Living with Animals 2 - Eastern Kentucky University

Living with Animals 2: Interconnections
Co-organized by Robert W. Mitchell, Radhika N. Makecha, & Michał Piotr Pręgowski
Eastern Kentucky University, Richmond, Kentucky, 19-21 March 2015
Conference overview
Each day begins with a keynote speaker, and follows with two tracks that run concurrently.
Locations: All talks are in the Crabbe Library. You enter the library from outside on the second
floor. If you follow straight through doorways from the outside, you will eventually arrive at the
Grand Reading Room, which is on the second floor. Room 108 is located on the first floor (the
basement), Rooms 201, 204G and 208 are located on the second floor, and the Saturday buffet
lunch and poster presentations are located on the third floor. If lost, just ask someone for help.
Coffee breaks: Breakfast foods, snacks and coffee/tea/water are available throughout the day.
Book display: Throughout the conference in Library Room 201, there is a book display. Several
university presses have generously provided books for your perusal (as well as order sheets), and
some conference participants will be displaying their books as well.
Art displays: We are pleased to have visual art in diverse media during the conference. Peter
Sherman’s ceramic animal sculptures, and Julia Schosser’s photography about human-animal
interaction and John Hochensmith’s photography concerning horses, can found in different areas
in the library during the conference. Claudia Medina’s film is presented on Friday afternoon. In
addition, during the poster sessions, Linda Brant will display her artwork memorializing animals,
and Lyn Miles will present artwork created by the sign-using orangutan Chantek, whose
development Lyn supported and studied starting when Chantek was quite young.
Thursday features the “Living with Horses” sessions, as well as concurrent sessions, and has an
optional (pre-paid) trip to Berea for shopping and dinner at the Historic Boone Tavern Restaurant.
Friday features the “Teaching with Animals” sessions throughout the morning and early
afternoon (which includes a boxed lunch during panel discussions and a movie showing and
discussion); “Living with Animals” sessions continuing in the late afternoon, and a Conference
Dinner at Masala Indian restaurant.
Saturday includes “Living with Animals” sessions throughout the day with intervening Poster
Presentations during a buffet lunch. In addition, there is the optional trip to the White Hall State
Historic Site (you pay when you arrive at the site).
Sunday includes an optional (pre-paid) trip to the Kentucky Horse Park.
NOTE: Boxed lunch, conference dinner, and buffet lunch are included in registration fee.
Parking on campus is located ONLY in the Commuter section of the Alumni Coliseum parking lot.
Be sure to avoid parking in the yellow Faculty E parking locations in the Alumni Coliseum lot, or you
may be towed. Saturday parking, however, can be anywhere on campus. Please contact us if you need
special assistance in traveling to and from locations.
Shuttle schedule is in the folder. Foothills Shuttle, phone: 859-624-3236, M-F, 8:30am-4:30pm.
You may also contact David Sowder at 859-893-4363 if you are having shuttle difficulties.
Posters can be put up on Saturday morning. Posters will be attached to a 3 feet x 4 feet poster
board on an easel. Pushpins will be provided.
Talks (other than keynotes) will be 20 minutes long, presumably 15 minutes for the presentation,
and 5 minutes for questions. If you wish to arrange your 20 minutes differently (e.g., 18 minutes
for presentation, 2 minutes for questions), speak with your session chair before your session.
Thursday, 19 March 2015
9:00-9:20am
Grand Reading Room
EKU President Michael T. Benson
Welcome to EKU
Robert W. Mitchell, Radhika N. Makecha, & Michał Pręgowski
Welcome to Living with Animals 2: Interconnections
9:20-10:25am
Radhika N. Makecha
Introduction to Ian Duncan
Ian Duncan
Asking the Animals
Living with Horses
10:45-10:50am
Angela Hofstetter
Introduction to Living with Horses
Chair: Angela Hofstetter
Grand Reading Room
(for Concurrent sessions—see following page)
10:50-11:50am
Grand Reading Room
Gala Argent
“Babysitters” and “Schoolmasters”: The Interpersonal, Intersocial and Intercultural Implications
in Learning to Ride and Be Ridden
Gwyneth Talley
Of Stallions and Men: Moroccan Masculinity in Traditional Horseback-riding
Fabienne Meiers
The Urban Horse: Equestrian Traffic and Horse Husbandry in Late Medieval Cities
11:50am-1:10pm
Lunch (Lunch can be purchased in Powell Building; see map)
1:10-2:10pm
Grand Reading Room
Hannah M. Biggs
Horse Books for Kids: World War II Adolescent Fiction, Film, and Television
Jopi Nyman
Rereading Sentimentalism in Anna Sewell’s Black Beauty: Affect, Performativity, and Hybrid
Spaces
Sarah Tsiang
Breeds for Needs: Type and Breed Names as a Reflection of the Horse-Human Relationship
2:30-3:30pm
Grand Reading Room
Keri Cronin
“Mendacious Representations?”: The Camera as Witness in the Battle Over the Live Export of
Horses in Early 20th Century England
Jessica Dallow
A “Galaxy of Distinguished Horses”: Schreiber & Sons and the Emergence of Equine Portrait
Photography
Angela Hofstetter
Reel/Real Horses: Animals, Visual Pleasure, and Narrative Cinema
Thursday, 19 March 2015 (Concurrent sessions to “Living with Horses”)
Living with Elephants
Chair: Radhika N. Makecha
10:50-11:50am
Room 108
Catherine Doyle
Keeper-Elephant Relationships: A Discussion of Patterns found in Keeper Perception of the
Human-Elephant Relationship, and the Potential for Disconnect.
Preston Foerder
What Do Elephants Know and When Do They Know It?
Ratna Ghosal, Andre Ganswindt Polani B Seshagiri, & Raman Sukumar
Endocrine and Behavioural Correlates of Musth in Male Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus)
11:50am-1:10pm
Lunch (Lunch can be purchased in Powell Building; see map)
Emotions
Chair: Laura Newhart
1:10-2:10pm
Room 108
Theo Verheggen
Embodied Cognition and Affect Attunement in Anthrozoological Research
Michele Merritt
Depressed Dogs, Heartbroken Humans, and a New Philosophy of Emotions
Melissa Burns-Cusato, Brian Cusato, & Amanda Glueck
Threats from the Past: Barbados Green Monkeys Retain Fear of Ancestral Predators for over
350 Years
Living with Dogs
Chair: Michał Pręgowski
2:30-3:50pm
Room 108
Helena Pycior
Collective Memory of the “First Dogs”: Privilege and Power of the “First Families” of the
United States
Michał Piotr Pręgowski
Dog Training as Taming Beasts: Canine Science versus Whispering
Scott Hurley
The Dog Fancy: A Site for the Intersection of Ableist, Healthist, and Speciesist Ideologies
Erica Feuerbacher & Clive Wynne
Most Dogs Prefer Food…But Sometimes They Don’t: Effects of Familiarity, Context, and
Schedule on Dogs’ Preference for Food or Petting
Optional (Pre-paid) trip to Berea with Dinner: ~5:00-9:00pm
Friday, 20 March 2015
9:00-10:05am
Grand Reading Room
Robert W. Mitchell
Introduction to Julia Schlosser
Julia Schlosser
Walking the Dog: An Exploration of Recent Lens-Based Images of Companion Animals
Concurrent Teaching with Animals Sessions Follow:
Teaching with animals 1
Chair: Mary Trachsel
10:30-11:30am
Grand Reading Room
Mary Trachsel
Ecological Consciousness Raising: Animal Studies in the Anthropocene
Jeannette Vaught
Animal Infiltrations: Teaching Animal Studies in Traditional Courses
Jack Furlong & Ellen Furlong
Melding Justice and Science: An Interdisciplinary Course, “Ape Sapiens: Wild Minds and
Captive Dignity”
Teaching with animals 2
Chair: Stephanie McSpirit
10:30-11:30am
Room 108
Joseph Tuminello
Teaching with Foer's Eating Animals
Elizabeth A. Lorenzen
Let's Strike while the Iron is Hot! Using the Cause of Equine Welfare as a Vehicle for Teaching
Information Literacy
Susan Rustick
Transforming Human Identity: Encounters in the Classroom through Animal Eyes
~11:40pm
Pick up BOXED LUNCHES OUTSIDE Room 108
12:00-1:30
Room 108
Robert W. Mitchell, Anne Perkins, & Erica Feuerbacher
Developing the Animal Studies/Anthrozoology Curriculum
Short Movie “Animal Blessings” and discussion by filmmaker
1:50-2:30
Grand Reading Room
Claudia Medina
Animal Blessings: Rituals of Appreciation as Pathways to Ecological Reconnection
Friday, 20 March 2015
Concurrent Sessions:
Animal Agency
Chair: Amy Nelson
3:00-4:00pm
Room 108
Jeanne Dubino
Listening to the Dogs: Orhan Pamuk and the Mongrelization of Fiction
Laura Keith
Creatures of Warfare: The Use, Misuse and Agency of World War I Animals
Amy Nelson
Canine Agency in the Soviet Manned Spaceflight Program
Animals in Ecological Cultures
Chair: Ed Frederickson
3:00-4:00pm
Grand Reading Room
Benjamin Z. Freed
Pleistocene Humans and Canids: A View from Studies of Primate Polyspecific Associations
Robert Michael Morrissey
Tall-Grass Ethnohistory: Indians, Europeans, and Other Animals in the Prairie Borderlands
Ed Frederickson, An Peischel, Greg Brann & Rick Griebenow
Potential Applications for Targeted Grazing to Enhance Ecosystem Services and Rural
Economies in Eastern Kentucky
Concurrent Sessions:
Animal Agency (continued)
Chair: Sarah Tsiang
4:20-5:00pm
Room 108
Magdalen J. Walton
Killer Whales or Whale Killers? A Routine Activities Analysis Introducing Agency Among Orca
Whales during the Capture of Orca Calves
Linda J. Sumption
“The tiniest glance”: Narrative, Wildlife, and the Recognition of Intimacy
Imagining Alternatives
Chair: Brett Mizelle
4:20-5:20pm
Grand Reading Room
Kathryn Kirkpatrick
“Every Polar Bear Alive”: Representing Animals in the Sixth Extinction
Ziba Rashidian
Epistemological Artifacts, or Death and the Specimen: Nabokov’s Butterflies, for Example
Brett Mizelle
Mary Griffith’s Odd Future: Real and Imagined Human-Animal Relationships in Antebellum
America
Conference Dinner at Masala Indian Restaurant: ~6:00-9:00
Saturday, 21 March 2015
9:00-10:05am
Grand Reading Room
Michał Pręgowski
Introduction to Marie-José Enders-Slegers
Marie-José Enders-Slegers
The Human Animal Bond and Further Professionalizing of Human-Animal Interventions:
Theories, Results and Challenges
Concurrent Sessions (Morning)
Communication and Connection
Chair: Sara Waller
10:30-11:50am
Grand Reading Room
Jane Desmond & Maria Lux
Thinking “Big”: Collaborative Processes between Artists and Scholars for Public Art Production
Martha Robinson
Avian Encounters: Connecting with Bird Lives through Live Streaming and Contemporary Art
Linda Brant
American Pet Cemetery Gravestone Image Pairings: A Visual Strategy for Exploring
Interspecies Relationships
Sara Waller, Christopher Kloth, & Mariana Olsen
Cats Talk Back: Feral & Socialized
Making Decisions for Animals
Chair: Matthew Pianalto
10:30-11:30am
Room 108
Miranda K. Workman
Euthanasia Decisions in the Sheltering Industry - A Critical Inquiry
Hazuki Kajiwara
Strong Bonds, Ambiguous Futures: Responses to the Fukushima Nuclear Disaster in Japan
Debra Vey Voda-Hamilton
When People are in Conflict about Animals
Saturday, 21 March 2015
POSTER SESSION AND BUFFET LUNCH
11:50am-1:50pm
Posters will be presented during a Buffet Lunch in the Library, Third Floor.
Be sure to talk with presenters to learn about their work and ideas.
Linda Brant
Mourning the Unknown and Honoring the Unmourned
Autumn Costelle
Expanding Horizons: The Goals and Achievements of EKU’s Animal Studies Club
Elena Cox
Lead Poisoning in Raptors: Impacts of Game Hunting with Lead Ammunition
Verda A. Davis
The Need for Anthrozoology in Veterinary Technology Curricula
Guadalupe Delgado, Victor Pataky, Richard Ford, Brian Cusato, and Melissa
Burns-Cusato
Dangerous Liaisons: Human-Monkey Interactions at a Wildlife Reserve
Kate Ford & Ellen Furlong
Moral Reasoning in Dogs
Tabitha Foster
Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR) Making a Positive Impact on EKU’s Campus
Ashley Hammond
Opt to Adopt: Lexington Humane Society
Tia G. B. Hansen, Mai Andreasen, Åsa H. Jansson, & Runa E. Gjellan
Belief in Profit Animal Mind Predicts Attitude to Profit Animal Welfare
Elena Iokimanskaya (Елена Иокиманская)
A Brief Overview of the Stray Animal Problem in Russia
Jessica Kraut, Stephanie AuBuchon, Connor Hughes, & Ellen Furlong
Self-Control in Dogs
Shane Locker
The Effects of Human Interaction and other Enrichments on Captive Tiger Stereotypy and
Exploration
Radhika N. Makecha
Paper Mache Giraffes and Puzzle Box Feeders, These are a Few of my Favorite Things:
Teaching Animal Enrichment Using Traditional and Applied Avenues
H. Lyn White Miles & Ross van der Harst
The Art, The Artist: The Orangutan Chantek’s Paintings and Found Art Assemblages
Pegah Naghib
Effect of Music on Horses
KiriLi N. Stauch, Stephanie AuBuchon, & Ellen Furlong
Domestic Dogs’ Understanding of Intentional and Goal Oriented Action
Brenden Wall, Anthony Bohner, Jeffrey Toraason, & Ellen Furlong
Good Dog! APPlications of Dog Science
Lucinda Woodward
Research and Development of the Pet Attribute Work Sheet (PAWS—for dogs)
Miranda K. Workman & Christy L. Hoffman
An Evaluation of the Role the Internet Site Petfinder Plays in Cat Adoptions
Saturday, 21 March 2015
Concurrent Sessions (Early Afternoon)
Sanctuary/Shelter/Adoption
Chair: Elan Abrell
1:50-2:50pm
Grand Reading Room
Elan Abrell
Captive Freedom: Multispecies Ethics in US Animal Sanctuaries.
Jessica Austin
Moral Stress, Meaning-Making, and Mourning: How Shelter Employees Process Euthanasia
Jennifer Blevins Sinski
“A Cat-sized Hole in My Heart”: Public Perceptions of the Companion Animal Adoption
Process
Humans and Animals in Animal Assisted Interaction Chair: Tia Hansen
1:50-2:50pm
Room 108
Chalotte Glintborg & Tia G. B. Hansen
Importance of a Dog for Recovery after Acquired Brain Injury: Two Case Stories.
Martha Sherrill
Animal Assisted Therapy for Adults with Communication Disorders: An Ethnographic Approach
to Therapeutic Human/Animal Interactions.
Gillian Squirrell
Working Dogs Working Lives: Cost-Effectively Frustrating Human and Animal Disposability
Saturday, 21 March 2015
Concurrent Sessions (Late Afternoon)
Animal-Human Comparisons and Identities
Chair: Joshua Kercsmar
3:10-4:10pm
Room 108
Joshua Kercsmar
Managing Livestock and Slaves in Barbados, ca. 1650–1816
Reiko Ohnuma
Animal Doubles of the Buddha
Keridiana Chez
Canine Connections in George Eliot’s Adam Bede and Middlemarch
Valuing and Using Animals
Chair: Radhika N. Makecha
3:10-4:30pm
Grand Reading Room
Bob Sandmeyer
The Value of a Varmint
Erin McKenna
Loving Pets Means Caring for Livestock
Radhika N. Makecha, Kathleen M. Dudzinski, Stan A. Kuczaj II, Otto Fad, & John
Anderson
Animals in Captive Settings: What Can We Learn From Them?
Jonathan L. Clark
Uncharismatic Invasives
Conference Farewell!
4:30pm
Optional trip to White Hall State Historic Site: ~5:45-7:30pm
Sunday, 22 March 2015
Optional (Pre-paid) Trip to Horse Park, ~8:00am-1:00pm
Abstracts in Alphabetical Order by Author’s Last Name
Elan Abrell
Captive Freedom: Multispecies Ethics in US Animal Sanctuaries.
Department of Anthropology, CUNY Graduate Center, New York
[email protected]
In the last decade animal rights activists have established thousands of sanctuaries across the
United States in an attempt to save tens of thousands of animals from factory farms, roadside
zoos, and other situations where, activists believe, animals are neglected, abused, or unjustly
slaughtered. In addition to saving the lives of individual animals, these activists seek to
challenge conventional ideas about the proper treatment of animals in contemporary US society.
They thus typically establish sanctuaries with the goal of creating spaces where animals can live
out the rest of their lives relatively free from human control. This principle guides efforts to
create lived spaces in which animals are treated as fellow subjects with interests and needs equal
to those of their human co-habitators, in contradistinction to the ways animals are treated as
property in more conventional contexts. While modeling these alternate ways of living with
animals, all sanctuaries must also balance animals’ freedom against concerns for their safety and
well-being. Indeed, practices of care limit animals’ freedom in multiple ways, from movement
to medical care to diet. Even the finite spatiality of the sanctuary itself necessarily limits animal
freedom. How sanctuaries navigate these dilemmas of captivity, however, is determined by
animals as well as their human caretakers. This article examines several different sanctuaries’
approaches to balancing the tensions between animal wellbeing and freedom to illustrate how
animals influence this balancing act. Drawing on work in queer ecology and ecofeminism, it
further argues that human-animal engagements within sanctuaries queer conventional species
relations, creating multispecies communities in which animals can—at least to some extent—
influence the ethical, social, and material conditions of their own rescue and care.
Jessica Austin
Moral Stress, Meaning-Making, and Mourning: How Shelter Employees Process Euthanasia
Canisius College, Buffalo, NY
[email protected]
Animal shelter employees face each day with the possibility of inhabiting antithetical roles: the
caretaker, charged with ensuring the safety and well-being of the wards in their custody; and the
executioner, overseer of these same animals’ untimely deaths. With shelter euthanasia estimates
reaching nearly three million adoptable animals per year, shelter workers shoulder a considerable
burden of grief, resulting in stress and manifesting in depression and even physical complaints,
such as sleep disturbance and headaches. While several authors describe coping mechanisms for
those whose work involves death, both in general and specifically tailored toward shelter
employees, little is written about how shelter workers mourn the animals they euthanize, and
how grieving for these beings shapes their sense of self. Separating coping from mourning
entails examining both the proximate mechanisms that shelter personnel employ in order to
continue to fulfill occupational obligations, as well as the ultimate emotional effects of ongoing
loss and how they influence understanding of one’s beliefs and values. In this study, semistructured interviews with shelter staff explored the myriad emotions that inform shelter workers’
outlook on their role in animal death, and how personal mourning rituals and practices occur and
empower them to continue in their bipolar role as both protector and life-taker. This presentation
will discuss the findings and perspectives gathered from six shelter employees who reported in
equal part mourning, not mourning, and mourning only specific animals. Ignoring the possibility
of mourning does a disservice to these individuals and perhaps quashes some of the benefits to
be realized both to employees and the animals who they may advocate for even more strongly
through forging a new sense of purpose. Strategies will be discussed for shelters to pay heed to
the benefits of recognizing both coping and mourning in their employees.
Hannah M. Biggs
Horse Books for Kids: World War II Adolescent Fiction, Film, and Television
Rice University, Houston, Texas
[email protected]
As World War II raged on, a new genre of children’s literature was developing out of both the
U.S. and England. This genre was the ‘horse book’: stories about children and their horses. Enid
Bagnold’s National Velvet was published shortly before WWII in 1935 in England; Mary
O’Hara’s My Friend Flicka was published in 1941 in the U.S.; Walter Farley’s The Black
Stallion in 1941, again in the U.S.; and Marguerite Henry’s, Misty of Chincoteague wrapped up
the influx of wartime horse literature out of the U.S. two years after the war’s end, published in
1947.
These four books are still considered reading staples for every equestrian youth. But the
question begs: what about the wartime era fostered stories of children and horses? And what is it
about the romantic aura of a child and his/her horse that is so attractive to children’s book
authors of the mid-20th century?
There is something intrinsic about the story of a child and his/her horse that captivated
wartime children, both on the page and later on the screen. This paper will parse out those
captivations, these four major authors’ fascination with the child-horse protagonist pair, and posit
that wartime children’s equestrian literature (starting in WWII) exemplifies some of the earliest,
most under recognized 20th-century portrayals of human-animal kinship. These kids’ books place
the entire emphasis of the novel on the intrinsic connection between a human and his/her animal.
In a period fraught with war and crumbling human and family relationships, the stuff of
children’s dreams at night, a child’s daytime playmate and, ultimately, his/her fantasy world of
fiction were populated and made better by an animal, a horse.
Linda Brant
Mourning the Unknown and Honoring the Unmourned
Independent Artist & Part-time Faculty Ringling College of Art and Design Sarasota, Florida
[email protected]
Is it possible to mourn the loss of unknown animals? How can animals that most people do not
consider “grievable” be recognized and honored? I explore these ideas through the process of
cleaning, polishing and re-presenting the bones of typically unmourned animals such as cow, pig,
lamb, turkey, and chicken. To clean is to purge, purify, detoxify, chasten, sanctify, improve, or
absolve. In many cultures, ritualistic exhumation and cleaning of human bones is considered an
act of love and reverence. Could the process of cleaning, sanding and polishing the bones of
common animals be similarly construed? Might it also be interpreted as an act of mourning,
honoring or atonement? The process of mourning the unknown and honoring the unmourned is
investigated through text, photographs and sculpture in this ‘hands on’ poster presentation.
Linda Brant
American Pet Cemetery Gravestone Image Pairings: A Visual Strategy for Exploring
Interspecies Relationships
Independent Artist & Part-time Faculty Ringling College of Art and Design Sarasota, Florida
[email protected]
The process of honoring can take many forms, ranging from traditional burials and funerary rites
to everyday acts such as story-telling, picture-making and memory production. Its traces can be
observed in American pet cemetery gravestones. Prior to the 1960s, animals were rarely honored
with individual gravestones. If they were, the stones were simple, indicating only the first names
of pets. As the quality of human and companion animal relationships changed, so too did pet
cemetery gravestones. Some of the major changes on gravestones include giving human first
names and surnames to pets, references to pets as family members, religious references and
symbols, references to the afterlife, the addition of color photographs and even websites for pets
(Brandes). Over the past two years, I have taken dozens of photographs of pet cemetery
gravestones in rural and suburban Florida cemeteries. I have selected the most compelling photos
and paired them with images from contemporary culture with the aim of highlighting the myriad
contradictions and inconsistencies in our treatment of nonhumans. In my presentation, I will
show a range of images from my fieldwork, demonstrating the historical changes in gravestones,
highlighting the interplay of human judgments, language, and chance factors in determining the
fate of non-human animals, and revealing the power of visual art to provoke informed discourse
on interspecies relationships.
Brandes, Stanley. "The Meaning of American Pet Cemetery Gravestones." Ethnology 48.2
(2009): 99- 118. Web. 15 Oct. 2013.
Melissa Burns-Cusato, Brian Cusato, & Amanda Glueck
Threats from the past: Barbados green monkeys retain fear of ancestral predators for over 350
years
Department of Psychology, Centre College, Danville, Kentucky
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
The ability to recognize and differentiate between predators and nonpredators is a necessary skill
for animals that engage in anti-predator behaviors. While there is evidence that both genetic and
experiential factors mediate predator recognition in various animal species, it is unknown which
of these two mechanisms the green monkey (Chlorocebus sabaeus) utilizes. A feral population
of green monkeys that has been isolated from predators on the island of Barbados for over 350
years offers a unique opportunity to investigate the mechanisms underlying predator recognition
in this species. Two experiments were designed to determine whether green monkeys’ ability to
recognize predators as threatening was largely genetic or learned. In separate experiments,
monkeys’ approach to visual representations of a leopard (exp 1) and a snake (exp 2) were
measured. In both experiments, monkeys showed less approach to the predator stimuli than
control stimuli. The results of these two experiments suggest that the green monkeys have
retained the ability to distinguish ancestral predators from ancestral non-predators despite not
having the opportunity to observe the predator-response pairing for over 50 generations.
Keridiana Chez
Canine Connections in George Eliot’s Adam Bede and Middlemarch
Department of English, Borough of Manhattan Community College, CUNY
[email protected]
Irrational instinct and uncontrolled passion were, for centuries, externalized onto the non-human
animal so that the human could conceive of himself as a uniquely rational being. But what if
animals also served as useful tools to comprise humanity by their inclusion? This talk grows out
of a larger project investigating how the middle classes on both sides of the Atlantic developed
the use of animal companions as emotional prostheses. The advent of modernity fueled a
dramatic change in attitudes towards animals, yet the goal of these humane attitudes was not, as
it would first appear, to fill emotional vacancies with animals, but rather to forge new
connections between humans via the animal. Expanding the model of prosthesis (as emotional
rather than physical, being rather than thing), I argue that the bourgeoisie used intimacies with
companion animals to enhance their capacity to feel and connect—transforming the concept of
the “animal” and of themselves as “human.”
The mobilization of affect on this large a scale was an achievement of narration: the
novel forged an imagined community between the “humane” subjects of England and the U.S. by
regulating the representation of human-animal relationships. In her fiction, George Eliot includes
emotive character sketches of canine subjects who are endowed with autonomous existence.
They often have little histories of their own and more willfulness in their ways of being. They are
empowered as judges of character, able to distinguish between persons they like and do not like
and act accordingly. Most importantly, they participated in the development of the nineteenthcentury discourse of human-dog relationships, particularly in the work dogs performed in
signaling the potential for and development of sanctioned interhuman connections. Adam Bede is
a representative example of a key moment in history where the dog transitioned from family
members into more exclusive personal attachments. In Adam Bede, Adam’s attachment to Gyp
enables him to maintain the ability to sympathize with others and is central to his moral
development. Eliot’s novels represent a process I am calling “togethering”—the process by
which the “irrational” animal Other is then reappropriated as an attached, affect-enhancing
instrument. My talk will explore how “man’s best friend” was central to the making of the
modern individual not only by exclusion, but by intimate inclusion.
Jonathan L. Clark
Uncharismatic Invasives
Department of Sociology, Ursinus College
[email protected]
Late on June 4th or early on June 5th, 2012, more than a year after a devastating
tsunami struck Japan, a massive commercial fisheries dock washed ashore on a popular Oregon
beach. But what was even more surprising than the dock itself were the passengers who had
come along for the ride. The surface of the dock was quite simply teeming with marine life,
including various invertebrates. The dock was quickly traced to the coastal town of Misawa,
Japan, where the tsunami had dislodged it with these “biofouling” organisms attached. Soon after
arriving on the scene, biologists from the Oregon Department of Fish and Wildlife (ODFW)
identified several potentially invasive species. As a precautionary measure, an ODFW team
scraped the organisms off the dock and then buried them above the high-tide line. Although
philosophers have explored the question of whether “invasive species” ought to be worthy of
moral consideration, there has been little anthropological or sociological research on how
invasive species managers think about these kinds of normative questions. Taking a descriptive,
ethnographic approach to the ethics of invasive species management, this paper examines how
the members of the ODFW team thought about the moral status of the potentially invasive
marine invertebrates on the dock. I argue that the position of these animals on two intersecting
scales of moral worth—the sociozoologic scale and the phylogenetic scale—rendered them
unworthy of moral consideration.
Autumn Costelle
Expanding Horizons: The Goals and Achievements of EKU’s Animal Studies Club
Animal Studies Program, Department of Psychology, EKU
[email protected]
This poster is an overview of the Animal Studies Club at Eastern Kentucky University and its
achievements. The Animal Studies Club was established in 2011 and continues to grow under
the guidance of Dr. Radhika Makecha and strong student leadership. The Animal Studies
program at EKU is unique, and the club celebrates this by promoting awareness of the major and
the field of Animal Studies. Our main purpose is to explore ideas, internship and career
opportunities, and to benefit the lives of animals. We plan events and trips that help our members
gain knowledge and hands-on experience outside of the classroom. With every speaker event and
trip we take, students make connections with professionals in the field. With visits to (e.g.) the
Primate Rescue Center, the Kentucky Equine Humane Center, and the Lexington Humane
Society, students get exposure to animal welfare issues and animals themselves, as well as to the
people who engage in humane action toward animals. The Animal Studies Club donates and
volunteers at these and other organizations. Our meetings have presentations on numerous topics,
most recently on dog chaining and abuse; the Ross University Vet School program; the pros and
cons of using trap, neuter, release with cats on campus at EKU; the politics of the New York
horse carriage business; rabbit rescuing and rescuers; and wildlife rehabilitation. Meetings
provide opportunities for Animal Studies majors to share their knowledge and ideas, and discuss
current animal-related issues. Students have presented to the club on reptiles, the exotic pet
business, wildlife rehabilitation, therapy and service dogs, and buying cruelty-free products. A
fundraiser, “Zoo to You Day,” educated children on animals through a day camp with various
presenters and live, well-cared-for animals. This semester our events include a trip to the Liberty
Nature Center, an overnight trip to the Cincinnati Zoo, and a presentation on the many internship
opportunities available. The Animal Studies Club is an asset to EKU’s students, whose active
participation in it allows the Club to thrive.
Elena Cox
Lead poisoning in raptors: Impacts of game hunting with lead ammunition
Virginia-Maryland College of Veterinary Medicine
[email protected]
Lead is a non-specific toxin that is known to affect a broad spectrum of physiological processes.
At levels as low as several parts per million, it can cause fatal neurological damage and organ
failure; milder symptoms are often secondary causes of death. Due to their morphology and
feeding behaviors, diurnal raptors have been disproportionately impacted by lead toxicosis.
Previous reports on lead impacts on waterfowl before and after existing lead shot bans,
combined with data from wildlife biologists and veterinarians, indicate that the most likely
source of lead for these birds is spent ammunition and bullet fragments left in gut piles or
carcasses. The birds present with symptoms of toxicosis after ingesting the lead fragments, rather
than being directly shot and targeted by game hunters. This report includes data from the
Wildlife Center of Virginia, a wildlife rehabilitation facility that receives approximately 2500
patients a year, many of them birds of prey. Patient data from wildlife rehabilitators provides a
window into significant causes of morbidity and mortality of wild species. The findings support
the high mortality rate of lead toxicity, and the high rate of morbidity within our sample of the
population. Considered with the human health risks associated with lead ammunition for
terrestrial game hunting, the consequences of lead toxicity in raptors show a substantial need to
further engage the hunting community as conservation partners.
Keri Cronin
“Mendacious Representations?”: The Camera as Witness in the Battle Over the Live Export of
Horses in Early 20th Century England
Department of Visual Arts, Brock University
[email protected]
This paper focuses on a 1914 film made as part of the fight against the live export of horses for
slaughter from England to continental Europe in the early 20th century. This film was made at the
insistence of a Norfolk reformer named Ada Cole, and was the culmination of a series of visual
tactics that Cole engaged in to bring attention to the issue. This was a significant undertaking, in
all likelihood the first time this technology was used in service of animal advocacy efforts. To
create the film Cole worked with both the RSPCA, who financed the endeavor, and the famed
Pathé firm. In November 1925, over a decade after the film was made, there were allegations that
it had been based on faked footage, and a high-profile court case saw Cole and members of the
RSPCA defending themselves against these allegations. In the end their accuser, Captain Robert
Gee, a Member of Parliament from Leicestershire, recanted his accusations and was sued for
slander.
In addition to providing good copy for the newspaper and fodder for the gossip mills, this
incident also illustrates some of the complexities relating to the use of imagery in animal
advocacy. Cole’s insistence upon obtaining film footage of this industry speaks to the broader
cultural associations that equate camera-based imagery with a form of documentary truth,
showing things as they really are. Some of the questions that this research raises include: How
do ideas about what constitutes “humane” and “cruel” activities get articulated through visual
means? How do images function in this context and what kinds of expectations do viewers have
of them? Why do certain types of images have more currency than others when it comes to
political activism? How do ideas about “witnessing” get complicated through different types of
visual material?
Jessica Dallow
A “Galaxy of Distinguished Horses”: Schreiber & Sons and the Emergence of Equine Portrait
Photography
Department of Art History, University of Alabama at Birmingham
[email protected]
This paper addresses the emergence of equine portrait photography in the United States through
an examination of Portraits of Noted Horses of America, a collection of fifty albumen prints of
the nation’s trotters and Thoroughbreds, published in 1874 by the Philadelphia-based firm
Schreiber and Sons. Patriarch Franz George and his sons specialized in animal portraiture,
photographing zoo animals, pets, livestock, and many of country’s most renowned race and show
horses of the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries. Though the Schreibers were not the
first artists to attempt a comprehensive portrait portfolio of American horses, they were the first
to successfully complete and publish one.
In the preface to Noted Horses of America, the Schreibers describe their photographs as
“taken from life,” with nothing omitted nor added “to make a picture to please the still more
pitiful vanity of an owner.” Their emphasis on accuracy and objectivity signals their audience’s
desire for truthful likenesses, but also emerges from the climate of nineteenth-century
Philadelphia, a city with a dynamic photographic community and a rich history of portraiture.
The Schreiber name is perhaps best known in connection to local artist Thomas Eakins, who
often relied on friend Henry Schreiber’s photographs for aids for his paintings. But in addition to
describing their images as “perfectly accurate”, the Schreibers also stress Noted Horses of
America’s value as a collection of images. The act of bringing together a “galaxy of
distinguished horses” affords the viewer the opportunity for comparative study, a means by
which to determine not only how heredity, evidenced by pedigree, but also how form and
proportion, shown through equine physique, produce speed and endurance. In this paper, I argue
that the combination of accuracy and comparative analysis is key to understanding how the
Schreiber’s book epitomized the conflicting ideologies of the American horse industry in the
post-Civil War period and helped to codify them. Though invigorated by modern technologies of
representation and scientific theories of training and selective breeding, the industry—and
consequently its attendant visual traditions—remained faithfully committed to early modern
models of animal portrait composition, natural history illustration, and comparative anatomy.
Verda A. Davis
The Need for Anthrozoology in Veterinary Technology Curricula
Canisius College, Buffalo, New York
[email protected]
Anthrozoology is a burgeoning field of study that examines human relationships with, attitudes
toward, and uses of nonhuman animal species. Anthrozoologists study animal behavior, animal
welfare, the human-animal bond, and animal ethics, to name just a few of the field’s concerns.
All of these issues are dealt with by veterinary technologist on a nearly daily basis.
Veterinarians are relying more and more on vet techs in private clinics and animal hospitals, as
well as in veterinary testing laboratories, biomedical research facilities, and in food safety roles.
Because of their position in the front lines of animal health, vet techs are considered a valuable
source of information regarding not only pets, but all nonhuman animals. Vet tech programs
focus mainly on the ever so important technical skills needed for veterinary medical and
diagnostic procedures. Some programs offer courses with sections on grieving clients, the
human-animal bond, and veterinary ethics, however, none to my knowledge currently offer an
explicit Anthrozoology course. I am proposing that an introductory course in Anthrozoology
should be offered in veterinary technology programs. This poster discusses why Anthrozoology
is important to veterinary technologists, what specific aspects of Anthrozoology should be
covered in a vet tech program, as well as a sample course syllabus. Because of the
multidisciplinary nature of Anthrozoology, I also offer suggested tweaks to the syllabus to make
the course suitable in a number of academic departments, including anthropology, sociology, and
animal science.
Guadalupe Delgado, Victor Pataky, Richard Ford, Brian Cusato, & Melissa Burns-Cusato
Dangerous Liaisons: Human-Monkey Interactions at a Wildlife Reserve
Department of Psychology, Centre College, Danville, Kentucky
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected];
[email protected]; [email protected]
Wild green monkeys living in the vicinity of the Barbados Wildlife Reserve have received daily
food provisions from staff for more than a decade. The purpose of provisions is two-fold: to
demotivate monkeys from raiding nearby fruit crops and to attract tourists that would like to
observe the free-ranging monkeys in their natural habitat. Unfortunately, some tourists ignore
posted signs warning of the dangers of approaching the wild, unpredictable monkeys too closely.
Tourists that tease, try to touch, or pick up the monkeys have been bitten or scratched. Such
incidents have also occurred at many locations where humans can interact closely with primates
(e.g., Monkey Island and Gibraltar). The present study examined the impact of a verbal greeter
at the entrance of the Reserve on the rate of potentially dangerous behaviors seen in tourists.
Subjects that were greeted with or without a verbal warning about touching the monkeys showed
significantly lower rates of dangerous behaviors than subjects that did not received a verbal
greeting. Implications for promoting safe interactions with these wild animals will be discussed.
Jane Desmond1 & Maria Lux2
Thinking “Big”: Collaborative Processes between Artists and Scholars for Public Art
Production
1
Department of Anthropology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; 2Independent Artist,
Champaign, Illinois
[email protected]; [email protected]
Responding to the conference’s emphasis on the visual and on interdisciplinary work, this joint
presentation by a scholar and a visual artist discusses our collaborative working process and the
larger issues that arise from it. We take our experience in developing ideas and visual renderings
for a billboard contest, called “Sky Gallery,” sponsored by our local art council last year, as a
starting example. Working together to develop ideas through discussion, engaging with
research on emergent topics, such as animal cognition, and then drafting and refining visual
designs for a large scale artwork, we created three separate designs that were submitted to the
competition, which drew about 60 entries. The 6 winning entrees are shown for 12 months each,
potentially reaching a mobile audience in the tens of thousands as the artworks appear in
changing billboard spaces throughout the micro-metropolitan area of Champaign-Urbana. One
of our three submissions, which features a short narrative in the style of a book illustration, was
selected, and continues to be on display. It depicts human-animal interactions in a public park,
while drawing attention to new understandings of crow cognition through a three-panel narrative.
In our contest of wills between humans and crows, the crows win.
We consider the process of working from scholarly sources to visual designs and back
again, the challenges of producing a politically engaged large scale public art piece that
addresses a relatively undifferentiated public (potentially anyone in the community in any
section of town in a car or public transportation), in a city with vested interests in agricultural
animals, and the crucial issue of legibility when your audience is moving quickly past your
image in traffic. We will also discuss briefly a mini-version of this project introduced into a
college course as a way of encouraging students to combine a targeted argument and public
rendering of their research.
In the latter half of the discussion, we move outward from these examples to consider the
larger issues they reveal: a) How might scholarly/artistic collaborations extend the work we are
able to do alone? Do they require/encourage a sort of “third voice” that belongs in between the
two arenas? b) How does the focus on the creation of an object change collaboration and c) how
do we work within the demands of sponsored public art—which must accommodate a variety of
community needs and desires—while still producing strong assertions about human-animal
relations?
Catherine Doyle
Keeper-elephant relationships: A discussion of patterns found in keeper perception of the
human-elephant relationship, and the potential for disconnect.
Director of Science, Research and Advocacy, Performing Animal Welfare Society (PAWS)
[email protected]
This discussion of the keeper-elephant relationship stems from a quantitative study that
investigated captive African elephant preferences for certain keepers as measured by responses
to olfactory and auditory cues. A key component in the study, which was conducted at a
sanctuary and a zoo, was a survey that provided its own interesting findings on keeper perception
of their relationships with the elephants. In the survey, 8 keepers (4 at each facility) rated their
relationships with each elephant; predicted how each elephant would rate other keeper-elephant
relationships; and predicted how each elephant would rate their own relationships, among other
information. Keepers participating in the study included 4 males and 2 females, between 25 and
44 years of age, with a range of 3 to 14 years of experience working with elephants. The study
analyzed keeper perceptions of relationships with the elephants; interactions and time spent with
the elephants in relation to keeper-elephant relationship ratings; and length of keeper experience
and relationship ratings.
Patterns were found in the perception of keeper-elephant relationships both inter-facility
and intra-facility. Keepers gave higher ratings to other keepers with more experience but
experience was not a factor in rating their own relationships with the elephants. Keepers tended
to rate their own relationships with elephants higher than those of other keepers. Keepers at the
zoo rated their relationships with the elephants significantly higher than did keepers at the
sanctuary. Keepers at the zoo also engaged more frequently than keepers at the sanctuary in nonhusbandry, non-training interactions with the elephants (e.g., giving treats, spending extra time
with them). The difference in time spent working with elephants at the zoo and the sanctuary
approached significance, with keepers at the zoo spending more time with the elephants.
These differences and the suggested bases for them imply the potential for a disconnect
between how keepers believe elephants perceive them and how elephants may actually view
relationships with their keepers. This can have safety implications for keepers and welfare
implications for captive elephants.
Jeanne Dubino
Listening to the Dogs: Orhan Pamuk and the Mongrelization of Fiction
English and Global Studies Appalachian State University, Boone, North Carolina
[email protected]
Dogs do speak, but only to those who listen.
Orhan Pamuk, My Name Is Red
One of the nineteen first-person narrators in Orhan Pamuk’s polyphonic novel, My Name Is Red,
is a stray dog who speaks directly to the reader in the third chapter, entitled “I Am a Dog.” What
most particularly exercises this stray is the hostility he encounters from the “hajis, hojas, clerics,
and preachers” who “persist in saying that dogs are impure” (My Name Is Red 12, 13). Pamuk’s
dog is not ashamed of being “a four-legged beast” (Red 13), and concludes by the end of his
chapter that he and his fellow canines are not the enemy; rather, the “boneheaded” clerics who
vilify dogs are themselves the “infidels” (Red 11, 14). In a book that celebrates multiculturalism
and hybridity, the dog becomes a spokesperson for a new and liberating way of looking at life,
one that counters the narrowminded fetishization of purity and singularity represented by the
clerics.
My Name Is Red is a Neo-Ottoman novel set in the late sixteenth century, when dogs
were considered unclean, or haram. But, as Kim Fortuny writes in “Islam, Westernization, and
Posthumanist Place: The Case of the Istanbul Street Dog,” stray dogs are still considered dirty
and taboo—and a sign of backwardness. Hence, programs of westernization, started in the
nineteenth century, have entailed, among other things, the elimination of dogs from the streets of
Istanbul. Pamuk writes about these efforts as well in his memoir Istanbul: “the state and the
school system have launched campaign after campaign to drive dogs from the streets, but still
they roam free” (44). As if in protest against this intifada on dogs, Pamuk continues to populate
the pages of both of his Neo-Ottoman novels (also The White Castle) and his contemporary ones,
including Silent House and The Museum of Innocence, with stray dogs.
This paper will connect the mongrelization of Pamuk’s fiction—that is, his resistance to
the singular vision of the clerics and also to the uniform, republican ideals of Kemalism—to the
presence of the dogs who roam free within its pages. At the same time, Pamuk is keenly
interested in the mongrels as mongrels. In one of his essays, “What I Know about Dogs,” Pamuk
wonders what it must be like to be a dog. Along with considering Pamuk’s attempts to portray
dogs from their points of view, I will return to the implied imperative of the headnote: we should
learn to listen to those who speak.
Ian J.H. Duncan
Asking the Animals
Professor Emeritus, Department of Animal and Poultry Science, University of Guelph, Guelph,
Ontario N1G 2W1, Canada
[email protected]
Human beings have been able to control some species of animals for thousands of years. For
example, they have been able to control horses for riding and draught, elephants and camels for
riding, oxen for ploughing, and dogs for a variety of tasks. One could consider this as “Telling
animals what to do”. During the 20th century, behavioral scientists began to understand
communication systems of various species of animals and how they pass information amongst
themselves. In the past 40 years, we have developed techniques of “Asking animals what they
feel” and these techniques will be described.
The argument will be developed that welfare is all to do with what the animal feels, with
the absence of negative subjective emotional states that are usually called "suffering" and,
probably, with the presence of positive subjective emotional states that are usually called
“pleasure”. Although subjective states are not directly accessible to scientific investigation, it is
possible to gather indirect evidence on how positive or negative an animal feels under particular
circumstances. Methods of "asking animals what they feel” will be illustrated with examples of
preference testing and motivational testing in a variety of species. Using these methods we can
get a more objective measurement of the effect on the animal of the conditions we impose on it
and the procedures we subject it to. These methods will help to answer the questions “Does it
matter to the animal?” and “How much does it matter?”
Marie-Jose Enders-Slegers
The Human Animal Bond and Further Professionalizing of Human-Animal Interventions:
Theories, Results and Challenges
Anthrozoology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Open University Heerlen, the
Netherlands, Valkenburgerweg 177, 6419 AT Heerlen, The Netherlands
[email protected]
The bond between humans and (companion) animals has existed for many thousands of years
Germonpre et al., 2009). The role that (companion) animals play differs in the various time
periods, the various societies and in the different life stages of humans. Since the last three
decades many empirical research findings about meaning and effects of human-animal relational
interactions have become available. Most emphasis is put on the positive effects of animal
assisted interventions for the wellbeing of humans, and particularly for the wellbeing and quality
of life of vulnerable populations (demented elderly, children with autism, psychiatric patients).
Also the health promoting factors of having a companion animal for the ’normal’ population
have been at the center of attention. Many social, psychological and physiological processes
were revealed and Social Support Theories, Attachment Theories as well as the functioning of
hormones were applied to explain the positive outcomes of human-animal interactions and
interventions (Enders-Slegers, 1993, 2000, Julius et al., 2013). I would like to present the current
state of affairs in this research on the human-animal bond and animal assisted interventions. In
addition, I would like to point out the state of current affairs in the multidisciplinary and
extended field of the practitioners, actually doing the animal assisted interventions. Theoretical
backgrounds and new insights, drawn from developmental psychology (Fogel, 1993), cultural
psychology (Voestermans & Verheggen, 2013), ethology (Hare & Tomasello, 2005),
evolutionary science and neuroscience (Panksepp, 2011; Porges, 2007) will be briefly addressed.
A final very important point that I would like to raise concerns challenges in the field of the
practitioners, such as a guaranteed quality of the interventions, continuing education of
professionals as well as volunteers, and the seminal challenge of safe guarding animal wellbeing
in the human-animal bond or animal assisted interventions.
Enders-Slegers, M.J. (1993). Investigation of the meaning for the elderly of a relationship with
companion animals, in Hicks, E.K. (Ed) Science and the Human-Animal Relationship.
Enders-Slegers, M. J. (2000). The meaning of companion animals: Qualitative analysis of the life
histories of elderly cat and dog owners. In E. S. Podberscek, J. A. Paul, & J. A. Serpell (Eds.),
Companion animals and us: Exploring the relationships between people and pets (pp. 209–236).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Fogel, A. (1993) Developing through Relationships. Origins of communication, self, and culture. Hemel
Hempstead, UK: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Germonpré, M., Sablin, M.V., Stevens, R. E., Hedges, R.E.M., Hofreiter, M., (2009). Fossil dogs and
wolves from Palaeolithic sites in Belgium, the Ukraine and Russia: osteometry, ancient DNA and
stable isotopes. Journal of Archaeological Science 36: 473–490.
Hare, M. & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9,
440-444
Julius H., Beetz A., Kotrschal K., Turner D., & Uvnäs-Moberg K. (2013). Attachment to Pets. New York:
Hogrefe.
Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: do animals have affective lives?
Neuroscience & Behavioural Reviews, V 35, 9, 1791-1804.
Porges S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116–143.
Voestermans, P.P.L.A., & Verheggen, Th. (2007). Cultuur & Lichaam. Een Cultuurpsychologisch
Perspectief op Patronen in Gedrag. Oxford, UK: Blackwell
Erica N. Feuerbacher1, and Clive D. L. Wynne2.
Most Dogs Prefer Food…But Sometimes They Don’t: Effects of Familiarity, Context, and
Schedule on Dogs’ Preference for Food or Petting
1
Department of Anthrozoology, Carroll College, Helena, Montana
2
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona
[email protected]; [email protected]
Despite our long standing relationship with dogs, little is known empirically about their
preferences for different types of human interaction. Previous research has indicated both petting
(McIntire & Colley, 1967) and food (Feuerbacher & Wynne, 2012) have reinforcing effects on
dog behavior and support social behavior towards humans (food: Elliot & King, 1960; social
interaction: Brodbeck, 1954). Which type of interaction dogs prefer and which might produce the
most social behavior from a dog has not been investigated. We assessed how dogs allocated their
responding in a concurrent choice between food and petting. Dogs received five 5-min sessions
each. In Session 1, both food and petting were continuously delivered contingent on the dog
being near the person providing the respective consequence. Across the next three sessions, we
thinned the food schedule to a Fixed Interval (FI) 15-s, FI 1-min, and finally extinction. The fifth
session reversed back to the original food contingency. We tested owned dogs in familiar
(daycare) and unfamiliar (laboratory room) environments, and with their owner or a stranger as
the person providing petting. In general, dogs preferred food to petting when food was readily
available and all groups showed sensitivity to the thinning food schedule by decreasing their time
allocation to food, although there were group and individual differences in the level of
sensitivity. How dogs allocated their time with the petting alternative also varied. We found
effects of context, familiarity of the person providing petting, and relative deprivation from
social interaction on the amount of time dogs allocated to the petting alternative.
Preston Foerder
What Do Elephants Know and When Do They Know It?
Department of Psychology, The University of Tennessee at Chattanooga
[email protected]
Elephants are generally considered to be extremely intelligent. However, until recently, there has
been little empirical research conducted on elephant cognition. This presentation will give an
overview of our current knowledge of elephant cognition. Elephants have been shown capable of
many cognitive abilities including visual and olfactory discrimination, numerosity, tool
manufacture, mirror self-recognition, and an understanding of cooperation. A focus will be on
my research which has shown elephant’s capable of insightful problem solving: spontaneous
problem solving without evident trial and error behavior. In humans this phenomenon has been
referred to as insight or the “aha” moment. Surprisingly, elephants had failed to exhibit insightful
problem solving in previous cognitive studies. I tested whether three Asian elephants (Elephas
maximus) at the National Zoo in Washington, D.C. would use sticks or other objects to obtain
food items placed out-of-reach and overhead. One elephant, a 7-year-old male elephant solved
the problem by moving a large plastic cube, on which he then stood, to acquire the food. In
further testing he showed behavioral flexibility, using this technique to reach other items and
retrieving the cube from various locations to use as a tool to acquire food. In the cube’s absence,
he generalized this tool utilization technique to other objects and, when given smaller objects,
stacked them in an attempt to reach the food. Previous failures to demonstrate this ability in
elephants may have resulted not from a lack of cognitive ability but from the presentation of
tasks that required trunk-held sticks as potential tools, thereby interfering with the trunk’s use as
a sensory organ to locate the targeted food. Preliminary results from ongoing problem solving
research on African elephants will also be included.
Kate Ford & Ellen Furlong
Moral Reasoning in Dogs
Department of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University
[email protected]; [email protected]
A sense of morality, values predisposing what is right (fair, just, kind) and what is wrong (unfair,
cruel, dishonest) appears universally across all humankind. All major cultures share support for
some values, such as self-respect, respect for others, and ‘the golden rule’—treat others how you
wish to be treated—and disdain for some sins, such as murder, theft and dishonesty (Kinnier,
Kernes & Dautheribes, 2000). Some moral behaviors, such as inequity aversion, the tendency to
do no harm and cooperation are found to exist in virtually all human adults. But where does
morality come from? Is it uniquely human or do we share some moral values with nonhuman
animals? To explore these questions domestic dogs—nonhumans with exceptional social
cognitive skills—were tested for moral values through a replication of a study on moral
reasoning in human infants (Hamlin & Wynn, 2011). Dogs watched a puppet show with a moral
and immoral actor—the moral actor helped a neutral character achieve a goal and the immoral
actor prevented the actor from achieving the goal. Dogs reacted stronger when the neutral puppet
chose to associate with the immoral hinderer than the moral helper, demonstrating that dogs, like
human infants, expect agents to choose to associate with morally ‘good’ actors. Though this is a
preliminary study it suggests that a sense of morality may not be uniquely human and may be an
evolved trait shared by humans and nonhumans alike.
Tabitha Foster
Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR) Making a Positive Impact on EKU’s Campus
Community Cats Volunteer; Animal Studies Program, EKU
[email protected], [email protected]
Similar to many institutions of higher education, Eastern Kentucky University is “home” to a
community cat population that has resulted from (1) cats that have been abandoned by their
owners, (2) stray cats that have become lost from their owners, and (3) feral (or community) cats
that have descended from abandoned and lost cats.
For decades, individuals at EKU have made significant contributions of their personal
money and time to control the community cat population on campus through humane
sterilization while also providing daily care for the cats. Understanding the need for an officially
recognized Trap-Neuter-Return (TNR) program, a group of volunteers, consisting of faculty,
students, staff and community members, submitted a formal proposal to help the University more
quickly reach the goal of fewer cats on campus through this proven humane method.
The TNR mission defined in the proposal was to immediately stabilize and ultimately
reduce the number of un-owned campus cats by working through the Madison County Humane
Society Animal League for Life. The University executed a Memorandum of Understanding in
August, 2014, which permitted the Community Cats Volunteer group to conduct TNR work and
associated care for the campus cats during non-work hours.
In December 2013, the number of community/free-roaming cats on campus was
estimated to be 125-150. Per the quarterly TNR report submitted the University on March 1,
2015, the estimated cat population is currently 68. The program appears to be working to reduce
the number of community cats, and treating humanely those that survive.
Ed Fredrickson1, An Peischel2, Greg Brann3, & Rick Griebenow4
Potential Applications for Targeted Grazing to Enhance Ecosystem Services and Rural
Economies in Eastern Kentucky
1
Department of Agriculture, EKU
2
Department of Agricultural and Environmental Sciences, Tennessee State University, Nashville
3
National Resource Conservation Service, Nashville, Tennessee
4
Director of Farms, EKU
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
With the exception of companionship, and establishment of social rank, human uses of livestock
are generally limited to providing society with recreation, meat, milk, and draft, or as a beast of
burden. The use of livestock for vegetation management and restoring vital ecosystem services
has received significantly less attention with most research being conducted in the western
United States where the costs of machinery and herbicides used to manage vegetation can be
greater than the resulting value of improvements in forage or other ecosystem services being
managed. Vegetation management using livestock is often termed “targeted grazing”. This
system uses knowledge of plant communities along with knowledge of both innate and malleable
differences in diet section that exists within and among livestock species. Innate differences
among species are widely known: for example, cattle prefer grasses and are less selective than
goats known for browsing shrubs and/or trees. It is also known that animals learn dietary
preferences in utero, or from examples provided by their parents early in life, and from their
peers as they mature. In this paper we examine the potential of targeted grazing to manage
vegetation (e.g. reduce invasive species such as kudzu) in eastern Kentucky and compare the
costs of targeted grazing with herbicide and mechanical applications currently being employed.
We hypothesize that the costs using targeted grazing are similar to other forms of vegetation
management yet the additional return in animal products provides income that can support rural
communities in Kentucky’s Appalachia region. We also describe existing methods of targeted
grazing and how and how they might be altered to meet eastern Kentucky’s unique conditions.
Lastly, we discuss the animal-human bond that results when using livestock for targeted grazing
and discuss how this bond can be used to improve results.
Benjamin Z. Freed
Pleistocene Humans and Canids: A View from Studies of Primate Polyspecific Associations
Department of Anthropology, Sociology, and Social Work, EKU
[email protected]
Researchers have debated whether recent archaeological finds between 20,000 and 35,000 years
ago from Siberia, Belgium, and elsewhere provide the earliest evidence of human domestication
of canids. Although the debate focuses on genetic and anatomical changes occurring in canids,
relatively little discussion has been devoted to the nature of the interaction between the species.
The purpose of this paper is to review and to provide examples of how primates as an order
associate with other species in the same habitat, and how these associations can have profound
evolutionary consequences. Polyspecific associations involving primates may form by chance,
but in most cases, these associations may provide a species increased or enhanced: predator
protection; foraging efficiency; childcare, mating, and social opportunities; and niche expansion.
Results from a quantitative study of two species of lemurs show that the species regularly
interact, recognize each other’s vocalizations, provide each other social benefits, and forage
more effectively together during periods of resource scarcity. Overall, polyspecific associations
often allow primates to co-evolve in habitats in which they might not have been able to exist
outside of association. Likewise, associations between Pleistocene humans and canids may have
existed and affected both species’ evolution, whether or not researchers find conclusive evidence
of canid domestication.
Ellen Furlong1 & Jack Furlong2
Melding Justice and Science: An interdisciplinary course, “Ape Sapiens: Wild Minds and
Captive Dignity”
1
Department of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University, Bloomington, Illinois;
2
Department of Philosophy, Transylvania University
[email protected]; [email protected]
"Ape Sapiens: Wild Minds and Captive Dignity" is a team-taught course that we will teach in
May of 2015. We believe that the details of the course itself may be of some interest and use to
colleagues at this conference, but our main aim in this paper is to examine the underlying
rationale; namely, that studying primate cognition shows us what kinds of duties we have toward
such intelligent, highly social creatures when it is found necessary to cage them.
Team-taught across two campuses, involving students from both -- Illinois Wesleyan
University and Transylvania University -- the course takes an evolutionary and comparative
approach to non-human primate cognition. Emphasizing how differences in evolutionary
pressures across species lead to differences in cognitive capacities allows us to explore the
specific psychological needs of each species. We will spend several days at the Louisville Zoo
and similarly at a primate sanctuary (the Primate Rescue Center in Nicholasville, Kentucky),
working with chimpanzees, gorillas, orangutans, siamang, several species of monkeys, and
lemurs, designing cognitively appropriate enrichment items for some of these species and
conducting research to explore their effectiveness. In the process, and especially at the sanctuary,
we will underscore the fact that primate minds are shaped for the wild, but those we will study
are captive, creating the need for reflection on what we owe them. This dual focus of the
scientific and the ethical most powerfully converge at our construction of a termite mound for
the chimpanzees at the Primate Rescue Center, leaving us to end the course asking: is there an
ethical pressure to create species-typical enrichments for such sapient apes?
The rationale behind the course has matured over years of discussions, especially
regarding the significance of comparative cognition, but Stephen Ross's important article, "How
Cognitive Studies Help Shape Our Obligation for the Ethical Care of Chimpanzees," and Lori
Gruen's work on non-human dignity helped us clarify and substantiate our thinking. In our paper,
we will elaborate on this analysis and look forward to fruitful dialogue and critique.
Ratna Ghosal1, Andre Ganswindt2,3, Polani B Seshagiri4, Raman Sukumara
Endocrine and behavioural correlates of musth in the male Asian elephants (Elephas
maximus)
1
Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore-560012, India
2
Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria,
Pretoria 002, Republic of South Africa
3
Department of Production Animal Studies, Faculty of Veterinary Science, University of Pretoria,
Onderstepoort 0110, Republic of South Africa
4
Department of Molecular Reproduction, Development and Genetics, Indian Institute of Science,
Bangalore 560012, India
The occurrence of musth, a period of elevated levels of androgens and heightened sexual activity,
has been well documented for the male Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). However, the
relationship between androgen-dependent musth and adrenocortical function in this species is
unclear. Therefore, our aim was to describe the behavioral characteristics and endocrine
correlates of musth in both phenotypes of Asian elephants, through non-invasive monitoring of
androgen and glucocorticoid metabolites in elephant feces. The study was carried out on wild
male elephants in Kaziranga National Park, Assam, India. The study was the first assessment of
testicular and adrenocortical function in free-ranging male Asian elephants by measuring levels
of testosterone (androgen) and cortisol (glucocorticoid - an indicator of stress) metabolites in
feces. During musth, males expectedly showed significant elevation in fecal testosterone
metabolite levels. Interestingly, glucocorticoid metabolite concentrations remained unchanged
between musth and non-musth periods. This observation is unlike that observed with wild and
captive African elephant bulls and Asian elephants in captivity. Our results show that musth may
not necessarily represent a stressful condition in free-ranging male Asian elephants.
Chalotte Glintborg & Tia G. B. Hansen
Importance of a Dog for Recovery after Acquired Brain Injury. Two Case Stories.
Aalborg University, Centre for Developmental and Applied Psychological Science, Denmark
[email protected]; [email protected]
WHO advocates development of more bio-psycho-social approaches to rehabilitation. When
looking beyond the physiological and cognitive consequences of acquired brain injury (ABI),
which are well addressed by traditional rehabilitation practices, survivors often face psychosocial consequences too. These include depression, anxiety and identity crisis. Since animal
presence has been shown to have positive effects on health, mood, and quality of life in other
circumstances, we wondered whether clients with ABI, who happen to have a family dog, see it
as important for their recovery. To investigate this, we examined how two clients with ABI,
“Marie” and “Jasper”, constructed their dog as part of their rehabilitation process and identity
transitions. Data were collected by semi-structured interviews, and discourse analytic concepts
were used to find participants’ self and dog constructions. In particular, concepts of positioning
and agency were employed to interpret the data. We found that the dog was constructed and
positioned as a family member, a welcoming distraction, and a motivator. Also, it was seen as
offering social and emotional support, closeness, and unconditional attention. We conclude that
companion animals and Animal Assisted Interventions may offer support that supplements
traditional rehabilitation practice in important ways, and that this possibility merits more
attention and research.
Tia G. B. Hansen, Mai Andreasen, Åsa H. Jansson, & Runa E. Gjellan
Belief in profit animal mind predicts attitude to profit animal welfare
Center for Developmental and Applied Psychological Science, Aalborg University, Denmark.
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
People may love pets but care little about the welfare of animals used for food. To accommodate
the need for such distinctions, the Pest Pest Profit Scale (PPP; Taylor & Signal, 2009)
differentiates attitudes to welfare for animals that are precieved as pets, as pests, or as
commodities (“profit animals”), respectively. However, the PPP has only been tested in Australia
and its remains to be seen whether the scale works in other parts of the world. Moreover,
although belief in animal mind (Hills, 1995) is a robust predictor for general attitude to animals
(e.g., Apostol et al, 2013), and Taylor & Signal (ibid.) found correlations between general
attitude to animals and attitude to welfare for profit animals, it has not been examined directly
whether belief in profit animal mind can predict attitude to profit animal welfare. Since it has
also been shown that categorisation of animals as “meat” reduces attribution of mind to them (e.g,
Bastian et al, 2012), this might not be the case. Thus, the aims of the present study were (1) to
replicate the PPP scale structure in a Scandinavian context, and (2) to examine the relationship
between belief in profit animal mind and attitude to profit animal welfare. With an internet-based
sample (N = 172; 73 % Danes and 25 % other Scandinavians, age 17-67 with mean = 26 years),
we (1) replicated the structure of PPP with a few caveats, and (2) found that belief in profit
animal mind (questions modified from Hills, 1995) predicted 38.5 % af the variance in attitudes
to profit animal welfare. Attribution of emotions and of conscious awareness were significant
predictors. We suggest that findings have implications for marketing related to “cruelty-free
meat” and profit animal advocacy.
References:
Apostol, L., Rebega, O. L., & Miclea, M. (2013). Psychological and socio-demographic
predictors of attitudes toward animals. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 78, 521-525.
Bastian, B., Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Radke, H. R. M. (2012). Don’t mind meat? The denial
of mind to animals used for human consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
38(2), 247-256. DOI: 10.1177/0146167211424291
Hills, A. M. (1995). Empathy and belief in the mental experience of animals. Anthrozoös, 8(3),
132-142. DOI: 10.2752/089279395787156347
Taylor, N., & Signal, T. D. (2009). Pet, Pest, Profit: Isolating differences in attitudes towards the
treatment of animals. Anthrozoös, 22(2), 129-135. 10.2752/175303709X434158
Ashley Hammond
Opt to Adopt: Lexington Humane Society
Development Manager
Lexington Humane Society
[email protected]
This past year, the Lexington Humane Society adopted over 4,000 animals into loving homes;
provided 3,500 free and low-cost spay/neuter surgeries through our Spay’sTheWay program;
educated over 2,000 individuals through our camps, tours and school outreach programs;
provided 700 animals lifesaving, in-home care through our foster program; and adopted 100 dogs
through our Canine Companions training program. Our mission, simply stated, is to Give Love,
Teach Love and Adopt Love!
It is estimated that less than 30% of owned pets are adopted from animal shelters. This
number is astonishingly low. We work hard every day to improve this statistic in our community,
but we need YOUR help! Nationally, it’s estimated that 7.3 million dog and cats enter shelters
every year and 2.6 million of them are euthanized due to overpopulation, health, and
temperament issues. To end pet overpopulation and preventable euthanasia, we work especially
hard to educate the public on the importance of spay/neuter and adoption. Knowledge is power,
and we are on a mission to educate the public on lifelong, responsible pet ownership and the
importance of making the choice to Opt to Adopt.
Angela Hofstetter
Reel/Real Horses: Animals, Visual Pleasure, and Narrative Cinema
Butler University, Indianapolis, Indiana
[email protected]
Can narrative cinema featuring horses represent the animal mind and interspecies
relationships in ways that cannot be reduced to images of their exploitation as objects of the
human gaze or dismissed as sentimental symbols? Are the powerful emotions expressed by
horses merely dramatic effects of acting, mise-en-scène, cinematography, and editing that
anthropomorphize a malleable equine tabula rasa for suspect ideological purposes, or could
onscreen relationships of reel horses reflect—even possibly affect—those of real horses?
In recognition of Marc Bekoff’s assertion that nonhuman animals benefit from
responsible representation, I look for traces of the increased concern for horse welfare in recent
Hollywood films such as Caroline Thompson’s Black Beauty, Robert Redford’s The Horse
Whisperer, and Stephen Spielberg’s War Horse against classic films such as Clarence Brown’s
National Velvet, Harold Schuster’s My Friend Flicka, and Carroll Ballard’s The Black Stallion.
Identifying formal features of the moments that suggest the horse is more than an object of our
gaze valued solely for visual pleasure offers a glimpse into how portrayals of equine subjectivity
might work against as well as reinforce speciesist narrative traditions. Moreover, finding a
cinematic grammar which grants a “voice” to the horse creates the possibility to reframe the
celluloid horse and human relationship as a conversation between partners with powerful
benefits.
Scott Hurley
The Dog Fancy: A Site for the Intersection of Ableist, Healthist, and Speciesist Ideologies
Assistant Professor of Religion, Luther College
[email protected]
Using personal observations of dog shows, American Kennel Club breed standards, and literature
about how to show and breed dogs as my primary source material, I argue in this paper that the
“dog fancy” (the promotion, breeding, and showing of dogs) contributes to capitalist agendas
which permit the manipulation, modification, and destruction of human and nonhuman animal
bodies for financial gain, reifies social and cultural constructions of normalcy (defined as ablebodied, beautiful, and healthy) for both humans and canines, and perpetuates views that
marginalize groups of human and nonhuman animals on the basis of their body shape and type as
well as their abilities to successfully perform certain mental and physical tasks. In doing so, I
demonstrate how the exploitation of canines parallels that experienced by people with disabilities
or illness. Employing the concepts of eco-ability explicated in Earth, Animal, and Disability
Liberation (2012), critical disability studies, and Harlan Weaver’s “becoming in kind,” I explain
the ways that relationships between humans and canines in the dog show world provide the
conditions for specific experiences of health, species, and ability that reinforce hierarchical
ideologies. Finally, I discuss how the category of “junk dog” is created in juxtaposition to that of
“show dog” and argue that humans, living vicariously through dogs, inscribe anthropocentric
notions of beauty and able-bodiedness on canine bodies resulting in such deleterious practices as
inbreeding and cosmetic alterations.
Elena Iokimanskaya (Елена Иокиманская)
A brief overview of the stray animal problem in Russia
Independent scholar, Pet shelter volunteer
[email protected]
You become responsible forever, for what you have tamed.
- Antoine de Saint-Exupery.
The exact number of stray dogs in Russia remains unknown. Unofficial data suggests there are as
many as 26,000 stray dogs live in Moscow alone. Packs of stray dogs several dozen animals
large became a widespread phenomenon in Moscow at the beginning of 21st century. Unlike
many other metropolises, Moscow has no laws against this. Packs may live in dumps, parks, side
streets, subways stations, and abandoned houses, where they are fed by local people. Others
inhabit establishments like warehouses, factories, car parking, and open air markets where they
are fed by staff and employees. Occasionally these dogs will act as guards, but most will lead a
nomadic life, migrating throughout the city. The average lifespan of a stray dog is about 2 years.
The tremendous population of stray dogs in Russia is due to two factors: a lack of
personal responsibility towards personal pet and incompetent management of the stray situation
by the government. These personal and governmental failures include:
Avoidance of fixing pets: Most of the nation’s population considers it inhumane to get their pet
fixed, as they see it is important to let their pet give birth. The pet owner will attempt to find
homes for their pet’s offspring, or hire someone who will look for them, unfortunately most pups
and kittens will end up on the street because the supply of new pets dwarfs the demand for new
pets. In some cases it is even the norm to simply drown the litter.
Choosing pets as fashion items, not companions: Often a pet is acquired as a symbol of
prestige, comfort, or own prosperity demonstration and not from a place of compassion or
caring. Pets will be chosen because “they match the wallpaper”.
Lost animals: Pet microchips are not yet available in Russia. Additionally, you’ll rarely find the
owner’s phone number or address on a dog’s collar and Russian cats almost never wear collars.
Lost pets are rarely retrieved. If there are rules for keeping dogs muzzled or leashed, they are
generally ignored and rarely enforced.
There are no limits on breeding: Anyone can breed dogs and cats, no license is required nor
are there any taxes levied against breeders. People choose the best of the litter and the rest will
up in streets or killed.
Lack of government attention: There’s no support from the government in bringing attention to
or resolving this issue. Promoting ideas like getting pets fixed and chipped is difficult with only
donations and volunteer time. Breeder regulation and taxation is not getting any government
attention. There’s no resources available from the government to help control the existing stray
population. Wide scale change will need the government’s attention. As a result of the
government’s failure to manage the stray dog population Russian cities began seeing dog-hunters
appearance in the 2010’s. These vigilantes will kill dogs using bait with fast acting poison.
In order to fix the drastic situation of stray dogs and cats in Russia the following
measures must be applied by the government:
1. Strict limitations on owners pets population and breeding (high taxes for unneutered pets);
2. Strict limitations on breeding activity (taxes, licenses, etc.);
3. Adopting animals from the shelters encouragement;
4. Active promotion of pet neutering in mass media.
Hazuki Kajiwara
Strong Bonds, Ambiguous Futures: Responses to the Fukushima Nuclear Disaster in Japan
Graduate School of Sociology, Rikkyo University, Japan
[email protected]
This presentation explores the interaction between human and animals after a nuclear disaster. It
reports on the responses of pet owners and their companion animals after the nuclear disaster on
11 March 2011 when the Fukushima No.1 nuclear power plant was seriously damaged by
tsunami and released large amounts of radiation. Areas near the plant were evacuated and then
divided into the three zones according to the level of radioactivity. While some dogs and cats
were allowed to live with their owners in temporary housing, many companion animals had to be
left behind. Some guardians return to their own house in evacuated areas each day in order to
feed their animals. Moreover, many activists concerned with animal welfare also enter these
areas to look after animals in spite of the radioactivity. Interviews, observation and fieldwork
were used to assemble ethnographic accounts for 15 pet owners aged 30-81 (including 5 male, 2
female, and 4 couples) who were assigned to temporary housing complexes where they still live.
Three findings emerged. First, some owners able to live in the temporary housing
complexes with pets nevertheless kept their pets at their uninhabited homes in the evacuated
areas for three-and-a-half years. Behind their behavior is a rural philosophy for companion
animals that is very different from that found amongst urban pet owners. Second, the attachment
of elderly guardians to companion animals is particularly pronounced. The approach to
evacuation taken by government officials gave priority to human beings. Animal protection
group understandably gave top priority to the animals. Each group tended to work independently.
As a result, the relationship between the pets being saved and pet owners being saved was
overlooked. It might be more fruitful to focus on human-animal relationship as the unit of
analysis. Third, the level of radiation has greatly influenced the owner's behavior. Although the
government claims it would be possible to return to the evacuated zone someday, many people
do not believe those claims. They live in a limbo, stuck in government housing without being
able to make a rational decision about their companion animals.
It is hoped that the findings will contribute to the small but growing amount of qualitative
research on human-animal relationships and interaction during and after nuclear disasters.
Laura Keith
Creatures of Warfare: the Use, Misuse and Agency of World War I Animals
College of Liberal Arts and Human Sciences, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University,
Blacksburg, Virginia
[email protected]
Despite the massive advances in military technologies during World War I, animals
played a pivotal role in the war effort. Indeed, animals by the thousands served in the war,
including horses, pigeons, cats, and even dolphins and camels. One type of animal in particular
proved to be especially versatile in its contributions to the war effort: man’s best friend, the dog.
Historians, beginning in the immediate aftermath of World War I and later World War II,
recognized the importance of animal contributions to the two world wars and began studying the
use of animals as forms of technology in their own right. Interestingly, it is this same focus on
animals that has persisted into historical discussions today. However, there has been much
development in the studies of wartime animals since the 1930s and 1940s: indeed, much of the
historiography in this field focuses not only on the myriad uses of animals but also ideas of
agency. Agency as a concept emerged in the 1980s among a group of historians who focused on
the capacity of an individual to act in the world. Later, this concept would be applied to
nonhuman entities as well. This is where the study of wartime animals and the theory of agency
converge.
Indeed, recent historians have suggested that animals were not merely a simple form of
technology. Instead, they argue that animals possessed agency, choosing to dedicate themselves
to the cause and serving their human masters to the best of their abilities. This is not to say that
all historians agree on this topic. In fact, there are those historians who believe that animals do
not have agency no matter how brave or valiant their actions may seem. By tracing the
historiography of the uses of canines in World War I and later scholars’ discussions of agency,
my paper will examine the limited ways the two historiographies currently intersect and the
differing opinions concerning the subject. By focusing on specific examples of military dogs’
work on the Western Front it will show how a more in-depth study of war dogs intertwined with
a study of such animals’ agency might enhance our understanding of inter-species interactions in
the context of war.
Joshua Abram Kercsmar
Managing Livestock and Slaves in Barbados, ca. 1650–1816
Lilly Postdoctoral Fellow & Lecturer in History, Valparaiso University, Valparaiso, Indiana
[email protected]
This talk argues that English sugar planters in Barbados developed overlapping strategies
for managing animals and slaves from the 1650s through 1816. Backed by legal codes that
equated Africans with livestock, Barbadian planters subjected human and animal laborers to
brutal work regimens with little or no concern for their well-being. Yet this was no mindless
endeavor. Planters thought carefully about how to extract maximum profit from the many oxen,
horses, mules, and cattle they employeda task that required skills in selection, appraisal of
worth, evaluation of personalities, mitigation of conflict when livestock grew testy, and the quick
replacement of dead animals with living ones through trade. As plantation records, account
books, and reports from observers make clear, planters applied these same skills in their dealings
with slaves. In the process, however, planters often encountered the limits of equating humans
with animals. Slaves’ sexuality, as well as their capacity to think, imagine, and resistnotably
during Bussa’s Rebellion (April 1816)complicated efforts to treat them merely as property.
Yet it was precisely slaves’ dual status as chattels and persons that enabled planters to equate
them with animals for as long as they did. For livestock, too, had personalitiesand they, too,
resisted human attempts to control them. Adapted from my book-in-progress, Animal Husbandry
and the Origins of American Slavery, this study answers the call of David Brion Davis, a
preeminent historian of slavery, to study human-animal interactions as a model for master-slave
relations. Moreover, it probes the larger question of how peoples’ treatment of animals shapes
their treatment of each other: a question that lies at the heart of any intersectional approach to
animal studies.
Kathryn Kirkpatrick
"'Every Polar Bear Alive': Representing Animals in the Sixth Extinction”
Department of English, Appalachian State University, Boone, North Carolina
[email protected]
How do poets represent non-human animals, especially those who are seriously endangered, in
what scientists now call the sixth mass extinction? Paula Meehan's "The Solace of Artemis"
suggests a way. Written for Iggy McGovern's “20/12: Twenty Irish Poets Respond to Science in
Twelve Lines," this poem models for its initial audience, delegates to the European Science
Open Forum in 2012, an undoing of the human/animal binary by using scientific data to recast
myth. Meehan employs the trope of shape-shifting, in this case between narrator and bear, to
revision human and nonhuman animal futures, in the process subverting distorted cultural
dualisms between science and art. Indeed, in her tenure as Ireland Professor of Poetry, Meehan
has so far made human relations with non-human animals central to her first two lectures, first in
Belfast in November 2013 with "Imaginary Bonnets with Real Bees in Them" and later in
Dublin in November 2014 with "The Solace of Artemis.” Moving beyond the binaries of poetry
and prose, art and science, intuition and logic, literature and politics, among others, Meehan
offers powerful correctives to the Cartesian dualisms that stymie our relations with the larger
than human world.
Jessica Kraut, Stephanie AuBuchon, Connor Hughes & Ellen Furlong
Self-Control in Dogs
Department of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University, Bloomington, Illinois
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Each year 6 to 8 million pet dogs enter dog shelters, approximately 60% of which are euthanized.
Unfortunately, many dogs are taken to shelters, returned to shelters, or euthanized, due to
behavioral problems such as fear, aggression, or separation anxiety. If we can identify dogs early
who may be prone to these behaviors we could help place them in homes that are prepared for
dogs with behavioral problems, reducing the rate at which they get returned to shelters. In
humans one strong predictor of negative outcomes is self-control. Four-year old children who
exhibit little self control are at higher risk of negative outcomes as adolescents and adults than
four-year old children who exhibit more self control (Eigsti et al., 2006). Thus, finding a reliable
measure of self-control in dogs may have real applications in placing dogs in appropriate homes
that are prepared to deal with potential behavioral problems. We constructed a plexiglass covered
wheel with an opening available for food that was rotated in front of dog subjects after Bramlett
and colleagues 2012. The dog had the choice to eat plain food (kibble) that reached the window
first, or to wait an additional period for good food (jerky treat). Most dogs successfully wait for
food, and most importantly, demonstrate variation in how long they wait. Follow up studies will
explore how long dogs can wait and whether this variation in self-control can predict behavioral
problems.
Shane Locker
The Effects of Human Interaction and other Enrichments on Captive Tiger Stereotypy and
Exploration
Octagon Wildlife Sanctuary, Punta Gorda, Florida; and Animal Studies, EKU
[email protected]
This research took place over the summer of 2014 at the Octagon Wildlife Sanctuary in Punta
Gorda, Florida. The purpose was to observe the effects of enrichment, including human
interaction, on captive tiger behavior. I examined whether human interaction was stressful or
beneficial (stress-reducing) for the tigers by looking for changes in stereotypy; whether or not
hiding flavorful meat induced exploratory behavior; and whether or not enrichment was followed
by rest. The subjects of the study were two male and four female tigers ranging from 2 to 14
years of age. Observation periods for each animal lasted a total of six hours and were separated
into three periods: pre-enrichment, enrichment, and post-enrichment. Pre- and post-enrichment
periods were observations that took place with no human contact with the large felids (i.e.
observations were taken from a hidden position where human presence was undetected). The
enrichment period consisted of two phases: in the first, spiced meat was hidden to entice
exploratory behavior; in the second, training routines and displays of affection by humans were
used. I used instantaneous scan sampling over five-minute intervals. The behaviors I examined
were exploration, stereotypy, and resting. The results provided evidence that human interaction
and other enrichment affected the tigers’ behavioral patterns by reducing stereotypic behavior
and increasing exploratory behavior: stereotypic behavior declined, and exploratory behavior
increased, for all tigers during the enrichment period, compared to the pre- and post-enrichment
periods. Tigers also rested more after the post-enrichment periods than during earlier periods. By
engaging in this study, I hoped to discover what works as enrichment for captive big cats.
Elizabeth A. Lorenzen
Let’s Strike While the Iron is Hot! Using the Cause of Equine Welfare as a Vehicle for Teaching
Information Literacy at Peacefield Equine Sanctuary
Cunningham Memorial Library, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, Indiana
[email protected]
Throughout time, the innate connection between horses and humans has remained indisputable.
Horses have been a part of every stage of human history and have been a helper to man for
centuries. However, it seems that in spite of this, sectors of the human race have taken the
horse’s giving nature for granted, and instead of horses being rewarded for their labor, they have
been punished for it. Listen while this speaker tells the story of how her passion for the cause of
equine welfare inspired her to reframe her career, drawing upon her lifelong experiences with
horses, and to use this inspiration to inform her work with students and other constituencies at
her university campus and local community. At an educational institution that places an
emphasis on community engagement and experiential learning, she has been able to take the
concept of embedded librarianship to a whole new level and engage the students in work with the
equines at her sanctuary in eastern Vigo County, Indiana. In doing so, the volunteers at
Peacefield Equine Sanctuary have been able to help the horses and learn the concepts of
information literacy simultaneously. In addition, new services to other non-profit organizations
have been conceived of and provided that emphasize information retrieval and dissemination.
During this process, a new teaching model has evolved that emphasizes the use of mobile
technologies and social media tools to create and share new types of information to participants.
For Further Reference:
Making Hay While the Sun Shines: Using the Cause of Equine Welfare as a Platform for
Teaching the Skills of Information Literacy. Special Issue of Indiana Libraries: Innovation in
Library Outreach and Instruction, Vol. 33 No 2 (2014).
https://journals.iupui.edu/index.php/IndianaLibraries/index
Radhika N. Makecha
Paper Mache Giraffes and Puzzle Box Feeders, These are a Few of my Favorite Things:
Teaching Animal Enrichment Using Traditional and Applied Avenues
Animal Studies, Department of Psychology, EKU
[email protected]
This poster will be an overview on the methods used to teach a special topics in psychology class
on animal enrichment at Eastern Kentucky University. Students in the course were assigned to
teams of two and assigned a species to design enrichment for from one of four facilities: The
Louisville Zoo (Louisville, Kentucky), the Primate Rescue Center (Nicholasville, Kentucky),
The Kentucky Equine Humane Center (Nicholasville, Kentucky), and Meadowbrook Farms
(Richmond, Kentucky). The first four weeks of the course involved students reading literature on
animal enrichment as well as researching the behavior, cognition, and enrichment of the species
assigned to them for a review paper on these topics. Additionally, after familiarizing themselves
with the literature on their respective species, each team had to submit an enrichment design for
their species as well as the rationale behind the design (using the literature they had researched).
The fifth and six weeks of the class involved each team obtaining materials for and constructing
their enrichment design. Additionally, each team had to create an ethogram and data sheet for
data collection on their species. During the final two weeks of the course, the entire class
traveled to each facility and watched each team implement their enrichment design as well as
used each team’s data sheet to practice collecting data on each species’ behavior. An additional
trip to the Cincinnati Zoo (Cincinnati, Ohio) was included in the course for educational purposes,
where students learned the differences in enrichment design for collection animals versus
animals set to be re-released. Finally, students had to submit a paper describing how their
assigned species reacted to their enrichment object as well as suggestions for future
improvements to their object. The course was offered to both undergraduate and graduate
students.
Radhika N. Makecha1, Kathleen M. Dudzinski2, Stan A. Kuczaj II3, Otto Fad4, John
Anderson2
Animals in Captive Settings: What Can We Learn From Them?
1
Department of Psychology, EKU
2
Dolphin Communication Project, Port Saint Lucie, Florida
3
Department of Psychology, University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg
4
Busch Gardens, Tampa, Florida
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected];
[email protected]; [email protected]
The presence of animals in enclosed facilities is widespread. This talk considers what can be
learned from animals in such facilities that can be used to improve animal welfare and
conservation. We begin with a brief history of animals in captivity and then address the shifts
over time in how these animals are/were treated, including an increased focus on animal wellbeing and welfare. Additionally, we address how studies of captive animals have contributed to
educational, conservation, rehabilitation, research, and welfare programs. Though animal
captivity may have a checkered past, current focus is on what we (both humans and animals)
gain from having animals in our care. Example benefits include the opportunity to conduct
cognitive, behavioral, and biological research to learn more about individual and group behavior
(etc.) of these species as well as how to use this information for welfare, conservation,
management, and educational purposes. For example, the National Zoo in Washington D.C. has
created the National Elephant Herpesvirus Laboratory in order to better understand diseases that
affect elephants, including the elephant endotheliotropic herpesvirus (EEHV). The results of this
research, much of which comes from studies on captive elephants, are then used to understand
the virus in both captive and wild populations in hopes of improving treatment of the disease.
One goal of this talk is to present the perspective that the subject of animals in captivity is
multifaceted and complex; increased understanding might help to dispel some of the negative
connotations that come with the topic.
Erin McKenna
Loving Pets Means Caring for Livestock
Department of Philosophy, Pacific Lutheran University, Tacoma, Washington
[email protected]
In my book Pets, People, and Pragmatism I sketched an approach of living with horses, dogs,
and cats that entails respecting the evolutionary history of these animal beings, the species
specific behaviors, the breed tendencies, and the individual variation. This approach, rooted in
the theory of American Pragmatism, stresses an experimental approach that accounts for
pluralistic and changing circumstances. Rather than seek absolute moral stands on issues
pertaining to the relations between human and other animal beings, this approach seeks to make
actual relations better and seeks to create dialogue and cooperation among currently opposed
groups (e.g. PETA and the AKC). One place where such dialogue rarely occurs is around the
issue of livestock. People who live with pets are involved in the livestock industry, whether they
themselves eat meat or not. Those who live with horses are buying feed from the same system
that provides feed for cattle, pigs, and poultry. Those who live with dogs and cats usually feed
some amount of meat products to these animal beings. Often this is done without much thought
for the lives of the livestock animals. While pets are loved and pampered, most livestock
currently living in the U.S. have few stable social relationships (with humans or others of their
species), live in cramped and unhealthy confinement, and are transported and killed under
stressful conditions. The irony of sacrificing one group of animal beings who are now generally
kept at a distance from most humans, in order to feed (and often overfeed) another group of
animal beings who live in close contact with humans, will be explored here. I will show that the
animals we commonly see as livestock have the same rich evolutionary histories, species specific
behaviors, breed tendencies, and individual variation that I argue need to be respected in pets.
So, if one loves and respects those animal beings commonly seen as pets, one should also love
and respect those animal beings commonly seen as livestock. This entails working to change the
current condition of most livestock in the U.S.
Claudia Medina
Animal Blessings: Rituals of Appreciation as Pathways to Ecological Reconnection
Independent Filmmaker
[email protected]
In this era of profound ecological and societal crisis, the language of reconnection to place, to a
deeper appreciation and understanding of the complexities and interrelationships within our
immediate environments is becoming increasingly compelling and resonant. The concept of local
resilience, and of finding ones way back to our place in our immediate eco cultural landscapes is
highlighted by how distant most of us have become from the basic processes of sustenance
required to survive and thrive. It is this disconnection that lends itself to a commodification of
every aspect of life, and to the profoundly disturbing nature of modern food production, which
configures animals as machines, to exploit seemingly without limits. The existence of factory
farming and all of it attendant consequences, to the land base, water systems, human health and
inherent cruelty to the animal and workers within this system, does nothing to inspire
acknowledgement and appreciation for the food that we eat. Conversely, when systems of food
and culture were localized, an appreciation of the land, animal, and water systems was inherent
in the ritualized aspects of life. One of the most compelling rituals found in rural traditions
throughout the world is some form of the animal blessing—a time when the animals that lived
alongside a family and community were brought together in an act of public appreciation. Within
these rituals, often framed within the dominant socio spiritual context, a deep appreciation of the
role of life, death, the animal as collaborator, and the understanding of ecological relationships
flourished. The animal became exalted, a source of life and a reminder of the cyclical nature of
our reality. The working or slaughter of an animal was understood within a system of
interrelationships that required an appreciation and gratitude for their role in the survival of a
community. In my presentation, I begin with an exploration of these rituals then and now
throughout the world, beginning with my own personal exploration of this theme in my short
film "Animal Blessings". This film was intended to re create this ritual within the context of my
grandmother's life, a ritual that has disappeared with the post war industrialization of rural Italy. I
explore the origins of animal blessing rituals and the ways in which this practice has evolved in
communities that seek to continue or revive a deep connection the land and animals that sustain
them. To view Animal Blessings film go to: https://vimeo.com/32122825 password "animal".
Fabienne Meiers
The Urban Horse: Equestrian Traffic and Horse Husbandry in Late Medieval Cities
University of Luxembourg Research Unit: IPSE Research Project: Villux 9 – Histoire des villes
luxembourgeoises Campus Walferdange Route de Diekirch L-7220 Walferdange, Luxembourg
[email protected]
Just like cattle, sheep and pigs, horses were part of the still agrarian influenced late medieval
townscapes. Written and iconographic sources as well as archaeological evidence give proof of
the extensive presence of horses inside and outside the city walls and their indispensability for
urban and interurban communication and mobility.
Since the thriving of European cities in the High Middle Ages, there was an increasing
demand for faster and more reliable exchange services comparable with those in the Roman
Empire: after the decline of the well-developed and regularly maintained Roman road network,
circulation of people, goods and services had become less effective, particularly given that
carriage traffic was virtually impossible on deteriorated roads. Consequently, equestrian traffic
gained more importance in the medieval period, as much in long-distance travels as in shorter
day’s journeys. In order to facilitate urban mobility and communication between cities, traffic
policies were developed, which consisted of road works, institutionally controlled mounted
courier services and provision of courier horses, as well as travel allowances and travel horses
for hire. In addition, the authorities adopted decrees that regulated animal waste disposal and
corpse removal to guarantee a hygienic living environment for both humans and animals. At the
same time specialized systems and structures for urban horse husbandry arose, e. g. stables for
mounts of the city authorities and distinguished guests; on the other side, horse rental stations for
the middle class emerged in the late 15th century.
The paper displays the characteristics, capacities and limitations of urban equestrian
traffic and horse husbandry in the Late Middle Ages and presents the importance and impact of
the human-horse relationship in the urban environment. Pragmatic documents such as (travel)
account books and legal texts were used as primary source base alongside municipal chronicles;
they were analyzed using a comparative and quantitative methodology. In addition, reflections of
the urban horse in material culture are also considered in order to emphasize a more dynamic
dimension of the phenomenon. To conclude, the value of the urban horse either as a daily
companion or as a mere commodity in medieval townscapes is discussed.
Michele Merritt
Depressed Dogs, Heartbroken Humans, and a new Philosophy of Emotions
Department of English and Philosophy, Arkansas State University, Jonesboro, Arkansas
[email protected]
In this paper, I defend three claims: first, the standard view of emotions, namely, that they play
little to no significant role in cognition, is flawed. Related to this first claim, I further argue that
the way the term ‘emotion’ is defined and deployed across several disciplines belies some very
compelling findings about what emotions might actually be and how they function. Last, I argue
that rethinking emotions in the ways I suggest is a project already underway in many ethological
studies of human-animal interactions, but in particular, those interactions taking place between
humans and dogs.
To support these claims, I begin by briefly examining recent discussions of emotions in
humans and why it is likely misguided conceive of them as entirely distinct from rational thought.
Next, I explicate and defend an even more contentious claim, that emotions are not simply
internally experienced phenomena, but are often, instead, highly interactive – they can be
genuinely shared. Background emotions, in particular, create a sort of atmosphere that is best
described as being generated and maintained in groups of two or more. Further, it is well
documented that the synchronicity resulting from long-term interactions, if disrupted, such as in
the case of a long-term partner dying, can be both physiologically and cognitively damaging.
Finally, I argue that these sorts of interactive emotions exist among the relationships we share
with animals, as well as in the relationships animals share with one another. I explore several
examples from Braitman’s (2014) recent work, wherein she discusses a wide variety of nonhuman animal emotions and how they are implicated. Of all the cases she discusses, however,
the dog-human pairings most resemble human-human interactions in terms of experiencing,
decoding, and responding to emotions. This claim is bolstered by some of the recent
neuroscientific findings (cf. Berns, 2013) regarding the way dogs’ brains process emotional
information. All of these studies taken together allow me to argue that when we interact with
other animals, especially dogs, the emotional resonance that emerges is not just a fleeting feeling,
but is a genuinely thoughtful interaction in which meanings are made and shared.
Berns, G. (2013). How Dogs Love Us: A Neuroscientist and His Adopted Dog Decode the
Canine Brain. New Harvest Press.
Braitman, L. (2014). Animal Madness: How Anxious Dogs, Compulsive Parrots, and Elephants
in Recovery Help Us Understand Ourselves. Simon and Schuster.
H. Lyn White Miles & Ross van der Harst
The Art, The Artist: The Orangutan Chantek’s Paintings and Found Art Assemblages
Project Chantek, Department of Anthropology, University of Tennessee at Chattanooga
[email protected]; [email protected]
Chantek is a 30 year old enculturated orangutan raised in a human setting at the University of
Tennessee at Chattanooga and taught to use gestural signs based on the American Sign Language
for the deaf. Using a participant observation ethnographic approach, Chantek was raised with a
small group of caregivers in a family setting on the University campus. Research with Chantek
focused on gestural sign acquisition and conversational ability; cognitive processes such as selfawareness, deception, and imitation, and on cultural and contextual processes and the acquisition
of cultural memes (units of cultural understanding, behavior, and interaction with the
environment). The latter included Chantek’s use of material culture, including everyday artifacts
such as tools, computers, vending machines, as well as visual media such as painting, and arts
and crafts. Chantek was encouraged to explore the affordances of the medium provided but was
not directly instructed to paint and construct, much in the manner of a parent who urges
creativity but does not directly provide lessons. Chantek began to finger paint around two years
of age and exhibited patterns and techniques similar to young children. He learned to use brushes
and nontoxic tempera paint and exhibited a dramatic style with simple fluid mandala-like
images. He reached at least the stage of borderline representational art of human three and a half
year old children, with a circular image evoking a face with two dots for eyes. In later years his
paintings came to resemble Asian calligraphy. His craft projects included stringing macaroni and
beads, tying knots, and weaving feathers and string onto sticks in wall hangings, mobiles, and
other found art assemblage. He also made necklaces, armbands, and bracelets out of rawhide,
wire, twine, silk cord, and stone, jade, crystal, wood, and glass beads and objects. Interestingly,
he created complex knots, some with a double loop allowing a necklace to be adjustable.
Chantek also learned to draw simple geometric shapes and a few letters to spell his name, as well
as X and O shapes for tac ac toe game. Art studies with great apes show that individuals develop
distinctive styles and like human art an observer can recognize the style and attribute it to the
animal artist. Although conducted in a human setting, the art research with Chantek supports
recent claims for cultural capacity of great apes in natural settings.
Robert W. Mitchell1, Anne Perkins2, & Erica Feuerbacher2
Developing the Animal Studies/Anthrozoology Curriculum
1
Animal Studies Program, Department of Psychology, EKU
2
Department of Anthrozoology, Carroll College, Helena, Montana
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
In 2012, Richard Timmins and others organized, under the auspices of the International Society
for AnthroZoology (ISAZ) a post-ISAZ-conference meeting to discuss the development of a
curriculum for the Anthrozoology major. This meeting derived from a growing interest in such a
major, initiated by Carroll College’s Human-Animal Bond Program started in 2011 by Dr. Anne
Perkins, which offered students an Anthrozoology Major focusing on developing skills for
understanding and working with animals, with specialized training in working with either dogs
or horses. Independently, Robert Mitchell and others at EKU initiated an Animal Studies major
in 2010, which includes in the curriculum courses in applied fields (agriculture, animal training,
co-op, law, conservation) as well as sciences (biology, physical anthropology, psychology) and
humanities (cultural anthropology, history, literature, philosophy, sociology). The Animal
Studies major also requires elective courses for students to develop specialized training in their
area of interest. Both programs offer unique opportunities for students to gain exposure with, and
learn about, animals and animal-human interaction. Representatives from each program will
discuss their program, and open a discussion as to the kinds of courses and experiences each
program offers, and if it is useful to develop a common curriculum across both majors to further
develop student involvement in the interdisciplinary field of study supported by ISAZ.
Brett Mizelle
Mary Griffith’s Odd Future: Real and Imagined Human-Animal Relationships in Antebellum
America
American Studies Program, California State University, Long Beach
[email protected]
Mary Griffith’s utopian novel “Three Hundred Years Hence” (included in Camperdown; or
News from Our Neighborhood, 1836), posits an odd future. Her time traveller, Hastings, learns
that in this future where machines provide labor and “brutal pastimes, thank heaven, have been
entirely abandoned,” that “the races of horses, asses and mules are almost extinct” and “so great
a curiosity now to the rising generation, that they are carried about with wild beasts as part of the
show.” Because of an epidemic of hydrophobia that killed “upward of one hundred thousand
people,” all dogs have been exterminated and “the breed will never be encouraged again.” Cattle
are pastured within moveable wire fences, but hogs and sheep are no longer allowed “to run
loose in the streets or on the road.” At the butcher’s market there were “no sights of blood, or
stained hands, or greasy knives, or slaughter-house smells.” Furthermore, “The meats were not
hung up to view in the open air, as in times of old; but you only had to ask for a particular joint,
and lo! A small door, two feet square, opened in the wall and there hung the identical part.”
This paper uses Griffith’s vision of a progressive future as an index to human-animal
relationships in antebellum America, arguing that her twenty-second century world engaged real
concerns expressed by nineteenth-century Americans about public health, animal welfare,
popular entertainment, and market culture. The mass killing of dogs, disappearance of horses and
mules, and the distribution of meat free of the traces of killing and processing in Camperdown
reflects debates in the public sphere about the usefulness and visibility of non-human animals.
After tracing the contestation over animals and meat in nineteenth-century America, one that
produced greater separation between human and nonhuman animals, I conclude by asking
whether our own visions of living with animals might similarly produce a world with fewer
animals in it and greater distance and concealment in the human-animal relationships that remain.
Robert Michael Morrissey
Tall-Grass Ethnohistory: Indians, Europeans, and Other Animals in the Prairie Borderlands
Department of History, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, Urbana IL, 61801
[email protected]
Recently, ethnohistorians have developed new insights about Native societies in the pre-contact
and colonial eras by focusing on the relationship between human cultures and the specific places
where they took form. Meanwhile, scholars of anthrozoology or human-animal studies have
developed new insights into human cultures by focusing on the relationship between people and
non-human species with whom they made their lives. This essay tries to bring people, place, and
animals together to imagine a new history of the Illinois Indians, who migrated to the tall-grass
prairies of the Illinois Valley in the 1500s precisely because of animals, building an entire
lifeway as pedestrian bison hunters in a distinctive landscape. They put bison at the center of
their cultural and spiritual lives, as revealed through archaeology, abundant contact-era linguistic
evidence, and often-ignored material culture. And yet their human-animal relations were not
static. European colonists brought not only new species to the tall-grass prairies, but also wholly
new ideas about the relationship between people and non-human nature. From 1500 forward, the
Illinois’ interactions with wild animals, game, and newly arriving livestock changed more in a
few centuries than they had for millennia. Animals' social, cultural, and economic functions
were reshaped by commodification and the market. As this happened, humans remade the
communities they had built around intimate and cooperative interactions between species. In this
paper, I consider how animals acted as ontological subjects in Illinois culture, playing roles as
other-than-human persons in myth and ritual, as well as material actors in everyday life. I also
sketch ethnohistorical techniques for recovering these relationships, and for understanding
change-over-time in human-animal relational ontologies in indigenous cultures like the Illinois.
Pegah Naghib
Effect of Music on Horses
Veterinary School, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran
[email protected]
Horses have played an important role in human life in different situations like industry,
transportation, agriculture, and sports for centuries. Researchers have studied the effect of resting
qualities on psychological stresses in horses. Most of this research concerns the evaluation of the
effect of horse fatigue on stress and physical behavior.
In this study, I examined the effect of housing condition on psychological stresses and the
effect of music on abrasion wound healing in horses. Three 4-years-old horses which were sent
to Shahriyar veterinary hospital had been driven for 72 hours with abrasion wounds on their
fetlocks. Long distance, warm weather, and wound’s pain have made them invasive. Their
wounds were washed with Ringer lactate, debridemented and bandage by the usage of Silver
sulfadiazine. Two of the horses were sent to stalls covered by straw and fed well. For the first
horse, classical music, and for the second one the Jazz music, were played for 12 hours a day
during the research, while the third horse was kept in stall with a concrete surface but was fed
well, and classical music was played.
The horse that was kept in a straw stall with classical music became calm and its wounds
recovered after thirty days while the horse in concrete stall with same music recovered
completely after eighty days. The horse kept in a straw stall with jazz music had not recovered
by the end of the trial, and its wound was more invasive than before.
It can be inferred from this research that the Jazz and classical music effects the psyche of
horses in that it had an effective result on their both action and the period of treatment. People
can influence the mental and physical healthiness of horses, and psychological curing is an
important part in speeding up the healing of horse’s body.
Amy Nelson,
Canine Agency in the Soviet Manned Spaceflight Program
Department of History, Virginia Polytechnic Institute, Blacksburg, Virginia
[email protected]
Sparked by geo-political rivalry and fueled by a faith in progress, a veneration of science and
technology, and a determination to harness nature to human ends, the space race might be
considered a quintessentially human drama. Yet in the years before Yuri Gagarin ushered in the
era of human space travel, many of the milestones in this unique Cold War contest were claimed
by dogs. Indeed from the initial clandestine launches of “rocket dogs” in 1951, to the highly
publicized, doomed voyage of “Laika” in 1957 and the celebrated journey of “Belka” and
“Strelka” in 1960, the prospects for human space flight were measured against the fates of the
stray dogs Soviet researchers used to test life support systems and investigate the effects of space
flight on living organisms. Like other experimental subjects, the space dogs functioned as
“boundary objects” providing a critical node of translation across different social worlds. Taking
the multivalent resonances of the “boundary object” as a starting point, this paper examines the
relationships between humans, dogs and a set of historically conditioned scientific practices to
contextualize the agency of the soviet space dogs. Extending recent research on “companion
species” and animal agency it shows how interactions between people and dogs in the laboratory
both reinforced and ran counter to the evolving celebrity of the space dogs, and suggests that
these animals’ significance as historical subjects involves what they did and their relationships
with human researchers, as well as how their contributions to the space race were perceived.
Jopi Nyman
Rereading Sentimentalism in Anna Sewell’s Black Beauty: Affect, Performativity, and Hybrid
Spaces
Department of English, University of Eastern Finland, Joensuu Campus
[email protected]
Anna Sewell’s 1877 horse novel Black Beauty is a classic work displaying human cruelty and the
insignificance of equine life in Victorian Britain. Through its portrayal of the life story of Black
Beauty from idyllic childhood to old life, narrated, as its original subtitle put it, as an
“autobiography” told by the horse himself, the novel criticizes contemporary animal-keeping
practices and ideologies. What is particularly significant is that the episodic and chronicle-like
narrative puts forwards its message of humane treatment by using sentimentalist discourse. In
Black Beauty, this emphasis on affect is, however, more than a mere strategy seeking to support
the ideological thesis of the novel as it is based on both historical and cultural ways of relating to
animal Others. The paper proposes a critical re-reading of Black Beauty that aims to read its
sentimentalism as a form of renegotiating the human–animal relationship amidst Victorian
modernity. What is at stake here is that the animals represented in Black Beauty, and the
discourse making them accessible, appear to enter a space conventionally defined as human and
dominated by human values and seek to redefine its preferred roles and identities. This means
that the novel’s animal representation is involved in the redefinition of the dichotomies such as
the human and the animal as discussed traditionally in Western philosophy since Descartes. As a
sign of this, the act of providing the “dumb beast” with a voice and history in the novel is not a
mere act of anthropomorphizing the animal. Rather, the representation of Black Beauty as a
speaking animal capable of story-telling aims to challenge the alleged human monopoly over
reason, and human identity as the natural way of being in the world, as the novel promotes
hybridized spaces of animal performance reconstructing conventional human–animal relations
and producing new sites of interconnections.
Reiko Ohnuma
Animal Doubles of the Buddha
Department of Religion, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire
[email protected]
Two centuries’ worth of work on the rich narrative traditions surrounding the life of the
historical Buddha have thus far failed to pay any attention to the episodes in his life involving
close interactions with animals. This paper will focus on three such episodes, taken from Pali and
Sanskrit sources from premodern India. In interpreting these episodes, I will argue that the
animals featured within them constitute “doubles” or shadows of the Buddha—illuminating his
character through identification, contrast, or parallelism with an animal “other.” The Buddha’s
horse, Kanthaka, serves as a scapegoat for the Buddha, mutely bearing the brunt of the criticism
and grief brought about when he first renounces the world—and suffering death as a result. An
elephant named Parileyyaka serves as a mirror for the Buddha, paralleling and thus validating
his questionable actions in an episode in which the Buddha becomes annoyed by his own
followers and abandons them, retreating into the forest without telling anyone and spending three
months in the company of this elephant. Finally, another elephant named Nalagiri serves as a
billboard for the Buddha’s power: Enraged with toddy and set loose against the Buddha in order
to assassinate him, he is immediately tamed by the Buddha’s charisma and power in a public
spectacle that converts millions of living beings to Buddhism—all through the force of the
Buddha’s person itself and without a drop of the Buddhist teaching. Yet even while these
“doubles” of the Buddha are made to serve their functions as scapegoat, mirror, and billboard,
the episodes in question also engage in several strategies of asserting human superiority to
animals and human dominance over the animal world. The Buddha, too, thus shares with the rest
of humanity a contradictory desire to both dominate and find oneself reflected in the animal
“other.”
Michał Piotr Pręgowski
Dog training as taming, dogs as wild beasts. Whispering versus canine science.
Department of Administration and Social Sciences, Warsaw University of Technology, Poland;
Fulbright Scholar-in Residence, Animal Studies Program, Department of Psychology, EKU
[email protected]; [email protected]
The human bond with dogs is multidimensional and unique. Dogs work with humans, train with
them, serve (some of) them, and provide them with psychological and emotional support. They
routinely leave human homes and are present in the public space, becoming unintentional actors
of the contemporary social life; many Western societies are preoccupied with topics such as dog
parks, dog waste or dog-friendly premises. Many people in the West describe dogs as members
of their families (Beck and Katcher 1983; Sanders 1999), play with them like they do with
children (Smith 1983; Serpell 1989), and communicate with them as with conspecifics (Mitchell
and Edmonson 1999; Sanders 1999). In the well known nature-culture divide, dogs stand out
from other animals (Nature) and can be seen as a part of the human domain (Culture). At the
very least dogs maintain a special status of a borderline species, one that resides in both entities
at the same time (Haraway 2003).
This presentation discusses how dogs, as well as their relationship with humans, are
constructed in the field of dog training in the 21st century. Canis lupus familiaris is nowadays a
very well-known species; canine science shed light at its behavior, evolution and cognition,
particularly in the last 25 years, and facilitated a better understanding of the species. The dog
ceased to be a “beast”, an unpredictable wildling of Nature subject to “taming”, something
visibly reflected in contemporary dog training philosophies grounded in academic findings. At
the same time, some dog trainers, including TV celebrities Cesar Millan and Jan Fennell,
disregard the academic findings and replace them with own founding myths. Their quasimystical approach offers alternative, unfounded in science, or simply outdated, explanations of
dogs and the rules of human-canine relationship. In such programs the metaphorical “beast”, a
fictional creature laid down to rest by science, is revived.
Helena Pycior
Collective Memory of the “First Dogs”: Privilege and Power of the “First Families” of the
United States
Department of History, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
[email protected]
The “first families” of the United States have used their privilege and power to embed their dogs
in the American collective memory. This paper analyzes three major stages in the promotion of
the memory of the “first dogs.” Focusing on Laddie Boy (Harding), Rob Roy (Coolidge), Fala
(Franklin D. Roosevelt), and Barney (George W. Bush), the paper traces the evolution of first
dogs from family pets to memorable historical figures through first families’ production of or
support for archives, books, statues, paintings, photographs, and film/video that gave voice to
their dogs. Moving through the three stages (each associated with a period of high respect for
dogs), the paper documents increasing incorporation of first dogs into the American memory as
comforting companions to presidents and individuals in their own right.
During the first stage Florence Harding and Grace Coolidge—supporters of their period’s
humane movement—pioneered the incorporation of first dogs into the American memory.
Although she destroyed many of her husband’s papers following his death, Harding pointedly
preserved documents relating to Laddie Boy. Concerned that Rob Roy be remembered, Coolidge
insisted that she and the dog pose together for her official portrait as first lady. In the second
stage, as thousands of dogs served in World War II, Roosevelt and Margaret Suckley—a member
of Roosevelt’s extended family—raised the memory of first dogs to a new level encompassing
documentary heritage and museum exhibition. They assured that the library and museum set up
by Roosevelt included eight boxes of “Fala papers” and a “Fala exhibit.” Coinciding with
increasing public appreciation of the human-animal bond, the paper’s third (and present) stage
has been one of even higher visibility of first dogs in the American memory. Thus the FDR
Memorial unveiled statues of Fala and Roosevelt sitting close to one another. The stage has also
been marked by the opening of the Presidential Pet Museum and George W. Bush’s
popularization and memorialization of Barney in creative and personal ways. The Bush White
House produced six “Barney Cam” videos. More significantly, Bush publicly memorialized
Barney. After leaving the White House he painted Barney’s portrait and, upon the Scottie’s death,
released the portrait with a personal tribute to the dog. The American public has generally
interpreted such exercises of the first families’ power as “memory makers” for their dogs as a
welcomed affirmation of close bonds shared by dogs and people across the nation.
Ziba Rashidian
Epistemological Artifacts, or Death and the Specimen: Nabokov’s Butterflies, for Example
Department of English, Southeastern Louisiana University, Hammond, Louisiana
[email protected]
My talk is part of a larger project on “the Death of the Animal” (a title I borrow from Paola
Cavalieri). Why the death of the animal? “Death” serves as a three-part diagnostic: 1. It points to
our present location within the unfolding of the sixth great extinction, where the diversity of life
on the planet is shrinking, precipitating, for those who accept the fact, a kind of existential
loneliness and, culturally, a host of productions aimed at reasserting the presence of animals,
even as they (and their habitats) disappear. 2. It points to our accelerating biotechnological and
genetic manipulation of animal bodies and reproduction, challenging our understanding of what
an animal is, and 3. It points to the fact that as a society our most common interaction with
animal beings involves their deaths—whether it be the production of meat, animal
experimentation for medical or other purposes, or the gathering of “specimens” in order to learn
more about newly discovered and/or dying species and varieties. It is this last issue—the
gathering of specimens, the “killing” in order to know and/or to preserve—that my paper will
investigate.
A fundamental assumption of my project is that the transformation of our relationship to
the animal kingdom is an outgrowth of what Foucault analyzed in The History of Sexuality and
his seminars as biopower and its biopolitical instantiation. Animal Studies scholars have
assumed that the birth of biopolitics governing human populations is coextensive with the
increasing subjection of animal bodies and lives to biotechnological and other interventions. My
paper will use biopower as a lens to focus on the scientific work on Lepidoptera by the novelist
Vladimir Nabokov, a self-taught researcher in the field who served as curator of lepidoptera at
the Museum of Comparative Zoology at Harvard. He spent years collecting, dissecting, and
writing about hundreds of species of butterflies and developed theories about the migration and
evolution of the species he devoted great energy to understanding: Polyommatus blues. His
scientific labors—only belatedly appreciated by professional scientists—reveal the strange
mixture of devotion and destruction that underwrites the epistemology of the specimen. While
Nabokov may differ from other scientists in that his engagement with the field came as an
amateur enthusiast, his work registers the way in which biopower structures the modes, methods,
and measures of scientific inquiry into animal being.
Martha Robinson
Avian Encounters: Connecting with Bird Lives through Live Streaming and Contemporary Art
Art History, Concordia University
[email protected]
Posthumanist thinking about the animal is often detailed through examples pulled from
contemporary art. Contemporary art practices combined with mediated observations of bird life
offer an unprecedented opportunity for interconnectivity between species beyond historic
practices of understanding birds that encompassed collecting, cataloguing and labelling. The
Cornell Lab of Ornithology hosts a number of bird (nest) cameras, offering close observation of
several avian species including Osprey, Red-tailed Hawks and Barn Owls. Each species resists
and deploys a human-centred understanding of bird life to varying degrees, from the very
accessible models of hawk family life to the almost incomprehensible (for humans) solitude of
the Laysan Albatross. Live streaming, chat moderators, and in some cases local birders on the
ground (BOGS), assist viewers in negotiating an understanding of what it is like to be a bird,
confronting bird behaviours that sit in anthropomorphic registers, and those which bear little
resemblance to a human understanding of family life. The Cornell bird cams offer an opportunity
to recognize the worlding of each species, a term coined by Ron Broglio (2011)—and related to
Jakob von Uexküll’s Umwelt, or enveloping world—to describe an environment specific to each
animal which includes all that it needs to flourish, as well as a sense of how the environment
appears for the animal. It is this concept of animal worlding which confronts the limits of
posthumanist scholarship, and interrogates the possibility of understanding the animal’s
perspective without reducing it to our own.
Connectivity with the remote and almost inaccessible world of the Albatross through the bird
cams, twinned with the poignant documentation of photographer Chris Jordan’s series of
photographs of dead albatross young: Midway: Message from the Gyre, double the points of
contact between species. This surface contact, as Broglio describes it, offers a productive reading
of the experience of viewers of both works: a possibility of representation for the species within
its worlding. Kari Weil (2012) argues that without this possibility of offering animals selfrepresentation, there is no promise of overcoming the speciesism and perfectionist hierarchies
that are antithetical to the posthumanist turn. This paper argues that these works establish liminal
zones where an understanding of a species— through representation which does not privilege
human language, and is rooted in the real world of the animal—is possible.
Susan Rustick
Transforming Human Identity: Encounters in the Classroom through Animal Eyes
English Department, Edgewood College, Madison, Wisconsin
[email protected]
How can we humans ever know our authentic selves while holding unexamined assumptions
about our separation from other sentient beings?
Western traditions assume a utilitarian dualism in which humans are the subjects and
other-than human animals are the objects. This presentation proposes that we humans should
question our assumed superiority through the collapse of subject-object dualism and an
engagement with all sentient beings as a communion of subjects. To quote from the PBS
documentary, My Year as a Turkey, “[Humans] do not have privileged access to reality.”
To demonstrate, this presentation will examine data from a first year college seminar
course, developed and taught by the presenter, which encourages students to look through the
lenses of the other-than-human. Such a change in perception has the potential to transform
human identity from a subject-object consciousness into a realization of humanity’s interdependence. Further, this transformation, if it occurs, might frame the students’ investigation of
the course goals of self-knowledge and their role in building a just and compassionate world.
Initially, a student’s assumption might be that who eats what or whom rightfully forms
the basis of an intrinsic value hierarchy. If this is critically questioned, the student might also
consider what eats humans, from bacteria and viruses to well-equipped larger predators. In
addition to philosophical questions, course content includes animal capacities and qualities, and
the human-animal bond.
Final papers delineate student development through the semester, from their initial selfreport of their awareness, proceeding through pivotal learning experiences and reflections, and
culminating in the answers to the three course questions through the lens of their knowledge of
themselves as an animal among animals: “Who am I? Who could I become? What is my role in
building a just and compassionate world?”
Bob Sandmeyer
"The Value of a Varmint"
Department of Philosophy, University of Kentucky, Lexington
[email protected]
The title of this talk is taken from an early essay by Aldo Leopold titled "The Varmint Question
(1915)." In this short piece, Leopold expresses strong hostility towards predatory animals in the
policy of game protection. In my presentation, I will trace the development of Leopold's views
on animals from his work with the Forest Service in early years of the last century to his last
years as Professor Emeritus of Game Management in Wisconsin. Leopold is an important figure
for animal theorists to study precisely because of his pioneering role in game, or what today we
call, wildlife management. In my presentation, I will show how, as his views matured, he gained
a deep appreciation for the role of predatory populations in the stable and integral functioning of
the land ecology. I will thus highlight the functional value of animals in his conception of the
land ethic. Perhaps Leopold's most well-known writing is his succinct articulation of his own
revelation on this matter in the essay called "Thinking Like a Mountain." To think objectively
like a mountain is to think ecologically and thus to recognize the central role of the predator, that
is, the wolf, in the mountain ecology of the Southwest. I will argue that the central question for
any Leopold scholar interested in animal studies, however, concerns the precise valuation of the
animal in his ethical theory. Do animals have value merely insofar as they preserve the integrity,
stability, and beauty of the land? Or is there a place for the intrinsic valuation of animals in
Leopold's land ethic? This is an important question, since if the answer is yea to the former but
nay to the latter then the land ethic not only allows for but commands the culling of destabilizing
"varmint" populations for the benefit of land health. This would by extension, it has been argued,
include theoretically the application of such a policy to human populations. I will present
evidence both from his writings and from anecdotes in his personal life that speak against this
fascistic interpretation of the land ethic. I will argue, in other words, that there is a place for the
intrinsic valuation of animals in Leopold's philosophy, however weakly expressed this may be in
his extant corpus.
Julia Schlosser
Walking the Dog: An Exploration of Recent Lens-Based Images of Companion Animals
California State University, Northridge
[email protected]
Traditionally, photographic images of pet or companion animals have existed on the periphery of
the contemporary fine art field, maligned for their assumed sentimental or nostalgic qualities.
However in the last twenty years, mirroring the rise of the animal studies discipline, a new and
significant aesthetic has emerged in the production of fine art images of pets. These images are
produced by contemporary artists using lens-based media, photography and video, in which an
“indexical” or one-to-one correlation exists between the animal and its image. This new aesthetic
acts in opposition to an older, more modernist use of images of pet animals in which pet animals
are considered anthropomorphically, and are denied a presence as individuals. In this talk, I will
trace the development of my own photographic and video-based works over the last fifteen years,
beginning with my early Spectra Polaroid images and ending with my most recent series of
photographic work entitled “Tether” (images from this series will be on view in the Crabbe
Library on the EKU campus during the course of the conference). In tandem, I will examine the
work of key artists who have influenced my work, as well as the major theoretical ideas, which
ground the work of these artists.
Over the last fifteen years, I have lived with and cared for eleven cats, one dog and one
rabbit. In my personal artwork, I primarily explore the lived experience of cohabitating with this
group of companion animals. I have investigated our mundane, daily interactions, employing
visual strategies designed to shift or subvert my persistent position of power in the group as both
sole human animal and image-maker. I often photograph without looking through the viewfinder,
using the camera’s self-timer to take sequential timed exposures. The resulting photographs
examine issues relating to domesticity, tending and care taking, negotiation of the needs of
various members of the household, the changing requirements of aging pet animals, my own
aging body, domestic hygiene and finally the mourning process which occurs as various
members of the household have died. I capture intimate views of the many, varied relationships
formed among and between the members of this interspecies community. As I investigate my
own work, I will also consider the artwork of John Divola, Martha Casanave, Keith Arnatt,
Carolee Schneemann, and Steve Baker among others.
Martha Sherrill
Animal Assisted Therapy for Adults with Communication Disorders: an Ethnographic Approach
to Therapeutic Human/Animal Interactions.
Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign
[email protected]
Animal Assisted Therapy is the addition of a trained animal (usually a dog) as an element of the
intervention provided by a professional, designed to meet specific criteria and goals in the
process of treatment. It is widely used at this time in the fields of mental and behavioral health,
education, and rehabilitation therapies (occupational, physical, recreational, etc.). AAT is
sporadically implemented in the field of communication disorders for pediatrics with
developmental or behavioral delays, as well as with adults with acquired neurogenic
communication disorders (traumatic brain injury, stroke, and dementia) without specific
pedagogical guidelines for clinicians. Despite its popularity in other fields, questions remain
regarding the ethical treatment and use of animals to address speech/language disorders, as well
as the potential for setting specific and realistic goals for treatment.
Current research in communication disorders has focused largely on pediatric interactions
with therapy animals (Severson 2014, Solomon 2010, Hurley 2014), and the social/motivational
impact of therapy animals in group settings such as skilled nursing facilities (Richeson 2003,
Berry et al 2012, Steed 2002). While clinical and anecdotal evidence abounds regarding the
positive social, motivational, and communicative impact of animals on adult patients, limited
research has been completed to establish methodological practices and recommendations for
Animal Assisted Therapy and adults with brain injury. To begin to answer these questions, a
connection between therapy animals and improved communication in adults with communication
disorders must be clearly established.
In my presentation I will present the preliminary findings of my research that targets
specifically the question of the relationship between the physiological and psychological
foundations of AAT and improved communication in adults with acquired communication
disorders. Results suggest that patients with a strong connection to animals prior to their injury
view animals as a communication partner, and therefore participate and communicate in AAT
sessions with greater success. These findings are based on observation and participation in
individual AAT sessions with a therapy dog at a rehabilitation hospital, and interviews with
patients and caregivers following intervention/interaction with therapy dogs. Suggestions for
future research include exploring the purposeful methodological treatments that meet both the
therapeutic goals of the patient and the ethical treatment and participation needs of the animal.
Jennifer Blevins Sinski
“A cat-sized hole in my heart”: Public Perceptions of the Companion Animal Adoption Process
Department of Sociology, University of Louisville, Kentucky
[email protected]
The companion animal adoption process differs between public shelter, non-profit all breed
shelter, non-profit all breed rescue and non-profit breed specific rescue organizations. While
some organizations require an application and fee, others require telephone interviews, vet
interviews and home visits. Little research has been done to date examining the difference in
organizational process and the impact on the potential adopter. This presentation provides the
results from a survey of 360 Bark magazine readers and 36 in-depth interviews. Adopters
viewed the adoption of companion animals as an ethical or moral obligation and their insights on
the experience are contained within. The researcher utilizes New Social Movement and Frames
theory to position adoption as social activism.
Gillian Squirrell
Working Dogs Working Lives: Cost-effectively Frustrating Human and Animal Disposability
Working Dog LLC, USA; Working Dog (UK) Ltd, U.K; Forget-Me-Not Farm, Sonoma, CA,
USA; & Humane Society University, Washington D.C., USA
[email protected]; [email protected]
This paper outlines the development of a multi-agency animal based intervention working with
adults sentenced to drug rehabilitation post incarceration and populations of shelter animals.
Working Dogs Working Lives works with the powerful human-canine bond to help
clients towards transformative learning and change. Over several years of field testing Working
Dog LLC has devised three novel programs helping clients with interpersonal skills, selfmanagement and employability. All three dog- based programs are grounded in the meta
principles of reward-based learning for the dogs and the clients and the modeling of pro-social
behaviors between species and within species. Through these program principles participants are
encouraged to think about motivation, encouragement, expectations and communications with
others, other species and with themselves.
The content detail of the various modules depends on the assessed learning needs of the
clients. The paper briefly outlines the context and content of the variations in the animal assisted
intervention. This includes discussion of an outreach program where dogs and activities travel to
clients; an immersive supervised work-placement at a humane society or shelter and a mentoring
program, requiring weekly volunteering to sustain pro-social commitment combined with the
opportunity to check-in with a mentor. The work placement, WorkOut, has three possible
different sets of learning goals depending on the learner’s needs. The content is shaped towards
either, a focus on basic and interpersonal skills, the development of pre-employability
expectations and skills or thirdly, a vocational module geared towards entry to pet or animal
industries. The paper briefly offers some findings from these interventions.
The paper explores the some of the architecture of Working Dogs Working Lives and
explicitly foregrounds:
 the theory of change underpinning and driving the intervention;
 the espoused model of change adopted within the intervention
 the assumptions about why the animal is integral to this animal assisted intervention
 the adoption of metrics for the consideration of the well-being and welfare of the animals
involved
 the nature of the assessment of what may be of service to the animals in the quest for
them to be adopted
 the way in which evaluative data may be collected from program participants
It is argued that this architecture is essential for an ethical and mutually beneficial animal
assisted intervention.
Finally the paper argues for the replication of Working Dogs Working Lives, a cost
effective, effective and manageable intervention.
KiriLi N. Stauch, Stephanie AuBuchon & Ellen Furlong
Domestic Dogs’ Understanding of Intentional and Goal Oriented Action
Department of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University, Bloomington, Illinois
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Human infants understand social cues such as goals and intentions. This sensitivity to social cues
is not uniquely human, as non-human primates share similar abilities (Call, Hare, Carpenter, &
Tomasello, 2004; Phillips, Barnes, Mahajan, Yamaguchi, & Santos, 2009). Is this complex social
reasoning uniquely primate? To explore this question we turned to the domestic dog. Dogs
possess some social cognitive skills, but do they also understand intentions and goals? In Study 1
we explored dogs’ understanding of intentions by presenting dogs with a researcher either unable
(i.e. she dropped a treat) or unwilling (i.e. she offered a treat and then pulled the treat away) to
provide the dog with a treat. Just like nonhuman primates, dogs spent significantly more time
close to the researcher when she was unable to provide the treat and significantly more time
away from when she was unwilling to provide a treat. These findings suggest that domestic dogs
understand human intentional action at least as well as non-human primates (Call, et al., 2004;
Phillips, et. al, 2009). In Study 2 we turned to dogs’ understanding of goals. Here we used a
violation of expectations paradigm (Woodward, 1996) in which dogs watched a researcher reach
for a ball on the right side of a stage and ignore a duck on the left. Next, the ball and the duck
switched sides – the ball was now on the left and the duck on the right. If dogs, like human
infants, treat the reaching as a goal to acquire the duck, they will spend more time looking (an
indication of surprise) when she reaches for the duck on the right (new goal, same movement)
than when she reaches for the ball on the left (same goal, new movement). If, however, the dogs
simply track movement, ignoring goals, they will look longer when the researcher reaches for the
ball on the left (old goal, new movement) than when she reaches for the duck on the right (new
goal, same movement). Dogs, like human infants, looked longer when the researcher appeared to
violate her goal suggesting that dogs encode the researcher’s goal much like nine-month-old
infants. Together, the findings from these studies suggest that domestic dogs share the ability to
understand intentional and goal oriented behaviors and that therefore these abilities are not
uniquely primate.
Linda J. Sumption
“The tiniest glance”: Narrative, Wildlife, and the Recognition of Intimacy
Department of English, Eastern New Mexico University, Portales, New Mexico
[email protected]
Researchers and writers devoted to the study and narration of wild creatures have, on the surface,
been viewed as extreme adventurers. The published work of Jane Goodall, Farley Mowat, and
Alexandra Morton – among many others – have intrigued a general readership for decades.
Their far wanderings, their committed solitude for months and years, and their willingness to
forego the comforts of home, have seemingly set them apart from the armchair travelers who
have so enthusiastically devoured their narratives of far-flung study.
However, as theorist Timothy Morton has suggested in The Ecological Thought, those
devoted chroniclers, the house-bound readers, and the subject wild creatures of those narratives
are part of “the mesh” of our existence, in which no actual centers or boundaries exist. He writes
that “really thinking the mesh means letting go of an idea that it has a center. There is no being
in the “middle” – what would the “middle” mean anyway? The most important? How can one
being be more important than another?”
This paper will investigate how the narratives of such writers as Goodall, Mowat and
Morton have collapsed that binary of the familiar and the extreme. I will investigate their
narrative “moves” which, in effect, bring the adventure home and destroy the artificial
boundaries that have previously denied our intimate experience of wildlife. There has been a
quiet revolution as a result of such narrative successes. Jane Goodall is as recognizable to us on
elite lecture stages, in fine clothes, as she is in photos from wild hillside homes of the
chimpanzees of Gombe. Alexandra Morton’s tales of orcas in Blackfish Sound and Echo Bay,
British Columbia now instantly connect to images of the troubled entertainment park SeaWorld
and the widely circulated documentary film, Blackfish.
As Lawrence Anthony notes in his narrative of life with an African elephant herd, “I had
finally grasped the essence of communicating with any animal, from a pet dog to a wild elephant.”
Here, months and years of patient, solitary interaction are delivered to thousands of general
readers. Anthony’s narrative expands the scene and slows the moment, through the delivery of
minute details and complex focalization. He reveals to us the significance of the “the tiniest
glance” between an enormous African elephant named Nana and a human being willing to
engage, and, in turn, wise enough to tell the tale.
Gwyneth Talley
Of Stallions and Men: Moroccan Masculinity in Traditional Horseback-riding
Department of Anthropology, University of California–Los Angeles
[email protected]
This paper presentation is based on my Master’s thesis that describes and analyzes the
interpretation of the Moroccan traditional horseback display known as fantasia or tbourida, as a
way to show masculinity through a traditional outlet. The fantasia consists of a group of horse
riders in traditional clothes armed with gunpowder rifles charging their horses about 200 meters
before simultaneously firing their rifles in the air. The beauty and difficulty of fantasia is the
synchronization–the charge of all the horses together and the simultaneous firing of the rifles, so
that only one shot is heard. The Arabic word for gunpowder is “baroud” and the display is often
referred to as tbourida, meaning “to release the powder” or “the powder games.” Riders are
mounted on Barb or Arab-Barb horses native to the region, typically all stallions as gelding is
seen as undesirable and useless horses.
Men of all ages participate in the fantasia and have various responsibilities within the
group as well as the performance of the fantasia at local festivals. Using Geertzian interpretive
anthropology, through the care of horses and using stallions for their warrior display we can
understand the interconnectedness of horses and Moroccan masculinity. Through historic and
modern participation in the fantasia we can understand how Moroccan men express their
masculinity via their horses, in their fantasia group, and their skills at the sport and how it relates
to the broader modern Arab masculine expectations.
Mary Trachsel
Ecological Consciousness Raising: Animal Studies in the Anthropocene
Department of Rhetoric, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa
[email protected]
Iowa governor Terry Branstad’s recent mandate that computer science be a required component
of Iowa high school science education illustrates the evolution of the competing forces American
historian Leo Marx described half a century ago as “the machine” and “the garden.” Branstad’s
suggestion that computer science courses can take the place of math or foreign language
instruction reveals the economic rationale behind the national emphasis on Science, Technology,
Engineering and Math. As humans rely more and more on computerized memory, digital
communication, and data management, a computer-savvy workforce becomes economically
imperative, and computer science becomes an increasingly promising field of employment.
Meanwhile, environmental scientists like Edward O. Wilson stress the need for an entirely
different kind of science education—one that cultivates intimate, experiential knowledge of the
natural world—one in which humans recognize themselves as one species among many in “the
garden.” Wilson’s approach to science education derives from the “biophilia hypothesis,” the
notion that scientific knowledge must combine with humanity’s innate aesthetic and emotional
inclination toward the natural world. If we don’t care about the natural world, Wilson and other
Biophilia-based science educators assert, we will not be motivated to take care of it, despite our
rational understanding of ominous changes in the biosphere.
In a STEM-dominated educational environment that stresses human communication with
machines, the interdisciplinary curriculum of Animal Studies can take the lead in modeling this
holistic, biophilic approach to science education. While embracing the scientific studies of
animal life and ecological systems, Animal Studies also employs the methods of the humanities,
including narrative, empathy, moral reasoning, aesthetic appreciation, interpretation and
imagination. My presentation argues that in the geologic era scientists are coming to call the
Anthropocene, holistic instruction in life science is more urgently needed than computer science
in isolation, and that Animal Studies is ideally positioned to lead the way.
Sarah Tsiang
Breeds for Needs: Type and Breed Names as a Reflection of the Horse-Human Relationship
Department of Languages and Literature, EKU
[email protected]
A language reflects the culture of its speakers. Horses have been a part of human history
for millennia, and since their domestication, horses have been bred to meet a variety of cultural
needs. Thus an examination of type and breed names may be revealing about the roles of the
horse in history and details of their cultural context. So, ‘Pit Pony’ recalls the little ponies who
used to work in the mines, while the ‘Tinker horse’ once pulled Gypsy wagons. The breed
names American Saddlebred and Tennessee Walking Horse evoke the image of a gentleman’s
riding horse, while the names Standardbred and American Quarter Horse recognize the racing
abilities of these breeds. The designation Thoroughbred highlights the strict attention to
bloodlines that typifies this breed. The importance of good breeding carries over into popular
speech, where “She’s a thoroughbred” is a compliment about having class.
Assertion of geographical and ethnic identities may be seen as new breeds in the New
World include ‘American’ in their names, while ‘Gypsy Horse’ contrasts with ‘Romany Horse’.
The small hobby horse of pre-Christian Ireland is recalled in the children’s toy hobby-horse, the
source of the modern word hobby, used to describe pastimes pursued for enjoyment. The Irish
Hobby is a related breed that is an ancestor of the Thoroughbred, and we can find “Irish hobby”
as slang for racing.
Of course a breed name book is not a historical record but it is an interesting source of
information about how horses and humans have interacted over time and how this interaction is
preserved in language.
Joseph Tuminello
Teaching with Foer's Eating Animals
Department of Philosophy and Religion Studies, University of North Texas, Denton, Texas;
Program Coordinator for Farm Forward
[email protected]
In this presentation, I provide an overview of the incorporation of Jonathan Safran Foer's book
Eating Animals in college and high school courses, including a discussion of its place in Animal
Studies curricula. While working with the nonprofit organization Farm Forward, I have
spearheaded a number of initiatives to promote the use of the book in educational settings.
Eating Animals has proven to be an accurate, accessible, and inspiring way to get students to
start thinking about relationships between human and nonhuman animals, as well as the nature of
food production and the exploitation of animals in industrial agriculture.
I chronicle the myriad ways that Eating Animals can and has been used by college and high
school educators in a variety of disciplines and interdisciplinary fields. I am in the process of
compiling an educational database which includes course materials, assignments, discussion
questions, and other activities centered around the book, as well as testimonials from educators
that have successfully incorporated it into their own courses.
Since 2012, I have organized and promoted an annual webinar series called the "Jonathan Safran
Foer Virtual Classroom Visits," where Foer has met with over 8000 students from college and
high school classes around the world to discuss ethical issues in food production, as well as
many of the other themes of Eating Animals. I provide an overview of this event, including a
brief clip of one of the 2014 sessions.
Jeannette Vaught
Animal Infiltrations: Teaching Animal Studies in Traditional Courses
Department of American Studies, University of Texas at Austin
[email protected]
While EKU has been successful in creating an animal studies major, and many institutions across
the US and internationally have approved animal studies courses in various disciplinary homes,
many of us spend most of our pedagogical time teaching core requirement courses. By
infiltrating core syllabi, animal studies scholars can extend the reach of animal studies ideas into
the undergraduate curriculum and work to normalize analysis of human-animal relations across
various disciplines.
At the University of Texas, I’ve taught two lower-level undergraduate seminars in
American Studies that do not focus on animals, but nevertheless incorporate animal studies
methodologies, texts, and topics. In this talk/workshop, I will share my strategies for bringing
animals into a syllabus that might be largely predetermined by departmental topic guidelines or
core course learning objectives. My first course, “The Scientific American,” provided a history
of modern scientific experimentation in the United States and lent itself well to animal topics.
The second, “The Cowboy Mystique in American Culture,” was a more straightforward history
course, yet animals enlightened the material in ways my students did not expect. It’s no news to
this audience that animal studies methodologies are excellent pedagogical tools for teaching race,
labor, and gender in broader historical contexts, and that students find scholarly work with
animals engaging. So as a major part of the talk/workshop, I wish to invite open discussion for
participants to share their own animal infiltrations in syllabi, exercises, and pedagogical
interventions in core courses from various disciplines across the humanities and the sciences.
Theo Verheggen
Embodied Cognition and Affect Attunement in Anthrozoological Research
Department of Anthrozoology & Cultural Psychology, Faculty of Psychology & Educational
Sciences, Open University of the Netherlands (OUNL), Heerlen, Netherlands
[email protected]
From cultural psychology and from the biology of cognition, I take the claim that the basic
orientation in human relations is an embodied one, not a purely intellectual one. Psychological
concepts like “ meaning”, “sense”, “identity” and even “mind” can only arise in interactions
between embodied agents, as I shall try to show. It is not required that these agents are all human.
Indeed, the elementary forms of meaning making can also be observed in human-animal
interactions, and even in animal-animal interactions such as in dogs or wolves. Most pet owners
know this of course; most psychologists and cognitive scientists tend to forget it.
I will argue that an embodied perspective on the origins of human cognition can be seamlessly
applied to the study of human-animal interactions. It may help us understand better how these
relations take shape, how they become meaningful, how they become persistent, and –also
important– how they may be operationalized in scientific research.
On this latter point, I intend to briefly address the work of Humberto Maturana and
Francisco Varela, Antonio Damasio, and Daniel Stern. These authors provide the theoretical
building blocks for an embodied view on cognition in which behavioral and affective attunement
are the key mechanisms. Stern’s theory has led to a few empirical studies within the field of
human-animal interactions. I will address how these studies have inspired our current research at
the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences at the Open University of the Netherlands,
in which we develop further techniques to study behavioral and affective attunement, as
important constitutive elements for the human-animal bond as well as for human and animal
cognition.
Debra A. Vey Voda-Hamilton
When People are in Conflict About Animals.
Attorney, Hamilton Law and Mediation, Armonk, New York
[email protected]
Conflicts arise, among people over the animals they love and care for, in animal welfare, human
animal bond studies, divorce, residential living arrangements, law enforcement, rescue and
veterinary malpractice to name only a few. These disagreements can be based on righteous
indignation, emotional misunderstandings, strong disagreements, hurt feelings or the need for
revenge. After the war of words has started, it is difficult to engage in a civil discourse toward
resolution. Disagreements like these often end up in court where the emotional part of the
argument, which is most important to the parties, is left unheard and therefore unappreciated.
After all, animals are still considered property under the law.
My discussion explores how a more ‘human and non human’ centered alternative dispute
resolution process, mediation, can be used to respectfully and confidentially support a
conversation on a disputed subject. Mediation provides the platform for healthy discussions
while also saving time, money and future relationships. Mediation is the means by which waring
parties can reach a peaceful end for the benefit of the animal(s).
Mediation assists parties in the exploration of many solutions to problems. The parties
can then self-determine an outcome that is the best for all. The mediator listens, understands,
appreciates and respects each parties position. Mediators hold a safe space for each party to
speak fully after which a possible shift in position to occur. The recent successful application of
mediation in divorce, medical malpractice and labor relation disputes shows promising results in
those venues and potential for it’s application in conflicts between people involving animals.
Courts are not equipped or allowed to respond to emotional arguments raised in support of
disputes involving animals. Animals are still seen as property in the eyes of the law. Mediation
encourages for party driven solutions to be explored from inception of a conversation to
agreement. Mediation is more responsive to the needs of the animal(s) as well as the people
involved in the disagreement. It provides a platform from which more party centered solutions
can be reached.
When living with animals, conflicts involving those animals arise and cannot always be
avoided. When they do arise, no one wants to stand by and watch as a bitter fight ensues which
can end in a less than responsive court order. My presentation explores how mediation can be
used effectively to find a more peaceful and efficient means to solution. Mediation is an animal
and people centric process. It can better handle conflicts between people involving animals
enabling everyone, human and non-human animal alike, to survive and thrive.
Brenden Wall, Anthony Bohner, Jeffrey Toraason, & Ellen Furlong
Good Dog! APPlications of Dog Science
Department of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Each year 6 to 8 million pet dogs enter dog shelters, approximately 60% of which are euthanized.
Unfortunately, many dogs are taken to shelters, returned to shelters, or euthanized, due to
behavioral problems such as fear, aggression, or separation anxiety. Many such behavioral
problems can be alleviated if dogs are afforded sufficient exercise; however, some owners are
unwilling or unable to provide dogs the exercise they need. Further, some dogs may be unable to
exercise due to physical limitations or other health concerns. One possible solution is, rather than
providing physical exercise, to provide dogs with mental exercise. We have developed a
cognitive “game” that could provide dogs this mental stimulation via interactive cognitive tasks
that owners could easily use to mentally engage their dogs. We are currently developing a series
of touchscreen computer tasks (such as object recognition and number discrimination tasks),
with the hope that, if dogs find them challenging and engaging, we can make them available to
owners broadly via an iPad app. Several dogs in our lab have learned to effectively use the touch
screen and have begun engaging with various cognitive games. In the near future, we hope such
touch screen task will transfer over into an app that will serve as an effective program to
minimize the amount of behavioral problems, and hence, the number of dogs sent to, returned to,
and euthanized in shelters.
Sara Waller1, Christopher Kloth2, & Mariana Olsen1
Cats Talk Back: Feral & Socialized
1
Department of Philosophy, Montana State University, Bozeman
2
Department of Philosophy, University of Nevada at Reno
[email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Little is known about feral cat vocalizations. Beyond the aversive response they elicit in humans,
the function of non-mating-based calls is unknown. Felis Catus in the feral state will congregate
into highly social, matrilineal colonies. Though usually solitary predators, cats cooperate in
young-rearing, with colonies often having “nursemaids” who feed many unrelated litters of
kittens, allowing mothers to hunt. While cats do not necessarily form male-female pair-bonds,
incidents of male cats hunting and sharing kills with colony kittens have been reported,
suggesting affiliative and even (possibly) altruistic responses among males. This study contains
both an empirical and a philosophical component.
The empirical study recorded the vocalizations of 12 feral cats in June, July, August and
September of 2014, with subjects ranging in age from 3.5 weeks to 14 months, as they were
exposed to humans in a foster home. Using Ramirez methods of friendly non-approach, strict
association of food with people, 11 cats were fully socialized and adopted into homes within 3
months, 1 was semi-socialized and became a barn cat. We discuss types of vocalizations
produced by the animals as they moved from shelter standards of feral to shelter standards of
socialized and adoptable.
The philosophical aspect of the work focuses on the nature of meaning and mental
representation in feline communication, honing notions of meaningfulness, intentionality, theory
of mind, and self-referentiality in response to the call categories the data suggests. We conclude
that studying human language alone fails to provide an accurate portrait of mental content as it is
revealed in communicative systems, and raise questions of possible infinite generativity in feline
call types and sequencing strategies.
Magdalen J. Walton
Killer Whales or Whale Killers? A Routine Activities Analysis Introducing Agency Among Orca
Whales During the Capture of Orca Calves
Department of Sociology, University of Tennessee at Knoxville
[email protected]
Animals in Human Society has become a topic of growing interest among many disciplines
including criminology. This field not only includes human interaction with pets and other
domesticated animals, but also wildlife. Orca whales have been animals of great interest to
human beings for many reasons: they are large, majestic, beautiful, and interesting creatures that
have become a focus of the entertainment and tourism industries for over a half of a century.
Despite these animals’ innocence and beauty they have become targets of poaching, capture, and
confinement. Routine activities theory, introduced by Cohen and Felson in 1979 was developed
as a criminological theory intended to explain the basic elements of time, place, objects and
persons in the development of crime and “routine activities.” In the past this theory has been
exclusively applied to crime and victimization of human beings. This paper seeks to explain how
it can be appropriately applied to the practice of hunting and capture of orca whale calves. In
addition to explaining criminal behavior and victimization of the human beings in this process, I
argue that adult orca whales can also serve as “guardians” of their offspring. With this I suggest
that orca whales not only possess feeling and emotions, but also agency to be involved in their
own policing within their pods.
Lucinda Woodward
Research and Development of the Pet Attribute Work Sheet (PAWS—for dogs)
Department of Psychology, Indiana University Southeast, New Albany, Indiana
[email protected]
An online survey of 1,345 dog owners nationwide presented 119 items from previous dog
behavioral rating forms to assess eight primary personality traits consistent with the interpersonal
circumplex theory of personality. Exploratory factor analysis (using a maximum likelihood
model with equimax rotation and Kaiser criterion, selecting only factors with Eigen value of 2.0
or higher ) identified recognizable octants aligned along the axes dominance and submission, as
well as one additional factor deemed “intelligence.” The final selection resulted in a 56-item
measure with four reverse scored items that demonstrated good psychometric properties
including internal consistency, and construct and predictive validity.
Miranda K. Workman
Euthanasia Decisions in the Sheltering Industry - A Critical Inquiry,
Canisius College,
[email protected]
Euthanasia decisions occur every day in animal shelters. In order to understand how those
decisions are made, this study investigated euthanasia decision making processes in shelters
across the United States. Respondents (n=62) to an online survey answered questions about
shelter demographics, euthanasia policies, variables for euthanasia candidacy, and specific case
studies. Only two-thirds (66%) of shelters were identified by respondents to have a written
euthanasia policy. Fifty percent (50%) of respondents indicate that medically-based euthanasia
decisions are not made by veterinary professionals. Medically based euthanasia decisions are
most likely to be due to terminal medical conditions, not treatable ones. Eighty-three percent
(83%) of respondents indicate that behavior-based euthanasia decisions are not made by animal
behavior professionals. Behavior-based euthanasia decisions are based primarily on risk
assessments and predicting likelihood of future behavior. The tools used to gather the
information are neither scientifically validated nor reliably predictive. The ethical perspectives
required to contemplate a medically-based euthanasia (Ethic of Care) are very different from that
necessary to make a behavior-based euthanasia decision (Utilitarianism/Deontology). In order to
improve euthanasia decision-making, each shelter should have a written euthanasia policy,
employ medical and behavior professionals to make those decisions, collaborate with researchers
to improve data collection leading to creation of better tools for behavioral assessment, and build
bridges with other organizations to help save lives.
Miranda K. Workman & Christy L. Hoffman
An Evaluation of the Role the Internet Site Petfinder Plays in Cat Adoptions
Canisius College
[email protected]; [email protected]
To better understand factors contributing to a cat’s adoption success, this study explored whether
there was an association between an adoptable cat’s popularity on the website Petfinder and the
cat’s length of availability on the adoption floor of a managed intake animal shelter. This study
also examined factors that contributed to a cat’s popularity on Petfinder and the percentage of
adopters who visited Petfinder prior to making adoption decisions. One-third of adopters
surveyed reported visiting Petfinder before adopting, and half of those had viewed their adopted
cat’s Petfinder profile. The number of clicks per day cats received on the site was negatively
correlated with length of availability. Age at adoption was positively correlated with length of
availability and negatively correlated with number of clicks per day. Primary coat color was a
strong predictor of number of clicks per day and length of availability. The only variable within
the photographer’s control significantly associated with number of clicks per day was whether
there was a toy in the photo. Although cats’ physical characteristics are strong predictors of their
popularity, strategic use of toys in cats’ photographs may promote adoptions of cats typically
overlooked.