Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use Pavica Sheldon A survey with 172 students was conducted at Louisiana State University to see what students’ motives are for using the Facebook, how individual differences relate to motives for Facebook use, and to what extent motives and individual differences can predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes of Facebook use. In light of the uses and gratification theory, the study found that people go to Facebook to fulfill needs traditionally fulfilled by other media but for their interpersonal communication needs first (relationship maintenance). Women were more likely to go to Facebook to maintain existing relationships, pass time and be entertained. On the other hand, men were more likely to go to Facebook to develop new relationships or meet new people. T The idea of a community accessible only via my computer screen sounded cold to me at first, but I learned quickly that people can feel passionately about e-mail and computer conferences. I’ ve become one of them. I care about these people I met through my computer… (Rheingold, 1993, p. 1) he range of communication media available to young people is rich, wide, and likely to continue to increase in the future. As the number and variety of media have increased across U.S. households (Louie, 2003), many questions about individuals’ media choice and use remain unanswered (Flanagin and Metzger, 2001). In 2006 about 88 percent of Americans age 12 through 29 went online. Digital Natives is the name of the group that has grown up with Internet technology. The Pew Internet Project (2006) found that the Internet’s major benefit is in helping people tap into social networks. One of these networks is Facebook, an Internet site created by Mark Zuckerberg, a Harvard undergraduate student, in February 2004. Facebook’s primary purpose, according its homepage, is to “share information with people you know, see what’s going on with your friends, and look up people around you.” (Facebook.com, 2007) As college students spend more time online than any generation before, it is important to know what gratifications they seek and obtain from the new media. As LaRose and Eastin (2004) suggested, the definition of Internet usage is too broad. So research in this area should distinguish Internet application (e.g., e-mail vs. online chat), functions or settings. Similarly, Papacharissi and Rubin Pavica Sheldon, a doctoral student at Louisiana State University’s Manship School of Mass Communication, won third place with this manuscript at the Southwest Symposium of the Southwest Education Council for Journalism and Mass Communication in October 2007. Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 39 (2001) suggested that, with the widespread use of CMC, we need a better understanding of personal and social attributes that predict why people use computermediated communication (CMC) and the outcomes of CMC-related behavior. CMC “blurs” traditional boundaries between interpersonal and mass communication, thus offering new opportunities for the way individuals relate to one another (Parks and Floyd, 1996). Although some educational institutions have raised their voices against Facebook, claiming that students may be addicted to the site and spend too much time there, there is little research on who uses the site, what brings people to Facebook, and what the outcome of their social networking is. What are user motives (gratifications sought) for using Facebook, how do individual differences (age, gender, education) relate to motives for Facebook use, and to what extent can motives and individual differences predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes of Facebook use? These are the questions this study is designed to answer. Uses and Gratifications Theory. Newhagen and Rafaeli (1996) suggested that uses and gratification (U&G) theory might well be suited to study the Internet. The theory explains how different people use the same media messages for different purposes to satisfy their psychological and social needs and achieve their goals (Katz, 1959). According to uses and gratifications theory, audiences differ in the gratifications they seek from the mass media. What needs and gratifications people are looking for can be grouped into the following categories: diversion (escape from problems; emotional release), personal relationship (social utility of information in conversation; substitute of the media for companionship), personal identity (value reinforcement, self-understanding), and surveillance (McQuail, Blumler, and Brown, 1972). Later, researchers added a few more categories. Generally, U&G theory focuses on motives for media use, factors that influence motives, and outcomes from media-related behavior. Many studies conducted after 1972 showed that different motives are linked to different media preferences, leading to different patterns of media exposure and use to different outcomes (Haridakis and Rubin, 2003). Ruggiero (2000) writes that new media like the Internet possess at least three attributes not commonly associated with traditional media: interactivity, demassification, and asynchroneity. Dicken-Garcia (1998) said that the Internet places stronger emphasis on interpersonal conversations than has been true of earlier media. Users say electronically what they might never say in person, sometimes taking on new personalities, ages, and genders. It more resembles word of mouth than the sort of communication one usually sees in newspapers and television (Dicken-Garcia, 1998). That is, many researchers see the Internet as a continuum between mass and interpersonal communication (Ruggiero, 2000). Uses and gratification theory has been criticized for focusing too narrowly on the individual (Elliot, 1974) and not explaining why people use a certain medium, or how a certain gratification is provided by using the medium. Many central concepts such as audience needs, gratifications, and motives remain unclear (Swanson, 1977). Scholars respond to those criticisms and are now measuring the distinction between gratifications sought and gratifications obtained. Some uses and gratifications studies have explored the relationship between gratifications 40 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 sought and gratifications obtained (GS and GO) and media choice, extending the research from description of gratifications to tests of the explanatory power of these gratifications (Dobos, 1992). These studies found that GO are stronger predictors of media exposure than GS. Cyber communities. Coley (2006) differentiates among three types of cyber communities. First, there are social networks such as Facebook and MySpace where young people create their profiles with private information and then revealing that information to their “cyber friends.” The main purpose of social networks is to make new friendships or to maintain those that already existed. The second type of cyber community is a chat system, which includes instant messaging (IM). The third type is blogs, personal websites with frequently updated observations, news, commentaries, and recommended links (Coley, 2006). Online social networks and Facebook. Online social networks encompass online dating sites, as well as popular social networking websites such as MySpace, Xanga, Live Journal, and Facebook. The difference between chat rooms and social networking sites is that the majority of communication in online social networks takes place asynchronously and within the network of “friends” that the user has established. Facebook.com is a social networking website which, according to nonacademic sources, allows people who use it several advantages. It allows users to stay in touch with old friends and those at other schools, to make new “friends,” to join “groups” that fit their interests, advertise their parties, check how many personal messages/wall posts they received from their friends, and see other people’s pictures and new features that Facebook continually adds. Many search for new people who have recently joined Facebook and whom they might know or want to meet. Another reason is that students are already online, and checking Facebook is a routine online behavior. Coley (2006) asserts that most students use Facebook for fun, to organize parties, and to find dates. They like the opportunity to find others with similar interests, students with whom they are in class, and in using Facebook, they feel a sense of community and connectedness. In 2006, Coley (2006) wrote that about 80 percent of colleges have Facebook, and 85 percent of students at those colleges have accounts. Sixty percent of them log on daily, around six times a day. According to Coley, Facebook is the ninth most-visited website, behind a similar online network called MySpace.com. Facebook features include a profile, status, friends, photos, shares, events, notes, groups, messages, an account setting, and a privacy setting. Facebook also offers a possibility to find a person from your yahoo or hotmail address list that has a Facebook account. A minimal Facebook profile only tells a user’s name, date of joining, school, status, and e-mail address. Withall (2005) best summarized the importance Facebook has for students: “Facebook.com has become our social Bible for definitive information on our classmates, crushes and high school peers we have not spoken to in who-knowshow long.” Motives for Facebook use. To look at the motive for Facebook use, one has to understand the history of research on traditional and new media. During the last 15 years, researchers have developed different motivational scales for Internet use. Most of the items they use came from the analyses of qualitative data, such Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 41 as answers to open-ended questions, essays and diaries (Vettehen and Van Snippenburg, 2002). According to Morris and Ogan (1996), the Internet can fulfill interpersonal and mediated needs. Except for interpersonal needs, Flaherty, Pearce, and Rubin (1998) found that people use computers to satisfy needs traditionally fulfilled by media (i.e., social interaction, to pass time, habit, information, and entertainment), and other needs (i.e., meeting people), which are fulfilled by new media. Flanagin and Metzger (2001) included in mass media needs mediated interpersonal needs such as feeling less lonely, relationship maintenance, problem solving and persuasion. For Parker and Plank (2000), a relaxation and escape factor predicted Internet usage. Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) found a social interaction factor as the only one that predicted going online. Other researchers found that the expectation of finding enjoyable activities online predicted the amount of consumption (LaRose, Mastro, and Eastin, 2001). Charney and Greenberg (2001) described eight gratification factors for the Internet (to keep informed, diversion and entertainment, peer identity, good feelings, communication, sights and sounds, career, and coolness). However, the assumptions of this study assert that certain Internet features, such as Facebook, may not be designed to fulfill all of those needs. According to the uses and gratifications model, a person’s social and psychological factors influence motives for communicating - their gratifications sought and gratifications obtained. Research on media using patterns suggests that demographic variables influence motives and behaviors. Thus, females were found to be more involved in online interpersonal relationships than men (Parks and Floyd, 1996). Behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use. Parks and Floyd (1996) found more developed personal relationships for those who posted more often and who had been posting online for a longer time. Uses and gratification researchers have suggested conceptualizing and measuring gratifications in terms of both gratifications sought and gratifications obtained from media use (Palmgreen and Rayburn, 1985). Gratifications sought are defined as the audience’s reasons are for using Facebook. Gratifications obtained are defined as being those benefits audiences believe they receive from the uses of Facebook or from their frequency of using Facebook. In this study, we measure “Facebook use” as the frequency of Facebook use and the duration of Facebook use (Rubin, 1983). We also measure the frequency of updating one’s Facebook profile. The measure of “relationship development” is operationalized as the number of friends people have on Facebook, and the percentage of friends they have never met in person. This study measured users’ satisfaction with Facebook gratifications. It also examined how much users would miss the site if it suddenly disappeared. Research Questions The first research question addresses student motives (gratifications) for using Facebook. Based on the differences in demographics, the second research question examines how students’ individual differences (age, gender, and education) relate to their motives (gratifications sought) for Facebook use. The third research question considers to what extent gratifications of Facebook use and demographics predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes on Facebook use. Attitudes and 42 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 TABLE 1 Respondents’ Education Level Education Frequency (n) Percent 173 100 5 92 56 17 3 Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Graduate Total 2.8 51.7 31.5 9.6 4.4 behaviors are measured as the frequency of Facebook use, duration of use, the number of Facebook friends, the number of people never met in person, satisfaction with Facebook, missing the Facebook. Method Sample and procedure. To address these questions, a survey of 172 students was conducted at a large southern research university. The survey sample consisted of students enrolled in two large communication classes. Ninety-three percent (n=172) of students had a Facebook account and seven percent (n=12) did not have the account. This number is larger than Coley (2006) found. Of those who had an account, 43 percent (n = 74) were male and 57 percent (n = 98) were female. The average age of respondents was 20 (M = 19.92, SD = 1.23). Most respondents were sophomores (52 percent) and business majors (see Table 1 and 2). Measurement. The survey asked participants to complete the questionnaire regarding their Facebook usage. Participation was voluntary, but students received credit if they completed the survey. Overall, they spent approximately five to seven minutes on the survey. Students who never had a Facebook account were excluded for the analysis. Demographics. Respondents indicated whether they were male or female and were coded dichotomously. Respondents were also asked their age. They were asked their educational level (freshman, sophomore, junior, senior, and other) and their field of study. TABLE 2 Participants’ Field of Study Majors Finance/Business Communication Studies Kinesiology Psychology Mass Communication General Studies Interior Design Human Ecology Other Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use Percent 25.2 5.8 4 3.4 3.4 2.8 2.8 2.8 48.8 43 Motives. A pool of gratification items was assembled from prior Internet gratifications studies (Flaherty, Pearce, and Rubin, 1998; Papacharissi and Rubin, 2000; Flanagin and Metzger, 2001). However, items were edited for duplication and redefined so that they fit Facebook users’ needs. In the questionnaire, respondents were asked how much they use Facebook for the certain reasons. A 5-point Likert Scale was used in rating 38 gratifications items, namely “5” (exactly) and “1” (not at all). Factor analyses extracted factors related to gratifications of the Internet. The factor analysis used a principal component solution and varimax rotation to find variable groupings, and specified the retention of factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0. This resulted in final six factors accounted for 60 percent of the variance. Facebook use and attitudes. To measure the frequency of Facebook use, respondents were asked how many hours they spend on Facebook on an average day and how often they log into their account. As a measure of duration of use, respondents indicated when they opened their Facebook account. We then calculated the number of years and months they had used Facebook. Respondents were also asked to determine how many Facebook friends they have. Facebook satisfaction was measured with a single-item: “Overall, how satisfied are you with the job the Facebook does in providing you with the things you are seeking?” Response options ranged from extremely satisfied (5) to not at all satisfied (1). Similarly, respondents were asked “If Facebook suddenly disappeared how much would you miss it?” Response options ranged from miss a lot (5) to not miss at all (1). Results The goal of the study was to find out to what extent motives and individual differences can predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes of Facebook. The data were analyzed using descriptive statistics, factor analysis, multiple regression and multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). For all analyses, the alpha level was set at .05. According to the analysis, the average time of a Facebook account was 18 months. Students in this sample reported that, on an average, they spent 47 minutes a day on Facebook. Fifty-four percent of the respondents logged into the account several times per day. Twenty- seven percent logged in once per day. Overall, 81 percent of students logged into Facebook on a daily basis. This is 21 percent more than Coley (2006) found. The majority of students (50 percent) changed their profile every few months. Nineteen percent changed their profile every day, and 19 percent 1 to 3 times per week. The majority of students had between 200 and 350 Facebook friends. Facebook motives. The first research question asked what motivates student to use Facebook. The final factor analysis yielded six interpretable factors. Table 3 presents the results of factor analysis. Factor 1 was labeled relationship maintenance (eigenvalue = 10.73). It contained six items (e.g. “To send a message to a friend,” “To post a message on my friend’s wall”) and accounted for 31 percent of the total variance after rotation. This factor suggests that Facebook was used to maintain relationships with existing acquaintances. It was labeled relationship maintenance because it focused on 44 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 TABLE 3 Motives for Facebook Use: Primary Factor Loadings Loading Eigenvalue Variance α Factor 1: Relationship Maintenance To send a message to a friend To post a message on my friend’s wall To communicate with my friends To stay in touch with friends Get in touch with people I know Get through to someone who is hard to reach Factor 2: Passing Time To pass time when bored It is one of the routine things I do when online To occupy my time To check my wall after I receive an e-mail from Facebook Factor 3: Virtual community Develop a romantic relationship Find more interesting people than in real life Find companionship Meet new friends To feel less lonely Factor 4: Entertainment To see other people’s pictures It is entertaining To read other people’s profile To enjoy it To see which of the people I know that joined the Facebook Factor 5: Coolness It makes me cool among my peers Have fun It is cool Factor 6: Companionship To feel less lonely No one to talk or be with So I won’t be alone .74 .70 .83 10.73 31 .90 3.94 11.2 .83 1.84 5.2 .80 1.62 4.6 .84 1.48 4.2 .76 1.41 4 .76 .78 .72 .58 .67 .61 .74 .74 .77 .70 .86 .65 .52 .59 .56 .67 .61 .62 .76 .66 .60 .51 .75 .83 Total variance explained = 60 percent maintaining relationships with existing acquaintances (Song, LaRose, Eastin, and Lin, 2004). Factor 2, passing time (eigenvalue = 3.94) contained four items (e.g. “To occupy my time,” “To pass time when bored”) and accounted for 11.2 percent of the Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 45 TABLE 4 Internet Motives Scale “I use the Facebook for the following reasons” Factor 1: Relationship Maintenance To send a message to a friend To post a message on my friend’s wall To communicate with my friends To stay in touch with friends Get in touch with people I know Get through to someone who is hard to reach Factor 2: Passing Time To pass time when bored It is one of the routine things I do when online To occupy my time To check my wall after I receive an e-mail from Facebook Factor 3: Virtual community Develop a romantic relationship Find more interesting people than in real life Find companionship Meet new friends To feel less lonely Factor 4: Entertainment To see other people’s pictures It is entertaining To read other people’s profile To enjoy it To see which of the people I know that joined the Facebook Factor 5: Coolness It makes me cool among my peers Have fun It is cool Factor 6: Companionship To feel less lonely No one to talk or be with So I won’t be alone M SD 3.62 3.61 3.84 3.92 3.49 3.37 1.19 1.31 1.22 1.22 1.19 1.31 4.10 4.04 3.52 3.85 1.07 1.24 1.33 1.29 1.15 1.22 1.18 1.60 .51 .58 .49 .87 3.67 3.58 3.05 3.61 2.25 1.18 1.21 1.24 1.19 1.15 1.52 2.62 2.22 1.12 1.23 1.22 1.28 1.50 1.26 .57 .92 .64 total variance. The motive is particularly salient to the Internet (Flaherty, Pearce, and Rubin, 1998). Factor 3, virtual community (eigenvalue = 1.83) consisted of five items (e.g. “To feel less lonely,” “To meet new friends”) and explained 5.2 percent of the total variance. This factor, as opposed to maintaining relationships with existing acquaintances, emphasized communication with people met through the Internet. It was named “virtual community” following Song et al. (2004) term. Factor 4, entertainment (eigenvalue = 1.62) consisted of five items (e.g. “To read other people’s profiles,” “It is entertaining”) and explained 4.6 percent of the total variance. However, the factor had a high mean score, suggesting entertainment as a strong gratification sought in Facebook use. 46 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 TABLE 5 Multiple Regressions: Facebook Motives and Demographics Relationship maintenance Passing time Virtual community Entertainment Coolness Companionship Gender (Female=1) Age Education Adjusted R² .63** .40* -.40* .42** -.02 -.14 -.16* -.18* -.08 -.04 -.08 -.05 -.08 .38* -.06 .01 .01 -.16 .16 .08 .03 .04 .00 .00 p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 Factor 5, coolness (eigenvalue = 1.48) consisted of three items (e.g. “It is cool,” and “Have fun”) and explained 4.2 percent of the total variance. It was named “Internet motive” by Charney and Greenberg (2001). Factor 6, companionship (eigenvalue = 1.41) consisted of three items (e.g. “To feel less lonely,” “No one to talk or be with”), and explained 4 percent of the total variance. It is connected with loneliness and regarded as one of mediated interpersonal technology needs (Flanagin and Metzger, 2001). Passing time (M = 3.88, SD = 1.23) and relationship maintenance (M = 3.64, SD = 1.24) factors had the highest mean scores. Entertainment (M = 3.23, SD = 1.19) was also a salient factor for using the Facebook. Less important reasons were coolness (M = 2.12, SD = 1.19), companionship (M = 1.35, SD = .78), and virtual community (M = 1.29, SD = 0.6) (Table 4). The internal consistency of each factor was assessed. Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .75 to .90 (Table 3). Motives and antecedents. The second research question examined how students’ individual differences relate to their gratifications sought in Facebook use. Six stepwise multiple regressions were run with different motives identified by the factor analysis and the demographic characteristics of age, gender, and educational level (Table 5.) Gender (β = .63**) and age (β = -.16*) were the significant predictors of respondents’ motivations to go on Facebook to maintain their existing relationships. Females and younger respondents went to Facebook for those reasons more than males and older respondents. The variables explained 16.3 percent of variance (F [3, 160] = 11.60, p < .01). Gender (β = .40*), age (β = -.18*), and education (β = .38*) predicted passing time motives, with more females than males, and more young people going to Facebook to pass time when bored. The variables explained 8 percent of variance (F [3, 160] = 5.6, p < .001). Gender was the only significant predictor of the use of Facebook to develop new relationships (“virtual community”). This time male respondents (β = -.40*) went to Facebook to meet new people, or to develop a romantic relationship more than female respondents (adjusted R² = 0.03; F [2, 161] = 3.32, p <.05). Gender was the only significant predictor of respondents’ going to Facebook for entertainment reasons (adjusted R² = .04; F [2,161] = 3.56, p < .05). More females (β =.42**) than males went to Facebook to be entertained and see other people’s profiles. Nothing significantly Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 47 predicted respondents going to Facebook to appear cool or relieve loneliness (“companionship” factor). Gender differences emerged also from the multivariate analysis of variance for four of six Facebook motives: relationship maintenance, F [1,162] = 24.15, p < .001; virtual community, F [1,162] = 6.5, p < .05; passing time, F [1,162] = 9.2, p < .01; and entertainment, F [1,162] = 7.15, p < .01. Females were more motivated than males to go on Facebook in order to maintain relationships, to be entertained and to pass time. On the other hand, males were more motivated to use Facebook for developing new relationships. Overall, results suggest that women are more likely than men to use Facebook to maintain existing relationships, pass time and be entertained. On the other hand, men are more likely than women to use Facebook to develop new relationships or meet new people. Younger respondents are more likely than older respondents to use Facebook to maintain existing relationships also to meet new people. Predictors of Behavioral and Attitudinal Outcomes Finally, the third research question asked, “To what extent can gratifications of Facebook use and demographics predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes on Facebook use?” Stepwise multiple regressions were run for dependent variables: the number of hours spent on Facebook, the number of logs into the account, months spent on Facebook, the number of Facebook friends, uploading the site, satisfaction with Facebook, and addiction to the site. The demographic variables (age, gender, education) were entered on the first step of the regression analysis. Six motives for Facebook use (relationship maintenance, passing time, virtual community, entertainment, coolness and companionship) were added next. Results showed that relationship maintenance (β = .23**) and passing time (β = .15***) positively predicted the number of hours students spend on Facebook. The first model with demographic variables explained 3.8 percent of the variance. However, when passing time and relationship maintenance factors were added, the adjusted R² increased 10 percent. Finally, only passing time and relationship maintenance motives were found to be significant predictors of the number of hours respondents spend on Facebook (Adjusted R² = .13, F [5,158] = 5.87, p< .001). Relationship maintenance (β = .50***), passing time (β = .60***), entertainment (β= .28***), and coolness (β=.16*) motives were significant predictors for how often students log into the account. The first demographics model explained only 7.6 percent of the variance. However, the passing time factor, entered next, added 16 percent to the total variance. Other motives, entered last, added significantly to the equation by increasing the total variance by 16 percent (Adjusted R² = .39, F [7, 156] = 15.92, p<.001]. Students who most often logged into their Facebook account were the ones who go there to maintain existing relationships, pass time when bored, be entertained or appear cool. Years-in-college (β = 3.86***) was the only predictor of students’ overall months spent on Facebook. The older the students, the longer they have had an account (Adjusted R²=.174, F [3,160] = 12.48, p<.001). 48 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 Relationship maintenance (β = .37***) and age (β = -.23***) were significant predictors of the numbers of friends students had on Facebook (Adjusted R²=.18, F [4, 159] = 10.14, p<.001). Students who were younger and who went to Facebook to maintain existing relationships had more friends than students who went for other reasons. According to Hecht,1 the number of friends is the measure of relationship development. The entertainment motive (β = .20*) and passing time (β = .45***) were significant predictors of changing a Facebook profile (Adjusted R² = .16, F [5, 158] = 7.41, p<.001) with the passing time motive explaining 10 percent of the overall variance. Therefore, students who were more likely to change their Facebook profile were those who were bored and wanted to occupy their time and be entertained. Relationship maintenance (β = .15*), entertainment (β = .20**), and passing time motives (β = .13*) were significant predictors of students’ satisfaction with Facebook (Adjusted R² = .14, F [6, 157] = 5.33, p<.001). Students who were on Facebook to communicate with their offline friends, to be entertained, read other people’s profiles and to occupy time when bored were more satisfied with the job Facebook was doing. Entertainment (β = .58***), relationship maintenance (β = .48***), passing time (β = .41***), and coolness (β = .18*) all predicted how much students would miss the site if it suddenly disappeared (Adjusted R² = .385, F [7, 156] = 15.59, p<.001). Entertainment motives explained 14 percent of the total variance. It seemed that the students who would most miss the site were those who went to Facebook to see other people’s pictures, to read their profile and to be entertained. Although demographics were not the most important predictors of behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use, the multivariate analysis of variance showed that female respondents had more Facebook friends (F [1,170] = 18.12, p<.001), were more satisfied with the job Facebook is doing (F [1,170] = 4, p<.01), would miss it more if it disappeared (F [1,170] = 14.87, p<.005) and spent more hours on Facebook (F [1,170] = 3.94, p<.005) than male respondents. This suggests that females were more involved in online interpersonal relationships than males (Parks and Floyd, 1996) and that they more readily develop relationship online. Discussion As college students spend more time online than each generation before, it is important to know the gratifications they seek and obtain from the new media. Many media outlets have warned about Digital Natives’ addiction to the social networking websites. However, there is little research on who uses them and why and what the outcome of their social networking is. In order to discover motives (gratifications sought) for using the Facebook, how individual differences (age, gender, education) relate to motives for Facebook use, and to what extent motives and individual differences can predict attitudinal and behavioral outcomes of Facebook, we conducted a survey with 172 students at a large southern research university. Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 49 Of the sample surveyed, 93 percent of students had a Facebook account and 7 percent did not have the account. Students in this sample reported that on an average they spent on Facebook 47 minutes a day. Overall, 81 percent of students logged into Facebook on a daily basis. The majority of students had between 200 and 350 Facebook friends. Most students go to Facebook to maintain relationships with people they know. Their motives include behaviors such as sending a message to a friend, posting a message on their friend’s wall, staying in touch with a friend or getting in touch with someone who is difficult to reach. Females go to Facebook for relationship maintenance more than males. A larger proportion of students, more so females than males, go to Facebook to pass time when they are bored or after they receive an e-mail suggesting them that someone had posted on their Facebook site. A significant number of students, more so females than males, use Facebook for entertainment reasons. A smaller number of people use it to develop new relationships or to meet new people, more so males and younger respondents than females and older respondents. This supports what Tewksbury and Althaus (2000) suggested: entertainment and passing time – gratifications typically associated with television and newspaper use – prove to be significant predictors of using Facebook. It also correspondents with the findings of Parks and Floyd (1996), which suggests that females are more involved in online interpersonal relationships than men are. The number of hours respondents spent on Facebook is correlated to maintaining relationships and passing time motives. Students who most often log into their Facebook account are the ones who go there to maintain existing relationships, pass time when bored, be entertained or appear cool. Younger students, females and those interested in maintaining existing relationships through Facebook have more Facebook friends than students with other interests. As Parks and Floyd (1996) noted, those who posted more often had developed a greater number of personal relationships. The data show that not many people go to Facebook to escape from problems in real life or to lessen loneliness by finding companionship. Factor analysis eliminated escape and companionship as motives for using the Facebook. Generally, the findings of this study are consistent with findings of Flaherty, Pearce, and Rubin (1998). Those findings suggested that people use computers to satisfy needs traditionally fulfilled by media (i.e., pass time, habit, information, and entertainment). It supported LaRose et al. (2001), which suggested that the expectation of finding enjoyable activities online predicted the amount of consumption a user was likely to engage in. According to the uses and gratifications model, a person’s social and psychological factors influence motives for communicating - their gratifications sought and gratifications obtained. In this study, we found that gender, and to lesser extent age and education of college students, were important predictors of using Facebook to maintain existing relationships and pass time, with women more likely to do it than men. Therefore, women tend to spend more time on Facebook than men, have more friends, are more satisfied with the job Facebook is doing and would have missed the site more if it suddenly disappeared. 50 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 Similarly to nonacademic sources, this study found that Facebook allows people to stay in touch with old friends and those at other schools, check how many personal messages/wall posts they received from their friends, and see other people’s pictures and new features that Facebook continually adds. Students also search for new people who have recently joined the Facebook and whom they might know or want to meet (Withall, 2005). Limitations. This study was limited in a few ways. First, the list of needs was not comprehensive. Inclusion of other needs may alter the findings. Second, the use of a convenience sample prevents a generalization of the findings to the population of Facebook users. Sampling restrictions hampered the potential external validity of the study. At the time the data were collected, Facebook had just opened access to the general public. If we had conducted the study several months later, after access to the general public had been granted, we would probably have obtained a different population for our sample, which would have hewed closer to providing external validity to the actual population of Facebook users. Future studies could compare demographic characteristics of people who have Facebook accounts with those who do not. They could measure social capital (bridging and bonding) in online and offline contexts. They could compare motivations for site’s use between high school students and college students and between different colleges and schools. Those limitations stipulated, this study had several strong points. First, its goal was to find unique dimensions of Facebook use unidentified in existing literature. It helped us to understand what motivates people to use Facebook, and what personal and social attributes affect its use. In light of uses and gratification theory, the study found that people go to Facebook to fulfill needs traditionally fulfilled by other media. But first and foremost, they use it to satisfy their interpersonal communication needs (relationship maintenance). This supports Dicken-Garcia (1998), which contends that the Internet places stronger emphasis on informal, interpersonal conversations than has been true of earlier media. It found what Flanagin and Metzger’s (2001) findings showed: mass media needs now include interpersonal needs such as relationship maintenance. Future studies should explore to what extent mass communication is a substitute for interpersonal communication and how people use false identities to communicate on Facebook. They could also test for other personal predictors of Facebook use, such as locus of control and for the relationships between selfdisclosure and relationship development on Facebook. Studies should be conducted using other theoretical approaches, such as the social penetration theory and agenda-setting theory. References Charney, T., & Greenberg, B. (2001). Uses and gratifications of the Internet. In C. Lin & D. Atkin (Eds.), Communication, technology and society: New media adoption and uses and gratifications (pp. 383-406). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton. Coley, T. (2006). Students and cyber communities. University of South Carolina. Dicken-Garcia, H. (1998). Internet and continuing historical discourse. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 75, 19-27. Dobos, J. (1992). Gratifications models of satisfaction and choices of communiStudent Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 51 cation channels in organizations. Communication Research, 19, 29-51. Elliot, P. (1974). Uses and gratifications research: A critique and a sociological alternative. In J. G. Blumler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses of mass communications: Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 249-268). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Flaherty, L. M., Pearce, K., & Rubin, R. B. (1998). Internet and face-to-face communication: Not functional alternatives. Communication Quarterly, 46, 250-268. Flanagin, A. J., Metzger, M. J. (2001). Internet use in the contemporary media environment. Human Communication Research, 27, 153-181. Haridakis, P. M., & Rubin, A. M. (2003). Motivation for watching television violence and viewer aggression. Mass Communication & Society, 6, 29-56. Katz, E. (1959). Mass communication research and the study of popular culture: An editorial note on a possible future for this journal. Studies in Public Communication, 2, 1-6. LaRose R., Eastin, M. S. (2004). A social cognitive theory of Internet uses and gratifications: Toward a new model of media attendance. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48, 358-377. LaRose R., Mastro, D., & Eastin, M. S. (2001). Understanding Internet usage: A social-cognitive approach to uses and gratifications. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 395-414. Louie, J. (2003). Media in the lives of immigrant youth. New directions for youth development, 111-130. McQuail, D., Blumler, J. G., & Brown, J.R. (1972). The television audience: Revised perspective. In D. McQuail (Ed.), Sociology of mass communications (pp. 135-165). Harmondsworth, Eng.: Penguin. Morris, M., & Ogan, C. (1996). The Internet as mass medium. Journal of Communication, 46, 39-50. Newhagen, J., & Rafaeli, S. (1996). Why communication researchers should study the Internet: A dialogue. Journal of Communication, 46, 4-13. Palmgreen, P., & Rayburn, J. D., II. (1985). A comparison of gratification models of media satisfaction. Communication Monographs, 52, 334-346. Papacharissi, Z., & Rubin, A. (2000). Predictors of Internet use. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 44, 75-196. Parker, B. J., & Plank, R. E. (2000). A uses and gratifications perspective on the Internet as a new information source, American Business Review, 18, 43-49. Parks, M. R.; & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication, 46, 80-97 also available at: Journal of Computer Mediated Communication, 46, http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol1/issue4/vol1no4.html Rheingold, H. (1993). The virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier. New York: Addison Wesley. Rubin, A. M. (1993). The effect of locus of control on communication motivation, anxiety, and satisfaction. Communication Quarterly, 41, 161-171. Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication & Society, 3, 3-37. Song, I., Larose, R., Eastin, M. S., Lin, C. A. (2004). Internet gratifications and 52 Southwestern Mass Communication Journal Spring 2008 Internet addiction: On the uses and abuses of new media. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 7, 384-393. Swanson, D. L. (1977). The uses and misuses of uses and gratifications. Human Communication Research, 3, 214-228. Tewksbury, D., & Althaus, S. L. (2000). An examination of motivations for using the World Wide Web. Communication Research Reports, 17, 127-138. Vettehen, P. G. H., & Van Snippenburg, L. B. (2002). Measuring motivations for media exposure: A thesis. Quality & Quantity, 36, 259-276. Withall, R. (18 November 2005). Facing the facts about Facebook. The Villanovan. Student Favorite: Facebook and Motives for its Use 53
© Copyright 2024