Why People Don't Take Their Concerns about Fair Trade to the Supermarket: The Role of Neutralisation Author(s): Andreas Chatzidakis, Sally Hibbert and Andrew P. Smith Source: Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 74, No. 1 (Aug., 2007), pp. 89-100 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25075446 . Accessed: 28/03/2013 04:09 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Business Ethics. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions (2007) 74:89-100 Journal of Business Ethics DOI 10.1007A10551-006-9222-2 ? their Concerns Why People Don't Take to the Supermarket: about Fair Trade The Role ABSTRACT. article It examines and theory of FT how explores the can neutralisation consumer context. the explaining for of knowledge KEY FT ethical WORDS: illustrative provides framework role of and behaviour, purchase that FT will in providing new generate behaviour purchase consumer in evident discrepancies research research neutralisation and gap, fair decision-making, eliminate ethical trade con purchase, neutralisation their norm their the processes by which individuals understanding in ethical consumption contexts cope with dissonance is important for scholars and practitioners. For scholars, for processes that explain attitude-behav accounting to bring further insights iour discrepancies promises to current understanding of ethical decision-making; neutralisation may prove to be an important moder to incorporate into extant theoretical of why practitioners, appreciation do not always translate into behaviour will tomarketing direction and management give practices, and particularly marketing intended communications, attitudes counter neutralisations various that consumer use. article begins by examining the core tenets of neutralisation the applicability theory and discussing to ethical consumer of the concept in behaviour The more consumers who to be profess sym pathetic with the aims of the Fair Trade (FT) move ment buy FT products at all or more regularly ?Cowe andWilliams (2000) andBird andHughes (1997)both confirm that Yet, segments Introduction don't impact have might violating and upon self-concept et social relationships It has been al., 1989). (Grove widely applied in a number of contexts but litde work has been done in the field of consumer behaviour. to Why the behaviour ating variable models. For behaviour. attitude-behaviour ethical sumerism, the consumers' of aspects research conceptual further to the purchase techniques being used in the FT attitude-behaviour to consumers' relation direction other A delineate of neutralisation its applicability Exploratory examples of neutralisation propositions tenets theoretical assesses critically products. SallyHibbert Andrew P. Smith explain people's lack of commitment to buying Fair Trade (FT) products, even when they identify FT as an ethical concern. Andreas Chatzidakis of Neutralisation This Springer 2007 that consumers most share various ethical concerns including FT, yet few translate their concerns into actual behaviour. When faced with a clear choice between fairly traded goods and mainstream products, those who make ,less ethical' choices experience dissonance? And if so, how do they restore equilibrium without changing their attitudes? This article addresses do in particular. Evidence general and to FT purchase from an exploratory qualitative study is then provided an outline for a theoretical framework after which that integrates the neutralisation of ethical construct consumer with behaviour existing explanations and FT purchase is given. This framework leads to the of research generation propositions aiming to address the issue of non-purchase of FT products by con an ethical concern. to it be recognise sumers who these neutralisation questions by applying theory and Neutralisation describes a Matza, 1957). (Sykes mechanism that facilitates behaviour that is either norm violating attitudes. When or in contravention they neutralise, of individuals expressed soften or What Social is neutralisation? norms behaviour play a crucial role in guiding et al., 2002). When (e.g. Davies This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions ethical social et al. 90 Andreas Chatzidakis norms are not guide to the degree internalised under behaviour all that they consumers circumstances, the dis may develop coping strategies to deal with sonance In 1957 Sykes and that they experience. Matza their seminal article on juvenile published to those of his/her can be perspective of the normative flexibility categorical to attributed the systems: rather than being norms social imperatives, or limited against killing does not the delinquent learns example, the moral injunction apply in time of war. Thus, patterns committed of her actions me fault with after the an attempt some higher to actualise order ideal or value; e.g. "To some what I did may I did it for my but appear wrong, family". to note that, while It is important these techniques as unethical behaviour, may be viewed following can make unethical and it, ultimately they precede behaviour nalised, are values in their applica "qualified guides terms in of time, place, persons, and social cir bility cumstances" 1957, p. 666). For (Sykes and Matza, for action, should find the society, complete opposition or rationalisa learns a set of justifications delinquent can tions, i.e. the techniques of neutralisation, which from self-blame and the blame of insulate him/her This they rip-offs they have engineered". to higher loyalties (AtHL): argument (5) Appeal is the result of that norm-violating behaviour suggested that rather than learning values or attitudes standing in a delinquency. They moral imperatives, others. joke to remain that help him/her thought to the normative system and qualify his/ as "acceptable" if not "right". (Grove rationalisations. Since known of deviance (Minor, 1981; Copes, 2003). Examples of its application include a variety of juvenile (e.g. Ball, 1966; Minor, 1981; Costello, 2000) as well as adult contexts deviance and 1981; Eliason (e.g. Levi, Dodder, of more their norm-violating behaviour. These techniques, consumer to context the adapted by Strutton et al. (1994, p. 254), are listed below along with generic genocide isational examples: neu by Sykes and Matza, one most has been of the theory widely and frequently cited theories in the sociology its formulation tralisation (1957) identified five categories Sykes and Matza labelled ,neutralisation that describe techniques' the different rationalisations that people apply to - inter possible. That is, once successfully can become devices they truly neutralising et al., 1989) as opposed to post behavioural 1999; Fox, 1999). It has also been the subject intuitive applications including the victimi sation of battered wives (Ferraro and Johnson, 1983), and the Holocaust rule management pageant mothers Calhoun, (Alvarez, 1997), 2003) enforcing (Fershing, of the "temporary deviant" in the United States organ and the label of (Heltsley and 2003). of responsibility (1) Denial argument (DoR): that one is not personally accountable for the norm-violating tors beyond one's behaviour control are because operating; fac Neutralisation (DoV): blame countered for personal the violated by arguing e.g. "It's party deserved whatever happened; I their fault; if they had been fair with me, would not have done it". the condemners (4) Condemning (CtC): deflec tion of accusations misconduct of by point out that those who condemn engage in ing similarly disapproved activities; e.g. "It's of FT et al. (1994) found that, in consumer con consumers tend to ratio texts, otherwise principled nalise their non-normative behaviour by appealing to the techniques of neutralisation. Other scholars have Strutton cause no party directly suffered as a result of the big deal in stealing it, it; e.g. "What's nobody will miss it?" of Victim the purchase products e.g. "It's not my fault, I had no other choice". of Injury (Dol): contention that per (2) Denial sonal misconduct is not really serious be (3) Denial actions and a applied neutralisation search in this domain to consumer behaviour, but re remains limited (Strutton et al., and Chan, 2002; Rosenbaum 1994, 1997; Mitchell and Kuntze, 2003). Most considered quite extreme of the extant studies have and were illegal behaviour based on one-offsurvey instruments, amethodological for approach that has been described as problematic investigating ethical behaviour (e.g. Crane, 1999) and also for validating the theoretical tenets of neutralisa tion theory in particular (see e.g. Fritsche, This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 2005). The Role There are strong reasons to believe that the need to justify/rationalise one's own behaviour spans all sorts of consumer as as activities, long they involve a concern. the negligence of ethical personal studies by Bersoff confirmed Experimental (2003) have (2001) and Fritsche the applicability of ? small rather than clearly i.e. failure to declare small both to relatively neutralisation deviant ethical breaches and drinking overpayment cans from non-recyclable respectively. Chatzidakis et al. (2004) explored the applicability of neutralisation theory in a variety of consumer contexts. found evidence They are techniques readily employed such as shoplifting, behaviours, that neutralisation in clearly deviant but also in more flexible contexts, such as recycling and normatively FT Each of these contexts varies buying products. and there is a need for dis however, considerably, crete treatment with context specific conceptualisa tion and matively consumption flexible have choices been that are more nor as behaviours described guided by personal moral beliefs and individual eth ical standards (e.g. Crane and Matten, 2004; Cher rier, 2005) rather than group or social norms that these apply under any circumstances. Consequently, are the contexts where attitudinally-incongruent behaviours should be most pervasive. This article builds probing more deeply the specific context ceptual contribution decision-making ating variable nally, construct the 91 in the context range of supporting FT and to uncover the that When justifications they employ. the application of existing theoretical there is a deductive to the research, aspect of exploring constructs but qualitative to re allow participants approaches count stories, examples and scenarios that illuminate the nature of those constructs within the particular context (Patton, 1990; Mason, 1996). This interplay of induction and deduction is particularly principles valued when neutralisation eliciting techniques (Fritsche and Mayrhofer, 2001), as it can also indicate the accessibility in people's mind, of arguments is an important which characteristic for effective neutralisation (Fritsche, have approaches tative on our earlier research by in into the use of neutralisation of FT. of It elaborates neutralisation the con to ethical its role as a moder by examining in relation to established article the models. it was that important familiar with the basic prin sample members some FT movement of the and felt ciples degree of it and these filters were with empathy A used during was re sample recruitment. cruited by asking visitors to a FT Roadshow (pro in central England to event) at a university motional participate convenience in the research. sample informants; of the informants were eighteen Fourteen were a total of and eleven females. There seven males staff or students at the university and four were in full-time employ ment in other organisations. The age range of the sample was from 20 to 50 years, with a bias towards the lower integrates the Theory with of Planned Behaviour - one of the more testable 1985, 1991) (TPB; Ajzen, models that has been applied to ethical decision ? and develops to research propositions making facilitate empirical of role of the neutralisa testing tion in consumer purchase of FT goods. to FT in relation were Fi neutralisation 2003). Accordingly, quali been widely for adopted into other applications of neu exploratory inquiries tralisation theory (e.g. Ferraro and Johnson, 1983; Hazani, 1991; Byers et al., 1999; Gauthier, 2001). In order to be able to discuss experiences of dis sonance research. Ethical ofNeutralisation Face ethical ages. to face concerns were interviews to conducted to FT, relevant explore the occurrence attitude-behaviour in of this domain, individuals discrepancies of dissonance and whether experiences use arguments in relation to their neutralisation-style FT consumption. Of particular concern for the re search design was the inherent problem of the per ceived social desirability in relation to of opinions issues (Crane, 1999; Nancarrow et al., 2001), the unattractiveness of favouring more selfish goals over socially-oriented et Davies al., 2002) goals (e.g. and the association between self neutralisation, ethical evidence: Preliminary Justifications for not buying FT products The evidence here was based preliminary presented a qualitative to examine how inquiry intended use readily individuals arguments neutralisation-style on needs and personal of FT presentation importance values (see Fisher and Katz, 2000). For these reasons, interviews were preferred to focus groups to avoid the social pressures of conforming This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions to perceived et al. Andreas Chatzidakis 92 norms the lead of dominant members and following of the group (e.g. Malhotra, 2004). To address the research partici social influence of the interviewer, to talk about pants were prompted consumer both in a projective/indirect 1993) and with Projections were explored Fisher, to the participant as well Discussion centred on being manner for ,other people' known as unspecified ,others'. which the interviews, the study aimed to each of the five associated with of examples to COSTA in bigger to me aren't subsidies, to going is problem...which and the minor that FT about about all agree signing the condemners our coffees other consumer... the small (CtC): for money FT...where round? If could they example, that can you should non-FT be save to coffee". it of any request it maybe a person wants request got you've look at the menu, if you letters print in way be not I like honest, very and nearest "I've thought in what tech of different trying the some and (CtC buying but in Salisbury's I things...and on same the a consideration, be I go shopping cal on keen might on". and in general...when I look for the cheapest to me". thing some that times I buy...but part be more I should of me is quite ethi lazy actu ally". "...and we then but whatever, to give an indication niques are presented below devices that are used in the FT context. it seems care coffee, where says "FT arguments neutralisation is too big to be dealt at the le that (3) Condemning am vidual's own priorities, whereas the second was coded DoR the person is suggesting because that their on the behaviour is contingent behaviour of a third Illustrative system". organizations "...to FT retailer. to an (4)Appeal to higher loyalties (AtHL): not if they were products two Of first these the excessively priced". examples, would be coded AtHL because it refers to the indi the buying DoR). to price/cost informants often referred example, when their behaviour; explaining typical quotes were "I always go for the cheapest things" and "I would party, view, my (by that contributes anything consumer... the FT...in buying are systemic... "I think that the issue of FT puts a lot of burden of In the coding process itwas apparent predetermined. an important that the framing of statements was of the should be coded. For determinant how data buy more trading fairness explore the use of the five techniques of neutralisa in the sense that the tion, the approach was deductive basic coding themes relating to these techniques were really trade ments". challenged but nearly all of them did so. Interviews behaviour, 20 and 75 min. lasted between of of those as the issue arose naturally in the course of was None of the participants to rationalise attitudes and incongruent recordings were made were later transcribed. As improved not for bad unfair ity of people overcome the individuals' Audio of I'm not doing FT) vel conversation. the feel causes "I think, the problem (and others') attitudes towards FT and the interviewer probed own to their talk about (and others') participants behaviour "I wouldn't the (see themselves. to reference a FT (2)Denial of Injury (Dol) (or denial of benefit): have this posed to be better coffee so I never this coffee coffee, buy coffee is FT which is horrible! And isn't it? I don't it's and sup like this it". are et al. (1989) note that some techniques heavily used than others in particular ethically situations. With respect to FT, appealing challenging to higher (AtHL), denial of responsibility loyalties Grove more (1)Denial of responsibility (DoR): so much "It more is so est money "I think don't "I I would think products when think much that promote actively a and anyway, expensive at the moment..." tight or supermarkets these shops more product don't explanation..." FT know and it's when enough, they FT I not...but, given reference referred to denial of victim and denial as Denial used neutralisation that exists are not tenuous (Dol; or what was were of Benefit) the more widely there was only techniques, while to CtC and none of the arguments (DoR) manifest in general, about passionate if I had realised the difference people hon things..." become is to be of injury (DoV). The informants on the responsibility grounds that they were that distribution and promotion of FT uninformed, denied products is inadequate or FT goods This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions are too costly. The Role ofNeutralisation 93 breaches in of injury (or of benefit) was based on notions such as that FT is just a marketing ploy or a small to contributes scale initiative which only marginally make. of Third World of aminority producers, if at all, and which does not lead to a systemic change in trading systems. External attribution was evident Ethical Itwas also related arguments. tensions which argument, highlighted in her own and other that the informant recognised to understand ethical decision-making Attempts of have increased substantially since the 1980s. Much contexts is the research in managerial/organisational Denial the everyday that choices people the welfare in both DoR and Dol to the CtC stakeholders' arguments In contrast, responsibilities. to higher loyalties related to social appealing financial constraints, convenience, variety perceived or the inferiority of FT products, perceived seeking self-oriented illustrating the tension between more and social responsibility. goals this re findings are only illustrative because demonstrate of course, search approach cannot, cannot that the causation and therefore prove The are arguments as used devices. neutralising However, that consumers the study did provide an indication have a range of accessible neutralisation techniques not to justify their actions when do buy FT they goods. Further research is required to demonstrate is at work in this context, but first that neutralisation relates to it is necessary to outline how neutralisation the to build in existing models used of ethical decisions. The next section constructs understanding gives a brief overview making been applied neutralisation of ethical decision that have how the act as a moderating might detailed these frameworks. A more construct variable within of how discussion of models models social psychological in this context and outlines and can be neutralisation integrated Behaviour the Theory of Planned is then (Ajzen, 1985, 1991) pursued. with (TPB) based literature decision-making on one ethical or another decision-making of the prominent positive and models such as Hunt (1986; 1992) general theory of marketing interac Trevino's ethics, (1986) person-situation tionist model, Ferrell and Gresham's (1985) contin Vitell's ethical decision for understanding model and (1991) issue-contingent making Jones' see Ford and Richardson, 1994; Loe (for reviews et al. 2000; O'Fallon and Butterfield, 2005). Rest's gency framework (1979) four-stage model as a major highlighted of moral influence is often judgment in this stream of research and Butterfield, (e.g. Jones, 1991; O'Fallon Its four fundamental components 2005). (1) rec a a moral moral issue, (2)making ognising judgment, (3) resolving to place moral concerns ahead of other and (4) acting on those moral concerns concerns, can be viewed as the underlying structure of all the ethical theories because, prominent decision-making different variables/constructs, despite emphasising some on one or more in focus way steps of this they model. Whilst the role of neutralisation stages of this process was (Vitell and Grove, 1987), research has empirical (McDonald Elsewhere addressed in some as early as 1987 unfortunately, subsequent remained limited very and Pak, 1996). in the consumer behaviour stream of and Vitell's research, Hunt (1986; 1992) theory of ethics and of Planned marketing Theory Ajzen's A moderating Ethical decision-making: for neutralisation? In a recent literature, review O'Fallon of role the ethical decision-making (2005) high lack of research into identi the surprising variables that may moderate key relationships fying of the existing models. ethical decision-making is to restore The primary function of neutralisation balance incongruent important when act people manner moderating (TPB; Ajzen, 1985, as the more and Butterfield lighted 1991) are identified frame commonly applied theoretical works (Chatzidakis et al., 2004). The small amount con of research that has specifically investigated Behaviour and, as variable in such, an attitudinally it may that explains be an ethical has concen support for the FT movement on and testing models based on the trated developing TPB 1999; Shaw et al. 2000; (Shaw and Clarke, Shaw and Shiu, 2002a, b, 2003). sumers' structure of in the fundamental commonality the ethical decision-making models suggests that the in of neutralisation relation to any conceptualisation The model that represents this four-stage This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions process, or part et al. Andreas Chatzidakis 94 thereof, can be relatively readily transferred. In this in relation the conceptualisation article we develop is arguably the most robust of in social psy models attitude-behavioural to the TPB. all the The TPB in a great and it has been successfully applied chology see e.g. Notani, (for reviews, variety of domains and Conner, 1998; Ajzen, 2001; Armitage 2001). most has been it the Crucially, applied frequently and tested in various aspects of ethical consumer behaviour, Clarke, 2002a, (Shaw and purchase et al., 2000; Shaw and Shiu, instances of consumer misconduct ethical including 1999; Shaw b, 2003), (Fukukawa, 2002) as well as more specific applica on tions e.g. software piracy (Chang, 1998), waste (Chan, 1998) and green consumerism (e.g. et al., 1999). and 1992; Kalafatis Shepherd, Sparks role of the moderating Therefore, conceptualising in relation to this theoretical neutralisation frame recycling in this and comparability promotes consistency nascent area of research. Further, it is in principle so long as open to the inclusion of other constructs work et al, a process (Strutton describes Neutralisation 1994, 1997). of restoring equilibrium without attitude change. Comment has been made more that generally or attitude-behaviour of cognitive consis social psychology have left the diverse tency within a to state without modes of returning balanced theories attitude (Hazani, 1991). Even change unexplored the cognitive dissonance literature, where a counter-attitudinal attitudes after performing within to remain in striking been found to that the focus has largely behaviour, opposition as opposed to the been on the arousal of dissonance, to attitude change; that lead processes subsequent hence generating little evidence regarding the nature behaviour have those of processes 2002). Accordingly, that there is surprising ways et al., 1990; Holland et al. (2002) observe little research on the different (Kunda, Holland in which people behaviour: incongruent tion and the associated their attitudinally justify The concept of neutralisa of neutralisation taxonomy increase TPB's power (Ajzen, they explanatory 1991, p. 199). Lastly, it is in line with all the ethical as models mentioned decision-making previously, is a theoretical contribution that promises techniques to build understanding of this gap. Despite the po tential to elaborate on the contribution of neutrali (from attitudes long as they allow for a step-by-step to intentions to behaviour) view of the cognitive to cognitive consistency and Chaiken, 1993 for a review), For process. point ethical all the above reasons, and as a particular, conceptualise and neutralisation, basis for future present empirical role of the moderating as a research propositions testing, in relation to the TPB. important momentum research Consumers often experience their own balancing that favours behaviour is clear and evidence example, being ethical moral and there concerns in actual always manifest For and Atalla, 2001). (e.g. Carrigan consumers to buy have been found environmentally their expression 1996) (Strong, to adherence behaviour tensions with are not attitudes behaviour internal that consumers' that hazardous products regardless of for greener alternatives shoplift regardless of their of concern and to societal guide and models the economic marketplace norms behaviour of area. this of psychological "norm-activation" denial", processes in incorporated being of ethical decision-making. model has been which For incorporates found to have as helping ability explanatory behaviour 1977; Schwartz and Howard, (Schwartz, 1981) and energy conservation (Tyler et al., 1982). TPB context the in the of shoplifting, Applying a case Fukukawa made for the inclu (2002) strong in desires societal well examples neutralisation to similar example when in are There "defensive rolefor neutralisation? (see Eagly in this article effort to understand established A moderating theories at integrating itwith the models adopted it may be an ethical decisions, where to build on the variable, moderating is directed starting up a new direction for research on in and support for FT decision-making for opening we sation contexts sion of "perceived unfairness" neutralisation point-of-view, such in the model. both of the From a above constructs represent a rather fragmented picture of the neutralising "defensive denial" relates to process: the of "denial of responsibility" whilst technique to unfairness" the of "denial victim". "perceived Hence, ethical account the of neutralisation into incorporation a more decision-models holistic promises of defensive psychological This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions mechanisms. The Role ofNeutralisation 95 serve Further indication that neutralisation might a significant role within the ethical decision-making is provided its underlying process by re-examining tenets in relation to a concept theoretical that has ? at more within attention least enjoyed organisa tional ethical decision-making research that is the addition "perceived unfairness"; while proposed in the ethical consumerism field, Shaw and col 1991). The moral intensity the concept convergent support for acknowledges nature of moral decisions. The the issue-contingent characteristics of the moral called issue, collectively realities of consumers who consistently psychological are in apparent contradiction behave in ways which to their expressed ethical concerns. Figure 1 incor moral intensity (Jones, are important determinants of intensity", a ethical decision-making and behaviour review, (for see O'Fallon and Butterfield, 2005). Jones (1991) six basic components of moral proposed intensity; "moral of magnitude social consequences, consensus, probability of effect, temporal immediacy, proximity are purported to and concentration of effect. These affect all steps in the ethical decision independently making as process, is neutralisation. Indeed, neu of reasoning are very much based on tralisation-types a biased interpretation of precisely those situational characteristics. For may of technique magnitude the probability the denial of injury example, on an act's downplaying or underestimating consequences rely of (negative) effects, whilst the denial on its be based lack of may temporal of victim immediacy. 1999; Shaw et al., 2000; leagues (Shaw and Clarke, Shaw and Shiu, 2002a, b, 2003) have suggested the and "self identity". addition of "ethical obligation" the model for the fails to account Still, inevitably in to TPB neutralisation the porates alongside and col additional variables Shaw proposed by for attitude-behaviour leagues to account discrep ancies At Conceptualising in consumers FT role of neutralisation purchase context. this of neutralisation theory norm a conventional of both acceptance situational lays and the the to it. Therefore, to contrary exceptions on the assumption studies based many underlying it does not assume the TPB, is always most role consistent with condition important in consumers' that people's behaviour their attitudes. Indeed, the to play a for neutralisation is that ethical decision-making an act should have a desire to commit and at the (that represents a less ethical alternative) same time have ethical bonds that require neutrali sation and Hughes, 1981; Dodder (e.g. Minor, individuals a TPB these ethical framework, 1987). Within attitudes towards a bonds could translate in positive norms. but also, in positive behaviour, subjective Whilst the in the heart the latter could be subsumed under the former (see e.g. Eagly and Chaiken, 1993, p. 178), such a distinction would be in conceptual agreement behaviour is an extension The TPB of the Theory of Reasoned and Fishbein, (TRA; Ajzen 1980), suggesting that behaviour in a specified is a direct situation, Action in turn is a intention, which and subjective norm. TPB differs by adding a new construct, perceived refers to an individual's control, which function of behavioural function of attitude from TPJV behavioural control behavioural While to is suggested intentions and behaviour. beliefs and the TPB has been found impact both to have some Behaviour explanatory ability, a large part of the ethical con sumer decision-making In still remains unexplained. this has been accounted for by sampling, general, and issues (see operationalisation behaviour-specific 1998; Ogden, e.g. Luzar and Cosse, 2003) or by the in the addition of further constructs. For example, context of consumer ethics, Fukukawa (2002) has Self-Identity Background Figure sation 1. The in the Characteristics direct theory and moderating of planned This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions effects behaviour. of neutrali 96 Andreas Chatzidakis with the original application of neutralisation theory to norm-violating instances as well as with the dis crete role of social norms, as highlighted in one way or another in most existing ethical decision-making models 1986, 1992; Ferrell and (e.g Hunt and Vitell, therefore Trevino, 1986;). We on the direct and moderating present propositions role of neutralisation from both attitudes and sub Gresham, 1985; norms jective behaviour, to intention, as follows: and then to et al. weakened Hence of neutralising by the acceptance the following propositions: P3a: The of neutralising be higher the acceptance liefs the weaker the relationship between ethical and behavioural intention. obligation P3b: The be liefs self actual of neutralising higher the acceptance the weaker the relationship between on consumers' has a direct, negative intentions behavioural individual to pur important tudes and behavioural Pic: The liefs be atti intention. of neutralising higher the acceptance the weaker the relationship between be sub intention. jective norms and behavioural P2a: Neutralisation has a direct and indirect intentions) negative chase behaviour. influence (via on actual FT pur P2b: The of neutralising be higher the acceptance the weaker the between relationship behavioural intentions and actual FT purchase liefs behaviour. is open to the inclusion of other con so long as they increase TPB's explanatory when power (Ajzen, 1991, p. 199). Accordingly, to behaviours that be ethical may applied guided by several studies have empirically confirmed concerns, The TPB structs the usefulness of two additional constructs, i.e. "ethical intention. ethical influence chase FT products. Plb: The higher the acceptance of neutralising liefs the weaker the relationship between and behavioural identity Within Pia: Neutralisation beliefs. several research, decision-making traits have been empirically established as determinants of ethical behaviour such as locus of control and cognitive gender, religion, see moral et al., reviews Loe (for development consumer and O'Fallon for 2000; Butterfield, 2005; in particular, see Vitell, 2003). In line characteristics with a TPB framework however, traits, personality and variables, values, intelligence, demographic other variables of this kind are considered "back factors" that "...influence behaviour and ground on intention their effects behavioural, indirectly by or control normative beliefs these and, through on attitudes, norms or percep beliefs, subjective tions of 197) That control" (Ajzen they are is, of the TPB and Fishbein, not 2005, but neglected are assumed to mediate p. the the components effects of background factors on intentions and actions. Since to directly they are not expected affect the relationships in the above described for the sake of parsimony and length we do not expand further on the considerations, influence of these factors. However, future research could benefit from identifying charac background propositions, and "self-identity" obligation" (e.g. Beck and Ajzen, 1991; Sparks and Shepherd, 1992; Sparks et al. 1995; Armitage and Conner, 1999). Research Shaw and and Clarke, 1999; by (Shaw colleagues that may the acceptance influence of neu as in beliefs to other tralising particular, opposed as of determinants behaviour atti such proximal tudes or subjective norms. For example, given that et al., 2000; Shaw and Shiu, 2002a, b, 2003) has established to the applicability of both concepts FT purchase in particular, therefore substantiating to influence the already been found of beliefs Ball, 1966; employment neutralising (e.g. Ward and Beck, this would 1990; Bersoff, 2001), mean that the effects of neutralisation in the ethical Shaw the proposition that support for FT may be guided by personal moral beliefs and ethical standards much more or universal than social norms values. As to atti in addition discussed however, previously norms tudes or subjective these types of ethical - as bonds in the constructs reflected of ethical and be may obligation self-identity equally teristics gender has decision-making women rather tional could factors be for process would stronger than men. Identification of addi ones particularly psychological valuable information about the ori provide of beliefs as well gin neutralising for future interventions. ways This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions as possibly suggest The Role research and Conclusions 97 ofNeutralisation directions et al., 1998). The few (e.g. Bohner a study on failure to declare include (Bersoff, 2001), one on environmen manipulated exceptions Ifmost consumers hold several ethical concerns/pro social attitudes, yet fail to behave accordingly (Bird andWilliams, and Hughes, 1997; Cowe 2000), then of ethical it is imperative theories that existing consumer to explain the evolve decision-making restore without in which ways equilibrium people an initial impetus for attitude change. To provide the this research, this article outlines neutralisation to consumption its applicability ory and discusses contexts ethical brea that involve relatively minor use in relation to FT. were There a tins; Fritsche, 2003) studies (embedding further measure in quantitative studies. The neutralisation final part of the article examines how the neutrali can be extant sation construct integrated with to build of ethical deci understanding to research and presents propositions sion-making relation facilitate empirical testing of the proposed models used ships in future research. The lack of literature on at an neutralisation intrapersonal level means that there is also a dearth of research that demonstrates the stage in the decision are em at which neutralisation process techniques precede an ployed. For example, does neutralisation act (rather than follow it)? It is often said that alter as an cannot be considered neutralisation natively, etiological represent theory not much of (im)moral more tions of deviant behaviour than behaviour. ex (Dunford post facto It would and is a primary this for direction on the from advancing understanding Apart such a research ethical decision-making, consumer's could endeavour for have important implications communications and public policy could be a neutralisation example, to alternative based strategies, emotional communication existing on example for or informational In fact, while of the phenomenon is increasingly discrepancies as a pertinent issue within ethical consumer viewed and Atalla, research 1996; Carrigan (e.g. Roberts, and Dawkins, 2001; Worcester 2005), there ismuch appeals. attitude-behaviour more to be done the most if to establish effective of guilt; (e.g. psychological approaches those discrep Bennett, 1996) for actively reducing ancies. A neutralisation-based approach could take inducement of advantage neutralisation consumers how understanding use to justify their techniques pursuing selfish goals (initially through qualitative data such as those reported here in the context of FT) and more then address the use of these mechanisms It head-on. is hoped that future experimental studies will pro a more vide theoretical and empirical appropriate as well basis for substantiating the above proposition as examine those in more effects directly communication/intervention-based relevant, environments. explana and Kunz, 1973; for count 1991; Dabney, 1995, p. 316; see e.g. Peretti-Watel, erarguments 2003). To an extent, this is amethdological problem: experimental in the neu been have approaches underrepresented Hollinger, free-riding; research. promising Further research is required to fully ac for the nature of neutralising arguments in the a scale to as a context FT and basis for developing minor commit and (e.g. shoplifting and Bayer, 1989) and on rape proclivity et support of FT, which (Bohner al.1998). However, is a relatively minor ethical breach, can be examined of the demographic they profile of the participants; all drawn from one geographic region of the to this type the analytical challenges common U.K.; count to intentions Schwarz actual marketing initiatives. For of research. on a questionnaire) acts delinquent to this study: a small conve number of limitations nience sample was used and itwas restricted in terms were (i.e. drinking from one-way similar and two methodologically an experimental manipulation in behaviour experimentally It ches and the particular case of buying FT products. from an exploratory evidence presents study that that the types of neutralising identified arguments consumers overpayment tally harmful literature; since for both ethical and prac tical reasons neither neutralisation (as the independent tralisation variables of interest variable) not many dependent can be easily extreme criminal behaviour) (e.g. Note For fined the agreements, tive working of purposes as goods this paper "purchased cooperative involving trading principles, conditions for ensuring the (Strong, 1996, p. 5). This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions FT under products equitable rather than a fair price producers and are de trading competi and fair suppliers" et al. 98 Andreas Chatzidakis References of Contemporary Criminal fustice 15(1), Amish', fournal 78-96. Ajzen, I. and M. Fishbein: 1980, Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behaviour (Prentice-Hall, Inc, Cliffs, NJ). Englewood Ajzen, I.: 1985, 'From Intentions to Actions: A Theory in J. Kuhland and J. Beckman of Planned Behaviour', to Behaviour (eds.) Action Control: From Cognition (Spring,Verlag, Heideberg), pp. 11-39. I.: Ajzen, 'The 1991, Organizational of Theory Behaviour Planned & Human Behaviour', Decision Processes 50, 179-211. I.: Ajzen, and 'Nature 2001, of Operation Attitudes', Annual Review of Psychology 52, 27?58. I. and M. Fishbein: 2005, 'The Influence Ajzen, on Attitudes in D. Behaviour', and M. T. P. Zanna Johnson (eds.) The Handbook of Attitudes. (Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ), pp. 173-221. Alvarez, A.: 1997, 'Adjusting to Genocide: The Tech of niques and Neutralization ScienceHistory 21(2), 139-177. Armitage, C. and M. Conner: of Theory Planned the Social Holocaust', 'Efficacy of the 2001, A Meta-Analytic Re view', British Journal of Social Psychology 40, 471-497. C. Armitage, J. Conner: 1999, 'Distinguishing Ball, R. A.: 1966, 'An Empirical Exploration of Neu tralization Theory', Criminol?gica 4(2), 22-32. Beck, L. and I. Ajzen: 1991, 'Predicting Dishonest Ac tions Using the Theory of Planned Behaviour', Journal ofResearch in Personality 25, 285-301. R.: of Horrific 'Effects 1996, Fear on Appeals Public Attitudes Towards AIDS', InternationalJournal of Advertising 15(2), 183-202. Bersoff, D. M.: 2001, 'Why Good People Sometimes do Bad Things: Motivated and Unethical Reasoning The Next Behaviour', K. Bird, The D. and Case of Phase G., Kerschbaum Coffee', "Fairly-Traded" M.-A. Business Ethics: S. B. Sturm, as 1998, 'Rape Myths Evidence for a Causal Impact Cognitions: of Anti-victim Attitudes on Men's Self-reported Like European Journal of Social Psychology Byers, B., B. W. Crider Crime Motivation: fender Neutralization and G. K. Biggers: 1999, S. Hibbert, A., 'Bias A Study of Hate Crime and Of used Against the Techniques D. Mitussis A. and Smith: 'Virtue in Consumption?', fournal ofMarketing Management 20(5/6), 526-543. to Ethical Sensitive Cherrier, H.: 2005, 'Becoming Behaviour: Consumption Uncertain Narratives and Unpredictable Consumer Research H.: of in an Survival in Advances World', 600-604. 32, 'Societal 2003, and quency Attachments, of Techniques Fre Offending Deviant Neutralization', Behaviour 24, 101-127. B. 2000, J.: Cowe, Test Theory', Consumers? The S. and of Social Control Deviant & Williams, R., of Neutralization 'Techniques A Critical Neutralization Behaviour 21, and 301?329. are Ethical (2000) Who Bank. Co-Operative Crane, A.: 1999, 'Are you Ethical? Please Tick Yes? or No? On Researching Ethics in Business Organiza Business Ethics 20, 237-248. tions', fournal of Crane, and D. A. pean Matten: Business 2004, Managing Corporate Perspective Ethics: Press, versity D.: Dabney, Davies, 'Neutralization Theft of G. R. Foxall andj. in and Medicines Behaviour Intention-behaviour Dodder, R. and (Oxford Uni and Deviance Supplies Deviant Nurses', J. D., Euro Oxford). 1995, Workplace: Hospital A Citizenship Sustainability in theAge of Globalization 16, Pallister: by 313-331. 2002, An Mythology: the 'Beyond Integrated of Recycling', Marketing Theory 2(1), 29-113. and S. P. Hughes: 1987, 'Collegiate Drinking - a Test of Neutralization Theory', fournal ofAlcohol and Drug Education 33(1), 73-85. Dunford, F. W. P. R. and Kunz: 'The 1973, Neutral ization of Religious Dissonance', Review of Religious Research 15(1), 2-9. Eagly, A. H. and S. Chaiken: 1993, The Psychology ofAtti tudes 28, 257-269. Proenviron 2004, Behaviour S. Rutz, and in Hong Kong', Behaviour: Waste Recycling fournal ofEnvironmental Management 52, 317-325. Chang, M. K.: 1998, 'Predicting Unethical Behaviour: A of the Theory of Reasoned Action and Comparison the Theory of Planned Behaviour', fournal of Business Ethics 17(16), 1825-1834. Model Consumerism: Effler: Neutralizing lihood of Raping', 3, 239-262. Communication 18(7), 560 mental the 159-167. Reinhard, and D. Ethics 'Ethical 1997, Hughes: A European Review 6(3), Bohner, of Business 'Mass 1998, Self-esteem: 72-90. Bennett, K.: Costello, Perceptions of Control from Self-efficacy: Predicting of a Low-fat Diet Using the Theory of Consumption Planned Behaviour', Journal ofApplied Social Psychology 29, Chan, Copes, Behaviour: and M. Behaviour?', fournal of Consumer Marketing 577. Chatzidakis, of B. Albarracin, Carrigan, M. and A. Attalla: 2001, 'The Myth of the in Purchase Ethical Consumer - do Ethics Matter Eliason, (Harcourt S. L. Neutralization States: Brace and R. A Research Jovanovich, A. Dodder: Fort Worth, 1999, TX). 'Techniques of by Deer Poachers in theWestern United Note', Deviant This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Behaviour!^, 233-252. The Role K. Ferraro, J. M. and J. Experience Process The Battering: 'How 1983, Johnson: Strategies', Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28(12), 1713-1724. Hollinger, R.: 1991, 'Neutralizing in theWorkplace: An Empirical Analysis of Property Theft and Production Women of Victimization', Social Problems 30(3), 325-339. O. Ferrell, and L. G. C. Framework Gresham: inMarketing', Making 'A Contingency 1985, Decision Ethical for Understanding to the Enforcement of to Snitch?" Techniques Misconduct', Occupational Sociological Perspectives 46(2), 149-178. Fisher, R. J.: 1993, 'Social Desirability Bias and the Validity of Indirect Questioning', Journal of Consumer Research 20(Sep), 303-315. Fisher, R. E. Katz: J. andj. 2000, Bias 'Social-Desirability R, ture', Journal of Business Ethics 13(3), 205-221. Fox, K. J.: 1999, 'Reproducing Criminal Types: Cogni tive Treatments for Violent Offenders in Prison', The Sociological Quarterly 40(3), 435-453. R. and Norm-contradictive Harmful Behaviour. Paper Taxon of Environmentally at presented the I. (2003). Pre-Behavioural Neutralization: and Myths and Account-Giving its Impact on Behaviour. Unpublished Fritsche, I.: 2005, 'Predicting Deviant Deviant by Behav iour. 26(5), 483-510. K.: Fukukawa, 2002, of Business D. Gauthier, K.: Ethics 14, and Neutralization Medicine Practice', S. J., & S. J., Vitell, Normative of Neutralization. Educators and Occupational in Veterinary 22, D. Strutton, and Structure', Deviant Holland, 2002, R. Behaviour (1989). R. W., 'Dissonance Moderator of Non M. Meertens on Internal the Road: K.: Levi, 1981, of Plan Examination', Journal 16(5), 441-460. for Motivated Case T. W., a Hit 'Becoming Very Deviant Reasoning', Man: in a Neutralization Career', Urban Life 10(1), 47-63. L. and Ferrell P. Mansfield: 'A Review 2000, E. J. and K. J. Cosse: 1998, to 'Willingnes Pay or Intention to Pay: The Attitude Behaviour Relation Evaluation', Journal of Socio-Eco ship in Contingent nomics 27(3), 427-444. Malhotra, N. K.: 2004, Marketing Research: An Applied Hall, (4th ed) (Pearson-Prentice Upper Saddle River, NJ) Mason, J.: 1996, Qualitative Researching (Sage, London) G. and P. C. Pak: 1996, 'It'sAll Fair in Love, McDonald, in Ethical War, and Business: Cognitive Philosophies Decision Making', Journal of Business Ethics 15(9), 973 W. W: UK L. Examination', J. Chan: Unethical I. Brace C, 2002, Attitudes Journal ofMarketing Management Desirable Journal 18, 5-26. and L. T. Wright: Little Sweet Lies: Dealing in Market Responses 'Investigating and Behaviours', Research', 'Tell me 2001, with Social The Mar keting Review 2, 55?69. 'The Good Notani, by Pageant Van Self-Esteem Vugt: as Justification A. S.: Control's 81-100. and External A of Neutralization: ,Techniques and Empirical and K. Consumers' Lies Tell me of Contemporary and M. 1981 V.-W. Mitchell, Symbols Banks Journal 24, Tsogas: and Ajzen's Theory ofResearch inCrime andDelinquency, July, 295-318. the Techniques Ethnography 20, 179-202. and T. C. Calhoun: 2003, Heltsley, M. Mother: Neutralization Techniques Used Mothers', 'The 1990, H. Psychological Bulletin 108(3), 480-498. Nancarrow, 1991, 'Aligning Vocabulary, Sociocultural Z.: Kunda, and M. East Cross-market Reconceptualization 467-490. 131-135. pp. A Behaviour: of Consumer Marketing Minor, Proceedings of the 1989 AMA Winter Conference, Hazani, M.: Lapses: Behaviour Consumer Model', 996. Techniques Behaviour Deviant for 99?119. 'Professional 2001, Framework in Consumption', Behaviour Deviance Grove, a 'Developing Ethically Questionable Journal ned R. Pollard, 'Green Marketing Orientation Manuscript. Behaviour Findings', S. P., M. 1999, Luzar, 14th An nual Conference of the International Association for Conflict Management, 24-27 June 2001, Cergy (Paris), France. Fritsche, by Indi Making Issue Contingent of Empirical Studies Assessing Ethical Decision Making in Business', Journal of Business Ethics 25(3), 185-204. Strategies New A Behaviour: in the Context omy Exemplified An Academy ofManagement Review 16(2), 366-395. Loe, Account (2001). 'Ethical Decision 1991, in Organizations: viduals Kalafatis, of Self-reported Values', Psychology & Marketing 17(2), 105-120. 1994, 'Ethical Ford, R. C. and W. D. Richardson: Decision Making: A Review of the Empirical Litera I. & Mayrhofer, 169?202. 12, pp.775-784. Jones, T. M.: and the Validity Fritsche, Behaviour and S. J. Vitell: 1986, 'A General Theory of Hunt, Marketing Ethics', Journal ofMacroMarketing 6, 5-15. Hunt, S. D., & Vitell S. J. (1992). The General Theory of in and Revision, Marketing Ethics: A Retrospective & in Ethics Irwin C Smith Marketing, QuelchJ (1992), S. D. 96. Fershing, J. L.: 2003, '"To Snitch or Not the Concept of Neutralization Applying Deviant Deviance', 87 Journal ofMarketing 49(3), 99 ofNeutralisation a 1998, 'Moderators of Perceived Behavioural in the Theory of Planned Consumer Behaviour', Journal of Psychology 7(3), 247-271. M. K. and D. Butterfield: O'Fallon, 2005, 'AReview J. of the Empirical Ethical Decision-making Literature: 1996-2003', Predictiveness Journal of Business Ethics 59(4), 375-413. This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions et al. 100Andreas Chatzidakis 'Some Problems with J.: 2003, Ogden, A Models: and Conceptual Pragmatic Shaw, D., E. Shiu and I. Clarke: 2000, 'The Contribution of Ethical Obligation and Self-identity to the Theory of Ethical of Planned Behaviour: An Exploration Social Cognition Health Analysis', Psychology 22, 424-428. M. Patton, 1990, Q.: Methods and Research Evaluation Qualitative Consumers', P.: 'Neutralization 2003, the and Theory of Risk: Some Evidence from Cannabis Use Among French Adolescents', British fournal of Sociology 54(1), 21-42. Rest, J. R.: 1979, Development in fudging Moral Issues Denial of Minessota (University J. A.: Roberts, Please Minneapolis). the Real Business M. S. tionship Between Kuntze: and R. Anomie 'The and Unethical and J. A. S. H. Howard: Model Decision-Making R. Rushton and Helping Behaviour: M. 1067-1093. Social, in Altruism', Sorrentino P. J. Hillsdale, and pp. NJ), 189-211. Schwartz, Generation Strutton, 1977, on Influences 'Normative Altruism', (eds.) Advances in Experi Vol 10. mental Social Psychology (Academic Press, New 1-2. pp. York), & Bayer, N., Eine A: anten Verhaltens of Causality ory of Deviant D. Ethical on Study and Sykes, D. and E. Approach', 2002a, International Investiga Preliminary and O. in Retail C. L. and E. 1997, a Is There Settings: of Business Journal Ferrell: Ethics 87-105. 16, Pelton: 'How 1994, in of Business Journal G. M. and D. A Matza: 1957, of Theory Research 30, A of Neu American Delinquency', in Making Interactionist Person-situation ofManagement Review Academy 253-260. 'Techniques Sociological Review 22(6), 664-670. L. K.: 1986, 'Ethical Decision Trevino, of 11(3), 601? 89/ Tyler, T. R., R. Orwin and L. Schurer: 1982, 'Defensive Basic and and High Cost Prosocial Behaviour', Applied Social Psychology 3(4), 267-281. Denial Vitell, S. J. and S. J. Grove: 1987, 'Marketing of Neutralization', the Techniques Journal Ethics and of Business Ethics 6, 433-438. Groups: Shiu: no. An in Formation Study', Exploratory 109-119. 'An Assesment fournal of Consumer Vitell, S. J.: Studies 26(4), 286-293. of Ethical Shaw, D. and E. Shiu: 2002b, 'The Role in Ethical Consumer and Self-identity Obligation Choice', Internationalfournal of Consumer Studies 26(2), 109-116. D. Ward, Ethics 'Consumer 2003, Ethics A. 43, and W. Research: Review, for the Future', Journal of and Suggestions Synthesis Business of Ethical in Ethical Consumer and Self-identity Obligation A Structural Equation Modelling Decision-making: 33-47. L. Beck: 'Gender 1990, and Dis honesty', The Journal of Social Psychology 130, 333?339. Worcester, and & R., Environmental Newholm, J. Dawkins: 2005, Attitudes,' Ethical ,Surveying in R. T. Harrison, D. Shaw (eds.), The Ethical Consumer (Sage pp. 189-203. Publications), Nottingham University Business School, University and E. Shiu: 2003, 'Ethics in Consumer A Multivariate Modelling Approach', Euro 1485-1498. pean fournal ofMarketing 37(10), Shaw, D. Choice: Gap'', tralization: f?r Umfragen, 'Belief 1999, Marketing Intelligence & Planning 17(2), Shaw, The report Zentrum Analysen. I. Clarke: Consumer of the Neutralization Unre Control, 617. 89/23) Instrument A Pelton S. J. Vitell D., Model', devi Nr. an (ZUMA-work Germany: und Methoden Shaw, A as Order Behaviour Mannheim, 23)]. E. Organizations: Fragen Kausalit?tspr?fung: (ZUMA-Arbeitsbericht Question Testing: der zur Neutralisationstheorie Untersuchung [Variation der Variationen 1989, Instrument als reihenfolge Zimmer and N. Consumers May Justify Inappropriate Behaviour Market Settings: An Application on the Techniques in L Berkowitz Schwarz, L. D., Neutralization', S. H: Social Consumerism"', - Consumerism 'Ethical Behaviour and Developmental Personality, Ethical Strutton, Altruism (eds.) Erlbaum, (Lawrence Perspectives. of "Green tion', Marketing Intelligence^ Planning 14(5), 5-13. Dis 'A Normative 1981, with N. Wieringa P., R. Shepherd, Sparks, manns: 'Perceived Behavioural 1995, of Rela Retail position', Psychology & Marketing 20(12), Schwartz, Horizons, 2003, Identification Psychology Quarterly 55, 388-399. and Dietary Change: An Exploratory 243-255. 24, Study', Appetite to the Growth C: 'Features 1996, Strong, Contributing Socially Responsible Forward? Step Sparks, P. and R. Shepherd: 1992, 'Self-identity and the Theory of Planned Behaviour: Assessing the Role of alistic Optimism 79-83. pp. fan-Feb, Rosenbaum, Press, 1996, Will Consumer 879 894. (Sage, London) Peretti-Watel, 16, Management ofMarketing Journal ofNottingham, Campus, Wollaton Road, IBB, UK Nottingham, NG8 fubilee E-mail: This content downloaded from 141.109.120.3 on Thu, 28 Mar 2013 04:09:21 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Uxac2@nottingham. ac. uk
© Copyright 2025