Copyright 1997 by The Geronlological Society of America Journal of Gerontology: SOCIAL SCIENCES 1997. Vol. 52B. No. 1, S37-S48 Loss to Follow-up in a Sample of Americans 70 Years of Age and Older: The LSOA 1984-1990 Adrienne H. Mihelic1 and Eileen M. Crimmins2 'Department of Population Dynamics, Johns Hopkins University. 2 Andrus Gerontology Center, University of Southern California. A LTHOUGH the increasing availability of panel data has •**• been a great boon to researchers studying characteristics that change with age and over time, analysis of panel data is complicated by the loss of panel members at followup interviews. All studies using panel data must investigate the extent to which loss to follow-up has occurred and whether the loss is likely to bias analytic results. Bias can be introduced when loss to follow-up is not random and is related to outcomes of interest. Such selection of the sample may result in confounded associations (Schaie, Labouvie, and Barrett, 1973; Siegler and Botwinick, 1979) and distorted prevalence estimates and transition rates (Corder, Woodbury, and Manton, 1992; Van den Berg, Lindeboom, and Ridder, 1994). For example, an analysis of health change in a panel of older persons would be selected with respect to the characteristic of interest if persons in worse health were more likely to become nonrespondents than others; this could result in biased estimates of the level of health of the sample and the correlates of health change. Samples of older persons are generally believed to be more susceptible to nonresponse selection because the proportion lost to follow-up tends to increase with age (Rodgers and Herzog, 1992). Studies of nonresponse have suggested that the differences between participants and nonrespondents may be greater among older persons than younger (Schaie, Labouvie, and Barrett, 1973). It is the magnitude of the difference between the respondents and nonrespondents that ultimately determines whether the sample is compromised by nonresponse. In this study, we are concerned with nonresponse that occurs at an interview after the baseline interview, often referred to as wave nonresponse. This type of nonresponse should be distinguished both from item nonresponse, which occurs when a person does not respond to a single question, and from initial nonresponse, which occurs when a person does not respond to the entire first interview. We also distinguish nonresponse among those alive from nonresponse due to mortality, which we view as a "legitimate outcome." The goal of this study is to evaluate whether there is nonrandom nonresponse in a multiwave panel study of older Americans (the Longitudinal Study of Aging) and whether this nonresponse is likely to significantly bias analytic results based on these data. To determine whether there is nonrandom nonresponse related to observed characteristics, a predictive model of wave-nonresponse is developed. Individual probabilities of nonresponse estimated from this model are used to construct weights for the remaining sample members which compensate for the loss to follow-up. Substantive univariate and bivariate results from data adjusted for nonresponse with these weights are compared with those from unadjusted data. In a second approach appropriate for multivariate analyses, the probability of sample selection (or loss to follow-up) is modeled simultaneously with a substantive outcome. This approach allows us to examine the effect of any unobserved characteristics jointly affecting nonresponse and our substantive outcome, and to evaluate the extent of sample selection bias in a multivariate context. An Overview of the Problem of Nonresponse As noted above, nonresponse is an issue in all panel surveys. In the first household interview of the National Medical Care Utilization and Expenditure Survey (NMCUES), 10 percent did not respond. By the fifth interview, conducted 15 months later, 22.1 percent of the sample were nonrespondents (Wright, 1983). About 5 percent of the Survey of Income and Program Participation (SIPP) sample did not respond to the first interview. By the ninth wave, 36 months later, this rate had accumulated to 22.3 percent (Kasprzyk and McMillen, 1987). Nonresponse to the annual interview of the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) was high in the first wave (24%), then leveled off to about 2 S37 Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Loss to follow-up is a problem in longitudinal samples, and the literature on response rates in panels of older persons suggests that they may be more vulnerable to nonrandom attrition and its consequent biases. The event history approach used in this study to determine the correlates of nonresponse addresses important shortcomings of previous analyses by incorporating time-varying covariates. Nonresponse is not random; persons of older ages, lower education, who live alone, rent (not own), have more functioning impairments, or have another sample person in the household are more likely to become nonrespondents. However, correction accounting for the effect of these correlates of nonresponse, as well as unobserved characteristics potentially affecting nonresponse, suggests that the association between these characteristics and the probability of nonresponse is not large enough to introduce bias. While these results are not portable to other analyses or panels, they do indicate that in this case, significant nonrandom nonresponse does not bias all related analytic results. S38 MIHELICAND CRMMINS How Do Persons Become ' 'Lost to Follow-up'' ? Nonresponse can occur because a person refuses to respond to the survey, because the person cannot respond to the survey, or because the person cannot be located. Refusal to respond may indicate distaste for responding to surveys in general, lack of interest in the particular survey, or competing uses for the potential respondent's time. Refusal may be related to gender. Women might be more wary of personal contact with interviewers; men might find long telephone surveys distasteful. This might be why men are more likely to be nonrespondents in some studies (Jay et al., 1993; Kalton et al., 1990; Streib, 1982), while in others they are not (Adams etal., 1990). In general, persons of lower socioeconomic status, those who have fewer years of education or lower incomes, are more likely to become nonrespondents (Chyba and Fitti, 1991; Streib, 1982). It is possible that persons with higher education could be more interested in the survey itself, or more aware of the importance of survey response to research, and thus less likely to refuse. Persons of lower socioeconomic status may have a greater distrust of survey organizations or less identification with the interviewers, and this may lead to more refusal. The relationship between socioeconomic status and refusal could explain the finding that African Americans have been more likely to be nonrespondents (Foley et al., 1990). Inability to complete a future interview is more likely among persons who are in worse health. Many studies have reported that nonrespondents are likely to be in poorer physical health, have a greater number of functioning difficulties, have worse self-reported health (Chyba and Fitti, 1991; Manton, 1988; Rodgers and Herzog, 1992; Speare, A very, andLawton, 1991; Streib, 1982) or have lower levels of cognitive functioning (Kalton et al., 1990; Schaie, Labouvie, and Barrett, 1973; Siegler and Botwinick, 1979). Difficulty locating sample members may be a particularly important problem among older respondents. Residential relocation among the old may occur because a household member dies or because an individual can no longer maintain independent living. Persons identified by the survey organization as contact persons are also likely to be older and thus more likely to die and dissolve their residences. Those who do not own their homes are more likely to become nonrespondents because they have fewer barriers to moving and thus are more likely to become unable to locate (Kalton, 1990; Speare, Avery, and Lawton, 1991). Family composition and marital status both have been linked to nonresponse (Speare, Avery, and Lawton, 1991). Persons who are living alone may be more likely to become lost to future interviews since they are less likely to leave someone behind who would know their whereabouts. Lillard's (1989) finding that household response rates were higher among those belonging to families with other households participating in the survey is consistent with this hypothesis. Age is almost always found to be a predictor of nonresponse (Adams et al., 1990; Rodgers and Herzog, 1992). This may be due to the fact that the older one is, the more likely one is to experience changes in residence that result in household dissolution. The institutionalized population may have even higher rates of nonresponse than the communitydwelling population because they are more difficult to locate or because of resistance from the institution or caregivers (Rodgers and Herzog, 1992). In addition to individual characteristics that may influence a person's propensity to continue responding to the survey, there are also certain survey attributes that may enhance the chances that a sample person will respond. The number of interviews a person is requested to complete may be related to respondent burden and thus to survey response (Kalton, Kasprzyk, and McMillen, 1989); however, comparisons of response rates among surveys with differences in the total number of interviews conducted suggest that frequency of interviews does not affect rates of nonresponse (Lillard, 1989). Nonresponse does accumulate over time; however, with a pattern of decline in response rate at the first follow-up interview, then a leveling off (e.g., Hill, 1992; Lillard, 1989; Streib, 1982). This initial flux might be caused by the weeding out of "not-interesteds." While we have used the literature to hypothesize the mechanisms through which response is affected, it is not clear from previous studies that these hypotheses are consistently supported by existing empirical work. Even in surveys of similar populations, the literature in this area is replete with conflicting findings which we would generally attribute to the lack of multivariate analyses or the omission of important variables. For instance, men are sometimes found to be more likely to be nonrespondents (Jay et al., 1993; Kalton et al., 1990; Streib, 1982), although sex has also been found to be insignificant (Adams et al., 1990). It is possible that whether or not a sex difference appears is related to whether or not living arrangements, marital status, and health are controlled. All of these characteristics are Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 percent at each interview, accumulating at about 44 percent in the twentieth wave (Hill, 1992). In their comparison of the characteristics and methodologies of several surveys related to health among older persons, Corder and Manton (1991) report the relative level of nonresponse in four longitudinal studies. Low nonresponse rates of about 4 percent are reported for persons selected on disability among those who had previously completed a screening interview for the initial 1982 interview of the National Long Term Care Survey (NLTCS). Among those eligible for reinterview seven years later, only 6 percent were lost to follow-up. On the other hand, in the NHANESI follow-up, which required a physical exam, nonresponse was a high 25 percent. According to Corder and Manton (1991), among surveys of the older population on health topics both the Longitudinal Study of Aging (LSOA) and the National Nursing Home Survey fall within the moderate range on nonresponse. While the new survey of Asset and Health Dynamics Among the Oldest Old (AHEAD) does not yet have a longitudinal component, 20 percent of the screened sample was not interviewed on the first wave (Hurd et al., 1994). These studies differ in their objectives, sampling strategies, observation plans, and methodologies, as well as in their levels of follow-up nonresponse; the issue of assessing the effect of panel nonresponse is, however, common to all of them. AN ANALYSIS OF NON-RESPONSE Correcting for Nonresponse If loss to follow-up occurs nonrandomly, that is, differentially selecting persons with certain characteristics out of the sample, attention must be given to correcting potential bias. Weighting is perhaps the most often used approach to unit nonresponse adjustment, although other techniques, such as imputation, are also used. One of the more commonly used methods for constructing weights is the application of response probability weights (Lepkowski, Kalton, and Kasprzyk, 1989; Rowland and Forthofer, 1993). Individual weights are constructed from the results of multivariate regressions predicting probability of nonresponse, and these weights are as unique as the characteristics predicting nonresponse (Iannacchione, Milne, and Folsom, 1991). The weights can be applied to correct descriptive univariate or bivariate statistics, including prevalence and transition rates, for a sample with nonrandom nonresponse. With a perfectly specified multivariate analysis in which all characteristics contributing to nonresponse are controlled, bias would be eliminated. Since perfect specification is not possible, one way to determine how much difference an adjustment for nonresponse will make is by making the adjustment using the best specified model possible and then comparing corrected and uncorrected analyses. While this is a common method for compensating for nonresponse, it assumes that those who are lost to follow-up are similar to continuing respondents on unobserved characteristics. If there are unobserved characteristics affecting both nonresponse and outcomes of interest, correcting only for observed characteristics will not eliminate the potential bias. Jointly estimating the probability of nonresponse and the substantive outcome of interest offers a way to correct for unobserved characteristics (Hoem, 1985; Tin, 1995; Van den Berg, Lindeboom, and Ridder, 1994). While most studies of attrition in panel surveys find nonrandom loss, several evaluations have found that correction has not altered the interpretation of the results (for example, Goudy, 1985; Kalton, Kasprzyk, and McMillen, 1989; Norris, 1987; Rowland and Forthofer, 1993). On the other hand, other studies have found that adjusting for attrition significantly changes some substantive findings (Hausman and Wise, 1979; Markides, Timbers, and Osberg, 1984). The reason for this is that whether or not nonrandom nonresponse translates into a biased sample depends on the relationship of the outcome of interest to nonresponse, how well-specified the model of nonresponse is, the size of the difference between respondents and nonrespondents, and the extent of the loss (Braver and Bay, 1992). The importance of sample attrition necessarily continues to be emphasized in virtually all analyses of longitudinal data (Corder, Woodbury, and Manton, 1992; Hsiao, 1986) because it is not possible to estimate whether or not a correction will matter until it is applied (Berk, 1983). METHODS Sample. — The Longitudinal Study of Aging (LSOA) is a four-wave panel survey that was designed to represent the community-dwelling population 70 years of age and older in the United States at the initial interview in 1984. The original LSOA interview took place among those eligible who responded to the 1984 National Health Interview Survey (NHIS). Among surveyed households, 96.4 percent responded to the NHIS (Ries, 1986), and 96.7 percent of these responded to the Supplement on Aging, the baseline interview for the LSOA (Kovar, Fitti, and Chyba, 1992). Three follow-up LSOA interviews are conducted at twoyear intervals, with the last interview occurring in 1990. When a sample member is unable to respond because of a physical or mental health problem, proxy responses are accepted. While the original interview was face-to-face, the follow-up interviews were done primarily by phone, using a mail interview where a phone interview could not be completed. Additional details of the survey design and interview protocol are available in Kovar, Fitti, and Chyba (1992). Data from the National Death Index (NDI) appended to the LSOA survey files provide confirmation and date of death. A sample person is assumed to be dead if a contact person reported that person as deceased or if that person has a "good match" with the National Death Index (Kovar, Fitti, and Chyba, 1992) and did not respond to any surveys subsequent to the date of death; sample persons are otherwise assumed to be alive. We make this assumption because evaluations of the accuracy of the National Death Index using match strategies comparable to that used by the LSOA indicate its sensitivity (correctly identifying persons known to be deceased) is approximately 97 percent and its specificity (not identifying those known to be alive as dead) is about 99 percent (Rich-Edwards, Corsano, and Stampfer, 1994; Williams, Demitrack, and Fries, 1992). Nevertheless, it is possible that a few persons we assume to be alive and nonrespondent could have died. This is especially true in the final year because of a considerable lag-time recording deaths in the NDI (Rich-Edwards, Corsano, and Stampfer, 1994), though this error is further minimized because we do not rely solely on the NDI data, but have contact persons' reports as well. Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 known to be quite different among older men and women. The literature is also ambiguous about the effect of household and family composition on nonresponse. Not being married is sometimes associated with nonresponse (Speare, Avery, and Lawton, 1991) and sometimes not (Streib, 1982). Living arrangements and marital status have both been linked to health. Streib (1982) controlled for health, whereas Speare, Avery, and Lawton (1991) did not. The response rate among African Americans is sometimes lower than that among others (Foley et al., 1990), but sometimes not (Streib, 1982). Perhaps race is confounded with socioeconomic status and health. Streib controls for these characteristics; Foley apparently does not. Analyses of nonresponse in panels of older persons have often unnecessarily focused on a limited range of characteristics, neglected to account for the possibility that individuals' characteristics may change over time, or ignored the contribution of unobserved characteristics. The consequences of these shortcomings are the presentation of spurious relationships, time-inappropriate linking of characteristics and outcomes, and a failure to fully account for nonresponse bias. S39 MIHEL1C AND CRIMMINS S40 Analysis. — This section begins with a description of how nonresponse and death contribute to the decrement of the sample over time. Both aggregate outcomes for the entire sample and individual patterns of response for the four waves are presented. Next, we turn to a multivariate analysis relating nonresponse to observed characteristics. Based on this analysis, we proceed with an application of a weighting approach to compensate for this loss and examine the effect on distributions and bivariate relationships. We conclude with an approach to multivariate analysis that allows correction for both observed and unobserved characteristics affecting nonresponse. RESULTS Multivariate Analysis of Loss to Follow-up To determine whether there are differences between the 20 percent of the sample that does eventually become lost to follow-up and continuing respondents, we use a multivariate analysis predicting nonresponse over the four waves of the LSOA. The outcome variable of interest is the first instance of nonresponse recorded for a panel member for any reason other than death. Persons who do not respond because they died are removed from the analysis for that and subsequent interviews. If the dead are retained and death is modeled as a competing risk, the estimated coefficients and standard errors for the relative risk of nonresponse are virtually identical to those presented here. We do not explicitly model death in this analysis, however, because this source of sample decrement is not an' 'error'' to be corrected for. Loss to death is an important part of the sample dynamic, however, and it should be taken into account in any substantive analysis. Nonresponse after the first occurrence is not considered in the equation because persons' characteristics at the beginning of those intervals are missing. In our analysis the event Table 1. Sample Nonresponse in Four Waves of the LSOA Wave 1 1984 n Wave 3 1988 Wave 2 1986 % n % n Wave 4 1990 % n % 2,676 1,857 618 5,151 52.0 36.1 12.0 100.0 1 A. Frequency and Percentages of Persons by Interview Status at Each Wave Interviewed Lost to death Nonresponse Total 5,151 — — 5,151 100.0 — — 100.0 4,114 645 392 5,151 79.9 12.5 7.7 100.0 3,314 1,291 546 5,151 64.3 25.0 10.5 100.0 IB. Percent of Those Alive on Previous Wave Not Interviewed on Next Wave Because of Death or Nonresponse Lost to death Nonresponse 12.5 7.6 14.3 12.1 14.7 16.0 14.1 18.8 1C. Percent of Those Alivf : Who Are Nonrespondents 8.7 ID. Percent of Those Alive Who Become Nonrespondents for the First Time Among Those Who Were Not Previously Nonrespondents 8.7 9.7 11.1 Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Description of Sample Dynamics The number of respondents diminished at each successive wave (Table 1A). Of the original 5,151 respondents, 80 percent were interviewed in 1986, 64 percent in 1988, and just over half of the sample was interviewed at the last wave in 1990. Three-quarters of this loss is due to death. By the second wave 12 percent of the original sample had died; by the fourth wave more than one-third was dead. Interestingly, even in this older sample, the percentage who died between interviews among those known to be alive at the preceding interview was fairly constant across the three intervals (Table IB). Between the first and second interviews 12 percent of the sample had died; between the third and fourth interviews about 15 percent had died. The percentage of the original sample not responding to the survey increased with each wave. Among those not known to be dead at each wave (Table 1C), the likelihood of nonresponse increased from almost 9 percent at the second wave to 19 percent at the fourth wave. The percentage becoming new nonrespondents (Table ID) does not increase nearly as dramatically, indicating that the likelihood of not responding to future waves is much higher for those who are earlier nonrespondents. This pattern of relatively low rates of increase in new nonresponse over time that accumulate to overall larger rates of total nonresponse has been observed by others (Kalton, Kasprzyk, and McMillen, 1989). The frequency of the observed three-interval pattern of response for individuals presented in Table 2 gives a fuller picture of how sample nonresponse affects our ability to trace changes in individuals' characteristics across interviews. For instance, "III" or an interview at each of the three follow-up waves, is the most common pattern of response. Eighty percent of the original respondents are interviewed at every wave before death. Another 10 percent of the sample is only missing at one interview and assumed to be still alive. Only 4 percent of the sample are never interviewed again after the initial interview, and one-third of these are dead before the fourth wave. Thus, even with this level of sample attrition, the changes in individual characteristics can be traced for most living members of the sample. S41 AN ANALYSIS OF NON-RESPONSE Table 2. Response Patterns for Three Waves of Follow-up Pattern Frequency Percent Description 2,431 47.2 IID ID. D.. 442 597 645 8.6 11.6 12.5 UN INI Nil 302 134 70 5.9 2.6 1.4 INN NIN NNI 128 47 41 2.5 0.9 0.8 IND NID 80 22 1.6 0.4 2.0% One more interview, dead ND. NND 50 21 1.0 0.4 1.4% Never interviewed again, dead 141 2.7 2.7% Never interviewed again, presumed alive 5,151 100.1 III 32.7% Interviewed until death 9.9% Two more interviews, still alive 4.2% One more interview, still alive 100.1 Note: I = Interviewed; N = Nonresponse; D = Dead. Period (ID.) = Previously deceased. of first nonresponse can occur at any one of the three interviews after the initial interview. A person-interval logistic regression is modeled; the log odds for the conditional probabilities of the event of first-time nonresponse occurring are estimated using SAS PROC LOGISTIC (SAS Institute, 1989). To enable us to take advantage of fresh information collected at each interview, a pooled data set is created in which each observation gets a unique record for each interval (n = 10,756) and characteristics at the beginning of the interval are related to response status at the end of the interval. This approach assumes that the error terms for each observation (in this case each person-interval) are not correlated. To evaluate the sensitivity of the results to this assumption, regression results from a generalized estimating equation (GEE), which treats the correlation across an individual's observations as a nuisance parameter, were compared with the results presented here (Groemping, 1994; Liang and Zeger, 1986; Lipsitz, Kim, and Zhao, 1994). There were no differences in patterns of significance or size of coefficients with and without the autocorrelation controlled. Our model of loss to follow-up includes characteristics that should be related to refusal to participate in the survey, inability to participate, and inability to be located. We hypothesize that being older, male, Black, having fewer years of education, not owning a home, currently residing in an institution, and reporting a greater number of functioning limitations will be associated with higher odds of nonresponse. An indicator for having another sample person in the household indicates family involvement in the survey. Our expectation is that having another family member in the survey will increase participation. An indicator of whether the follow-up where loss occurs is the first, second, or third wave after baseline is also included. This allows us to evaluate whether the rate of first-time nonresponse increases at successive interviews independent of changes in other characteristics. The record for each person-interval contains two kinds of independent variables. The first are those variables which typically remain constant over adult life: sex, race, and years of education. The second type of variable was collected or can be inferred at each interview: age, living arrangement, whether the respondent lives in an institution, home ownership, the presence of another sample member, and functioning ability. The operational definitions of most of the variables are straightforward. Functioning ability is operationalized as the number of 13 ADL and IADL functions a respondent reports having difficulty with (bathing, dressing, eating, transferring, walking, getting outside, toileting, shopping, preparing meals, using the telephone, managing money, doing light housework, and doing heavy housework). While this approach makes the best use of the repeated measures that are available, a kind of measurement error may still arise because the interview at which these characteristics are measured occurs at least two years prior to the nonresponse, and the characteristics could change in that interval. Results. — Table 3 compares the full sample at baseline, those who respond to each interview, those who live but become nonrespondents, and those who die. The bivariate data in this table suggest that those lost to nonresponse are more likely to be female, Black, older, have lower education, live alone, not own a home, or have more functioning problems than those who are interviewed at each opportunity. Death selects those who are male as well as those who have more functioning problems. While Table 3 gives a picture of the bivariate relationships, the effect of these variables when other characteristics are controlled is shown in a series of logistic regression models (Table 4). Four nested models were estimated so that the effects of successively including groups of predictor variables covering different types of hypothesized effects could be compared. Model 1 includes demographic charac- Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 NNN 47.2% Completed all interviews M1HELIC AND CRMM1NS S42 Table 3. Comparison of Characteristics at Baseline for the Total Original Sample, for Those Actually Interviewed at Each Wave, for Those Who Become Nonrespondents at One or More Waves But Are Eligible To Be Interviewed, and for Those Who Become Deceased During the Course of the Survey Full Respondents n = 2,431 64% 11% 37% 76% 67% 11% 37% 80% 72% 13% 44% 67% 56% 9% 35% 75% 60% 13% 2% 1% 36% 78.21 9.76 1.62 71% 12% 1% 0% 37% 76.56 10.27 0.90 62% 14% 2% 0% 35% 77.88 9.16 1.20 44% 13% 4% 2% 36% 80.52 9.35 2.76 teristics which are hypothesized to be related to the likelihood of refusal. Model 2 adds characteristics of living arrangements which we expect to relate to loss of contact. Model 3 adds an indicator of functioning which should be related to ability to respond, and Model 4 includes characteristics of the survey. This approach should clarify why some studies in the literature find that certain variables are related to nonresponse while others do not. The odds ratios and the log-odds ratios indicating the probability of being a first-time nonrespondent relative to being a continuing respondent are reported in Table 4. When only the demographic variables are included in the model, we find that women are more likely to be nonrespondents than men, holding race, age, and education constant (Model 1). When living arrangements are controlled, women are no more likely than men to become nonrespondents (Model 2). Thus, women do not appear to be refusers but to become lost because of gender differences in living arrangements; persons who live alone are more likely to become nonrespondents. Blacks are no more likely than non-Blacks to become nonrespondents when education is controlled. Years of education remains significant, and living alone becomes significant when level of functioning is controlled (Model 3). Persons who are better educated may find themselves more interested in the survey itself, or they may have more of an understanding of the potential usefulness of the survey; it is also possible that they are less intimidated by the interviewer. As hypothesized earlier, persons who live alone may be less likely to leave someone behind who knows their whereabouts when the interviewer is not able to locate the person; and renters, being more likely to move, have a greater chance of being lost to follow-up than those who own their home (Models 3 and 4). Although it may be difficult to follow people into nursing homes from the community, those persons who have completed an interview in an institution are no more likely to become nonrespondents than those living in the community. Those Who Become Deceased Between 19841990 Interviews n = 1,857 Both number of functioning problems and age are consistent predictors of nonresponse. Age could be reflecting unmeasured health dimensions that result in inability to respond. Or, in this sample of persons over 70 years of age, the older cohort may be more averse to survey participation or contact with strangers. The chance of institutionalization increases with age, and persons moving into institutions may be disproportionately lost to follow-up, either because they cannot be located, because they are unable to respond, or because a proxy or the institution refuses in their behalf. Having another sample person in the household is strongly related to nonresponse. This could mean that having another sample person in the house increases the burden of response so that both members do not feel a need to participate. They may feel that one person telling the "family story" is sufficient. However, in interpreting the effect of another household member in the survey, the reference category must be clarified. When living alone is included along with having a sample person in the household, the omitted reference group becomes living with a nonsample person. Living with another sample person and living alone are associated with about the same increase in relative risk of nonresponse compared with persons who are living with other nonsample persons. Another sample person must be older because the sample is limited to those 70 and over. Those living with nonsample persons would generally be living with someone younger, who was not eligible for the survey. Thus, the effect of a sample member in the household could really be the effect of an older household context versus a younger household context. In that case, the relationship between nonresponse and having another sample member in the house could be caused by a greater inability to locate older households. The coefficients of the interval variables indicate whether (and how) the odds of becoming a nonrespondent differ at the second and third intervals with reference to the first (Model 4). The significance of the dummy variable for the third interval indicates that those persons responding at the Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Female Black Lives alone Owns home Functioning difficulties None One Six Thirteen Other sample person Mean age Mean years of education Mean number of functioning difficulties Original Sample n = 5,151 Alive 1984-1990 but Nonrespondent at One or More Interviews n = 863 S43 AN ANALYSIS OF NON-RESPONSE Table 4. Regression Coefficients and Odds Ratios for Predictors of Nonresponse (standard errors in parentheses) Model 2 Model 1 Intercept Female Black Age Years education Log Odds Odds Ratio Log Odds Odds Ratio Log Odds Odds Ratio Log Odds Odds Ratio ^t.186** (0.495) 0.175* (0.075) 0.058 (0.109) 0.029** (0.006) -0.056** (0.009) 0.01 -3.648** (0.503) 0.086 (0.079) 0.034 (0.110) 0.026** (0.006) -0.054** (0.010) 0.122 (0.075) 0.290 (0.270) -0.502** (0.075) 0.03 -3.308** (0.513) 0.056 (0.080) 0.007 (0.110) 0.021** (0.006) -0.050** (0.010) 0.154* (0.076) 0.202 (0.271) -0.488** (0.075) 0.039** (0.012) 0.04 -3.375** (0.525) 0.064 (0.080) 0.018 (0.110) 0.019* (0.006) -0.055** (0.010) 0.321** (0.092) 0.2376 (0.274) -0.502** (0.075) 0.041** (0.012) 0.316** (0.094) 0.026 (0.083) 0.220* (0.087) 0.03 1.19 1.06 1.03 0.94 Lives in institution Owns home No. limitations 1.09 1.03 1.03 0.95 1.13 1.34 0.60 Other sample person in household Interval 2 Interval 3 Chi-square df p-value 6191.64 covariates 76.626 4 .0001 6139.79 m2 v ml 51.853 3 .0001 6129.62 m3 v m2 10.175 1 .01 1.06 1.01 1.02 0.95 1.17 1.22 0.61 1.04 1.07 1.02 1.02 0.95 1.38 1.27 0.60 1.04 1.37 1.03 1.25 6112.06 m4 v m3 17.559 3 .001 Response Categories: 1 = 935 (nonresponse), 0 = 9,360 (response) *p< .05;**p< .001. third interview after baseline were more likely to be lost than respondents to the first interview after baseline. There was no significant difference in the likelihood of loss between the first and second interviews after baseline. As noted earlier, the LSOA has a complex sample design. To examine the sensitivity of these results to the violation of the independence of observations assumption, we again employ GEE (Groemping, 1994), now correcting the standard errors for correlation within the sampling clusters. We find our results unchanged. The probability of nonresponse. — Having identified which characteristics are associated with the relative risk of nonresponse in this sample, the next question is: How does variation in these characteristics affect the absolute probability of becoming a nonrespondent? To explore this question, in Table 5 the mean probability of becoming a nonrespondent is estimated for selected categories of each of the significant predictor variables using the results of Model 4 from Table 4. These probabilities provide useful information on the absolute size of the effect of certain characteristics in this sample. For example, if our entire sample were age 70 and each individual retained all other observed characteristics, the mean probability of becoming a first-time nonrespondent would be 7.7 percent. If the whole sample were 90 years old, the probability of becoming a nonrespondent would increase approximately 3 percentage points to 10.8 percent. Similarly, having another sample person in the household, regardless of whether that person chose to respond or not, results in an increase of 3 percent in the probability of nonresponse to the subsequent interview. If our entire sample had difficulty with all 13 ADLs and IADLs, the mean probability of nonresponse would increase from .0849 (having no difficulty with any functions) to . 1695. This is the largest contrast among the characteristics listed. Of course, any given person would have some combination of these characteristics. In this sample, the highest probability of becoming a nonrespondent (.32428) occurred in the final interval for a 95-year-old Black woman who reported no formal education, was living alone at the time of the third interview; she was not institutionalized, but neither was she a homeowner. She reported having difficulty with 5 of 13 possible ADL and IADL tasks. The lowest observed probability, .02867, occurred in the first interval for a 70year-old White man with 18 years of education, living with his spouse at that first interview in 1984 in a home he owned, and reporting no difficulty with any ADL or IADL. The Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Lives alone -2 Log L Model 4 Model 3 MIHELIC AND CRIMM1NS S44 Table 5. Predicted Mean Probability of Becoming a Nonrespondent for Specific Values of Significant Independent Variables With All Other Characteristics as Observed Variable Value Age Age 70 Age 90 Mean Probability .0769 .1083 Standard Error .0003 .0004 Table 6. Comparison of Means, Standard Deviations, and Sample Sizes for Number of ADLs with Difficulty and Number of IADLs with Difficulty at Each Wave of Follow-up with LSOA Sample Weights and With Weights Constructed To Correct for Probability of Nonresponse Number of ADLs with Difficulty LSOA Weights Nonresponse Weights Number of IADLs with Difficulty LSOA Weights Nonresponse Weights .0982 .0660 .0003 .0002 Living Arrangement Lives with others Lives alone Mean = 1.09 Mean = 1.10 Mean = 1.07 Mean = 1.08 = 1.86 SD = 1.67 SD = 1.93 SD 1986 SD = 1.67 N = 4146 N = 4458 N = 4054 N = 4358 .0804 .1070 .0003 .0004 Tenure Type Does not own home Owns home Mean = 1.19 Mean = 1.22 Mean = 1.04 Mean = 1.07 1988 SD = 1.98 SD = 1.66 SD = 2.16 SD = 1.81 N = 3294 N = 3812 N = 3785 = 3271 N .1242 .0795 .0004 .0002 Functioning Problems (ADL/IADL) No difficulties Difficulty with 6 functions Difficulty with 13 functions Mean = 1.29 Mean = 1.34 Mean = 1.09 Mean = 1.13 1990 SD = 2.08 SD = 1.74 SD = 2.38 SD = 1.99 N = 2562 N = 2542 N = 3235 N = 3208 .0845 .1051 .1667 .0003 .0004 .0007 Other Sample Person in Household No Yes .0835 .1104 .0004 .0005 Respondent at 3rd Wave No Yes .0860 .1046 .0003 .0004 median probability for the sample was .08220. Several people had this precise probability of becoming a nonrespondent. They were 78-year-old non-Black women with eight years of education, living alone in their own homes, and reporting no difficulty in ADLs or IADLs. Each had responded to the first two interviews. Compensating for Nonresponse Actual probabilities of nonresponse are calculated for each individual responding in 1984, 1986, and 1988 using the significant coefficients from Model 4, Table 4. These probabilities are then used to construct weight adjustment factors to compensate for any bias in the remaining sample due to nonrandom nonresponse over the three survey intervals in the LSOA. The effect of using the adjusted weights is illustrated by comparing results using only the weights provided with the survey to adjust for the sampling strategy with the weights that compensate for sample loss as well as the sample design. Constructing weights. — Because the LSOA is a multistage cluster sample, the LSOA sample weight is provided to adjust for baseline nonresponse to the SOA and the subsampling of certain populations. The weight adjusts the baseline sample so that it is representative of the noninstitutionalized population that is 70 years of age and over in 1984 by sex, race, and age. We use this sample design weight for the baseline data and then modify it cumulatively at each successive wave to compensate for loss to follow-up. The weights for dates after 1984 are calculated by multiplying the weight for the previous date by (1 + the probability of nonresponse). When these weights are applied, the contribution of those persons who actually respond is inflated to also represent those who did not respond, based on characteristics included in the computation of the probabilities. Thus, the weights alter the distribution of the characteristics in the sample to reflect the greater propensity of persons with certain characteristics to become nonrespondents. Effect of weighting for nonresponse. — Table 6 compares the mean number of ADLs and IADLs in 1986, 1988, and 1990 calculated in two ways: weighting only with the normalized LSOA weight and weighting for loss to follow-up. Using the LSOA weight, by 1990 the sample responding to ADL questions had diminished to 2,562. However, the nonresponse weights increase the weighted sample size of those responding to this question by 673 persons, making the weighted sample just 19 shy of what the total sample would have been in 1990 if the only source of nonresponse had been death. (The difference of 19 is due to item nonresponse to this question.) Since those in poor physical functioning are less likely to respond, the use of nonresponse weights results in slight increases in all the means. The size of the difference gets larger in later years as there is more loss to follow-up and because the loss has been accumulating. However, the differences between the two estimates for any one year are statistically and substantively insignificant. Examining sample change in functioning over the 1984 to 1990 period, the increase in the average number of ADLs would be .24 with adjusted weights and .20 without. For IADLs the increase would be .05 versus .02. While greater, the difference is not significant. Note that these tests of significance are performed using the actual, not the weighted, sample size. Table 7 shows the differences resulting from the two sets of weights in the estimated percentage making a two-year transition from no difficulty performing an ADL or IADL to Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Education 8 years school 16 years school AN ANALYSIS OF NON-RESPONSE Table 7. Percent of Those Having No Difficulty at a Given Wave But Having Difficulty With One or More ADL or IADL at the Next Wave Nonresponse Weights 34.44% 1008 26.04% 451 28.22% 386 34.52% 1080 26.29% 520 28.15% 466 Persons 70-84 Years Old in 1984 1 9 8 4 ^ 1986 32.97% 908 1 9 8 6 ^ 1988 25.31% 424 1988-^ 1990 27.63% 368 33.02% 971 25.53% 488 27.54% 444 Persons 85 Years and Older in 1984 1 9 8 4 ^ 1986 57.86% 100 1986 -» 1988 47.78% 27 1988 ^ 1990 50.79% 18 57.8 110 47.94% 32 49.49% 23 Full Sample 1984 -» 1986 1986-> 1988 1 9 8 8 ^ 1990 Table 8. Parameter Estimates for Incident Disability, With and Without Correcting for Selection Bias From Loss to Follow-up (standard errors in parentheses) Variable difficulty with at least one function. Using these weights to adjust for nonresponse makes little difference in either sample means or proportions making transitions even among the oldest persons in the sample, who are presumably both more frail and more likely to become lost to follow-up. Correcting/or nonresponse with a bivariate probit model. — Most researchers ultimately use multivariate analyses, however, and the weighting method proposed above is not typically used in that context. A bivariate probit with selection (Greene, 1992) allows us to simultaneously estimate the probability of nonresponse and the probability of making a transition of substantive interest, in this case, incident ADL disability. Comparing the results of this simultaneously estimated model with those from a univariate probit indicates the effect of not taking attrition into account. Furthermore, we are able to determine whether results are biased by some characteristics we have not observed, such as distaste for survey response, cognitive impairment, or circumstances associated with moving into a nursing home. The correlation between the error terms for the two equations (rho), representing the unobserved influences common to both the process of disability and attrition, is included in the estimation. (For details of the method see Greene [1993] and for another application see Defo [1992].) The results of the bivariate probit are presented in Table 8 along with probit results for the two equations estimated separately. Note that the dependent variable for the selection equation is the same as the nonresponse outcome from Table 4, but the coding is inverted (1 = respondent) so the signs are opposite. The data are again pooled for a person-interval analysis. But, since the outcome of interest is the first report Univariate Probits Dependent Variable: Disability Constant -3.835** (0.285) Female -0.009 (0.038) Black 0.070 (0.062) Years of age 0.039** (0.003) Years of education -0.020** (0.005) Under poverty threshold 0.089 (0.056) Missing poverty indicator 0.003 (0.051) History of heart disease 0.256** (0.053) History of cancer 0.016 (0.058) History of stroke / cardiovascular 0.385** (0.095) Diabetes 0.353** (0.067) Arthritis 0.378** (0.037) Osteoporosis 0.221* (0.107) Difficulty with hearing 0.089 (0.095) Difficulty with vision 0.609** (0.092) Very low weight 0.099 (0.068) Very high weight 0.205* (0.063) Rho Log likelihood N = 6,090 Dependent Variable: Response Constant Female Black Years of age Years of education Lives alone Lives in institution Owns home Other sample person in household Interval 2 Interval 3 Log likelihood N = 6,628 Bivariate Probit With Selection -3.82** -0.011 0.071 0.038** -0.019* 0.087 0.004 0.255** 0.017 (0.288) (0.039) (0.060) (0.004) (0.007) (0.055) (0.052) (0.053) (0.059) 0.385** 0.352** 0.378** 0.220* 0.089 0.608** 0.098 0.205* 0.107 (0.091) (0.067) (0.037) (0.109) (0.094) (0.091) (0.068) (0.062) (0.484) 1.873** -0.067 0.040 -0.012* 0.039** -0.083 -0.990 0.309** (0.349) (0.049) (0.079) (0.004) (0.007) (Q.059) (0.811) (0.053) -3213.7 Status 1.869** -0.067 0.038 -0.012* 0.039** -0.086 -0.960 0.308** (0.355) (0.050) (0.078) (0.004) (0.006) (0.061) (0.760) (0.052) -0.101 (0.060) 0.017 (0.054) -0.050 (0.058) -1815.4 -0.099 (0.060) 0.022 (0.054) -0.044 (0.059) -5029.1 *p< .05;**p< .001. of ADL disability, the sample is selected at the beginning of each interval for those with no ADL disability. This reduces the number of observations to 6,597 and accounts for the now nonsignificant effect of living alone. Rho itself is not significant, and there are only very small differences in the estimates and levels of significance when rho is controlled, suggesting minimal bias in these estimates due to loss to follow-up. DISCUSSION Our analysis has demonstrated the dramatic sample loss in a panel survey of older persons. Decrement to the sample occurs through two distinct sources: loss to follow-up and death. Clearly, when a sample of older persons is followed for even a few years, there is tremendous loss to death. In this sample, over a third of the original population was lost to Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 LSOA Weights S45 S46 MIHELICAND CRIMMINS sample of older persons is not random. Characteristics associated with nonresponse may be used to develop targeting criteria for those interested in the retention of sample persons. Loss to follow-up could be lowered if survey organizations made special efforts to maintain contact with renters and those who live alone. In addition, extra effort might be devoted to retaining those in worse health and the oldest old. Those with less education might be converted to continuing response with extra educational effort. Although our results are consistent with our general typology of nonrespondents as people who refuse, are unable to respond, and who cannot be located, a useful elaboration of this analysis would consider predictors of nonresponse by reason for nonresponse. Indeed, if sufficient heterogeneity between types of nonresponse exists, the salience of the predictors considered here may have been obscured because we have grouped all types of nonresponse. Unfortunately, in this survey reasons for not-completed interviews were insufficiently differentiated for such an analysis. We cannot overemphasize the need to accurately record some detail about why interviews are not completed. It will not be possible to either improve response rates or more effectively evaluate the extent of nonresponse bias without this information. In order to evaluate whether this nonrandom nonresponse biases analytic results, we constructed response probability weights based on the characteristics observed to increase the probability of nonresponse. The approach to developing weights suggested here allows for optimal use of each sample member's contribution to the survey by estimating response probabilities within each interval and applying these to form a cumulative weight. Weighting on these characteristics restored the characteristics of this sample to what they would have been without the loss, but did not significantly affect univariate and bivariate distributions. Ideally, initial nonresponse (nonresponse to the baseline interview) could be included in this phase; in practice, information on the characteristics of these persons is frequently unavailable. In this study, partial compensation is made because the LSOA weights on which the nonresponse weights are based adjust the distributions of age, sex, and race to account for initial nonresponse to the NHIS and SOA, which comprise the baseline sample of the LSOA. Ultimately, we need to assess the effect of nonrandom nonresponse in a multivariate environment. We have presented a substantive analysis predicting incident disability and evaluated it for nonresponse bias with correction for selection by attrition. By including the correlation between the error terms of the two equations, this procedure also adjusts the coefficients for the unobserved characteristics affecting both processes. Thus, we can observe whether our original coefficients changed (i.e., whether they were biased), and also whether the unobserved characteristics themselves had a significant effect on incident disability. In this survey, for our outcome of interest, unobservables did not have a significant effect and did not bias our results. We find, then, that nonresponse in this sample of older persons is not random. Also, however, we find that this nonrandom nonresponse does not produce bias of any consequence. Though significantly associated with the outcome, our observed variables were not strongly enough correlated Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 death in six years. We set out to determine whether loss to follow-up in this sample is random; whether nonrandom loss significantly biases analytic results when evaluated with correction based on observed characteristics; and whether there are unobserved differences between those who become lost and continuing respondents, and whether those differences alter our conclusions. The LSOA has a significant amount of nonresponse, reaching almost 20 percent of those alive by the fourth wave. Those who are older, have lower educational attainment, live alone, have worse functioning, and rent their homes are more likely to become nonrespondents. We had hypothesized that certain characteristics previously found to be associated with nonresponse were acting as proxies for characteristics that were not included in those analyses. Among the variables previously identified as ambiguous in their relationship to attrition, we find that sex becomes insignificant when measures of living arrangement are included. And, once functioning ability is controlled, living alone emerges as a significant predictor of becoming a nonrespondent. We do not find any effect of race, a characteristic that several previous studies found to predict nonresponse. We had expected persons living in an institution to be more prone to loss to follow-up because they might be more difficult to locate, because of the possibility of resistance from the institution or caregivers, or because of increased frailty beyond what has been included in the model. Among persons who have been interviewed in an institution, nonresponse at subsequent interviews is not more likely than for community-dwelling persons. Although previous analysis finds that persons moving into institutions are disproportionately lost to follow-up (Crimmins, Hay ward, and Saito, 1994), we are not able to ascertain the extent to which this loss contributes to sample selection. Underrepresentation of the institutionalized population poses an important threat to the representativeness of older samples. However, while it may be especially difficult to follow people into an institution, once they have been successfully followed, they are not more likely to be nonrespondents. Frequently surveys collect information from multiple members of a single household so that studies involving household behavior may be completed. We find in this survey that having another sample person in the household increases the probability of nonresponse considerably, relative to living with other nonsample persons. Households with two sample persons are older, and as such are less likely to have job ties, and may more freely move for amenities, or social or environmental support. Those living with older respondents are also more likely to become widowed during the course of the survey, and this may be the catalyst for a move or a loss of interest in survey participation. Household response behavior is clearly an area in which further research would be useful. The lesson from this finding is not that survey designs including multiple sample members from a single household should be avoided, as this would result in a loss of valuable information. Rather, awareness that such sample persons may be more prone to loss to follow-up might call for special attention to retaining these sample members. These findings suggest that wave nonresponse in this AN ANALYSIS OF NON-RESPONSE While our analyses showed both nonrandom nonresponse and negligible nonresponse bias for the substantive outcome considered, there is no rule of thumb to ascertain whether bias exists for other outcomes or for other panel studies. The evaluation of the effects of the nonrandom nonresponse in this survey is a single case study, applicable for others undertaking analysis with the LSOA, and hopefully a useful demonstration of how to approach nonresponse evaluation for those using other panel data. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This project was supported by Grant RO3 HS08459 from the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research, and T32 AG0O237 and R01 AG11235 from the National Institute on Aging. Address correspondence to Dr. Adrienne Mihelic, Department of Population Dynamics, School of Hygiene and Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, 615 N. Wolfe St., Baltimore, MD 21205. E-mail: amihelic(Sihpcsun01 .sph.jhu.edu REFERENCES Adams, M.M.E., P.A. Scherr, L.G. Branch, L.E. Herbert, N.R. Cook, A.M. Lane, D.B. Brock, D.A. Evans, and J.O. Taylor. 1990. "A Comparison of Elderly Participants in a Community Survey with Nonparticipants." Public Health Reports 105:617-622. Berk, R.A. 1983. "An Introduction to Sample Selection Bias in Sociological Data." Sociological Methods and Research 3:386—396. Braver, S.L. and R.C. Bay. 1992. "Assessing and Compensating for SelfSelection Bias (Non-representativeness) of the Family Research Sample." Journal of Marriage and the Family 54:925-939. Chyba, M M . and J.E. Fitti. 1991. "Response Experience in a Longitudinal Study of Older Persons." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 135-140. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Corder, L.S. and K.G. Manton. 1991. "National Surveys and the Health and Functioning of the Elderly: The Effects of Design and Content." Journal of the American Statistical Association 86:513-525. Corder, L.S., M.A. Woodbury, and K.G. Manton. 1992. "Loss to Followup Assessment: Application of Grade of Membership Methods to Aged Population Longitudinal Sample Loss to Follow-up in the National Long Term Care Survey." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 357—361. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Crimmins, E.M., M.D. Hayward, and Y. Saito. 1994. "Changing Mortality and Morbidity Rates and the Health Status and Life Expectancy of the Older Population.'' Demography 31:159-175. Defo, B.K. 1992. "Mortality and Attrition Processes in Longitudinal Studies in Africa: An Appraisal of the Iford Surveys." Population Studies 46:327-348. Foley, D.J., L.G. Branch, J.H. Madans, D.B. Brock, J.M. Guralnik, and T. Franklin Williams. 1990. "Physical Function." In J.C. CornoniHuntley, R.R. Huntley, and J.J. Feldman (Eds.), Health Status and Well-being of the Elderly: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey-I Epidemiologic Follow-up Study. New York: Oxford University Press. Goudy, W.J. 1985. "Sample Attrition and Multivariate Analysis in the Retirement History Study." Journal of Gerontology 40:358-367. Greene, W.H. 1992. LIMDEP: User's Manual and Reference Guide, Version 6. New York: Econometric Software. Greene, W.H. 1993. Econometric Analysis. New York: Macmillan. Groemping, U. 1994. SAS Macrofor Longitudinal Data Analysis: Generalized Estimating Equation, Version 2.03. Fachbereich Statistik, Univeritaet Dortmund, Germany. Hausman, J.A. and D.A. Wise. 1979. "Attrition Bias in Experimental and Panel Data: The Gary Income Maintenance Experiment." Econometrica 47:455-473. Hill, M.S. 1992. The Panel Study of Income Dynamics: A User's Guide. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hoem, J. 1985. "Weighting, Misclassification, and Other Issues in the Analysis of Sample Surveys of Life Histories." In J. Heckman and B. Singer (Eds), Longitudinal Analysis of Labor Market Data. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Hsiao, C. 1986. Analysis of Panel Data. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Hurd, M , W. Rodgers, B. Soldo, and R. Wallace. 1994. Asset and Health Dynamics Among the Oldest Old: An Overview of the Survey. Paper presented at The AHEAD Early Results Workshop, Ann Arbor, MI. Iannacchione, V.G., J.G. Milne, and R.E. Folsom. 1991. "Response Probability Weight Adjustments Using Logistic Regression." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 637-642. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Jay, G.M., J. Liang, X. Liu, and H. Sugisawa. 1993. "Patterns of Nonresponse in a National Survey of Elderly Japanese." Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences 48:S143-S152. Kalton, G. 1990. Comment on "Theory and Issues in Weighting Survey Data." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 147149. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Kalton, G., D. Kasprzyk, and D.B. McMillen. 1989. "Nonsampling Errors in Panel Surveys." In D. Kasprzyk, G. Duncan, G. Kalton, and M.P. Singh (Eds.), Panel Surveys. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Kalton, G., J. Lepkowski, G.E. Montanari, and D. Maligalig. 1990. "Characteristics of Second Wave Nonrespondents in a Panel Survey." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 462-467. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Kasprzyk, D. and D.B. McMillen. 1987. "SIPP: Characteristics of the 1984 Panel." Proceedings of the Social Statistics Section: 181-186. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Kovar, M.G., J.E. Fitti, and M.M. Chyba. 1992. "The Longitudinal Study of Aging: 1984-1990." Vital and Health Statistics 16(28). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Lepkowski, J . M , G. Kalton, and D. Kasprzyk. 1989. "Weighting Adjustments for Partial Nonresponse in the 1984 SIPP Panel." Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods: 296—301. Washington, DC: American Statistical Association. Liang, K. and S.L. Zeger. 1986. "Longitudinal Data Analysis Using Generalized Linear Models." Biometrics 73(l):13-22. Lillard, L.A. 1989. "Sample Dynamics: Some Behavioral Issues." In D. Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 with the probability of nonresponse for the resulting weights to make much difference in the univariate and bivariate distributions. However, these weights did not compensate for differences between respondents and nonrespondents that were not observed. The second diagnostic/adjustment procedure we employ, the bivariate probit, would tell us if we had omitted any such characteristics, but the outcome of this procedure — the lack of significance of rho — suggests that we had not. This tells us that although nonrespondents were different from respondents on observed characteristics, they did not differ on unobserved characteristics. Another example of a similar analysis that finds significant differences on observed characteristics but no difference on those unobserved is presented by Whitehead, Groothuis, and Blomquist (1993). In that evaluation, however, adjusting for observed characteristics did change results. In this study, significant differences between our respondents and nonrespondents exist on observed variables only, but these differences are apparently not so large that adjusting for them alters our results. This is an important point, because it indicates that nonrandom nonresponse does not always fatally compromise the sample. An important caveat, however, is that although there is a close relationship between the result of compensating and the strength of the relationship between the predictive characteristics and the outcome (Braver and Bay, 1992; Miller and Wright, 1995), it is not possible to tell whether an adjustment will make a difference until it is tried (Berk, 1983). S47 S48 MIHELIC AND CR1MM1NS Effects in a Fourteen-year Study of Adult Intelligence." Journal of Gerontology 28:328-334. Siegler, I.C. and J. Botwinick. 1979. "A Long-term Longitudinal Study of Intellectual Ability of Older Adults: The Matter of Selective Subject Attrition." Journal of Gerontology 34:242-245. Speare, A.J., R. Avery, and L. Lawton. 1991. "Disability, Residential Mobility, and Changes in Living Arrangements." Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences 46:S133-S142. Streib, G.F. 1982. "Participants and Drop-outs in a Longitudinal Study." Journal of Gerontology 21:200-209. Tin, J.S. 1995. "A Model of Attrition and Income for Dynamic Longitudinal Surveys." Applied Economics 27:705-717. Van den Berg, G.J., M. Lindeboom, and G. Ridder. 1994. "Attrition in Longitudinal Panel Data and The Empirical Analysis of Dynamic Labour Market Behaviour." Journal of Applied Econometrics 9: 421-435. Williams, B.C., L.B. Demitrack, and B.E. Fries. 1992. "The Accuracy of the National Death Index When Personal Identifiers Other than Social Security Number Are Used." American Journal of Public Health 82:1145-1147. Whitehead, J.C., P.A. Groothuis, and G.C. Blomquist. 1993. "Testing for Nonresponse Bias in Contingent Valuation: Analysis of a Combination Phone/Mail Survey.'' Economics Letters 41:215-220. Wright, R.A. 1983. "Survey Design and Administration of the National Medical Care Utilization and Expenditure Survey." In Priorities in Health Statistics: Proceedings of the 19th National Meeting of the Public Health Conference on Records and Statistics (DHHS-PHS841214):I77-18O. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Received June 9, 1995 Accepted September 12, 1996 Downloaded from http://psychsocgerontology.oxfordjournals.org/ at INSERM on July 16, 2013 Kasprzyk, G. Duncan, G. Kalton, and M.P. Singh (Eds.), Panel Surveys. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Lipsitz, S.R., K. Kim, and L. Zhao. 1994. "Analysis of Repeated Categorical Data Using Generalized Estimating Equations.'' Statistics in Medicine 13:1149-1163. Manton, K.G. 1988. "A Longitudinal Study of Functional Changes and Mortality in the United States." Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences 43:S\52,-S16\. Markides, K., D. Timbers, and G. Osberg. 1984. "Aging and Health: A Longitudinal Study." Archives of Gerontological Geriatrics 3:33-49. Miller, R. C. and D.W. Wright. 1995. "Detecting and Correcting Attrition Bias in Longitudinal Family Research." Journal of Marriage and the Family 57:921-929. Norris, F.H. 1987. "Effects of Attrition on Relationships Between Variables in Surveys of Older Adults.'' Journal of Gerontology 42:597-605. Rich-Edwards, J.W., K.A. Corsano, and M.J. Stampfer. 1994. "Test of the National Death Index and Equifax Nationwide Death Search." American Journal of Epidemiology 140:1016-1019. Ries, P. 1986. "Current Estimates from the National Health Interview Survey, United States, 1984." Vital and Health Statistics 10(156). Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. Rodgers, W.L. and A.R. Herzog. 1992. "Collecting Data about the Oldest Old: Problems and Procedures." In R.M. Suzman, D.P. Willis, and K.G. Manton (Eds.), The Oldest Old. New York: Oxford University Press. Rowland, M.L. and R.N. Forthofer. 1993. "Adjusting for Nonresponse Bias in a Health Examination Survey." Public Health Reports 108:380-386. SAS Institute. 1989. SAS/STAT Users Guide, Version 6, Volume 2 (4th ed.). Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc. Schaie, K.W., G.V. Labouvie, and T.J. Barrett. 1973. "Selective Attrition
© Copyright 2024