COMMUNICATION 10.1177/009365002237830 Fujioka, Austin • Family RESEARCH Communication • December 2002 YUKI FUJIOKA ERICA WEINTRAUB AUSTIN The Relationship of Family Communication Patterns to Parental Mediation Styles A telephone survey of parents (n = 216) of third-, sixth-, and ninth-grade children examined the relationship between reported family communication patterns and parental mediation styles. Concept orientation predicted parental involvement in both positive (endorsements of TV messages) and negative (counter-reinforcement of TV messages) mediation as well as critical discussion of a variety of issues with a child. Socio orientation predicted positive mediation and was associated with coviewing, but it was not related to the parental practice of either negative mediation or critical discussion. The results suggest that parents with an open communication style are more likely to make use of discussion-based intervention strategies applied to television. A parent’s more control-oriented style translates into reinforcement of TV messages. Possible negative influences of media messages on children have long concerned parents, educators, policy makers, and scholars. In conjunction with this concern, many have examined how parents influence children’s responses to television messages (e.g., Atkin, Greenberg, & Baldwin, 1991; Austin, 1993a; Corder-Bolz, 1980; Krcmar, 1996; Nathanson, 1999). Although a number of studies have examined parental influences, we still know little about parents’ motivations for discussing media messages or the results of such discussions. The literature on media effects suggests that parents have four major routes by which they can modify the effects of television messages. These include intentional activities such as (a) making restrictions on a child’s TV viewing and (b) active mediation via critical discussion of television COMMUNICATION RESEARCH, Vol. 29 No. 6, December 2002 642-665 DOI: 10.1177/009365002237830 © 2002 Sage Publications 642 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication messages with a child (Atkin et al., 1991; Austin, 1993a; Corder-Bolz, 1980; Huston & Wright, 1996). Also included are more subtle, often unintentional activities such as (c) the general style of communication patterns that exists within the family and (d) the example parents set as models for children to imitate (Desmond, Singer, Singer, Calam, & Colimore, 1985; McKechnie, 1977; McLeod & Chaffee, 1972; Medrich, 1979). A sizable literature suggests that parents do not make extensive use of existing means for intentional influence (e.g., Bower, 1973; Comstock, 1975; Corder-Bolz, 1980; Lyle & Hoffman, 1972). The literature also suggests that parents may not sufficiently recognize the importance of their indirect methods of influence, frequently serving as poor role models or cultivating communication patterns within the family that do not help children develop effective decision-making skills (e.g., Alexander & Fry, 1990; Austin & Nach-Ferguson, 1995; Flay & Sobel, 1983; Kandel & Logan, 1984). A variety of studies suggests that general discussion styles in the family (e.g., Andreasen, 1990; Austin, 1993a, 1995; Krcmar, 1996) can help explain media-specific discussions and effects. More specifically, general styles may help explain the motivations for discussion along with the likely results of various discussion strategies. According to research on family communication norms, parents differ in the style of communication they apply to parent-child interaction. Parental communication can be located along two relatively independent dimensions: (a) stressing the value of challenging ideas without much concern for controversy and (b) emphasizing deference and harmony and the avoidance of controversy (Chaffee, McLeod, & Atkin, 1971). This study explores how these parenting styles are associated with parents’ use of more specific strategies that tend to reinforce or counter-reinforce messages about television, known as positive and negative mediation (Austin, Fujioka, Bolls, & Engelbertson, 1999; Austin, Knaus, & Meneguelli, 1998). Family Communication Patterns (FCP) The family communication patterns model (Chaffee et al., 1971; McLeod & Chaffee, 1972) was developed specifically to aid the investigation of media socialization issues. Scholars have found the FCP model useful for predicting a variety of socialization outcomes in contexts such as public affairs, academic achievement, and persuasion. The model also has withstood critical reexamination by a number of scholars (e.g., Ritchie, 1991; Ritchie & Fitzpatrick, 1990; Tims & Masland, 1985). In particular, scholars have explored ambiguities in the meaning of the constructs. For example, Ritchie (1991; Ritchie & Fitzpatrick, 1990), expressing concerns consistent with 643 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 those of other scholars, argued that socio orientation inventory items may measure parental control, the assertion of parental power, rather than harmonious relationships. They also noted that strategies associated with the socio style of communication may not lead to a harmonious relationship.1 According to both Ritchie (1991) and Austin (1993b), socio orientation reflects a desire for harmonious interpersonal relationships in the family, and the measures may reflect the parent’s efforts to achieve harmony through the emphasis of conformity and control. Accordingly, socio-oriented parents report an interest in telling their children to avoid controversy and arguments. In contrast, concept-oriented parents tend to consider communication a tool to convey and share views. Conflict, controversy, and resolution all can occur through candid discussion. As conceptualized by Ritchie (1991), who focused on the meaning of communication to each participant rather than the goal of communicative acts as perceived by the parent, socio orientation represents strategies used by parents who want to protect the status quo and is thus more internally focused. Concept-oriented parents focus on making their children capable of handling information and understanding the world, thus reflecting a more external focus. Parents can embrace both of these orientations, one but not the other, or neither. McLeod and Chaffee’s (1972) conceptualization of two dimensions to communication style represented a contrast to the prevailing unidimensional views of parenting style. For example, Baumrind (1968) conceived of a single dimension that subsumed behavior management and communication characteristics. According to Baumrind, authoritarian parents seek strict obedience (control of power) from the child and discourage give and take (communication). Authoritative parents, however, might make rules but demonstrate more openness to their child’s opinions and more willingness to adjust their behavior accordingly, still reflecting an emphasis on power but encouragement of communication (Baumrind, 1968). At the other end of the continuum from authoritarian parenting, Baumrind referred to parents deemphasizing power as permissive. Burleson, Delia, and Applegate (1995) similarly summarized that control-based orientations emphasize imperatives and commands, discouraging open communication and reflection. Parents who use styles more focused on reflection enhancement tend to incorporate reasoning and explanation in their attempts to guide the child’s conduct. Similar to the family communication patterns model, both theories conceptualize patterns of behavior management and the openness of discussion as important elements. Unlike the FCP model, however, the Baumrind and Burleson et al. models do not conceptualize them as separate but related aspects of parental communication style. 644 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication The distinction between patterns of behavior management and openness of discussion is especially relevant to the consideration of the relationship between general parenting styles and media-related parenting strategies. For example, patterns of behavior management should be related to the extent to which a parent makes use of rule-making strategies or coviewing. Openness of discussion should be associated with the extent to which a parent actively engages a child in critical discussion of television. Thus, it makes sense that parents’ general style of communication should correspond to their discussion patterns regarding specifically the media along two dimensions of behavior-management orientations and open discussion. Relating Parenting Styles to Parental Mediation of Television Parent-child discussion patterns related to media often are referred to as parental mediation, defined as the active discussion of television content with a child (Austin, 1993a). Mediation is conceptually distinct from other parental behavior such as coviewing (watching a television program with a child) and rule making (prohibiting certain television programs for a child). Mediation has been considered an important role for parents (Austin et al., 1999; Nathanson, 1999), helping children (a) determine whether and how television presents the actual world (categorization), (b) decide whether to endorse or condemn television messages (validation), and (c) obtain additional information that demonstrates the potential relevance of television messages to real life (supplementation) (Austin, 1993a; Messaris, 1982). Parental mediation can contribute to children’s understanding of persuasive intent of television by immunizing them with skeptical or critical thinking (e.g., Austin, 1993a; Austin, Pinkleton, & Fujioka, 2000). Two dimensions of active discussion can be distinguished, called negative mediation and positive mediation (Austin et al., 1999, 2000). Positive mediation refers to parental endorsement of television messages or portrayals, whereas negative mediation refers to parental counterarguments or contextualization of television messages. These mediation strategies differ conceptually from other existing constructs such as restrictive mediation (rule-making strategies) or coviewing (a parent and a child watch TV together) in which parents do not necessarily engage in discourse with a child.2 According to the Austin et al. (1999) conceptualization, parents may use mediation that comprises positive reinforcement, negative reinforcement, or a combination of the two mediation strategies. In general, parents tend to 645 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 engage in negative mediation when strongly concerned with media influence on their child and/or when skeptical about media messages. In contrast, parents tend to use positive mediation when they possess a low level of skepticism and when they have positive attitudes toward television in general. Positive mediators are more likely to watch more television and coview with the child but are less likely to discuss television content than negative mediators (Austin et al., 1999). This coincides with Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, and Marseille’s (1999) finding that children tend to watch more television overall in families that participate in a lot of social coviewing. Relating the FCP to Positive Mediation Scholars note that family norms and behaviors regarding television tend to reflect general family norms and behaviors (Andreasen, 1990; Krcmar, 1996). For example, families that tend to have rules about other things also tend to have rules about television use. Similarly, Valkenburg et al. (1999) and Austin et al. (1999) both acknowledged that parents’ comments about television do not always focus on content or provide critical commentary but instead sometimes just reflect the natural extension of daily conversation. This type of discussion would be more suggestive of positive mediation, whose nature is sometimes described as unintentional or happenstance (Austin et al., 1999). Negative mediation appears to require more intentional effort, with discussion focused on specific topics such as advertising techniques or portrayals of violence. Given the spontaneous nature of positive mediation, we would expect to receive reports of positive mediation from both concept- and socio-oriented parents. Although the styles of communication differ, both orientations represent some involvement in communication. Chaffee, McLeod, and colleagues (Chaffee et al., 1971; Chaffee, McLeod, & Wackman, 1973) demonstrated that parents who emphasize neither concept nor socio orientation appear to have little input overall into their children’s lives. They described this type of parent as “laissez faire,” indicating an overall lack of parental influence. A parent’s emphasis on either communication style therefore suggests some involvement in communication, and it appears that the default strategy of involvement regarding television is positive mediation. Although both concept- and socio-oriented parents may use positive mediation, they may use it differently. In general, concept (communication) orientation is associated with an open mode of parent-child communication such 646 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication that parents and children express and exchange their ideas and opinions freely and frequently. It follows, then, that concept-oriented parents would disclose their reactions to television messages, both positive and negative, while encouraging analytical discussion of television content with their child. In fact, concept-oriented parents seem more likely to provide broader contextual information and use nondirective statements including questioning about and commenting on what they see on television (Messaris & Kerr, 1983), consistent with the parenting patterns that cultivate reflective abilities (Burleson et al., 1995). Concept-oriented parents tend to disagree with television messages and engage in critical discussion with a child (Messaris & Kerr, 1983), not necessarily to seek children’s conformity or agreement but to exchange and share opinions. Concept-oriented parents may, therefore, both express themselves and also ask children’s opinions on television messages. This open communication style would likely include some endorsement (positive mediation) of exemplary characters and behaviors and some counterreinforcement (negative mediation) of less desirable content. Socio orientation is positively associated with positive attitudes toward television and greater television use (Chaffee et al., 1971), characteristics that also correspond to positive mediation (Austin et al., 1999). Socio-oriented parents, however, may not have discussions as wide-ranging as those of concept-oriented parents. In a family emphasizing harmonious relationships, media messages may instead represent opportunities to point out things on which family members can agree. The control-oriented aspects of socio orientation identified by Ritchie and Fitzpatrick (1990) and by Austin (1993a) also seem predictive of positive mediation because families may use the television as a tool for maintaining family harmony and conformity. This prediction is consistent with Lull’s (1990) findings that families watch television together to accomplish a variety of things. One reason to watch together is to enjoy each others’ company without much risk of conflict, consistent with socio orientation’s goal of maintaining harmonious relations. Lull noted that another reason can be to give family members such as parents the opportunity to demonstrate power over the viewing experience, consistent with socio orientation’s emphasis on conformity and parental control. Thus, television coviewing and discussion can be inspired by varied motivations. This leads to the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 1: Concept orientation will positively predict reported parental engagement in positive mediation. Hypothesis 2: Socio orientation will positively predict reported parental engagement in positive mediation. 647 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 FCP, Negative Mediation, and Topic-Related Discussion The styles of communication are expected to diverge in their relationships with negative mediation. Considering the open mode of communication between concept-oriented parents and their children, it is not difficult to predict a positive relationship between concept orientation and parental engagement in negative mediation. The concept-oriented parents may express their disagreement with what they see on television and invite a child’s opinion. Or a child may express a feeling about a specific message or portrayal, which may initiate active discussion, which may lead to negative mediation. Moreover, concept-oriented parents’ tendencies toward skepticism should motivate them to engage in discussion-based negative mediation. Socio-oriented parents, however, tend to display their desire for more control by means of explicit directives rather than through give-and-take discussion (Carlson, Grossbart, & Walsh, 1990; Krcmar, 1996). Scholars have suggested that this reflects a more authoritarian power structure in the socio-oriented family (e.g., Desmond, Singer, & Singer, 1990; Krcmar, 1996; Ritchie, 1991). Consistent with these scholars, we expect that socio-oriented parents will behave in this way toward television. For example, Messaris and Kerr (1983) reported that socio-oriented mothers of elementary school children were more likely to give directive comments about television than were concept-oriented mothers but were less likely to engage in nondirective kinds of discussion than were concept-oriented mothers. Thus, although socio-oriented parents report watching more television than do conceptoriented parents, they may not speak up when they see something with which they disagree, and if they do express disagreement, they will be less likely to solicit their children’s opinions in an open discussion. Because Austin (1993a) found socio orientation unrelated to a general index of parental mediation, it may be that socio-oriented parents do counterreinforce some messages but do not emphasize analytical discussion. As a result, we submit a research question on the relationship between socio orientation and negative mediation along with a hypothesis regarding the relationship between concept orientation and negative mediation: Hypothesis 3: Concept orientation will predict reported parental engagement in negative mediation. Research Question 1: What is the relationship between socio orientation and negative mediation? 648 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication That the concept-oriented communication style relates to open communication of issues seems clear from existing scholarship. It has been shown that concept-oriented parents encourage children to express themselves and engage in discussion of a variety of issues (e.g., Austin, 1993a; McLeod & Chaffee, 1972). Moreover, according to Burleson et al. (1995), families emphasizing reflection enhancement tend to engage in more discussion overall. In addition, relationships between concept orientation and communication affect appear to exist, which may make concept-oriented family environments more conducive to the discussion of difficult issues. For example, Krcmar (1996) reported that children of control-oriented (socio-oriented) parents were less likely to show positive affect—that is, warm expression— toward their parent during decision making. Interestingly, however, control (socio) orientation was unrelated to warm expression exhibited by the parents. Krcmar suggested that because socio-oriented parents had confidence in their authority, they considered affect “irrelevant” to gaining a child’s compliance. Consistent with this view, Austin (1993a) found that a “warm” relationship between parent and child was associated with concept orientation but not with socio orientation. Austin (1995) further reported that communication warmth, interaction serving the purpose of enjoyment and emotional support (Austin, Roberts, & Nass, 1990), allowed the children to broach risky discussion topics with their parents. Although Austin (1993a) found that concept- and socio-oriented communication styles did not directly predict communication confidence, the two studies together suggest an indirect relationship between concept orientation and openness through warmth from the child’s point of view. Taken together with Krcmar’s (1996) study, these data suggest a positive relationship between concept orientation and the discussion of various issues for the parent. These studies do not suggest that socio-oriented parents care less about or feel less warm toward their children, but they do suggest that these parents use more restricted patterns of communication. These restricted patterns appear to make children feel less comfortable discussing a wide-ranging array of topics, but these restricted patterns do not appear to lead parents to feel similarly inhibited. Thus, existing scholarship suggests that: Hypothesis 4: Concept orientation will predict more frequent parent-child discussion of a variety of issues as reported by parents. 649 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 FCP Orientations, Coviewing, and Use of TV Rating Systems Although the primary interest of this study lies in the relationships between overall communication styles and discussion styles regarding television, it is nonetheless relevant to briefly consider relationships of communication style and other aspects of parental involvement with media. Of the four forms of parental involvement discussed previously, we have focused on one form of intentional activity—active mediation via critical discussion of television messages—and one form of more subtle or unintentional activity—the general style of communication patterns that exists within the family. We now briefly consider the other form of indirect activity—the example parents set as models for their children to imitate—and the other form of intentional activity—that of making restrictions on a child’s TV viewing. More specifically, we offer two predictions regarding the likely associations between communication style and coviewing patterns along with the likely associations between communication style and the use of a directive approach to management of the viewing environment. It seems clear from previous research that positive attitudes toward television and frequent use of television are associated with coviewing (Alexander, 1990; Austin, et al., 1999). Coviewing refers to a parent and a child watching television programs together. In fact, a relationship between socio-oriented parenting and coviewing already has been demonstrated (Chaffee et al., 1971). Parents who emphasize negative mediation tend to watch less television overall and therefore may coview less as well. Coviewing is not a necessary precondition for critical discussion of television (Austin et al., 1999), and indeed coviewing tends to associate with greater viewing time and less critical viewing (Atkin et al., 1991; Chaffee et al., 1971, 1973; Desmond et al., 1985; Medrich, 1979). It appears that the relationship between socio orientation and coviewing may be partly an artifact of opportunity. In other words, this relationship may be a side effect of more viewing time overall in the family. Nevertheless, consistent with Lull’s (1990) findings regarding the social uses of television, this relationship also may reflect the desire for harmonious shared experiences in a controlled environment. This combination of factors makes it likely that we will find a relationship between socio orientation and coviewing, consistent with previous studies. Hypothesis 5: Socio orientation will positively predict coviewing as reported by the parent. 650 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Because of socio orientation’s apparent emphasis on directives and control, it would be informative to know whether socio-oriented parents use an alternative strategy to express their disapproval of television through rule making regarding television, particularly if they do not use much negative mediation. Krcmar (1996) reported that when parents and children were asked to select a TV program to watch, parents and children among conceptoriented families had consultations, whereas parents directed the decision among socio-oriented families. Carlson et al. (1990) also found that controloriented parents tended to regulate children’s exposure to television. Both of these findings suggest that socio-oriented parents may make particular use of television advisory ratings. When Krcmar and Cantor (1997) compared the role of television ratings in decision-making processes for both parents and children, they found that parents mostly made negative comments about the ratings (e.g., “We don’t like this show”) and used more commands (e.g., “Don’t watch this”). Such comments are consistent with socio orientation, reflecting the belief that the parent is the one who makes decisions about the television shows their children will watch. This suggests that socio-oriented parents may be particularly likely to consult the television ratings system. Thus, we predict: Hypothesis 6: Socio orientation will positively predict parental use of the new TV ratings system in selecting programs for the child as reported by the parent. Methodology Sample A telephone survey (n = 216) was conducted with parents of third-, sixth-, and ninth-graders in two Washington state communities. Those parents (60% women, 40% men) whose child had completed an in-class survey prior to the phone survey were asked to participate in the study using a randomization procedure to obtain male and female respondents. The study obtained a 78% response rate. The survey indicated that 62% of the respondents had completed college. The respondents’ family income was relatively higher than the state median income, with 64% making more than $50,000 per year. Although an overwhelming majority indicated they were White (90%), the sample reflects the population of the state in terms of ethnic representation (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1995). 651 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 Measures The descriptive statistics for all variables used in the analysis, all of which were based on self-reported perceptions of beliefs and behaviors, are displayed in Appendix A. Socio (five items, alpha = .67) and concept orientations (five items, alpha = .65) were measured by the traditional family communication patterns inventory (e.g., Tims & Masland, 1985) on 4-point scales (4 = often, 1 = never). Positive and negative mediation were measured by the mediation questions used in Austin et al.’s (1999, 2000) studies. Positive mediation (six items, Cronbach’s alpha = .64) and negative mediation (five items, Cronbach’s alpha = .68) were measured on 4-point scales (4 = often, 1 = never). Parent-child discussion (four items, Cronbach’s alpha = .70) was measured by asking parents how often they talk about television, advertisements, and health-related issues such as alcohol and tobacco using a 4-point scale (4 = often, 1 = never). Coviewing (five items, Cronbach’s alpha = .66) was measured by asking how many days of the week they watch prime time, sports, educational TV, news, and TV movies with their child. In addition, five demographic variables, including child’s gender, child’s grade (third, sixth, or ninth), parent’s gender, parent’s formal education (highest education completed), and parent’s annual income, were measured and used as control variables. Analysis All the hypotheses were tested via hierarchical regression. Five control variables (child’s gender, child’s grade, parent’s gender, parent’s income, and parent’s education) were entered first, followed by the independent variables entered as a block. There were no significant relationships between demographic variables (child’s gender, child’ grade, parent’s gender, parent’s education, and parent’s income) and either socio orientation or concept orientation in the present sample except for one. Parent’s education (highest education completed) had a negative relationship with socio orientation, r = –.24, p = .001. Because of the nature of the sample, which overall was highly educated and included slightly more mothers (60%) than fathers, interaction of the demographic variables (child’s gender and grade and parent’s gender, education, and income) and the independent variables (socio orientation and concept orientation) were assessed. The analysis was conducted through moderated regression in which all interaction terms were entered after the additive effects of each predictor had been entered. 652 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Results Descriptive Statistics Mean scores for all variables and indices used in the analysis are displayed in Appendix A. Socio orientation and concept orientation were unrelated (r = –.05, ns). Hypothesis Testing Results of hierarchical multiple regression tests of hypotheses are shown in Table 1. The test of Hypothesis 1 examined whether concept orientation would predict parent involvement in positive mediation. As predicted and as shown in Table 1, higher scores on concept orientation did predict more frequent positive mediation, β = .22, t(175) = 2.98, p = .003. Hypothesis 2 stated that socio orientation reported by parents would lead parents to engage in positive mediation. As hypothesized, socio orientation was found to be a significant predictor of positive mediation, β = .27, t(175) = 3.53, p = .001. Also, although unanticipated, the study found the interaction between socio orientation and education (B = –.15, p < .05) a significant predictor for positive mediation, R2change = .02, F = 2.27, df = (2, 167), p = .11. The overall equation including the interaction term reached R2 = .17, F (9, 167) = 3.78, p < .001. Following the procedure recommended by Aiken and West (1991), we obtained the slopes (unstandardized betas) for the socio orientation variable at the high level (b = –.71) and low level (b = –1.32) of education. The educational level was determined by 1 SD below (low) or above (high) the mean education level. The test of Hypothesis 3 assessed whether concept orientation would predict parental involvement in negative mediation. As expected and as shown in Table 1, higher scores on concept orientation did predict more frequent negative mediation, β = .26, t(177) = 3.47, p = .001. The test of the research question assessed the relationship between socio orientation and negative mediation. Socio orientation was not related to negative mediation, β = .04, t(177) = .52, ns. Hypothesis 4 stated that higher scores in concept orientation reported by parents would predict more parent-child discussion of a variety of issues. Consistent with the hypothesis, concept orientation was found to be a positive predictor of discussion, β = .19, t(176) = 2.59, p = .01. 653 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 Table 1 Results of Hierarchical Regression Tests of Hypotheses Dependent Variables Independent Variables Positive mediation Socio orientation Concept orientation Socio × Education Socio × Concept Negative mediation Parent’s gender Socio orientation Concept orientation Coviewing Education Child’s gender Income Socio orientation Concept orientation Discussion Socio orientation Concept orientation TV ratings use Education Socio orientation Concept orientation R2 Change F Change .11 10.57*** .02 2.27 .02 .07 3.95* 6.08** .08 .03 .02 .06 Beta df F Probability .27*** (7, 169) 0.22** –.15* (9, 167) 0.04 4.15*** .15* .04 0.26** 3.95* 5.45** (1, 177) (3, 175) 14.32*** –.28*** (1, 174) 5.21* –.16* (2, 173) 5.12* –.17* (3, 172) 6.28** .23** (5, 170) .12 .04 3.76* –.06 .19** (2, 175) .04 .07 6.38* 6.84*** –.19** (1, 173) .28*** (3, 171) –.02 3.78*** 14.32*** 9.94*** 8.49*** 7.92*** 3.76* 6.38* 6.83*** Note. Control variables (child’s gender and grade and parent’s education and income) are entered first using the stepwise procedure; thus, they enter only when they are significant. Standardized betas reported from the block of entry. Independent variables entered using the forced entry procedure in a single block. Interaction terms entered using the forced entry procedure in a final block. When interactions were examined, control variables were forced into a block previous to the interaction terms. Only the interaction statistics associated with a block that included statistically significant entries are reported, including a nonsignificant entry if it was entered in the same block as a significant interaction. Higher number indicates higher, more frequent, or positive. Gender: male = 0; female = 1. *p ≤ .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001. The analysis also supported Hypothesis 5, which proposed that socio orientation would predict coviewing. As expected and as shown in Table 1, parents who were high in socio orientation watched television with their child more frequently, β = .23, t(174) = 3.17, p = .002. Concept orientation was unrelated to parent-child coviewing, β = .12, t(174) = 1.67, ns. The test of Hypothesis 6 examined whether socio orientation reported by parents would predict parental use of the television ratings system in selecting television programs for the child. Consistent with the prediction, those parents high in socio orientation tended to use the TV ratings system, β = .28, t(173) = 3.68, p < .001. Being high in concept orientation had no influence on parental use of the TV ratings system, β = –.02, t(173) = –.23, ns. 654 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Discussion Research has suggested that parental input can effectively counteract possible negative influences of media messages on children while cultivating their potential for positive effects. Research also has suggested that some types of parental input can make worrisome media effects more likely. To understand when and how parents make use of more or less productive strategies, it is valuable to explore how mediation patterns reflect more general patterns of communication within the family. As a result, this study examined the relationship between family communication style and parental use of different mediation strategies. Findings To investigate the links between general family communication norms and television-specific behaviors, this study tested hypothesized relationships between family communication patterns constructs and parental mediation strategies, coviewing, and the use of the new television ratings system. As hypothesized, concept orientation predicted parents’ frequent use of both negative mediation (counter-reinforcement of television messages) and of positive mediation (endorsement of television messages) as well as frequent discussion of issues more generally. Also as hypothesized, socio orientation predicted frequent use of positive mediation and coviewing. No relationship was predicted or found between socio orientation and discussion-oriented behavior such as negative mediation or general discussion of issues. As hypothesized, socio-oriented parents also reported more frequent use of the TV ratings systems in TV program selection for the child. General Implications of the Results Previous research on the effects of parental mediation has indicated that it is discussion-based parental mediation more than coviewing or rule making that helps equip the child with critical skills necessary to combat possible negative media influence (Alexander, 1990; Andreasen, 1990; Austin, 1993a; Austin et al., 1990; Nathanson, 1999). Discussion that encourages reflection has been shown especially beneficial for children’s development of communication competence, which assists with problem solving, academic achievement, and self-control (Burleson et al., 1995). Unlike negative mediation, positive mediation appears to take place on a happenstance basis in response to 655 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 frequent enjoyment of programming (Austin et al., 1999; Valkenburg et al., 1999). As a result, positive mediation appears to heighten potential persuasive effects of media messages, particularly to the extent it occurs together with increased viewing and a less reflective strategy of parental discussion. This seems to be confirmed by Austin and Chen’s (in press) and Austin et al.’s (2000) findings that parental reinforcement of media messages was associated with increased students’ expectancies and desirability for drinking alcohol, which in turn related to more consumption of alcohol. The results of this study relate these mediation strategies to socio and concept orientations, which have previously been connected to media-related outcomes such as persuadability and public affairs involvement. The findings suggest that communication style and media-related effects are related partly as a function of how they translate into discussion strategies regarding media messages. Implications Regarding How Mediation Strategies Reflect Family Processes These results support the view that family communication styles provide a consistent, sometimes subtle guide for a child’s behavior and information seeking. In particular, the results of this study further confirm the utility of the family communication patterns model when studying family life and the media, even given the methodological and conceptual limitations discussed by a number of scholars. The FCP constructs do appear to represent a communication structure that provides a frame of reference to family members to guide the construction of social reality in new situations, as asserted by McLeod and Chaffee (1972). These contexts include the child’s exposure to and interpretation of television content. For example, the results suggest that socio orientation can be seen as the expression of a parent’s desire to seek conformity and quiescence through the assertion of power and control over family members. This mode of communication appears to transfer to the ways parents interact with their children regarding television. For example, socio-oriented parents tend to make rules or use TV ratings systems, but they engage in less critical discussion. This is problematic because open discussion that encourages reflection seems important to the development of valuable media literacy skills. These findings therefore add support to the contention that family interaction regarding television and the media both reflects and affects family dynamics and socialization processes in a broader sense. Andreasen (1990) noted that communication during television viewing and about television 656 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication teaches children what kind of informational help they can get from their parents more generally. The finding that concept orientation relates to the tendency to discuss a variety of issues supports that view. Other scholarship also reinforces the value of considering mediation in the context of family processes more generally. Alexander (1990) noted that even beyond the extent to which families watch television and discuss it, television helps define the context of family interaction as well as the meaning of that interaction. Moreover, television can help facilitate interpersonal communication for purposes such as conflict management, relaxation, maintenance of family solidarity, and the transmission of values (Carlson et al., 1990; Lull, 1990; Moore & Moschis, 1983). As Lull (1990) demonstrated, families use television in a number of ways that includes the exercise of authority or the opportunity for discussion. Implications of the Findings for Parents’ Management of Media Influences Scholars have suggested that parental influence is most effective when parents exhibit management styles based on explanation and interplay along with psychological support (Gecas, 1981, 1992). A child is more receptive to parental influence attempts when receiving parental support (e.g., warmth via daily conversation) along with direction (e.g., critical discussion and viewing skills). Parental mediation given in an open communication environment thus may be a key to obtaining a child’s acceptance of a parent’s intended interpretation of television messages. Current research suggests that if the goal of parental mediation is to teach children critical skills to help a child cope with media messages, it seems that this can be best achieved when (a) parents engage in mediation that encourages reflection; (b) a child interprets the messages as the parents intend, which also requires open discussion; and (c) a child willingly accepts the messages given by the parents or model examples endorsed by the parents. The data from this study suggest that concept-oriented parenting leads to mediation strategies that accomplish these objectives. The results further suggest that socio-oriented parenting does not prevent parents from engaging in beneficial mediation strategies, but it may in some cases lead to counterproductive results. Socio-oriented parents, therefore, may have more need for media literacy interventions. Concept-oriented parents appear to have an advantage when it comes to managing media influences. The use of negative mediation may be necessary but not sufficient to produce favorable outcomes, and concept-oriented 657 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 parents appear to use both positive and negative mediation. Positive mediation appears to have value despite its potential drawbacks because parents need to point out exemplary characters and behaviors to which children might wish to aspire rather than focusing only on the negative aspects of media portrayals. Moreover, because the reality of media messages is that they excel at portraying desirable imagery, parents who only condemn messages may lose credibility. This could be why concept-oriented parents make use of both positive and negative mediation strategies, although these data have not explicitly investigated the motivations behind the relationships between concept orientation and mediation strategies. Scholars and activists attempting to encourage parental involvement in children’s media use and interpretation may find it useful to consider the extent to which mediation patterns reflect more general patterns of communication in the family. For example, to the extent negative mediation reflects concept orientation, media activists might question whether they should focus their efforts on encouraging discussion of television specifically or whether they should emphasize open discussion of issues more generally. Austin (1993a), based on children’s reports, found that parental active mediation (critical discussion), not family communication patterns, significantly affected a child’s level of skepticism (critical viewing skills). Those results, however, did not examine reports of positive and negative mediation separately. The current study, based on parents’ reports, suggests that general communication styles have indirect influence on a child’s skepticism through positive or negative mediation variables. Limitations and Recommendations for Further Study Parental mediation appears to comprise a relatively consistent indicator of the family socialization environment and more specifically of communication style. This merits further investigation because these data reflected parents’ self-reports of general communication behaviors regarding television rather than communication from both parents and children about specific topics or programs. The level of generality in these data may have obscured a complex variety of behaviors and interpretations based on differing perspectives. This study has identified relationships between the FCP orientation styles and mediation strategies but far from perfect correspondence between them. It would be useful to investigate the results of consistency or inconsistencies between general communication styles and mediation strategies. Concept orientation in conjunction with negative mediation may be an 658 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication especially useful form for parental input because it represents critical evaluation in combination with warmth and open discussion. Indeed, Krcmar (1996) reported that the concept-oriented style, which was less prescriptive, elicited more compliance from children regarding program selection than the socio-oriented style, which was more dictatorial. These findings also suggest a number of other avenues for further study. For example, this study did not incorporate the child’s perspective, which should be considered to achieve a better understanding of the relationships among communication norms, mediation patterns, and children’s beliefs and behaviors (Austin, 1992; Ritchie, 1991). It also is important to explore how beliefs and behaviors relevant to family communication patterns and mediation differ as a child grows older. No grade-related differences were found in this study, but other studies (e.g., Fortman, Clarke, & Austin, 1998; Meadowcroft, 1986) have demonstrated age-related differences in communication style and mediation strategies. Further research with fathers also would be advisable given that mothers were slightly overrepresented in this sample. It also should be noted that this study was based on generally highly educated parents’ reports about their communication patterns and mediation behavior. These data indicate that some relationships among education, mediation strategies, and FCP orientations exist that merit further exploration. For example, the results suggested that socio orientation has both a direct influence on positive mediation and also an interaction effect that is conditional on parent’s education. It appears that socio orientation works against parental engagement in positive mediation for those parents with high education. That is, socio-oriented parents with high education may be less likely to engage in positive mediation. Together, this and previous studies suggest that although general communication styles and mediation strategies are distinct, FCP styles affect parental choices of specific mediation strategies and children’s responses to the parents’ mediation messages. Future research needs to examine this link by using both parents’ and children’s reports along with observational or other behaviorally based data to achieve a better understanding of the processes involved in parental mediation effects. Conclusion This study has established that it is fruitful to explore connections between family communication patterns and parental involvement in specific mediation strategies. An open family communication style, commonly known as concept orientation, tends to be associated with critical viewing and open 659 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 discussion between parents and children. A style such as socio, emphasizing conformity, associates with reinforcement of messages and rule making but not discussion-oriented parental guidance regarding television messages. That general family communication patterns are reflected in parental choices of specific mediation strategies is especially informative for educators and media literacy practitioners who aim to enhance the effectiveness of parental input about media as a socialization tool. Parents’ daily interaction with a child can help alleviate negative media influences and promote positive media influences. A better understanding of the relationships among parenting styles, mediation strategies, and socialization outcomes should help parents better harness this potential. Appendix A Means, Standard Deviations, and Reliabilities of Variables Variable Index: Negative mediation How often you Say that something you’ve seen somebody do on TV is not OK Say that something you’ve seen in a TV ad is not OK Say that something on TV is not real Tell your child more about something you’ve seen on TV Explain to your child what ads on TV are trying to do Index: Positive mediation How often you Say you like a product in a TV ad Say you like a person or character on TV Say you agree with something on TV Say that something on TV often happens in real life Repeat something you’ve heard on TV Index: Socio orientation Tell your child your ideas are correct and that they shouldn’t argue with you? Answer your child’s argument by saying they’ll know better when they grow up? Tell your child not to say things that make people angry? Tell your child there are some things that just shouldn’t be talked about? Tell your child they shouldn’t argue with adults? 660 N Mean SD Range Alpha 214 3.39 0.47 1 to 4 214 3.43 0.64 1 to 4 214 214 3.23 3.25 0.78 0.81 1 to 4 1 to 4 214 3.76 0.51 1 to 4 214 212 3.27 2.65 0.70 0.46 1 to 4 1 to 4 212 212 212 2.24 2.89 2.97 0.76 0.66 0.56 1 to 4 1 to 4 1 to 4 212 212 210 2.57 2.57 2.24 0.79 0.76 0.59 1 to 4 1 to 4 1 to 4 210 2.28 0.93 1 to 4 210 2.75 0.93 1 to 4 210 1.80 0.94 1 to 4 210 2.41 0.94 1 to 4 210 1.93 0.80 1 to 4 .69 .65 .67 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Appendix A (continued) Variable Index: Concept orientation Tell your child they should look at both sides of an issue? Encourage your child to question other people’s opinions? Tell your child getting their ideas across is important even if others don’t like it? Tell your child every member of the family should have some say in family matters? Say kids know more about some things than adults do? Index: Discussion Please indicate whether you talk about each of the following things with your child Talk about TV Talk about ads Talk about alcohol Talk about tobacco or smoking Index: Coviewing How often in the past week Watched prime time TV together Watched sports programs together Watched an educational show Watched news programs Watched a movie Income Education Grade N Mean SD Range Alpha 209 3.32 0.47 1 to 4 209 3.72 0.54 1 to 4 209 3.29 0.73 1 to 4 209 3.28 0.86 1 to 4 209 3.62 0.66 1 to 4 209 213 2.72 3.22 0.87 0.57 1 to 4 1 to 4 .70 213 213 213 213 211 3.50 3.04 3.09 3.24 1.64 0.69 0.84 0.79 0.82 1.20 1 to 4 1 to 4 1 to 4 1 to 4 0 to 7 .66 211 211 211 211 211 210 211 200 2.22 1.14 1.51 1.84 1.49 3.59 3.95 1.78 2.00 1.69 1.69 2.20 1.57 1.10 0.97 0.66 0 to 7 0 to 7 0 to 7 0 to 7 0 to 7 a 1 to 5 b 2 to 5 c 1 to 3 .65 Note. High score indicates an answer in agreement with the statement or a more frequent behavior. a. Income: 1 = < $15,000; 2 = $15,001 to $34,999; 3 = $35,000 to $49,999; 4 = $50,000 to $79,999; 5 = more than $80,000. b. Education: 1 = less than high school; 2 = high school graduate; 3 = some college; 4 = college graduate; 5 = graduate work. c. 1 = third grade; 2 = sixth grade; 3 = ninth grade. Notes 1. Ritchie (1991) and Fitzpatrick (Ritchie & Fitzpatrick, 1990) also argued that the family communication patterns (FCP) cannot validly assume a shared view of perceived communication patterns between parent and child, and a number of studies has shown considerable differences in perceptions (Austin, 1993b; Tims & Masland, 1985). In response to these concerns, Ritchie and Fitzpatrick (1990) proposed a revised FCP model that measures perceived parental control (socio orientation) and 661 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 communication (concept orientation) within the family. The development of their measurement model relied on adolescent respondents and appears too complex for use with young children, for whom the force of parental communication patterns presumably would have the most potency. Nevertheless, Ritchie’s (1991; Ritchie & Fitzpatrick, 1990) analysis of the FCP model is useful for clarifying what FCP measures represent from each family member’s perspective with results that correspond with Austin’s (1993b) analysis of the FCP with children as young as 10. 2. Positive mediation generally parallels the construct of “social coviewing” proposed by Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, and Marseille (1999), which focuses on watching together for enjoyment purposes rather than instructional purposes. The two conceptualizations, however, do embody important distinctions. Whereas Valkenburg et al. combined the act of coviewing with positive affect, the Austin, Fujioka, Bolls, and Engelbertson (1999) positive mediation construct focuses on affirmative commentary and does not assume concurrent viewing. The Valkenburg et al. scale focuses on the act of coviewing, whereas the Austin et al. construct focuses on the act of discussion. The emphasis on discussion and its valence in the Austin et al. conceptualization of mediation further distinguishes it from the Valkenburg et al. conceptualization. The “instructive/evaluative” mediation construct proposed by Valkenburg et al. parallels and extends Austin’s (1993a) earlier mediation index, which combined nonvalenced and negatively oriented commentary. Although Austin et al. found that the negative and positive items loaded on different factors in a principal components analysis, Valkenburg et al.’s measures loaded on a single factor. Scholars have not yet compared the two largely complementary conceptualizations simultaneously to verify and refine the distinctions between the two approaches. References Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Alexander, A. (1990). Television and family interaction. In J. Bryant (Ed.), Television and the American family (pp. 211-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Alexander, A., & Fry, V. H. (1990). Interpreting viewing: Creating an acceptable context. In W. A. E. S. Thomas (Ed.), Communication and culture: Language, performance, technology and media (pp. 236-243). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Andreasen, M. S. (1990). Evolution in the family’s use of television: Normative data from industry and academe. In J. Bryant (Ed.), Television and the American family (pp. 3-55). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Atkin, D., Greenberg, B. S., & Baldwin, T. F. (1991). The home ecology of children’s television viewing: Parental mediation and the new video environment. Journal of Communication, 41, 40-52. Austin, E. W. (1992). Parent-child TV interaction: The importance of perspective. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 36, 359-361. Austin, E. W. (1993a). Exploring the effects of active parental mediation of television content. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 37, 147-158. 662 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Austin, E. W. (1993b). The importance of perspective in parent-child interpretations of family communication patterns. Journalism Quarterly, 70, 558-568. Austin, E. W. (1995). Direct and indirect influences of parent-child communication norms on adolescent’s tendencies to take preventive measures for AIDS and drug abuse. In G. Kreps & D. O’Hair (Eds.), Relational communication and health outcomes (pp. 163-183). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Austin, E. W., & Chen, Y. J. (in press). The relationship of parental reinforcement of media messages to college students’ alcohol-related behaviors. Journal of Health Communication. Austin, E. W., Fujioka, Y., Bolls, P., & Engelbertson, J. (1999). How and why parents taking on the tubes. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43, 175-192. Austin, E. W., Knaus, C., & Meneguelli, A. (1998). Who talks how to their kids about TV: A clarification of demographic correlates of parental mediation patterns. Communication Research Reports, 14, 418-430. Austin, E. W., & Nach-Ferguson, B. (1995). Sources and influences of young school-age children’s general and brand-specific knowledge about alcohol. Health Communication, 7, 1-20. Austin, E. W., Pinkleton, B. E., & Fujioka, Y. (2000). The role of interpretation processes and parental discussion in the media’s effects on adolescents’ use of alcohol. Pediatrics, 105, 343-349. Austin, E. W., Roberts, D. F., & Nass, C. I. (1990). Influences of family communication on children’s television-interpretation processes. Communication Research, 17, 545-564. Baumrind, D. (1968). Authoritarian vs. authoritative parental control. Adolescence, 3, 255-272. Bower, R. T. (1973). Television and the public. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Burleson, B. R., Delia, J. G., & Applegate, J. L. (1995). The socialization of person-centered communication: Parents’ contributions to their children’s social-cognitive and communication skills. In M. A. Fitzpatrick & A. L. Vangelisti (Eds.), Explaining family interactions (pp. 34-76). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Carlson, L., Grossbart, S. L., & Walsh, A. (1990). Mothers’ communication orientation and consumer socialization tendencies. Journal of Advertising, 19, 27-39. Chaffee, S., McLeod, J. M., & Atkin, C. K. (1971). Parental influences on adolescent media use. American Behavioral Scientist, 14, 323-340. Chaffee, S., McLeod, J., & Wackman, D. (1973). Family communication patterns and adolescent political participation. In J. Dennis (Ed.), Socialization to politics: Selected readings (pp. 349-364). New York: John Wiley. Comstock, G. (1975). The evidence so far. Journal of Communication, 25, 25-34. Corder-Bolz, C. R. (1980). Mediation: The role of significant others. Journal of Communication, 30, 106-118. 663 COMMUNICATION RESEARCH • December 2002 Desmond, R. J., Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1990). Family mediation: Parental communication patterns and the influence of television on children. In J. Bryant (Ed.), Television and the American family (pp. 293-310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Desmond, R. J., Singer, J. L., Singer, D. G., Calam, R., & Colimore, K. (1985). Family mediation patterns and television viewing: Young children’s use and grasp of the medium. Human Communication Research, 11, 461-480. Flay, B. R., & Sobel, J. L. (1983). The role of mass media in preventing adolescent substance abuse. In T. J. Glynn, C. G. Leukefeld, & J. P. Ludford (Eds.), Preventing adolescent drug abuse (DHHS Publication No. ADM 83-1280, pp. 5-35). Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services. Fortman, K. K. J., Clarke, T. L., & Austin, E. W. (1998). Let’s talk about what we’re watching: Parental behavior towards children’s gender and age regarding television viewing. Communication Research Reports, 15, 413-425. Gecas, V. (1981). Context of socialization. In M. Rosenberg & R. Turner (Eds.), Social psychology: Sociological perspectives (pp. 165-199). New York: Basic Books. Gecas, V. (1992). Socialization. In E. F. Borgatta & M. L. Borgatta (Eds.), Encyclopedia of sociology (Vol. 4, pp. 1863-1872). New York: Macmillan. Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1996). Television and socialization of young children. In T. M. MacBeth (Ed.), Tuning in to young viewers: Social science perspectives on television (pp. 37-60). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kandel, D., & Logan, J. A. (1984). Patterns of drug use from adolescence to young adulthood. I. Periods of risk for initiation, continued use and discontinuation. American Journal of Public Health, 74, 660-666. Krcmar, M. (1996). Family communication patterns, discourse behavior, and child television viewing. Human Communication Research, 23, 251-277. Krcmar, M., & Cantor, J. (1997). The role of television advisories and ratings in parent-child discussion of television viewing choices. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 41, 393-411. Lull, J. (1990). Families’ social uses of television as extensions of the household. In J. Bryant (Ed.), Television and the American family (pp. 59-72). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lyle, J., & Hoffman, H. R. (1972). Children’s use of television and other media. In E. A. Rubinstein, G. A. Comstock, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television and social behavior: Vol. 4. Television in day-to-day life: Patterns of use (pp. 129256). Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. McKechnie, R. J. (1977). Parents, children and learning to drink. In R. J. McKechnie (Ed.), Alcoholism and drug dependence (pp. 451-456). New York: Plenum. McLeod, J. M., & Chaffee, S. (1972). The construction of social reality. In J. S. Tedeschi (Ed.), The social influence processes (pp. 50-99). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine. Meadowcroft, J. (1986). The impact of family communication patterns on political development: The child’s role. Communication Research, 13, 603-624. Medrich, E. A. (1979). Constant television: A background to daily life. Journal of Communication, 29, 171-176. 664 Fujioka, Austin • Family Communication Messaris, P. (1982). Parents, children, and television. In G. Gumpert & R. Cathcart (Eds.), Inter/Media (2nd ed., pp. 580-598). New York: Oxford University Press. Messaris, P., & Kerr, D. (1983). Mothers’ comments about TV: Relation to family communication patterns. Communication Research, 10, 175-194. Moore, R. L., & Moschis, G. P. (1983). Role of mass media and the family in development of consumption norms. Journalism Quarterly, 60, 67-73. Nathanson, A. I. (1999). Identifying and explaining the relationship between parental mediation and children’s aggression. Communication Research, 26, 124-143. Ritchie, L. D. (1991). Family communication patterns: An epistemic analysis and conceptual reinterpretation. Communication Research, 18, 548-565. Ritchie, L. D., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1990). Family communication patterns: Measuring intrapersonal perceptions of interpersonal relationship. Communication Research, 17, 523-544. Tims, A. R., & Masland, J. L. (1985). Measurement of family communication patterns. Communication Research, 12, 35-57. U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1995). Statistical abstract of the United States 1993 (115th ed.). Washington, DC: Department of Commerce, Economic and Statistics Administration. Valkenburg, P. M., Krcmar, M., Peeters, A. L., & Marseille, N. M. (1999). Developing a scale to assess three styles of television mediation: “Instructive mediation,” “restrictive mediation,” and “social coviewing.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43, 52-66. 665
© Copyright 2024