Kathleen Thies, PhD, RN Associate Professor, Graduate School of Nursing University of Massachusetts Medical School Worcester, MA John Travers, EdD Professor, Lynch School of Education BostOn College Chestnut Hill, MA ~ ~ JONES AND BARTLETT PUBLISHERS Sudbury. Ma,;sacllusetts BOSTON TORONTO LONDON SINGAPORE
© Copyright 2024