AN INVESTIGATION OF A BULLYING PREVENTION PROGRAM IN SCHOOL SETTINGS A Proposal Presented to the Faculty of California State University, Stanislaus In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Psychology By Scott Michael Charlton February 2015 CERTIFICATION OF APPROVAL AN INVESTIGATION OF A BULLYING PREVENTION PROGRAM IN SCHOOL SETTINGS by Scott Michael Charlton Signed Certification of Approval Page is on file with the University Library Dr. Victor Luevano Associate Professor of Psychology Date Dr. Annie Guichard Associate Professor of Psychology Date Dr. Rosanne Roy Associate Professor of Psychology Date Dr. Emily Branscum-Higuera Lecturer of Psychology Date © 2015 Scott Michael Charlton ALL RIGHTS RESERVED DEDICATION This work is for the victim. Though the bent scales of injustice may not be equalized today, we will work for remedy. You are not alone. This work is dedicated to the bully. The fear and anger that lead to such hurtful actions must be tormenting. May you that harms others heal and find peace with our help. This work is dedicated to the bully-victim. With calm perseverance there will be genuine connections with others. Most of all, this work is dedicated to the defender. It takes courage and strength to swim against the stream. May we find the defender within and protect each other with zeal. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Funding for a small portion of this work was provided through a California State University grant. This work is possible because of the participation of the schools sites and administrators, especially Karen Vail and Kevin Triance. Over thirty students from CSU, Stanislaus supported this work through the assessment, kick-off event, classroom meetings, and data entry. One source of significant support was Adelin Ansari, who continued assessment and feedback for a year following the current work. Dr. Branscum’s guidance throughout the intervention was most helpful, but her inspiration has expanded my willingness to engage the juicy stuff that life has to offer. Dr. Luevano has been continually supportive, collaborative, and patient throughout the journey. Finally, I would like to thank Bruce and Colleen, amazing parents, who have championed my efforts and moral philosophy. v TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE Dedication ............................................................................................................... iv Acknowledgements ................................................................................................. v List of Tables .......................................................................................................... viii List of Figures ......................................................................................................... ix Abstract ................................................................................................................... x Literature Review.................................................................................................... 1 Bullying Roles ............................................................................................ Bullying Defined ......................................................................................... Types of Bullying Behavior ........................................................................ Gender Differences ..................................................................................... Negative Effects of Bullying ...................................................................... Bullying Prevention .................................................................................... Purpose and Hypothesis .............................................................................. 1 8 10 15 16 18 22 Method .................................................................................................................... 24 Participants .................................................................................................. Materials and Variables .............................................................................. Procedure .................................................................................................... Data Analysis .............................................................................................. 24 26 27 29 Results ..................................................................................................................... 30 Discussion ............................................................................................................... 35 Intervention ................................................................................................. Interpretation ............................................................................................... Limitations and Future Directions .............................................................. Applications ................................................................................................ 35 36 38 40 vi References ............................................................................................................... 41 Appendices A. B. C. D. E. Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire ........................................................... 62 Consent Letter ............................................................................................. 75 Parent Information Pamphlet ...................................................................... 76 Kickoff Event Script ................................................................................... 84 First Class Meeting Outline and Script ....................................................... 100 vii LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1. Participants Assessed with BVQ by School Site and Year............................... 24 2. Gender of Participants Assessed with BVQ by Year........................................ 25 3. A Comparison of the Likelihood of Reported Bullying Roles Before and After Intervention.................................................................................................... 31 4. A Comparison of Pre and Post Intervention Victimization Severity ................ 32 5. A Comparison of Genders on Bullying Role and Type for Assessment 1 ....... 33 6. A Comparison of Genders on Bullying Role and Type for Assessment 2 ....... 34 viii LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 1. Bullying Circle .................................................................................................. 2 2. Program Implementation .................................................................................. 28 ix ABSTRACT Bullying is pervasive across schools in the United States. The bullying circle describes the roles of bully, bystanders, defender, and victim. Bully-victims, students who are both victims and bullies, are of special concern. Bullying consists of direct and indirect behaviors that are mean and hurtful. Many problems are associated with bullying and can be reduced with intervention. Two elementary school sites instituted the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program which intervenes at individual, classroom, school-wide, and community levels. Intervention consisted of an initial assessment, kickoff event, classroom meetings, feedback sessions, and subsequent assessment. The Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire was used to assess 496 participants pre-intervention and 483 participants post-intervention in grades 3 through 8. Participants in all grades received the kickoff event and classroom meetings. Participants were less likely to be a victim (RR = 0.92), bully (0.97), or bully-victim (0.97) after program implementation. Victimization severity was lower after intervention (d = 0.09). Risk ratios further showed that participants were less likely to join a bullying situation underway (0.97) and more likely to defend a student being bullied (1.15). There was minimal support for gender differences pre intervention, wherein girls were at more likely to have indirect victim roles (1.10), while less likely to have direct victim roles (0.93). Additionally, boys were more likely to be a joiner in a bullying situation (1.11). These gender differences diminished post intervention. x LITERATURE REVIEW The American public has become more concerned about bullying of school children over the past decade (Dervin, 2011). Part of this increase in concern is due to high profile media coverage of school bullying cases (Noronha, 2012). These anecdotal cases begin to capture the seriousness of bullying in America. As a result, there has been increased awareness and surveillance of bullying behavior in schools; that is capturing the pervasiveness of bullying. Peleg-Oren, Cardenas, Comerford, and Galea (2012) examined data from 45,000 students in the United States and discovered that more than half of students reported being either a victim or bully in the previous 30 days. Research on childhood aggression increased in the late 1960’s. This research was generally conducted in natural environments, such as schools and child care settings. Dan Olweus, a pioneering bullying researcher described the nature of bullying and how to intervene. Olweus identified types of bullying behavior and the roles of children in bullying situations. The Olweus Bullying Prevention Program (OBPP) was developed using evidence-based practices (Olweus, 1997). OBPP has been manualized for use in the United States (Olweus et al., 2007). The main goals of OBPP are to establish contingencies that address bullying behavior and structure an environment that enables students to pursue prosocial roles. Bullying Roles Bullying occurs as a group process and often involves several children who assume specific roles within a given bullying situation. Olweus illustrates these roles 1 2 with the “bullying circle” (Juvonen & Graham, 2001). The bullying circle describes the roles that children and adolescents have within a given bullying situation. The major roles within the bullying circle are the bully, active and passive supporters, disengaged onlookers, victim, and defenders (see Figure 1). The OBPP defines the bullying roles with behaviorally descriptive terms, such as students who bully or target. Figure 1. Roles of students involved in bullying situations. Adapted with permission from “Olweus Bullying Prevention Program Schoolwide Guide,” by Dan Olweus, Copyright 2007 by the Hazeldon Foundation. Group bullying strategies coexist with one-on-one bullying. However, in group bullying situations, the roles can influence power differentials. For instance, when a bully recruits friends or acquaintances to assist bullying, that bully has more 3 social leverage and physical power over the victim. Empirical support for these roles has been demonstrated by researchers (Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996). The bullying roles are not fixed and can be utilized in intervention and prevention programs (Sutton & Smith, 1999). Bullying status is used to identify those who have been bullied, bullied others, or participated in both over a period of time. Self-reported information is used to identify bullying status and is a fair representation of the real experiences those students (Theriot, Dulmus, Sowers, & Johnson, 2005). Status labels the patterns of individuals’ role in the bullying circle. Victim The person who is the target of the aggression is the victim. The victim experiences the direct and subsequent negative consequences of the bullying event. These negative consequences stem from the aversive nature of aggression, which includes physical harm and/or social consequences. Emotional responses that the victims experience include fear, guilt, and shame (Camodeca & Goossens, 2005). The victim, by definition, has less power than the bully. Individual social factors are recognized by their peers. Victims are viewed by peers as unhappier and deficient in social skills (Fox & Boulton, 2005). A wide range of socially dysfunctional traits are also associated with those who experience victimization, such as increased self-isolation and emotional volatility (Frizzo, Bisol, & Lara, 2012). Victims may not utilize skills or elicit the aid of others to defend themselves because of differences in social ability or standing. 4 Mouttapa, Valente, Gallaher, Rohrbach, and Unger (2004) found that students with high friendship nominations are not victims, but low peer nominations is not a reliable predictor of victim status. The number of friendships may not predict victimization, but quality friendships play an important role in helping students to recover from the negative effects of bullying (Kendrick, Jutengren, & Stattin, 2012). Bullied students without quality friendships and poor social abilities are of significant concern, due to poorer outcomes (Glick & Rose, 2011). Bully The main, initial source of aggressive behavior is the bully. The bully can either engage in the aggressive behavior themselves or choreograph assistants. These assistants are also likely to be bullies (Farmer et al., 2010). The bully often has an immediate peer group who serve as assistants. Students who bully also tend to associate with controversial or deviant peers (Farmer et al., 2010). The bully’s immediate friendship group includes those who also participate in aggressive behavior (Mouttapa et al., 2004). Caravita, Gini, and Pozzoli (2012) found that bullies are often strong leaders and are well liked by teachers. Fellow students rate bullies high in popularity, even though they do not want to associate with the bully themselves. This means that the bully is often capable of gathering support from bystanders. There are a host of problems faced by those who bully others. For example, students who bully have an increased likelihood of future conduct problems and alcohol use (Peleg-Oren et al., 2012). Adolescent students who have a history of 5 bullying others are also at far greater risk of school delinquency. Students who bully others had higher levels of violence and other anti-social behavior ten years later (Bender & Lösel, 2011). Additional conduct related problems, such as drug use and rule violations, are higher for students who bully than non-involved peers (Radliff, Wheaton, Robinson, & Morris, 2012). The psychiatric diagnosis of Conduct Disorder has features associated with bullying, namely the violation of others’ rights (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Caravita et al. (2012) showed that students with the bully role have higher acceptance of moral transgression and higher emotional disengagement than their peers. These moral belief systems make it more acceptable for the bully to continue aggression and cause further victimization. Bully-Victims Students who have been bullied by others and have bullied others themselves are considered bully-victims. Pellegrini, Bartini, and Brooks (1999) and Schwartz (2000) termed these students “aggressive victims.” These aggressive victims will often retaliate against the aggressor after the fact or aggress against a student unrelated to their prior victimization (Swearer, Wang, Maag, Siebecker, & Frerichs, 2012). The connection between bullying and victimization events is not always clear and may not have any reciprocal basis. However, learned aggression is typically assumed in child samples and being bullied has been identified as a risk to being a perpetrator (Berthhold & Hoover, 2000). Bully-victims often cite their own victimization as part of the reasoning for their aggression (Schwartz, 2000). 6 Wienke Totura, Green, Karver, and Gesten (2009) demonstrated that bullyvictims tend have high rates of internalizing problems, like anxiety and depression. Teachers rate these bully-victims as moody, having poor relationships, and more learning difficulties than other students. Radliff et al. (2012) found that drug use and delinquency are highest in bully-victims compared to bullies, victims, and those described as noninvolved. The authors also found that bully-victims receive large amounts of discipline at school and lack the quality of friendships of those in other statuses. Unnever (2005) found that bully-victims are a distinct group from either bullies or victims. Bully-victims have different early socialization experiences and often experience more physical bullying. Disadvantageous psychological factors, such as low self-control, are highest in children who have the status of bully-victim. Camodeca, Goossens, Meerum Terwogt, and Schuengel (2002) argued that a bullied student may react to their lack of control by bullying others. Bully-victims rate higher in proactive and reactive aggression than victimized only peers. The bully-victim is uneasy and unable to genuinely connect with others. Perren, Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, Malti, and Hymel (2012) found that bully-victims find bullying an acceptable tactic for peer negotiation. The bully-victim approves of the violation of moral rules more than other students. These researchers suggest social skills training to promote empathy and cooperation. Bystanders Peer approval is highly regarded by students and is therefore highly influential (Crocker, Luhtanen, Cooper, & Bouvrette, 2003). Peer support can offer a strong 7 rewarding mechanism for a variety of behaviors, including bullying. This support comes from being accepted by specific peers and is demonstrated by continued friendships and enhanced popularity (Olthof & Goossens, 2008). There are two distinct categories of bystanders defined by the functional relationship to bullying behavior; students who actively support or passively support the bullying situation. The active supporters cheer on the bullying and encourage aggression. Fanger, Frankel, and Hazen (2012) found that active supporters often command the bully to use specific insults, reward the bully with praise, or mirror the bully’s aggression. Bystanders in the passive support role do not support bullying the victim nor do they intervene on the victim’s behalf. Salmivalli, Voeten, and Poskiparta, (2011) found that active support in the presence of passive support is the largest maintenance contingency of bullying behavior. These authors suggest that passive support of bullying increases bullying behavior because of the seemingly accepting attitude of bystanders. Many students report no bullying perpetration or victimization. These noninvolved students have less problems in school than bullies or victims (Bender & Lösel, 2011). The non-involved student can fulfill several roles in the bulling circle, but most predominately serve as bystanders. These students’ attitudes and reactions to bullying or defending in the classroom play a pivotal role in affecting the likelihood of future bullying (Salmivalli et al., 2011). The majority of the noninvolved students report wanting to help victims and are responsive to prosocial change (Bauer, Lozano, & Rivara, 2007). 8 Defenders The most altruistic bystander behavior is to defend against bullying using positive and non-aggressive actions. The presence of defenders in classrooms is associated with less bullying (Salmivalli et al., 2011). Defending behavior occurs, but is much less common than non-participation in bullying events. A defending student makes a choice to defend that may include risk. The adoption of additional risk, whether physical or social, for another student’s benefit exemplifies a fairly advanced level of personal development and social responsibility (Shakoor et al., 2012). Defenders have high class ranking in terms of popularity and friendship, ranking high with peers and adults (Caravita et al., 2012). Identifying defenders and rewarding their actions are useful tactics to reduce bullying and increase peer support for victims (Flaspohler, Elfstrom, Vanderzee, Sink, & Birchmeier, 2009). Peer support is necessary because students without other defenders in their friendship circle are less likely to continue to intervene on the behalf of victims (Farmer et al., 2010). Bullying Defined A student is being bullied when another student, or several other students, repeatedly says or does mean and hurtful things to him or her on purpose (Black, Washington, Trent, Harner, & Pollock, 2010; Olweus, 1997). The characteristics that define bullying are recurrent, hurtful actions and an imbalance of power between 9 victim and bully. This definition is useful to researchers to better identify the behavior, but also to teach children and adults as part of bullying prevention efforts. Bullying consists of behaviors that have harmful effects on those involved. These effects include physical, social, and emotional consequences to the victims of aggression (Turner, Exum, Brame, & Holt, 2013) and perpetrators (Camodeca & Goossens, 2005). School avoidance and excessive absences are seen in students who experience bullying victimization (Bender & Lösel, 2011). Bullying causes additional harm to students not directly involved by negatively affecting the learning environment (Skapinakis et al., 2011). The repetitiousness of bullying is one factor that separates bullying from isolated aggressive violence or other forms peer conflict. The experience of bullying is not only an isolated event, but an ongoing situation. The duration of bullying is critical to the deleterious aftereffects, typically the longer the duration the more severe the effects. For example, being bullied weekly is associated with severe outcomes, suicidal ideation, and attempts (Skapinakis et al., 2011). The imbalance of power involved in bullying can come in many forms. Physical power imbalances can be seen as differences in size or strength. Social status, like popularity, can also determine power imbalance. Individual differences in intelligence, ethnicity, and social skills can work to increase or decrease the power differential between peers (Bettencourt & Farrell, 2013). Developmental or psychological difficulties can also present an imbalance of power and increase the risk of being bullied. For example, students with Autism Spectrum Disorders have 10 been shown to be bullied more than typically developing students (Chen & Schwartz, 2012). Hanish and Guerra (2004) suggest that the ability to navigate potentially dangerous situations and to defend oneself from aggressors is part of normal child and adolescent development. However, some students have a reduced ability to defend themselves from aggression, making them more vulnerable. Minority groups and other special populations are of particular concern, as they have higher rates of victimization and more severe effects than those in the majority (Fedewa & Ahn, 2012; Peskin, Tortolero, & Markham, 2006). Sexual minority students, such as homosexual or transgendered students, are bullied with particularly severe effects (Hightow-Weidman et al., 2011). Group dynamics are another factor related with power imbalance, wherein the size of the group or groups alone can create an imbalance of power. Forber-Pratt, Aragon, and Espelage (2013) demonstrated that large groups engaged in bullying in may be difficult to distinguish from gang activity in large schools. These schools with known gang problems have higher rates of bullying separate from gang violence in general. Gang violence may represent a different type of aggression than bullying, but gang members are more likely to be bullies or bully-victims than students without gang membership (Bradshaw, Waasdorp, Goldweber, & Johnson, 2013). Types of bullying behavior There are a variety of ways school-age children are bullied. The aggressive behaviors that bullies us varies in intensity, focus, and type. The description of 11 current and recent bullying behavior is of clinical importance to schools. School-wide interventions and individual treatment can be more effective and efficient with a good understanding of the types of bullying occurring in a given population (Marini, Dane, Bosacki, & YLC-CURA, 2006). Two broad categories of bullying, direct and indirect, are useful to delineate bullying and describe bullying behavior. Direct and indirect bullying are differentiated by the time and proximal relationship between the aggressor and victim. In direct bullying there is an immediate exchange between bully and victim. Indirect bullying tends to be more prolonged and often involves a social group. Direct and indirect bullying and victimization are moderately correlated and often used together (Marini et al., 2006). Victims of bullying face a variety of types of bullying behavior and are bullied with combinations of bullying type. Wang et al. (2010) found that adolescent students who endure several types of bullying were at the greatest risk of negative physical and psychological outcomes. Additionally, students who report long-term bullying or multiple bullying types need the most attention and resources. Direct bullying One of the most overt negative behaviors seen within school bullying is aggressive violence. The use of physical contact, either to injure or coerce, is the most easily identifiable type of bullying behaviors by teachers and students (Pellegrini & Bartini, 2000). A single episode of aggressive violence is not considered bullying, nor is a mutual affray. However, these single episodes can be part of a pattern that is 12 bullying and represent risk for future violence (Nansel, Overpeck, Haynie, Ruan, & Scheidt, 2003). Bullying that is overtly displayed by the aggressor toward the intended victim is direct bullying. Direct bullying manifests as physical assaults, verbal threats or insults with the victim present. Although direct bullying is typically overt, it is not always observed by individuals other than the bully and victim (Barnes et al., 2012). Students will often attempt to hide aggression from teachers and other adults at school to avoid negative consequences. Physical assault is typically against school rules. Physical aggression represents serious risk of injury. Interestingly, the students who are most likely to report injury are relational bullies, not physical victims (Dukes, Stein, & Zane, 2010). These victims are intimidated by aggression and property violations. Serious aggressive bullying behaviors are also possible and include physical violence and sexual aggression. Sexual assaults are a form of direct bullying and represent a significantly high level of conduct severity. Negative mental and physical effects are often experienced long after childhood sexual assaults (Ullman & Brecklin, 2003). Direct bullying does not require physical contact to be harmful. Yelling, name calling, and threats are overt and occur directly from bully to victim. Repetitious and negative name calling deeply impact some victims (Vicars, 2006). Nishina, Juvonen, and Witkow (2005) demonstrated that verbal bullying resulted in social difficulties and physical symptoms similar to bulling with physical violence. Despite the saying that starts sticks and stones, words can hurt. 13 Indirect bullying Bullying is not always immediate or overt. Indirect bullying uses an intermediary, often social pressure, to cause harm to the victim (Huitsing & Veenstra, 2012). Spreading false rumors and excluding victims from play are common forms of indirect bullying. Indirect bullying utilizes the complexity of interpersonal relationships and social interaction, which is why indirect bullying is also referred to as relational bullying (Elsaesser, Gorman-Smith, & Henry, 2013). Students who use relational bullying, will often use tactics such as shaming, excluding, and rumors (Wang, Iannotti, & Nansel, 2009). Indirect bullying is not always easy to observe and often takes place far from adults who would intervene (Kahn, Jones, & Wieland, 2012). Indirect bullying is difficult to measure by observation or teacher nomination because it is covert. However, even when seen, teachers and administrators are less likely to intervene with indirect bullying than direct bullying (Barnes et al., 2012). Bullies and bully-victims who use relational bullying have greater internalized problems (Hoglund, 2007) and beliefs that normalize bullying behavior than their peers (Marini et al., 2006). These students are also likely to use direct bullying. This is especially concerning because students who bully with relational strategies are more likely to report carrying a weapon (Dukes et al., 2010). Social exclusion is a method of indirect bullying that has an adverse effect on psychological characteristics including social avoidance, emotion tolerance, and empathy (Smart Richman & Leary, 2009). Even simple exclusions from games can 14 reduce self-esteem, belonging, and cause hurt feelings. Exclusion causes an experience of suffering that closely relates with physical pain (Nordgren, Banas, & MacDonald, 2011) and has long term health implications (Knack, Gomez, & JensenCampbell, 2011). Bernstein and Claypool (2012) demonstrated that even simulated exclusion affects physical pain sensitivity. Research participants who were excluded from games were more sensitive to experimentally induced pain, whereas exclusion from future life events led to numbing. Peer rejection and exclusion impact the classroom experience and attendance. Buhs, Ladd, and Herald (2006) found that students who report peer rejection are not as engaged in classroom activities and are less likely to attend school when compared with their non-rejected peers. Additionally, classrooms with high rates of exclusion have adversely affected attendance and student participation. Students who are excluded from cooperative activities or have few friends may be missing an important protective factor. Holt and Espelage (2007) showed that social support from peers helps students overcome difficulties and ameliorate psychological symptoms. Even moderate peer support appears to improve the resulting problems faced by those students who have been bullied. Cyberbullying A modern method of bullying behavior that takes advantage of electronic networking and social media is cyberbullying. Several highly publicized cases of cyberbullying that resulted in the suicide of the victim provide case evidence and 15 highlight the need for community response (Fagan & Huet, 2007). Cyberbullying appears later in childhood than traditional forms of bullying, peaking in adolescence. Cyberbullying has an important relationship to the bullying that is happening in school or the community (Erentaitė, Bergman, & Žukauskienė, 2012). There is often a continuation of the coercive bullying relationship that began at school involved in the later cyberbullying. Kowalski, Morgan, and Limber (2012) found that students who were bullied by traditional methods are more likely to be also victimized by cyberbullying than non-involved peers. Cyberbullying occurs online, but this does not equate to a lack of oversight by parents or school officials. Many schools have a mandate to follow up on aggressive behavior that is initiated within the school or is affecting students (Willard, 2011). School social workers, resource officers, and local police often have the tools to intervene with cyberbullying bullies and victims (Slovak & Singer, 2014). Common interventions include actions against bullies and emotional support for victims. Gender Differences Putallaz et al. (2007) found that gender is one of several demographic factors related to types of bullying behavior. Self and teacher reports of bullying behavior documented that boys are more likely to engage in direct bullying than girls. Girls appear to use indirect bullying about the same as boys. Similarly, Smith, Rose, and Schwartz-Mette (2010) found equivalent levels of self-reported indirect bullying between genders. Despite similar experiences, these students were more accepting of indirect bullying by girls than boys, whereas direct bullying had low acceptance by 16 both genders. Spriggs, Iannotti, Nansel, and Haynie (2007) reported that students, school personnel, and family members typically held a stereotypical perspective that girls are more likely to use indirect bullying than boys. This nation-wide sample of 11,033 students supports that indirect bullying for boys and girls is similar, while physical aggression and direct bullying may be slightly higher in boys. Gender differences also occur in the role of defender. Girls are more likely to defend others experiencing bullying victimization (Crapanzano, Frick, Childs, & Terranova, 2011). This is especially true for younger girls (Caravita et al., 2012). Sainio, Veenstra, Huitsing, and Salmivalli (2011) found that the defender-victim dyad are typically same gender pairs, but girls were more frequent and likely defenders. Idsoe et al. (2012) identified gender differences related to bullying victimization frequency and severity of psychiatric symptoms related to Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Despite boys being twice as likely to be victimized daily, girls tend to have more avoidant and intrusive symptoms related to PTSD. While not highly researched or easy to detect, gender nonconformists seem to have the worst reports of bullying victimization outcomes, depressive symptoms, and diagnoses (Roberts, Rosario, Slopen, Calzo, & Austin, 2013). Negative Effects of Bullying Bullying is hurtful; the resulting negative effects from bullying can last into adulthood (Allison, Roeger, & Reinfeld-Kirkman, 2009). Storch, Brassard, and Masia-Warner (2003) identified how early victimization produces patterns of fear and avoidance, resulting in higher rates of future mental health and academic problems. 17 Poor school performance, as demonstrated by achievement tests and grade point average, is evidenced in children who are bullied (Schwartz, Gorman, Nakamoto, & Toblin, 2005). These deficits can be exacerbated and further compounded by added stressors in the child’s life (Schwartz, Lansford, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2013). Cornell, Gregory, Huang, and Fan (2013) demonstrated that students not involved in bullying are affected when bullying is perceived as common or not responded to by adults. Schools with more negative perceptions about bullying on campus had worse school attendance and academic performance. Bender and Lösel (2011) demonstrated that students who bully others are more likely to have higher school delinquency, more violent behavior, and higher levels of antisocial traits. Many of these behaviors at school and antisocial traits in bullies are associated with conduct problems that continue into adulthood (Kokkinos & Panayiotou, 2004). Idsoe, Dyregrov, and Idsoe (2012) found that victims and students who bully others daily are each more likely to have avoidant symptoms within the clinical range than non-involved peers. Students who identified as both victim and bully had more severe symptoms. One of the most negative effects related to bullying is suicidal ideation and attempts in children. Dickerson Mayes et al. (2014) found that bullying victimization was a common element in the relatively few cases of suicidal ideation in children without psychiatric disorders. Meltzer, Vostanis, Ford, Bebbington, and Dennis (2011) found that adults who were bullied as children were twice as likely to attempt suicide compared with those not bullied. This experience of bullying was more 18 impactful than other suicide related factors, such as depression and other mental illnesses. Kaminski and Fang (2009) found an association between peer victimization and suicide in United States samples. The presence of victimization within the last year predicted suicidal ideation and behavior beyond other variables related with suicide. History of frequent bullying victimization is related to suicide attempts in adult females and suicide completions in adult males (Klomek et al., 2009). Bullyvictims have worse outcomes of those in any status; their rate of suicidal ideation is three to four times more likely than the non-involved (Heikkilä et al., 2013). Bullying Prevention Teachers, administrators, and other professionals often have an ambiguous definition of bullying (Arora, 1996). Bradshaw, Sawyer, and O’Brennan (2007) demonstrated that untrained school staff are often hesitant to intervene and seldom intervene with both the victim and bully. Additionally, students and teachers likely underreport bullying when unware and untrained. Successful child and adolescent prevention programs overcome hesitancy and inefficacy with training, repeated assessments, and clear behavioral contingencies (Durlak & Wells, 1997). Bullying prevention programs have shown the largest effect in reducing direct bullying (Brown, Low, Smith, & Haggerty, 2011). The effects of bullying prevention programs on indirect and overall bullying are typically small (Black et al., 2010). One potential reason for this small effect on overall bullying is an initial increased reporting of bullying behavior and a reduction of actual bullying. Amundsen and 19 Ravndal (2010) found fewer student problems, such as substance abuse and school delinquency, among late adolescent students who participated in a bullying prevention program in elementary school compared to students without early bullying prevention. A widely utilized bullying prevention program is the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program (OBPP). Olweus found that reducing the complex and pervasive problem of bullying in school-age children had to encompass and integrate multiple intervention domains. The OBPP uses four domains to reach students and reduce bullying: individual, classrooms, school wide, and community. Intervention in the individual domain consists of the contingencies of speaking with staff, negative consequences for bullies, positive consequences for defenders, and phone calls to parents/guardians. Classroom meetings and the kickoff event are key components of the classroom and school wide domains, respectively. The community domain is inclusive to the other domains. Families, guardians, and other community members are encouraged to be involved at every level. Assessment Accurate assessment is a crucial aspect of intervention. The standard behavioral measurement technique in studies that involve children is observation. Observation can account for detailed descriptions of environmental and contextual variables (Atlas & Pepler, 1998). However, observational assessment has limitations with large population sizes and has high costs for use in school-wide assessments. Peer nominations are often used to asses power imbalances between students, but are 20 also time and resource intensive (Green, Felix, Sharkey, Furlong, & Kras, 2012). Bullying assessments that utilize self-report questioners are more practical for use in school-wide assessments. The Bully Victim Questionnaire (BVQ) developed by Olweus is one of the standards in bullying assessment and has demonstrated internal reliability and validity (Kyriakides, Kaloyirou, & Lindsay, 2006). The BVQ offers a practical method for obtaining a descriptive bullying data, including bullying type and frequency in school settings. Additionally, the BVQ assesses location of bullying victimization. The frequent bullying locations are important for school staff to know because these areas are ripe for intervention. Bullying hotspots are most likely to be in the hallways, lunchrooms, and playground areas (Vaillancourt et al., 2010). School administrators and teachers can make adjustments to the positioning of supervisors or staff around these hot spots. School-wide Implementation A committee of caring individuals directs and manages OBPP implementation. Ideally, the committee is a blended mix of teachers, administrators, parents, and community members. Regular committee meetings are used to plan, schedule, and even modify implementation to fit the needs of the students. One of the most important goals of the committee is to adapt bullying consequence contingencies to preexisting conduct codes. Ttofi and Farrington (2012) found that the best consequences for bullying are consistent applied with corrective instruction, but are not harsh or zero tolerance. 21 A kickoff event is held after the initial assessment to educate students about the bullying prevention program. The kickoff event is a large assembly, held to signal to students that the school is actively working to counteract bullying. During this assembly the students are taught what bullying is and is not. The school rules about bullying are taught and rehearsed. Additionally, the contingencies and consequences for both bullies and victims are explained. This information can be conveyed in a fun and interactive manner with the aid of college students, civic groups, or other community members. Classroom Meetings Salmivalli and Voeten (2004) found that student attitudes about bullying and classroom norms are strong predictors of bullying behavior. Student and teacher attitudes that were accepting of bullying reinforced aggression, while prosocial attitudes reinforced defending behavior. Successful classroom interventions will need to take advantage of pre-existing behaviors that support prosocial behavior, while working to extinguish bullying and peer aggression. Weekly class meetings are included to encourage dialogue with students and teach prosocial skills. One of the goals of class meetings is to model and develop conflict resolution strategies. Class meetings can be challenging to implement, but have been shown to reduce bullying in the classroom and improve students’ selfconcept (Edwards & Mullis, 2003; Sorsdahl & Sanche, 1985). Student and teacher relationships can also be improved through class meetings resulting in increased trust, better communication, and improved cooperation (Emmett, Monsour, Lundeberg, & 22 Russo, 1996). There can be wide applicability to class meeting including crisis support. Bullock and Foegen (2002) showed how teachers successfully utilized class meetings to provide emotional stability and reassurance to students after tragedies. Class meetings can also involve language arts learning to address bullying. Narrative strategies that address bullying have been developed (Teglasi, Rahill, & Rothman, 2007). Incorporating stories and class discussion with concepts about bullying can be useful, especially for younger children. The classroom meeting leader, typically the teacher, educates students and encourages positive student interaction. Concepts such as the bullying circle or how to disagree without being disagreeable are taught in early meetings. Specific information from the school-wide assessments allows leaders to establish goals for class meetings. Students are encouraged to be active participants and are involved in identifying problems or defining discussion topics over the course of OBPP. Purpose and Hypotheses The implementation of the OBPP has the expressed goal of reducing bullying behavior on campus. One way to conceptualize this change is to have prosocial movement around the bullying circle, bullies become bystanders, bystanders become defenders, and so on. As a result of OBPP implementation there will be fewer bullies and victims. This effect will be evidenced by a reduced risk for participants to have the role of victim, bully, and bully-victim. Additionally, victims will be bullied with less severity after OBPP implementation. Furthermore, participants will be at less 23 risk of having a joiner role, while more likely to have the role of defender after OBPP implementation. Gender differences in bullying type and bullying role will be also be explored. Males will be at higher risk for having a direct victim role than females. Females will be at higher risk of having indirect victim role. Girls will be at greater odds to be a defender and at less odds to be a joiner than boys. 24 METHOD Participants Two schools participated in implementing the OBPP. One elementary school (K-6) and one combined elementary and middle school (K-8). Both schools are in the same school district in northern California. Participants in grades three through eight were assessed as part of a bullying prevention program. Kindergartners, first and second graders were not assessed, but received classroom meetings and attended kickoff events. All students at school on the day of the event or assessment participated as no parents opted out and all participants assented. Age, name, individual identifiers, and other demographics were not collected to provide anonymity to students and comply with school practices. Grade and gender of the students were collected with the BVQ (see Tables 1 and 2). Table 1 Participants Assessed with BVQ by School Site and Year Grades Site 1 Site 2 Year 3 4 5 6 7 8 Total 2011 28 34 41 32 135 2012 0 28 35 36 99 2011 42 51 41 56 100 72 362 2012 43 47 49 54 88 103 384 Note: Third graders at site 1 were not assessed post-intervention due to errors in printing. 25 Table 2 Gender of Participants Assessed with BVQ by Year School Year Boys Girls Total 2011-2012 243 253 496 2012-2013 223 258 483 26 Materials and Variables The Olweus Bully Victim Questionnaire (BVQ) is a 39 item measure used to collect self-reported information about bullying and victimization occurrences (see Appendix A). Early questions include grade, gender, and number of good friends. Bullying victimization (questions 4 – 13) and perpetration (questions 24 – 33) are rated on a five point frequency scale, from no bullying in the past several months to several times per week. A sample of indirect bullying is “Other students left me out of things on purpose, excluded me from their group of friends, or completely ignored me” (question 6). A sample of direct bullying is “I was threatened or forced to do things I didn’t want to do” (question 10). Other questions identify location of bullying, student attitudes about bullying, and the schools response to bullying. Inclusion criteria for having a role of bully or victim required a response of greater than only once or twice. Participants that indicated victimization at rates equal or greater than 2 to 3 times per month in questions 4 – 13 were classified as a victim for any bullying; questions 6 and 8 for indirect bullying; and questions 5, 7, and 9 – 12 for direct bullying. Participants that indicated perpetration at rates equal or greater that 2 to 3 times per month in questions 24 – 33 were classified as a bully for any bullying; questions 26 and 28 for indirect bullying; and questions 25, 27, and 29 – 32 for direct bullying. The role of bully-victim required an indication of both victimization and perpetration equal or greater than 2 to 3 times per month for questions 4 – 13 and 24 – 33. Joiners are participants who indicated they could join bullying a student they didn’t like (yes or yes maybe on question 36). Defenders are 27 participants that indicated they usually react to someone being bullied by trying to help the bullied student (question 37). Victimization severity scales were constructed by averaging responses from victims on three scales. The total bullying scale is the mean of victimization items (questions 4 – 12), except for the write in question (question 13). The indirect bullying scale is the mean score of items associated with indirect bullying (questions 6 and 8). The direct bullying scale consist of the mean score of items associated with direct bullying (questions 5, 7, and 9 – 12). Unanswered questions were excluded from the averaging. Procedure Program implementation followed a general pattern of assessment, initial and ongoing intervention, and reassessment (see figure 2). Participants’ parents/guardians were sent a passive consent letter (see appendix B) and information pamphlet (see appendix C) that describes the OBBP, including assessment, events, and classroom meetings. No parents opted out of the assessment, events, or classroom meetings. Participants in grades three through eight were assessed with the BVQ two to three months after the school year began. Proctors for the assessments were trained students from California State University, Stanislaus (CSUS). Participants were assessed in their classrooms, during the course of a normal school day. Participants were informed of the purpose of the assessment and that participation was voluntary. Participants were also informed there was no penalty or reward for completing the assessment. Assent was confirmed verbally, by asking participants if they are willing 28 to begin. Any protests were to be treated as dissension, in which participants were to be instructed to continue to sit at their desks and engage in non-participatory tasks like reading or drawing. However, no protests occurred. Parent Information Assessment 1 (2011) Summary Reports Kick-off Event Classroom Meetings Assessment 2 (2012) Summary Reports Classroom Meetings Figure 2. Flow chart representing the steps of the procedure. Program implementation incorporated assessment, feedback, intervention, and ongoing classroom meetings. Descriptive information about bullying behavior as reported in the BVQ was given to each school site in the form of summary reports. Summary reports include location, type, and frequency of victimization. Overall trends or high percentages of bullying were flagged to signal school intervention. The school administrators used these reports at their discretion. A kickoff event was held at both schools in February of 2012. The kickoff event was coordinated with each school’s principal. Students from CSUS performed 29 skits that incorporated core concepts of the OBPP, including bullying roles, the school rules about bullying, and the consequences for bullying (see Appendix D). Skits were made grade-appropriate and relevant by grouping them as follows: kindergarten through third grades, fourth through sixth grades, and seventh through eighth grades. At the end of the event, the students were given pencils and wrist bands that were imprinted with “Be a Buddy Not a Bully.” CSUS students initialized classroom meetings for teachers and students by leading the first meeting (see Appendix E). Teachers were given an outline of rules and rationale of the classroom meetings. CSUS students discussed the purpose of class meetings, established meeting norms, and provided information about bullying. Additional topics of negotiation, defending, and class meeting goals were introduced. Teachers were expected to continue classroom meetings through the school year. Data Analysis The data obtained from the BVQ were analyzed to identify the risk of having a bullying role (victim, bully, bully-victim, joiner, and defender), before and after OBPP implementation. Identifying the absolute risk of the bullying roles pre and post intervention with chi square comparisons indicates program effectiveness. Additionally, victims’ mean score on each victimization scale, before and after OBPP implementation, was compared using independent samples t tests. Gender differences in bullying role were analyzed, similarly to pre and post intervention comparisons, with absolute risk ratios and chi square tests. Gender differences were analyzed separately by year. 30 RESULTS There were fewer participants with a victim role post intervention than preintervention (see Table 3). This difference was significant for indirect victimization and approached significance for any type of victimization. Similarly there were fewer bullies post intervention. This difference was significant for indirect bullying and marginally significant for any type of bullying. There is also marginal support for fewer participants with a bully-victim role post intervention. There were less participants with a supporter role post-intervention than preintervention, but this difference was not significant (see Table 3). There was no significant difference between the ratio of participants noticing bullying postintervention (317 noticed/151 did not notice) compared to pre-intervention (343 noticed/149 did not notice), χ2 = 0.44, p = .51. Furthermore, participants that noticed bullying were not at significantly greater odds of having a defender role postintervention than pre-intervention (again see Table 3). 31 Table 3 A Comparison of the Likelihood of Reported Bullying Roles Before and After Intervention Pre Post Risk Ratio [95% CI] χ2 Any 186/496 154/483 0.92 [0.84,1.01] 3.41a Direct 142/496 126/483 0.97 [0.89,1.04] 0.78 Indirect 130/496 94/483 0.92 [0.86,0.98] 6.42** Any 41/496 26/483 0.97 [0.94,1.00] 3.19a Direct 15/496 14/483 1.00 [0.98,1.02] 0.13 Indirect 21/496 10/483 0.98 [0.96,1.00] 3.74* Any 35/496 22/483 0.97 [0.94,1.01] 2.79a Supporter 44/493 30/468 0.97 [0.94,1.01] 2.14 Defender 238/343 233/317 1.15 [0.91,1.47] 1.37 Victim Bully Bully-Victim Notes: ap <.10, *p < .05, **p < .01. Pre and Post columns are the ratio of participants with that bullying role to the sample population. Risk ratios less than one indicate less risk post intervention. Bullying roles are inclusive. 32 The mean scores of victims’ reported bullying type pre and post-intervention were compared using t tests. Participants that reported no bullying in each type of bullying were excluded from analysis. Bullying victimization severity was not significantly less post intervention than pre intervention for total bullying, direct bullying, and indirect bullying, ps = .22, .71, .28 respectively (see Table 4). Table 4 A Comparison of Pre and Post Intervention Victimization Severity Pre Post Mean SD Mean SD t (df) Cohen's d [95% CI] Total 1.79 0.78 1.72 0.77 1.22(715) 0.09[-0.06,0.24] Direct 1.76 0.78 1.74 0.76 0.37(601) 0.03[-0.13,0.19] Indirect 2.31 1.00 2.22 0.95 1.09(552) 0.09[-0.74.0.26] 33 Gender differences in bullying roles was largely not statistically significant for both assessment 1 (see Table 5) and assessment 2 (see Table 6). However, in assessment 1 male participants were at statistically greater odds to be a bullying supporter than female participants. There was marginal support that male participants were at greater odds of having a direct victim role than females in assessment 1. These differences did not carry over to assessment 2. Table 5 A Comparison of Genders on Bullying Role and Type for Assessment 1 Female Male Risk Ratio [95% CI] χ2 Any 95/252 91/243 1.00 [0.87,1.14] 0.01 Direct 64/252 78/243 1.10 [0.98,1.23] 2.72a Indirect 73/252 57/243 0.93 [0.84,1.04] 1.94 Any 17/252 24/243 1.04 [0.98,1.09] 1.60 Direct 5/252 10/243 1.02 [0.99,1.06] 1.91 Indirect 10/252 11/243 1.01 [0.97,1.04] 0.10 Bully-Victim 14/252 21/243 1.03 [0.98,1.09] 1.73 Supporter 11/240 33/208 1.11 [1.05,1.17] 13.10** Defender 129/180 108/162 0.85 [0.62,1.17] 1.00 Victims Bullies Notes: a = p < .10, **p < .01. Female and Male columns are the ratio of students with that bullying role to the sample population. Risk ratios greater than one indicate that male participants have a greater proportion in the role than female participants. Risk ratios less than one indicate that female participants have a higher proportion in that role than male participants. 34 Table 6 A Comparison of Genders on Bullying Role and Type for Assessment 2 Female Male Risk Ratio [95% CI] χ2 Any 79/258 74/223 1.04 [0.92,1.17] 0.36 Direct 64/258 61/223 1.04 [0.93,1.15] 0.40 Indirect 52/258 41/223 0.98 [0.90,1.07] 0.24 Total 14/258 12/223 1.00 [0.96,1.04] 0.01 Direct 7/258 7/223 1.00 [0.97,1.05] 0.08 Indirect 3/258 7/223 1.02 [0.99,1.05] 2.30 Bully Victims 11/258 11/223 1.01 [0.97,1.05] 0.12 Supporter 12/252 18/215 1.04 [0.99,1.09] 2.52 Defender 130/177 103/140 1.01 [0.69,4517] 0.01 Victims Bullies Notes: Female and Male columns are the ratio of students with that bullying role to the sample population. Risk ratios greater than one indicate that male participants have a greater proportion in the role than female participants. Risk ratios less than one indicate that female participants have a higher proportion in that role than male participants. 35 DISCUSSION Implementation of the bullying prevention program coincided with a reduction of bullying involvement. Participants were less likely to have a victim, bully, or bully-victim role after program implementation. Participants’ reports reflect an increased awareness and compliance with non-aggression, but not a greater willingness to defend others being bullied. OBPP shifted students away from the negative roles, evidenced by fewer bullies and victims after intervention. However, there were not more defenders, suggesting further intervention is required to promote the defender role. Differences in bullying roles between genders were largely rejected. There was minimal support that boys are more likely to report being victims of direct bullying, while girls showed a trend to be indirectly bullied more than boys. Boys were more willing than girls to join in bullying a student they did not like preintervention. Intervention The assessment, kick-off event, and initial classroom meetings were implemented largely according to plan. There was not a single protest from any participant. The reading of each assessment item to grades three and four provided an interaction that the participants reported enjoying. Participants also seemed to enjoy the kick-off event and classroom meetings. Printing errors occurred for one grade, in one school for Assessment 2, but did not appear to cause distress to participants. 36 One barrier to program implementation was found in developing a robust and diverse committee. The committee’s main purpose is to facilitate program adoption and encouragement in the community domain. As such, there was limited community involvement from businesses, families, and community organizations. These community alliances are designed to support students and school staff. Community involvement expands preventative factors outside of schools’ influence (Holt, Raczynski, Frey, Hymel, & Limber, 2013). Committee responsibilities were primarily handled by the school principal and implementation team during staff meetings. Capital resources would alleviate this barrier by providing funding for advertisement, additional events, and hiring support staff. Interpretation The lack of a control group or staggered interventions does not allow cause to be attributed directly to intervention. Additionally, statistical power was reduced in order to ensure the level of anonymity needed to comply with educational code. Therefore, paired t-tests and repeated measure ANOVAs were not appropriate. There are many natural variables between school sites: size, location, administrators, and number of grades, but these variables were not explored at this time. The effect sizes, though small, are indicative of a clear change in bullying behavior, especially when considering that approximately six percent of the entire population moved out of the victim role, from 38% to 32%. Similarly, about three percent moved out of the bully role after intervention, from 8% to 5%. While not captured by the BVQ, students verbalized to teachers and staff how they felt about 37 bullying and what steps can be taken when bullying happens. Objective school-wide changes in responses on the BVQ indicate changes in the system, a changing tide of norms. Students know not to bully, are less likely to bully, and as a result there are fewer victims. However, student’s decisions about defending victims has yet to improve. The effect of this intervention may be smaller than interventions using like protocols implemented more robustly, namely at the classroom level. Olweus and Limber (2010) demonstrated a dose response curve at multiple intervention sites. The effect of this intervention may be minimized by an overestimating bias in self-report of bullying after intervention. Frey, Hirschstein, Edstrom, and Snell (2009) found that behaviorally observed rates of bullying and aggression decrease at a greater rate than self-reported bullying. One reason for higher reports is an increase in awareness by students. Additionally, these researchers found that students’ beliefs about program effectiveness are reinforced by continuing adult efforts. The lack of gender differences found is similar to the findings of Crapanzano et al. (2011). These authors suggest the largest difference between genders is that boys are more likely to stay in the same bullying role from year to year, while girls bullying role is more variable. Bevans, Bradshaw, and Waasdorp (2013) analysis of self-report bullying behavior supports the two factor model of direct and indirect bullying. However, these authors suggest that bias underscores the larger amounts of direct bullying boys experience and the more indirect bullying that girls experience. Some specific 38 behaviors such a pushing and shoving are much more common in boys and spreading rumors is more common in girls, but aggregate out in the two factor model. Limitations and Future Directions Low and Van Ryzin (2014) discovered that prosocial attitudes among school staff and students strengthen intervention effects. However, negative attitudes or a school climate that has a high acceptance of aggression does not negate effectiveness. Changing staff and student attitudes and behavior takes time and the current intervention was limited in scope and duration. Polanin, Espelage, and Pigott's (2012) meta-analysis of several bullying prevention programs reveals that increasing prosocial bystander engagement takes additional interventions above and beyond current bullying prevention programs. Data for adherence to classroom meetings and following through on behavioral contingencies were not recorded, but both school sites reported difference among teachers. Biggs, Vernberg, Twemlow, Fonagy, and Dill (2008) demonstrated that adherence to bullying prevention programs drives outcomes, yet is highly variable. Teachers often report partial and zero adherences. These researchers demonstrated that the primary barrier to adherence was preexisting teacher attitudes. Additionally, the most helpful and adherent teachers had the best defending students. Teachers are five times more likely to implement evidence based programs when trained specifically on adherence (Hanley et al., 2009). Cyberbullying is likely to be more commonplace as electronic social networking and internet access continue to grow. Computer based assessments are 39 available (Verlinden et al., 2014), including an electronic version of the BVQ, that address cyberbullying. Cyberbullying will most likely be reduced and prevented with elements of the OBPP; however further research on overlapping prevention is needed (Low & Espelage, 2013). Cyberbullying will likely respond to bullying prevention programs when addressed by parents/guardians, educators, and communities (Smith et al., 2008). It seems clear that the role of defender needs more attention. Defenders need to be supported in their use of advanced moral and prosocial skills. Defensive tactics and community skills can be taught and practiced in classroom meetings, but effects take several rounds of intervention to become palpable (Azad & Amiri, 2012). Empathy is an established motivator for defenders (Nickerson & Mele-Taylor, 2014) and may be increased through training (Frey et al., 2009). Continued contingency management and support systems will create an environment wherein practiced defenders will act. Additional research is needed to further assess and understand the development of defenders. School-based counseling services are a likely ally in bullying response and prevention. McElearney, Adamson, Shevlin, and Bunting (2013) found that school counseling is an effective resource to support victims, bullies, and bully-victims. Stadler, Feifel, Rohrmann, Vermeiren, and Poustka (2010) identified support from teachers and school staff as a protective factor against peer victimization, especially for middle and high school students and students with mental illness. Counseling at school will support these students and foster those protective child-adult connections. 40 Applications The primary application of the current research is to provide validation for bullying prevention programs in primary schools. The current implementation of OBPP provides a model of community based intervention for bullying, specifically a collaborative partnership between universities and public schools. Public schools are often resource limited and unable to fund comprehensive intervention programs. Schools benefit by having trained intervention teams, as future professionals gain valuable experience. REFERENCES 42 REFERENCES Allison, S., Roeger, L., & Reinfeld-Kirkman, N. (2009). Does school bullying affect adult health? Population survey of health-related quality of life and past victimization. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 1163– 1170. doi:10.3109/00048670903270399 American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington D.C. Amundsen, E. J., & Ravndal, E. (2010). Does successful school-based prevention of bullying influence substance use among 13- to 16-year-olds? Drugs: Education, Prevention & Policy, 17, 42–54. doi:10.3109/09687630802095591 Arora, C. M. J. (1996). Defining bullying: Towards a clearer general understanding and more effective intervention strategies. School Psychology International, 17, 317–329. doi:10.1177/0143034396174002 Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the classroom. The Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86–99. doi:10.1080/00220679809597580 Azad, N. E., & Amiri, S. (2012). Effectiveness of Olweus Bullying Prevention Program on Iranian boys. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 18, 175–183. doi: 10.1014/2013-23146-001 Barnes, A., Cross, D., Lester, L., Hearn, L., Epstein, M., & Monks, H. (2012). The invisibility of covert bullying among students: Challenges for school 43 intervention. Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 22, 206–226. doi:10.1017/jgc.2012.27 Bauer, N. S., Lozano, P., & Rivara, F. P. (2007). The effectiveness of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program in public middle schools: A controlled trial. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, 266–274. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.10.005 Bender, D., & Lösel, F. (2011). Bullying at school as a predictor of delinquency, violence and other anti-social behaviour in adulthood. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 21, 99–106. doi:10.1002/cbm.799 Bernstein, M. J., & Claypool, H. M. (2012). Social exclusion and pain sensitivity: Why exclusion sometimes hurts and sometimes numbs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 185–196. doi:10.1177/0146167211422449 Berthhold, K. A., & Hoover, J. H. (2000). Correlates of bullying and victimization among intermediate students in the Midwestern USA. School Psychology International, 21, 65–78. doi:10.1177/0143034300211005 Bettencourt, A. F., & Farrell, A. D. (2013). Individual and contextual factors associated with patterns of aggression and peer victimization during middle school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 285–302. doi:10.1007/s10964012-9854-8 Bevans, K. B., Bradshaw, C. P., & Waasdorp, T. E. (2013). Gender bias in the measurement of peer victimization: An application of item response theory. Aggressive Behavior, 39, 370–380. doi:10.1002/ab.21486 44 Biggs, B. K., Vernberg, E. M., Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., & Dill, E. J. (2008). Teacher adherence and its relations to teacher attitudes and student outcomes in an elementary school-based violence prevention program. School Psychology Review, 37, 533–549. Black, S., Washington, E., Trent, V., Harner, P., & Pollock, E. (2010). Translating the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program into real-world practice. Health Promotion Practice, 11, 733–740. doi:10.1177/1524839908321562 Bradshaw, C. P., Sawyer, A. L., & O’Brennan, L. M. (2007). Bullying and peer victimization at school: Perceptual differences between students and school staff. School Psychology Review, 36, 361–382. Bradshaw, C. P., Waasdorp, T. E., Goldweber, A., & Johnson, S. L. (2013). Bullies, gangs, drugs, and school: Understanding the overlap and the role of ethnicity and urbanicity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 220–234. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9863-7 Brown, E. C., Low, S., Smith, B. H., & Haggerty, K. P. (2011). Outcomes from a school-randomized controlled trial of steps to respect: A bullying prevention program. School Psychology Review, 40, 423–443. Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children’s classroom engagement and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 1–13. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.1 45 Bullock, C., & Foegen, A. (2002). Constructive conflict resolution for students with behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders, 27, 289–295. Camodeca, M., & Goossens, F. A. (2005). Aggression, social cognitions, anger and sadness in bullies and victims. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 186–197. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00347.x Camodeca, M., Goossens, F. A., Meerum Terwogt, M., & Schuengel, C. (2002). Bullying and victimization among school-age children: Stability and links to proactive and reactive aggression. Social Development, 11, 332–345. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00203 Caravita, S. C. S., Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2012). Main and moderated effects of moral cognition and status on bullying and defending. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 456–468. doi:10.1002/ab.21447 Chen, P.-Y., & Schwartz, I. S. (2012). Bullying and victimization experiences of students with autism spectrum disorders in elementary schools. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 27, 200–212. doi:10.1177/1088357612459556 Cornell, D., Gregory, A., Huang, F., & Fan, X. (2013). Perceived prevalence of teasing and bullying predicts high school dropout rates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 138–149. doi:10.1037/a0030416 Crapanzano, A. M., Frick, P. J., Childs, K., & Terranova, A. M. (2011). Gender differences in the assessment, stability, and correlates to bullying roles in 46 middle school children. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 29, 677–694. doi:10.1002/bsl.1000 Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., & Bouvrette, A. (2003). Contingencies of self-worth in college students: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 894–908. doi:10.1037/00223514.85.5.894 Olweus, D., Limber, S., Crocker Flerx, V., Mullin, N., Riese, J., & Snyder, M. (2007). Olweus Bullying Prevention Program: Schoolwide Guide. USA: Hazelden. Dervin, D. (2011). 2010: The Year of the Bully. Journal of Psychohistory, 38, 337– 345. Dickerson Mayes, S., Baweja, R., Calhoun, S. L., Syed, E., Mahr, F., & Siddiqui, F. (2014). Suicide ideation and attempts and bullying in children and adolescents: Psychiatric and general population samples. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention, 35, 301–309. doi:10.1027/0227-5910/a000264 Dukes, R. L., Stein, J. A., & Zane, J. I. (2010). Gender differences in the relative impact of physical and relational bullying on adolescent injury and weapon carrying. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 511–532. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2010.08.001 47 Durlak, J. A., & Wells, A. M. (1997). Primary prevention mental health programs for children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 115–152. Edwards, D., & Mullis, F. (2003). Classroom meetings: Encouraging a climate of cooperation. Professional School Counseling, 7, 20–28. Elsaesser, C., Gorman-Smith, D., & Henry, D. (2013). The role of the school environment in relational aggression and victimization. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 235–249. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9839-7 Emmett, J. D., Monsour, F., Lundeberg, M., & Russo, T. (1996). Open classroom meetings: Promoting peaceful schools. Elementary School Guidance & Counseling, 31, 3–10. Erentaitė, R., Bergman, L. R., & Žukauskienė, R. (2012). Cross‐contextual stability of bullying victimization: A person‐oriented analysis of cyber and traditional bullying experiences among adolescents. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 181–190. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00935.x Fagan, K. & Huet, E. (2007). Cyberbullying's tragic toll. San Francisco Chronicle. Retrieved from http://www.sfchronicle.com Fanger, S. M., Frankel, L. A., & Hazen, N. (2012). Peer exclusion in preschool children’s play: Naturalistic observations in a playground setting. MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 58, 224–254. doi:10.1353/mpq.2012.0007 Farmer, T. W., Petrin, R. A., Robertson, D. L., Fraser, M. W., Hall, C. M., Day, S. H., & Dadisman, K. (2010). Peer relations of bullies, bully-victims, and victims: 48 The two social worlds of bullying in second-grade classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 110, 364–392. doi:10.1086/648983 Fedewa, A. L., & Ahn, S. (2012). The effects of bullying and peer victimization on sexual-minority and heterosexual youths: A quantitative meta-analysis of the literature”: Corrigendum. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 8. doi:10.1080/1550428X.2012.653768 Flaspohler, P. D., Elfstrom, J. L., Vanderzee, K. L., Sink, H. E., & Birchmeier, Z. (2009). Stand by me: The effects of peer and teacher support in mitigating the impact of bullying on quality of life. Psychology in the Schools, 46, 636–649. doi:10.1002/pits.20404 Forber-Pratt, A. J., Aragon, S. R., & Espelage, D. L. (2013). The influence of gang presence on victimization in one middle school environment. Psychology of Violence. doi:10.1037/a0031835 Fox, C. L., & Boulton, M. J. (2005). The social skills problems of victims of bullying: Self, peer and teacher perceptions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 313–328. doi:10.1348/000709905X25517 Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Edstrom, L. V., & Snell, J. L. (2009). Observed reductions in school bullying, nonbullying aggression, and destructive bystander behavior: A longitudinal evaluation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 466–481. doi:10.1037/a0013839 49 Frizzo, M. N., Bisol, L. W., & Lara, D. R. (2012). Bullying victimization is associated with dysfunctional emotional traits and affective temperaments. Journal of Affective Disorders, 1, 48–52. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.046 Glick, G. C., & Rose, A. J. (2011). Prospective associations between friendship adjustment and social strategies: Friendship as a context for building social skills. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1117–1132. doi:10.1037/a0023277 Green, J. G., Felix, E. D., Sharkey, J. D., Furlong, M. J., & Kras, J. E. (2012). Identifying bully victims: Definitional versus behavioral approaches. Psychological Assessment, advance online publication. doi:10.1037/a0031248 Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2004). Aggressive victims, passive victims, and bullies: developmental continuity or developmental change? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 17–38. doi:10.1353/mpq.2004.0003 Hanley, S., Ringwalt, C., Vincus, A. A., Ennett, S. T., Bowling, J. M., Haws, S. W., & Rohrbach, L. A. (2009). Implementing evidence-based substance use prevention curricula with fidelity: The role of teacher training. Journal of Drug Education, 39, 39–58. doi:10.2190/DE.39.1.c Heikkilä, H.-K., Väänänen, J., Helminen, M., Fröjd, S., Marttunen, M., & KaltialaHeino, R. (2013). Involvement in bullying and suicidal ideation in middle adolescence: A 2-year follow-up study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 95–102. doi:10.1007/s00787-012-0327-0 Hightow-Weidman, L. B., Phillips, G. I., Jones, K. C., Outlaw, A. Y., Fields, S. D., & Smith, J. C. (2011). Racial and sexual identity-related maltreatment among 50 minority YMSM: Prevalence, perceptions, and the association with emotional distress. AIDS Patient Care and STDs, 25, S39–S45. doi:10.1089/apc.2011.9877 Hoglund, W. L. G. (2007). School functioning in early adolescence: Gender-linked responses to peer victimization. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 683– 699. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.683 Holt, M. K., & Espelage, D. L. (2007). Perceived social support among bullies, victims, and bully-victims. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 984–994. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9153-3 Holt, M. K., Raczynski, K., Frey, K. S., Hymel, S., & Limber, S. P. (2013). School and community-based approaches for preventing bullying. Journal of School Violence, 12, 238–252. doi:10.1080/15388220.2013.792271 Huitsing, G., & Veenstra, R. (2012). Bullying in classrooms: Participant roles from a social network perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 494–509. doi:10.1002/ab.21438 Idsoe, T., Dyregrov, A., & Idsoe, E. C. (2012). Bullying and PTSD symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 901–911. doi:10.1007/s10802012-9620-0 Juvonen, J., & Graham, S. (2001). Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized. USA: Guilford Press. 51 Kahn, J. H., Jones, J. L., & Wieland, A. L. (2012). Preservice teachers’ coping styles and their responses to bullying. Psychology in the Schools, 49, 784–793. doi:10.1002/pits.21632 Kaminski, J. W., & Fang, X. (2009). Victimization by peers and adolescent suicide in three US samples. The Journal of Pediatrics, 155, 683–688. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.04.061 Kendrick, K., Jutengren, G., & Stattin, H. (2012). The protective role of supportive friends against bullying perpetration and victimization. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 1069–1080. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.014 Kevin Fagan and Ellen Huet. (2013). Cyberbullying’s tragic toll. San Francisco Chronicle (10/1/2007), A1. Klomek, A. B., Sourander, A., Niemelä, S., Kumpulainen, K., Piha, J., Tamminen, T., … Gould, M. S. (2009). Childhood bullying behaviors as a risk for suicide attempts and completed suicides: A population-based birth cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 254– 261. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318196b91f Knack, J. M., Gomez, H. L., & Jensen-Campbell, L. A. (2011). Bullying and its longterm health implications. In G. MacDonald & L. A. Jensen-Campbell (Eds.), Social pain: Neuropsychological and health implications of loss and exclusion. (pp. 215–236). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. 52 Kokkinos, C. M., & Panayiotou, G. (2004). Predicting bullying and victimization among early adolescents: Associations with disruptive behavior disorders. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 520–533. doi:10.1002/ab.20055 Kowalski, R. M., Morgan, C. A., & Limber, S. P. (2012). Traditional bullying as a potential warning sign of cyberbullying. School Psychology International, 33, 505–519. doi:10.1177/0143034312445244 Kyriakides, L., Kaloyirou, C., & Lindsay, G. (2006). An analysis of the Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire using the Rasch measurement model. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 781–801. doi:10.1348/000709905X53499 Low, S., & Espelage, D. (2013). Differentiating cyber bullying perpetration from non-physical bullying: Commonalities across race, individual, and family predictors. Psychology of Violence, 3, 39–52. doi:10.1037/a0030308 Low, S., & Van Ryzin, M. (2014). The moderating effects of school climate on bullying prevention efforts. School Psychology Quarterly, 29, 306–319. doi:10.1037/spq0000073 Marini, Z. A., Dane, A. V., Bosacki, S. L., & YLC-CURA. (2006). Direct and indirect bully-victims: Differential psychosocial risk factors associated with adolescents involved in bullying and victimization. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 551–569. doi:10.1002/ab.20155 McElearney, A., Adamson, G., Shevlin, M., & Bunting, B. (2013). Impact evaluation of a school-based counselling intervention in Northern Ireland: Is it effective 53 for pupils who have been bullied? Child Care in Practice, 19, 4–22. doi:10.1080/13575279.2012.732557 Meltzer, H., Vostanis, P., Ford, T., Bebbington, P., & Dennis, M. S. (2011). Victims of bullying in childhood and suicide attempts in adulthood. European Psychiatry, 26, 498–503. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.11.006 Mouttapa, M., Valente, T., Gallaher, P., Rohrbach, L. A., & Unger, J. B. (2004). Social network predictors of bullying and victimization. Adolescence, 39, 315–335. Nansel, T., Overpeck, M., Haynie, D., Ruan, W., & Scheidt, P. (2003). Relationships between bullying and violence among US youth. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 157, 348–353. Nickerson, A. B., & Mele-Taylor, D. (2014). Empathetic responsiveness, group norms, and prosocial affiliations in bullying roles. School Psychology Quarterly, 29, 99–109. doi:10.1037/spq0000052 Nishina, A., Juvonen, J., & Witkow, M. R. (2005). Sticks and stones may break my bones, but names will make me feel sick: The Psychosocial, somatic, and scholastic consequences of peer harassment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 37–48. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_4 Nordgren, L. F., Banas, K., & MacDonald, G. (2011). Empathy gaps for social pain: Why people underestimate the pain of social suffering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100 120–128. doi:10.1037/a0020938 54 Noronha, C. (2012). Amanda Todd: Bullied teen shared story, then committed suicide. Christian Science Monitor, N.PAG. Olthof, T., & Goossens, F. A. (2008). Bullying and the need to belong: Early adolescents’ bullying-related behavior and the acceptance they desire and receive from particular classmates. Social Development, 17, 24–46. Olweus, D. (1997). Bully/victim problems in school: Facts and intervention. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, 495–510. doi:10.1007/BF03172807 Olweus, D., & Limber, S. P. (2010). Bullying in school: Evaluation and dissemination of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 80, 124–134. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01015.x Peleg-Oren, N., Cardenas, G. A., Comerford, M., & Galea, S. (2012). An association between bullying behaviors and alcohol use among middle school students. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 32, 761–775. doi:10.1177/0272431610387144 Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2000). An empirical comparison of methods of sampling aggression and victimization in school settings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 360–366. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.360 Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies, victims, and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group affiliation and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 216–224. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.216 55 Perren, S., Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E., Malti, T., & Hymel, S. (2012). Moral reasoning and emotion attributions of adolescent bullies, victims, and bully‐ victims. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 511–530. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02059.x Peskin, M. F., Tortolero, S. R., & Markham, C. M. (2006). Bullying and victimization among Black and Hispanic adolescents. Adolescence, 41, 467–484. Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., & Pigott, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of schoolbased bullying prevention programs’ effects on bystander intervention behavior. School Psychology Review, 41, 47–65. Putallaz, M., Grimes, C. L., Foster, K. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D., & Dearing, K. (2007). Overt and relational aggression and victimization: Multiple perspectives within the school setting. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 523– 547. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.05.003 Radliff, K. M., Wheaton, J. E., Robinson, K., & Morris, J. (2012). Illuminating the relationship between bullying and substance use among middle and high school youth. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 569–572. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.01.001 Roberts, A. L., Rosario, M., Slopen, N., Calzo, J. P., & Austin, S. B. (2013). Childhood gender nonconformity, bullying victimization, and depressive symptoms across adolescence and early adulthood: An 11-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 143–152. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.006 56 Sainio, M., Veenstra, R., Huitsing, G., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Victims and their defenders: A dyadic approach. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 144–151. doi:10.1177/0165025410378068 Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1–15. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:1<1::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-T Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections between attitudes, group norms, and behaviour in bullying situations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(3), 246–258. doi:10.1080/01650250344000488 Salmivalli, C., Voeten, M., & Poskiparta, E. (2011). Bystanders matter: Associations between reinforcing, defending, and the frequency of bullying behavior in classrooms. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 668–676. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.597090 Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in children’s peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181–192. doi:10.1023/A:1005174831561 Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L. (2005). Victimization in the peer group and children’s academic functioning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 425–435. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.425 Schwartz, D., Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2013). The link between harsh home environments and negative academic trajectories is 57 exacerbated by victimization in the elementary school peer group. Developmental Psychology, 49, 305–316. doi:10.1037/a0028249 Shakoor, S., Jaffee, S. R., Bowes, L., Ouellet‐Morin, I., Andreou, P., Happé, F., … Arseneault, L. (2012). A prospective longitudinal study of children’s theory of mind and adolescent involvement in bullying. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 254–261. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02488.x Skapinakis, P., Bellos, S., Gkatsa, T., Magklara, K., Lewis, G., Araya, R., … Mavreas, V. (2011). The association between bullying and early stages of suicidal ideation in late adolescents in Greece. BMC Psychiatry, 11. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-22 Slovak, K., & Singer, J. B. . (2014). School social workers’ responses to victims and perpetrators of cyberbullying: Results from a national survey. School Social Work Journal, 39, 1–16. Smart Richman, L., & Leary, M. R. (2009). Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model. Psychological Review, 116, 365–383. doi:10.1037/a0015250 Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., & Tippett, N. (2008). Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 376–385. doi:10.1111/j.14697610.2007.01846.x 58 Smith, R. L., Rose, A. J., & Schwartz-Mette, R. A. (2010). Relational and overt aggression in childhood and adolescence: Clarifying mean-level gender differences and associations with peer acceptance. Social Development, 19, 243–269. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00541.x Sorsdahl, S. N., & Sanche, R. P. (1985). The effects of classroom meetings on selfconcept and behavior. Elementary School Guidance & Counseling, 20, 49–56. Spriggs, A. L., Iannotti, R. J., Nansel, T. R., & Haynie, D. L. (2007). Adolescent bullying involvement and perceived family, peer and school relations: Commonalities and differences across race/ethnicity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 283–293. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.04.009 Stadler, C., Feifel, J., Rohrmann, S., Vermeiren, R., & Poustka, F. (2010). PeerVictimization and Mental Health Problems in Adolescents: Are Parental and School Support Protective? Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41, 371–386. doi:10.1007/s10578-010-0174-5 Storch, E. A., Brassard, M. R., & Masia-Warner, C. L. (2003). The relationship of peer victimization to social anxiety and loneliness in adolescence. Child Study Journal, 33, 1–18. Sutton, J., & Smith, P. K. (1999). Bullying as a group process: An adaptation of the participant role approach. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 97–111. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1999)25:2<97::AID-AB3>3.0.CO;2-7 Swearer, S. M., Wang, C., Maag, J. W., Siebecker, A. B., & Frerichs, L. J. (2012). Understanding the bullying dynamic among students in special and general 59 education. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 503–520. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2012.04.001 Teglasi, H., Rahill, S., & Rothman, L. (2007). A Story-Guided Peer Group Intervention for Reducing Bullying and Victimization in Schools. In J. E. Zins, M. J. Elias, & C. A. Maher (Eds.), Bullying, victimization, and peer harassment: A handbook of prevention and intervention. (pp. 219–237). New York, NY US: Haworth Press. Theriot, M. T., Dulmus, C. N., Sowers, K. M., & Johnson, T. K. (2005). Factors relating to self-identification among bullying victims. Children and Youth Services Review, 27, 979–994. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2004.12.024 Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2012). Bullying prevention programs: The importance of peer intervention, disciplinary methods and age variations. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 8, 443–462. doi:10.1007/s11292-0129161-0 Turner, M. G., Exum, M. L., Brame, R., & Holt, T. J. (2013). Bullying victimization and adolescent mental health: General and typological effects across sex. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41, 53–59. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2012.12.005 Ullman, S. E., & Brecklin, L. R. (2003). Sexual assault history and health-related outcomes in a national sample of women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 46–57. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.t01-2-00006 Unnever, J. D. (2005). Bullies, aggressive victims, and victims: Are they distinct groups? Aggressive Behavior, 31, 153–171. doi:10.1002/ab.20083 60 Vaillancourt, T., Brittain, H., Bennett, L., Arnocky, S., McDougall, P., Hymel, S., … Cunningham, L. (2010). Places to avoid: Population-based study of student reports of unsafe and high bullying areas at school. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 25, 40–54. Verlinden, M., Veenstra, R., Ringoot, A. P., Jansen, P. W., Raat, H., Hofman, A., … Tiemeier, H. (2014). Detecting bullying in early elementary school with a computerized peer-nomination instrument. Psychological Assessment, 26, 628–641. doi:10.1037/a0035571 Vicars, M. (2006). Who are you calling queer? Sticks and stones can break my bones but names will always hurt me. British Educational Research Journal, 32, 347–361. doi:10.1080/01411920600635395 Wang, J., Iannotti, R. J., & Nansel, T. R. (2009). School bullying among adolescents in the United States: Physical, verbal, relational, and cyber. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 368–375. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.021 Wienke Totura, C. M., Green, A. E., Karver, M. S., & Gesten, E. L. (2009). Multiple informants in the assessment of psychological, behavioral, and academic correlates of bullying and victimization in middle school. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 193–211. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.04.005 Willard, N. (2011). School response to cyberbullying and sexting: The legal challenges. Brigham Young University Education & Law Journal, 2011(1), 75–125. APPENDICES 62 APPENDIX A OLWEUS BULLY/VICTIM QUESTIONNAIRE 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 APPENDIX B CONSENT LETTER 76 APPENDIX C PARENT INFORMATION PAMPHLET 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 APPENDIX D KICKOFF EVENT SCRIPT KINDERGARTEN – 3rd GRADE SKITS SKIT 1 Synopsis: Three kids are playing jump rope on the playground. A girl wanted to join in but the bully and henchmen told her to go away. Sadly, she walks away and the defender comforts her. They walk to tell the yard duty to tell her what had happened. Later the yard duty discusses the bullying incident with the Principal. Kids 1 – 3: singing a jump rope song. Cinderella dressed in yellow went upstairs to kiss a fellow; made a mistake and kissed a snake. How many doctors does it take? 1, 2, 3, 4... Victim: walks up to group, stands around. Can I have a turn? Kids 1 – 3: ignore the victim and sing: Ice cream soda Cherry on top. Who’s your girlfriend I forgot A, B, C, D . . . Victim: Can I have a turn now? Bully: No, I already told you yesterday that you can’t play. You’re not our friend. Henchman: Yea! Passive supporter: hangs head and looks uncomfortable Victim: walks away rejected, sniffling Defender: sees the victim crying, runs up to him/her. What happened? Victim: They don’t want to play with me; they said I’m not their friend! Defender: Let’s go tell the yard duty. Victim: I don’t want to, I’m scared. Defender: I’ll go with you 85 Victim: Ok Victim and Defender: walk up to yard duty. Victim: Megan, Nikki, and Amanda won’t let me jump rope with them. I keep asking to play and they tell me to go away, I’m not their friend. Yard Duty: Well that’s not nice of them. I am sorry this happened to you. I will go talk to them and I will let the principal know what happened. Someone will follow up with you about this. Why don’t you and hector go play on the upper field for a while? Victim and Defender: walk to field. Victim: Thanks for helping me. Defender: You’re welcome. I was following the rule “We will help others when we see bullying.” Do you want to play ball with me? Yard Duty: approaches the bully group. I heard that you bullied Moua. You know the rules about bullying. We will not bully others. Because this happened, you will need to give me the jump ropes and sit on the bleachers for the rest of recess. Also, I will be telling Principal Julie what happened here and she will follow up with you. Blows whistle to indicate the end of recess. Yard Duty: Hi Principal Julie. I need to share with you a bullying incident that happened on the playground at recess. Hector and Moua came to me to tell me that Moua was bullied by Megan, Nikki, and Amanda. I think Megan was the main one bullying. I had them sit out the last few minutes of recess and told them they would be hearing from you. Principal: Thank you Yard Duty. I will follow up with hector and let them know what a good job he did. I will also talk with Moua to make sure she feels safe. I will also speak with Megan, Nikki, and Amanda to see what consequences should follow. SKIT 2 Synopsis: This is a non-bullying scene. It is after recess and the yard duty has blown the last whistle. All the kids form a line and when the yard duty is not looking 2 kids cut the line and the kids who witness the cutting mistook it for bullying. The yard duty with the help of another kid explains why this was not a bullying incident and then sends the two cutters to the end of the line. 86 In beginning of this scene the kids are playing on the playground. 3 students are playing hopscotch and 3 are playing jump rope, 4 students are playing four squares. Yard duty doesn’t blow her whistle until 15 sec. in. Yard Duty: blows her whistle, Kids, it’s time to go back to class! Some kids rush to form a line, while others ignore the whistle and keep on playing. Everyone manages to line up. The kid in the middle can’t stay still and starts playing with the kid in front of him/her. Kid 1: stop playing with my hair or I’m going to tell. Kid 1: Yard duty, he’s playing with my hair. Yard duty: Hector, stop playing with her hair and keep your hands to yourself. The kid stopped and kept his/her hand behind his/her back. Attention is now focused on the 2nd kid in line. Kid 2: psssttttt to kids 4 and 5. The yard duty isn’t looking, come to the front. You can cut me!” Kid 4 and Kid 5: hesitate Kid 2: Come on! She’s not looking! Kid 1: You can’t cheat, you’re cutting. Kid 3: Stop cutting that’s not fair, we were here first. I think you’re bullying us. I’m going to tell. Yard duty they’re bullying us by cutting. Yard duty: Who’s cutting? Kid 3: The two of them. Yard duty: Nikki and Hector, I’ve never seen you two cut before this isn’t like you guys at all. Megan, this is not bullying it is cutting. Megan, can you explain to Marybel why this is cutting and not bullying? Kid 1: This is not bullying because Nikki and Hector didn’t do it on purpose to hurt us. Yard duty: Good job Megan! Do you see the difference now? 87 Kid 3: Yes, I do. Yard duty: Nikki and Hector, even though this is not bullying it is still wrong. You two need to go to the back of the line. All of the Kids: Yay! Yard duty rules! Thank you yard duty! SKIT 3 Synopsis: In this scene the teacher is helping another student with math. The rest of the kids are supposed to complete a math worksheet by their selves. However, this is not the case. The bully and follower bully a student because he is hyper. Defender saw what was happening and told the teacher about it. The teacher discusses bullying with the bully and followers and then sends them to the principal’s office. Everyone is working on their math worksheet quietly except for one kid. The kid keeps on humming, picking on an eraser, and rocking. The kid hums loud enough for others to hear but not loud enough for the teachers to hear. Bully: Why can’t you ever stay still? You’re so dumb? You’re nothing but a dummy! You’re going to get in trouble. Victim: ignores them and keeps on going Follower 1: Yeah dummy stay still and stop. Follower 2: Oooo you’re going to get in trouble. You’re distracting all of us. Victim: gets irritated; throws pieces of the eraser at the bully and the followers. Follower 3: You need to be more like us, and stop being dumb. You need to listen to the teacher. Victim: I am listening to the teacher. Followers: No you’re not! Bully: sticks out his/her tongue. Hyper dummy. Victim: curls up and covers his/her face. Defender: Stop being so mean to her. That’s not right. Raises hand. Teacher they’re being mean to her.” 88 Teacher to victim: What’s wrong? Victim: removes hand from face, and point to the followers and bully. They called me dumb and said that I was going to get in trouble. Teacher: I’m so sorry that this has happened to you. I will talk them and Principal Julie will follow up with you. Moua, I want to thank you for helping out Christina. You did an awesome job and I want you to keep up the great work! Teacher: walks over to bully and followers, I know what you guys have been up to. What are our school rules about bullying? Bully and followers stayed quiet so the teacher asks again. Teacher: What are our school rules about bullying? Bully: The school rules say that we will not bully others. Follower 1: We will try to help students who are bullied. Follower 2: We will try to include students who are left out. Follower 3: If we know that somebody is being bullied, we will tell an adult at school and an adult at home. Teacher: I’m glad that you guys were able to remember the school rules but I’m disappointed that none of you followed it. Remember that it doesn’t matter who you are, the rules apply to everyone. Bully: But Christina was really annoying and we just can’t help it Teacher: There are no excuses for bullying and bullying is bullying. We treat everyone the same no matter what they’re like. Here are the referrals; I’m sending all of you to Principal Julie’s office. GRADE 4 – GRADE 6 SKIT 1 Synopsis: Cyberbullying - Three girls are hanging out at home talking amongst themselves about the clothes a girl was wearing at school. They log onto Facebook and send the victim a message regarding her appearance. One girl will be the bully, one will be the henchmen, and the other will be the defender. At home the victim reads the message. The victim’s mom finds out and calls the school. The next day 89 before school starts the defender goes to the victim and tries to comfort her. The principal then comes and talks to them about the situation. Bully: Did you see what Megan was wearing today? Henchmen: That shirt she was wearing was so ugly and dirty. Defender: I thought it looked nice. Bully: Well you’re wrong, it was hideous! Henchmen: We should tell her something on Facebook. Bully: That’s a great idea, quick bring me my laptop Henchmen brings laptop. Defender: We should just leave her alone. Bully: Oh shut up and be quiet, we do what I say… logs on Facebook…found her home page. Now what to say…hmmm…oh I know, “you should try getting some new clothes ‘cause what you were wearing today was just plain ugly and dirty. You looked like a homeless person.” Defender: C’mon now…just leave her alone, she doesn’t deserve it. (On the other side of the stage) Victim: Oh I have a new message from Moua I wonder what she said? “You should try getting some new clothes ‘cause what you were wearing today was just plain ugly and dirty. You looked like a homeless person.” Starts crying. Dad: walks in. What’s wrong Megan? Victim: This girl from school told me I was wearing an ugly shirt and that I look like a homeless person. Dad: She did what? I’m going to call your school right now! The next day before school starts Defender: goes up to the victim. I’m sorry for what Moua said, I tried to stop her but she wouldn’t listen to me. Don’t pay any attention to what she says. She’s just mad that you’re prettier than her. Victim: Thank you! I’m glad to know who my true friends are. 90 Principle: walks up to Victim and Defender. I heard what happened. Thank you Amanda for following the school rule: “We will try to help students who are bullied.” I want you to know that I am going to follow up with the other students involved. Do you feel safe to be around them? Victim: Yea, I’m okay. Especially since Amanda let me know that we are still friends. Principal: Glad to hear that. You girls have a great day. Megan, I will be checking in with you in a little while to make sure everything is still okay. SKIT 2 Synopsis: Non-bullying. During recess a group of kids are playing soccer. One kid gets mad that someone scored a goal on him. He kicks the ball over the fence so that no one can play anymore. The kids then move on to play a different game. Kid 1: Let’s go play soccer for recess, I’ll go get the ball and meet you all on the field. Goes to get ball while the rest go to the field Kid 2: Yes! You got the best ball we have! Now let’s get the game started. Kid 3: Since we have 12 people, let’s play 6 vs. 6. Kid 4: Ok, I’ll be goalie. Kid 5: I’m going to score so many goals on you! Kid 4: I don’t think so. I’m the best goalie here. (Game starts, play for 20 seconds) Kid 5: Goalllll!! Ha-ha Kid 4: You got lucky. A couple plays later, 15 seconds later Kid 5: Goalllll!! I told you I’m going to be scoring lots of goals Kid 4: You won’t score anymore goals on me. Right away Kid 5 scores another goal 91 Kid 5: Goallll!!! Kid 4: gets angry and kicks the ball over the fence. Now no one will play anymore! Kid 1: What the heck, I had to check out that ball. You have to go tell yard duty. Kid 4: Sorry! I just get so mad when Kaoshoua shows off. Kid 2: I guess we have to play something else now. Go tell yard duty and we will meet you at the hoops. They all go play basketball SKIT 3 Synopsis: This scene is an ADHD child (Provocative Victim) who acts very obnoxious and continues to jump in the middle of others conversations at the wrong time. This child is looking for friends and is very friendly but often does not know how to interact so not to annoy other students. There are a group of children on their lunch break talking outside talking in a circle. Child 1: Oh my gosh you guys will never guess what I did this weekend Child 2: Really…something exciting? Child 1: Yes, very exciting but you guys have to guess. Provocative victim: runs up just to catch the words “you guys have to guess.” Have to guess what guys? Child 3: roles his/her eyes and says. What Amanda did this weekend? Provocative victim: Oh, oh, I want to guess…Um let me see did you go to Disneyland, or did you go to….hmm… Child 1 interrupts loudly: No I went to my cousin’s house in San Francisco and we went on a boat ride. Anyway, the boat was so much fun…we Provocative victim: jumps in abruptly and loudly. I went on a boat one time and it was so much fun… Child 3: No one cares! Child 1: staring at provocative victim. What did you do this weekend? 92 Child 3: Probably nothing interesting because you have no friends… because you are a loser. Child 1: Whispers to the friend closest to her. Let’s go talk over here, where Nikki won’t bother us. Provocative victim: Whatever! Who cares about going to San Francisco anyway? I go to SF all the time. AND, I have lots of friends just so you know. Children 2 & 3: Ha, yea right. Child 1: Why are you even over here right now? Provocative victim: Well…umm…I thought you guys were my friends. Child 1: Well you were wrong…you are a pest and we aren’t your friends. Children 4 & 5 look shocked and see that these comments are really hurting the provocative victim. Child 4: Hey guys, you are being mean and really excluding Nikki. Hey Nikki, I will be your friend. Child 1: You will? Whose friend are you, mine or these losers? Pointing to the provocative victim. Child 4: puts head down and does not answer. Child 1: Just get out of here! Victim runs off in tears as the other children laugh except for child 4, who follows after the victim. Child 4: Let’s go tell the teacher. They shouldn’t treat you like that. Bullying isn’t right. Victim: I’m afraid. Child 4: It is ok. I will go with you. Both children go into their classroom and find their teacher. 93 Child 4: Nikki is being made fun of. The other kids are calling her names and being mean to her. Teacher: That’s not right. Do these children always talk to you that way? Victim: Yes, they are never nice to me! They always call me names, and make fun of me. Child 4: She wasn’t doing anything. She was just trying to talk to all of us. Teacher: It’s not your fault .These children are bullying you. They will be sent to the principal and will get in trouble. Bullying is never ok! GRADE 7 – GRADE 8 SKIT 1 Synopsis: Reverse Bullying. The bully asks basketball star (victim) to the 8th grade dance. The victim politely declines her invitation, yet the bully is not willing to take “no” for an answer. She becomes angry and begins to call him “gay.” The next day at school, the bully and her group of friends (henchmen) target him in front of onlookers, throwing him dirty looks, whispering to each other, and giggling. The victim tells his coach about the harassment, and Coach explains to him that this is an instance of bullying. The coach talks to the victim, bully, and henchmen. Bully: (female) standing on one side of the stage, expectantly waiting. Henchmen: (also female) are standing a few paces back watching. Victim: (male) walks onto stage near the bully dribbling a basketball and carrying a water bottle. Bully: Hi Hector. How was practice? Victim: Hey Marybel, pretty good. Coach was in a good mood since we beat our rivals last night. Bully: Well that’s good . . . the game last night was pretty fun, it was so exciting when you made that shot at the buzzer. You’re like the most popular guy at school now. Victim: Oh, yeah. Chuckles uncomfortably 94 Bully: So . . . the 8th grade dance is coming up, and I think we should go together! You’re the most popular guy in school right now, and I’m obviously popular, we would make the perfect couple! Victim: Uh . . . I’m sorry, I can’t. Bully: What?!?! Why not?? Are you already going with someone else? Victim: No. Bully: Well then why won’t you go with me? Are you going to be out of town? Victim: No Bully: I don’t get it. Looking more and more agitated. Victim: Look, I just don’t want to go to the dance with you okay? I’m sorry. I’ll see you tomorrow. Turns and walks away Bully: shouts after the victim. Yea well you’re just gay! Victim: turns around. What? Bully: Anyone who would turn down a chance to go to the dance with me must be gay. Victim: shakes his head and walks off Bully: stomps over to her henchmen and whispers to them. . . . Next day . . . Bully and Henchmen standing around in a group. Disengaged Onlookers are standing nearby. Victim: walks by with a basketball under his arm. Bully and Henchmen throw Victim dirty looks, whisper to each other, and giggle. Bully: Hey Hector. Where’s your boyfriend!? Henchmen snicker Victim glances at the group, shakes his head, and walks off. 95 Bully and Henchmen resume whispering, giggling, and snickering . . . Later in the day . . . Victim: Hey coach, you got a second? Coach: Absolutely, what’s up bud? You enjoying the stardom? Victim: Eh, not really. Coach: looks quizzically at Victim. Victim: Well, after I made that shot, the next day this girl decided she wanted to go to the 8th grade dance with me. I told her no, as nicely as I could, but now she’s all mad at me. Which I guess I could understand, but she won’t stop calling me gay, and she and all her friends keep whispering about me and laughing. It’s getting pretty annoying. She was yelling about my “boyfriend” in the hall earlier, everyone heard. Coach: Well, son, it’s not acceptable for her to bully you like that. Victim: Bully me? But she’s a girl and I’m a guy, how can she bully me? Coach: By calling you names, harassing you, and making fun of you in front of others, is she hurting you? Victim: Well yea, I guess. Coach: And she’s doing this on purpose because you wouldn’t go to dance with her? Victim: Yea. Coach: Well, bullying occurs when someone, on purpose, hurts someone else physically or emotionally by embarrassing them and making them feel bad in front of others. Remember her doing the same thing to Kyle a couple of weeks ago? Victim: Yea. Coach: And remember our rules against bullying. Victim: How could I forget? Rule #1: We will not bully others. Coach: Right. So, you also know that I will follow up with Marybel about this and I will check back in with you after a while to make sure it has stopped. 96 Victim: Thanks, Coach. SKIT 2 Synopsis: Non-bullying. Boy 1 and Girl are standing in the hall discussing the movie they are going to go see that evening. Boy 2 walks up and invites Girl to the same movie. Boy 2 realizes that Boy #1 has already asked Girl out and becomes agitated. Boy 1 and Boy 2 start to get worked up and eventually come to blows. A teacher breaks up the fight and sends them both to the principal’s office. Girl is standing in the hallway talking to Boy 1. Boy 2 walks up. Boy 2: Hi Scott. What’s going on??? glares at Boy 1. Girl: Oh we’re just talking about the new movie that’s coming out tonight. Boy 2: Oh yea? I heard that’s supposed to be good. Do you want to go see it with me? Girl: Umm, Scott and I are already going to see it tonight. motioning to Boy 1. But you could come with us, we could get a whole group together. Boy 1: Hector isn’t coming with us! Girl: looks surprised. Boy 2: Wait a second; you’re going to a movie with him??? Girl: Uh, well I was . . . Boy 2: Shouting at Boy 1. You aren’t going to a movie with her! Boy 1: Try and stop me! Boy 2: Dude, I’ve been trying to ask her out for a week! Boy 1: Well I just did! Boy 2: lunges for Boy 1, they begin to fight. Teacher: runs up and breaks up the fight. What’s going on here!?! Boy 2: This jerk just asked out my girl! 97 Boy 1: She said yes! . . . And she isn’t “your” girl! Boy 1 and Boy 2: lunge at each other again. Teacher: separates them once again. That’s it you two, the principal’s office. Now! Teacher marches off with Boy 1 and Boy 2. SKIT 3 Synopsis: Cyberbullying/Drug Use. A group of students pass around a bottle of prescription medicine. The victim chooses not to partake. As a result, she is called “Saint” and “Goody Two Shoes.” That night, the bully posts multiple status updates on Facebook that degrade the victim and sends the victim threatening text messages. The victim is torn between wanting to fit in and doing the right thing. The next day, the victim comes to school looking distraught. The defender asks what is wrong and they discuss the situation. The defender encourages the victim to tell his/her Mom and offers to go with her. Victim, Bully, Henchmen, and Disengaged Onlookers are standing around outside during recess. Henchman 1: passes a bottle of pills to the bully. Bully: Sweet! Henchman 1: My brother said he’ll keep giving them to me as long as I do his math homework. Bully: takes a pill and passes the bottle to Henchman 2. Henchman 2: Well then you better keep doing it! Henchman 2: takes a pill and passes it to the victim. Victim: does not take a pill and passes the bottle to Henchman 3. Bully: What, are you too good to take some? Victim: No, I just don’t want to. Henchman 3: takes a pill. What’s the big deal? 98 Victim: looks uncomfortable. I don’t want to do drugs. Bully: Ok, Saint Kelsy. Henchman 1, 2, and 3: all laugh. Bully: to victim. Come on, take one. Victim: No thank you. Bully: You really are a goody two shoes. It’s just Megan’s brother’s ADHD pills. Henchman 1: Quick, put them away, here comes Mrs. Julie! Henchman 3: hands the pills to the bully, who shoves them in pocket. Bully, Henchman 1, 2, and 3 begin to scurry away. Bully: You better not say anything, Kelsey. . . . The next day . . . Victim: walks on stage, looking distraught. Defender: Hi, Kelsey! Victim: Hey. Defender: Are you okay? Victim: No, you’re lucky you were out sick yesterday. Defender: I am? Why? Victim: Marybel brought some pills to school yesterday, and when I wouldn’t take any, they started pressuring me. I didn’t take any, but now she is calling me “Saint Kelsey” and “Goody Two Shoes” on Facebook and threatening me not to tell anyone about the pills. Marybel said if they get caught, it will all be my fault. Look at all these texts from them. Shows phone to Defender. Defender: reads aloud: “Hey Saint Kelsey, I know you think you’re too good for us, but you better not tell anyone about the pills! You’re just a pathetic loser who doesn’t want to have any fun!” 99 Victim: Maybe I should have just taken a pill. Defender: What!? No way! You can’t let Marybel bully you into taking drugs! Victim: But she is going to make my life miserable! Defender: Not if I can help it. We have to tell someone. Remember the Rule? Victim: Yea, “If we know that somebody is being bullied, we will tell an adult at school and at home.” Defender: So let’s tell your mom after school. Victim: But I’m nervous. Defender: Why, you didn’t do anything wrong. Victim: I know, but still. Marybel told me not to say anything. Defender: But if we tell your mom then she can help us figure out what to do. Victim: Yea, she is pretty good at that. Defender: So we can talk to her after school? Victim: Yea. Defender: Good. 100 APPENDIX E FIRST CLASS MEETING OUTLINE AND SCRIPT 101 102 103 104
© Copyright 2024