Exploring Kawaii 1 Running head: EXPLORING KAWAII IN JAPANESE COLLEGE WOMEN Exploring Kawaii in a Sample of Japanese College Women: A Mixed-Methods Study Stephanie Klapper Paul H. Nitze Scholars Program St. Mary’s College of Maryland Exploring Kawaii 2 Abstract Kawaii is a Japanese concept that roughly translates to “cuteness” in English. Though many essays about kawaii have been written over the years, few empirical studies have been conducted. This exploratory study used mixed methods to examine the meaning of kawaii to Japanese college women at Akita International University. A measure of kawaii was developed and administered with the Bem Sex Role Inventory to 64 students. Individual interviews were conducted with 18 students from the surveyed sample. Results indicated that although all interviewed participants thought that most young women want to be kawaii, over 70% also believed that it was not strange to not want to be kawaii. The participants also discussed socially acceptable alternatives to being kawaii such as kireii (beautiful) and kakkoii (cool). Additionally, there was much variation in the interviewed participants’ perceptions of kawaii style. The surveyed participants rated the importance of the characteristics on the kawaii characteristics scale in relation to their importance to the definition of kawaii. The interviewed participants then refined the kawaii characteristics scale and gave examples. The hypotheses, that there would be a significant positive correlation between self-rated kawaii and masculinity, and a significant negative correlation between self-rated kawaii and femininity, were not supported. There was no relationship between self-rated kawaii and masculinity or femininity. Because masculinity in Japan has been correlated with career self-efficacy, vocational maturity, and career commitment, the data suggests that self-rated kawaii women are not at a disadvantage in these areas as compared with other women. This is important in light of the finding that there was also no positive or negative relationship between self-rated kawaii and the priorities of having a career or having children. The results imply that kawaii may be more related to appearance than to personality. Implications for future research are discussed. Exploring Kawaii 3 I would like to thank the following people for their help with this St. Mary’s Project: My mentor Dr. Renée Peltz Dennison for all of her assistance The administration at Akita International University for their support The students at Akita International University for their participation The Office of International Education for all of their hard work on study abroad programs Dr. Elizabeth Nutt Williams and Christina Torres for introducing me to qualitative research Dr. Debbie O’Donnell for discussing kawaii in her cross-cultural psychology class Zenaida, Larry, James, and Marilyn Klapper for their great support and understanding Maxwell Bloch for always providing me with help when I asked and needed it the most Exploring Kawaii 4 Exploring Kawaii in Japanese College Women: A Mixed-Methods Study In the past four decades, a pop culture phenomenon known as “kawaii” has permeated through Japanese society. “Kawaii” translates into English as “cute,” though that is not a perfect translation. More completely, it means “childlike, sweet, adorable, innocent, pure, simple, genuine, gentle, vulnerable, weak, and inexperienced social behavior and physical appearance” (Kinsella, 1995, p. 220). Centuries ago, kawaii referred only to small children, animals, or objects. Today, anything cute in Japanese society- from company logos to pornography- can be described as kawaii (McVeigh, 2000b). Kawaii is so much a part of the national consciousness that even the police use a cute cheerful mouse-like mascot in their public safety campaigns (McVeigh, 2000b). With kawaii becoming such a part of mainstream culture, scholars have found it increasingly difficult to define the word concisely (Akita, 2005). Though kawaii is a pervasive concept in Japanese culture, it has special importance to young Japanese women. In the 1970s teenage girls and young women started to use the word to describe themselves. They proceeded to dress, speak, act, eat, and even write cutely (Akita, 2005). Based on the literature, being a kawaii as a young woman is defined by specific characteristics which are not applicable to other kawaii beings, such as animals. These include characteristics of appearance (clothing, makeup, hair, accessories) and behavior (language, writing style, actions). The importance of the kawaii phenomenon among young women rests in both material and social implications. Japan’s total population is approaching 128 million people (Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications [MIAC], 2009), with 6.65 million1 young women between the ages of 15 and 24. Though these women comprise only about 5% of the total population, they 1 This number was calculated by using the ratio of Japanese men to women between the ages of 15-64 (Central Intelligence Agency, 2009) and applying it to the number of Japanese men and women between the ages of 15-24 (MIAC, 2009) Exploring Kawaii 5 have an enormous amount of economic power (Kinsella, 1995). Japanese teenagers make up the fourth highest teen spending market in the world (Krauss, 2001). High school girls alone spend approximately $2.5 billion every year (Drake, 2001). Young women in their 20s retain large disposable incomes by continuing to receive free room and board from their parents while they work (Chandler & Kano, 2003). In fact, more than half of women in their 20s own a Louis Vuitton handbag, a luxury item that can cost hundreds or thousands of dollars (Chandler & Kano, 2003). The amount of wealth that young Japanese women possess, combined with a strong desire to be kawaii, make kawaii a powerful force in the Japanese economy. More importantly, the desire to describe oneself as kawaii may have profound social impacts on young Japanese women. In western cultures, as well as in Japan, instrumental traits such as leadership abilities, ambitiousness, and strength have been linked to positive characteristics such as mental health, self-efficacy, and self-esteem. By socializing women to act young, innocent, and weak, kawaii may be putting young women at risk. This idea will be discussed in more detail in the gender roles section of this literature review. Although there are many uses for the word “kawaii” in Japanese society, this literature review will specifically explore the meaning and implications of the word to young women who consider themselves kawaii. A Review of the Literature Kawaii The Beginning In 1974, large numbers of teenage women invented a new style of writing Japanese characters which resembled the script of a child just learning how to write. These characters differed from normal characters in that they were rounded, written left to right rather than Exploring Kawaii 6 vertically, used thin lines rather than varied thickness, had little cartoon pictures inserted randomly throughout the text, and included some English. The style was dubbed “kawaii.” By 1985 approximately 5 million people were using the new form of writing (Kinsella, 1995). In the late 1970s, companies started to capitalize on the teenage desire to be kawaii. Though the kawaii writing style was banned in many schools, several magazines, comics, word processors, and other writing-based media adopted the new style to appeal to teenage tastes. Sanrio, a Japanese company that specializes in merchandise with cute characters, started producing cute stationary, toys, toiletries, lunch boxes, bags, towels, and other novelties. These fashionable “fancy goods” were small, pastel, round, soft, frilly, and distinctly styled in a European or American way rather than in a traditional Japanese way. Soft, small, infantile, mammalian, and round stuffed animals with no body appendages were very popular. These stuffed animals often seemed confused, vulnerable, or lacking confidence. Shops specializing in cute objects became very popular, especially in large cities (Kinsella, 1995). Characteristics of Kawaii After the 1970s, women adopted many different methods to convey kawaii. These included changing their voices, content of speech, mannerisms, food, makeup, clothing, hair, accessories, belongings, and toiletries. By 1992, the word “kawaii” was estimated to be “the most widely used, widely loved, habitual word in modern living Japanese” (CREA, November 1992, p. 58; as cited in Kinsella, 1995, p. 220). Communication. Young Japanese women often engaged in altered modes of speaking to appear kawaii. The most basic of these is to speak in a highly pitched voice to appear more childlike (Bremner, 2002). A native professor at a women’s junior college from 1989 to 1998, observed that her students would often smile or giggle to avoid completing their sentences Exploring Kawaii 7 (Akita, 2005). They would also use onomatopoeias, abbreviated expression, and slang such as “I like English xx2” instead of “I like English very much.” At times, they would refer to themselves in the third person rather than in the first. For example, one might say “Yukari did it,” rather than “I did it.” In Japan this behavior is normally only accepted for children aged 2-7. Actions. Kawaii women during the 1980s also acted more infantile than other women. For example, kawaii women stood pigeon-toed, smiled much of the time (Akita, 2005, p. 45), stomped their feet (Bremner, 2002), took faltering steps, blushed, cried, and giggled more than non-kawaii women (Kinsella, 1995). They also ate very sweet food, such as ice cream, cake, and puddings. These foods are strongly associated with children in Japan (Kinsella, 1995, p. 231). These sweets became so popular in the 1980s that fancy ice cream shops started opening all throughout Japan (Kinsella, 1995). At times, males reinforced the cute food trend. In one essay a woman describes her boyfriend switching her order to ice cream when she asked for black coffee (Akita, 2005). Kawaii women also changed their manners. For example, some kawaii women sat with their friends on the floor of a train or platform and talked loudly, which is considered very rude for adults but acceptable among children (Akita, 2005). Clothing. In the 1980s a company based in Tokyo called Pink House LTD became so well known for its cute outfits that the people who bought its clothing were said to have been part of the “Pink House Movement.” Cute clothing included white, pink, and pastel colors with frilled puffy sleeves and ribbons. Extra small shirts were decorated with cartoon characters and slogans. White tights, frilly ankle socks, and knee length school girl socks completed the outfit. Even underwear was built to be small with large amounts of elastic so that they would shrink to look like little girls’ panties when not in use (Kinsella, 1995). Exploring Kawaii 8 Since then, several kawaii styles have come and gone. In the late 1980s, a fashion magazine called “Cute for Independent Girls” pushed a “cheeky tomboy” sort of cuteness. Popular clothing included tight white t-shirts, nursery colors, cartoon characters, and tight little sweaters. The magazine showed pictures of teens trying to look “bad and cute” at the same time (Kinsella, 1995, p. 229). In 2003, young girls started buying fake school uniforms to wear outside of school. In rare cases, girls wore real school uniforms, such as the coveted traditional sailor suit uniform. But for the most part, most had embellishments—such as shorter skirts and many colorful bows-- which would usually not have been allowed at school. The trend was so popular that some schools started hiring famous Japanese fashion designers to make their uniforms in order to attract students (Zaun, 2003). In 2006, several kawaii fashions developed in three Tokyo neighborhoods. One was simply called “kawaii,” a preppy but cute fashion that used ruffles, lace, bows, and embroidery. Another was called “glamorous girls,” in which many of the girls identified themselves as “erokawaii” or “cute sexy.” This fashion style mimicked famous movie stars, and included short skirts, skinny denim, big movie star sunglasses, tinted hair, and colorful high heels. Meanwhile “eighties and natural” girls used natural colors for their clothing, which contrasted with their colorful hair and makeup ("The tribes of Tokyo," 2006). Kawaii fashion has obtained a reputation for its fickleness. Japanese fashion is determined by consumer teenagers more so than in most other nations. Tokyo high school students in particular have the most influence. These girls spend approximately $275 per month on clothing, three times the amount spent by high school students in other parts of the country. These girl consumers decide amongst themselves what new fashion is kawaii, and clothing stores rush to develop the right clothing before the girls change their minds. Kawaii fashion can range Exploring Kawaii 9 from frilly pink shirts to vinyl miniskirts, and each fashion lasts only a few months at a time. After something is deemed kawaii in Tokyo, fashion magazines quickly advertise the clothing to the rest of the country. However, the industry moves so quickly that by the time the rest of Japan is scrambling to get the coveted clothing, Tokyo has already moved on to the next fashion. Tokyo consumers are so powerful that clothing companies typically hire teenage Tokyo girls with no training as clothing designers in order to gain an edge in the kawaii market (Drake, 2001). Hair, makeup, and hygiene. Hair and makeup styles change depending on what is currently kawaii. Few elements are common among different styles of kawaii hair, but two staples are butterfly clips and young girl barrettes (Bremner, 2002). For example, the previously mentioned kawaii girls wore mascara, blush, paled foundation, and colored hair ("The tribes of Tokyo," 2006). The “glamorous girls” wore iridescent makeup and tinted hair. The “eighties girls” wore colorful makeup and colorful accessories in their hair. But whatever the fashion, hair and makeup can take a lot of time to perfect. This is not surprising, considering that the cosmetics market in Japan is the second largest in the world (Miller, 2006). Perfume, hair products, skin products, and makeup sales accounted for approximately $15 billion in 2002 (Japan External Trade Organization, 2003 as cited in Miller, 2006). Some of these sales included soap, shampoo, cream, toothbrushes, and perfumes meant for babies and young children, but instead sold to young women in their early 20s ("From the cradle," 1995). Belongings. Nearly any object can be turned into a kawaii accessory if a company is willing to make it. Even something as routine and functional as a cell phone can be deemed fashionable or unfashionable (Katz & Sugiyama, 2006), kawaii or not kawaii (Nelson, 2006). According to Kinsella (1995), this makes perfect sense. The fantasy world of kawaii is not Exploring Kawaii 10 always easily applicable to real life situations. For example, acting childish does not necessarily help with school or work performance. However, using kawaii every-day items can allow women to enjoy feeling kawaii at any time of the day (Kinsella, 1995). The high demand for these items is apparent from the diversity of available products. Hello Kitty, a popular kawaii character in Japan, is available on hundreds of objects. According to McVeigh (2000a), Sanrio has invented the following Hello Kitty merchandise: stickers, coffee mugs, glasses, calculators, watches, towels, pillows, toothbrushes, lunch boxes, pens, pencils, garbage pails, golf bags, boxer shorts, safes, luggage, scooters…bank-books, and cash cards…toys, handbags….telephones and televisions…spatulas, sets of dishes, pans, pots, bowls and cups, a tea kettle, coffee maker, shaved ice machine, hair dryer, cookie mold, toaster, toaster oven…waffle maker…car…wallets, sunglasses, pens [and] pocketbooks (p. 229-230). These are only some of the Hello Kitty items available. Because young women can never look or feel perfectly like a child, there is no upper limit to how much money it is possible to spend on products to achieve the ideal kawaii persona. By 2005, Hello Kitty products alone had drawn in $400 million for Sanrio (Bland, 2007). Even after women in their late 20s and 30s stop trying to embody the kawaii ideal, they still continue to buy cute objects which are useful at home, such as aprons and vacuum cleaners. This makes the market for kawaii products so large that it has been called “consumutopia” (McVeigh, 2000a). Symbolism and Uses of Kawaii Kawaii and adulthood. Kinsella (1995) conducted a survey among 18-30 year old men and women in Tokyo in 1992 to better understand what kawaii meant to this age group. Her results showed a strong dichotomy between childhood and adulthood. Adulthood was perceived Exploring Kawaii 11 as very negative, with descriptions such as “harshness of having to support a family,” “responsibility,” “a dirty world of power,” “dreams disappear,” “strictness,” “can’t stop working,” “free time is lacking,” and “lonely” (Kinsella, 1995, p. 42). In a society where almost all men and a few women work for one company their entire lives and are expected to stay extremely late virtually every night, it is understandable that they do not look forward to growing up. Many women who marry are resigned to focusing nearly all of their concentration on their family without the prospect of a career or the emotional support from an absent husband. Often women delay marriage, live with their parents, and work part-time to escape these dismal scenarios (Zielenziger, 2006). Meanwhile, kawaii was associated with words such as “childlike,” “innocent,” “naïve,” “unconscious,” “natural,” and “inability to deal with every day life” (Kinsella, 1995, p. 238-239). Above all, kawaii was described as a time when warm emotional contact was possible and sincere emotions could come out from hiding. While adulthood was perceived as artificial and isolating, kawaii was associated with happiness, naivety, and unconscious harmony with others (Kinsella, 1995, p. 238-239). Kinsella (1995) concluded that the Japanese see adulthood as a time of imprisonment and responsibility. Kawaii could be useful to young adults by providing an escape from a bleak view of adulthood. (Kinsella, 1995). If young women truly view kawaii as sincere, naïve, and unconscious, then the most ironic part is that so many young women are “burikkos,” literally “fake children.” Many young women intentionally invest money in kawaii clothing, makeup, accessories, and hair products, and intentionally engage in kawaii behaviors so that they can achieve the ideal of being kawaii. But the idea of kawaii seems to hinge on the idea that it is natural; it is not something that one can try to embody through effort or consumerism. Exploring Kawaii 12 Thus, the very ideal that many “kawaii” women try to embrace always eludes them (Kinsella, 1995). If kawaii does provide an escape from reality, it is artificial. Authority. Another more cynical approach to kawaii is its potential usefulness in power relationships. In Japan, vertical relationships with clear authority figures are a very important part of society (Nakane, 1970 as cited in McVeigh, 2000b). From the time Japanese children can talk, they must learn to show proper respect to their elders. When school starts, they learn that they are to sit quietly and learn from their teacher without making suggestions or asking questions. The hierarchal system is so strong that high-ranking businessmen consistently discourage young Japanese workers from suggesting creative ideas or trying to quickly work their way to the top. Salary men slowly rise up through the business world in due time, regardless of individual ability (Zielenziger, 2006). Female workers have the lowest position in this system, working mostly as stagnant clerical assistants for male bosses. Although women make up nearly half of Japan’s work force, in 2005 women held only 10.1% of all corporate and government managerial jobs. In contrast, women held 42.5 percent of managerial jobs in the United States (Fackler, 2007). McVeigh (2000b) argues that kawaii helps young women create “amaeru” and “amayakasu” relationships with their male bosses at work. Amaeru literally means “to solicit the indulgence of another,” while amayakasu means “to indulge, spoil, or pamper” (McVeigh, 2000b, p. 144). Though typically used to describe the relationship between a mother and her children, these two words also capture the “sugary emotional bonding between superiors and inferiors” (McVeigh, 2000b, p. 144). One person argues that kawaii allows women to be indulged by their superiors “simply by nature of [its] silliness. How angry can you be at plastic barrettes and ankle socks? How seriously can you take a subculture that is, by definition, Exploring Kawaii 13 profoundly inane?” (Schoemer & Chang, 1995). Thus, by acting cute, women can create sugary emotional bonding with their superiors and be looked upon more favorably. This theory is supported by research finding that cuteness in babies is correlated with adults’ desire to take care of them (Glocker, et al., 2009). Kawaii’s usefulness to women in the workforce sometimes leads to the abuse of kawaii as reflected in this young woman’s statement: “I pretend to be cute at the office where I work part-time. This is because I do not want to be bothered with complicated human relationships” (McVeigh, 2000b, p. 144). However, kawaii’s usefulness for women is strongly tied to its usefulness for men. Men do not like to be called kawaii, as this denotes weakness. It may be that when women act kawaii, they mimic a person who is submissive, meek, and compliant. Because obedience reinforces one’s position in the hierarchy, women effectively accept their low spot in the Japanese hierarchy and take their “proper” place as a Japanese woman. One study conducted by Hildebrandt and Fitzgerald (1979) shows how deeply gender stereotypes about cuteness penetrate. Cute babies are more likely to be perceived as female regardless of actual gender, which indicates that cuteness is associated with females from the beginning of life. Outside of the workforce, corporations or people in authority positions may also find kawaii to be useful, which would explain military recruitment posters which depict cute Japanese women saying “Peace People Japan—Come On!” (Frühstück, 2007). If state agencies, educators, large companies, the police, and the military can convince others that they are not intimidating, then they may have an easier time with persuasion and control (McVeigh, 2000b). It makes them more likeable. However, kawaii can also take authority away from those in power, such as teachers with students. For example, when a professor was scolding her classroom of students, one young girl exclaimed “Teacher, your earrings are so kawaii!”(Akita, 2005). The class Exploring Kawaii 14 laughed and stopped listening to what the teacher had to say. Being called kawaii had effectively robbed her of her power (Akita, 2005). Conformity and individuality. Another explanation for the persistence of kawaii is that it allows young people to conform to a group while still showing some amount of individuality. In Japan, conformity and belonging to a group is highly valued among young people. Advertisements for clothing will often have a “fit in with the group” theme as opposed to the “dare to be different” themes more popular in the west (Zielenziger, 2006). Nonetheless, after adhering to a certain dress code, young people often find small ways to assert their individuality (Mcveigh, 2000a). For example, a young clique that wears and buys everything with Hello Kitty can still show individuality through which products they buy. Hello Kitty’s mood and size can make a difference (McVeigh, 2000a). Kawaii provides a common dress code, but also provides options for alteration. Another explanation is that acting kawaii allows young women to express independent thoughts while still appearing to conform to the majority. Unfortunately, because they act vulnerable and immature, they have trouble having their ideas taken seriously (Akita, 2005). Sexuality. Japanese men admit that they prefer cute women over beautiful women because they do not feel as comfortable with beautiful women. Beautiful women appear to have sexual experience, while cute women appear to be naïve and innocent. Thus, beautiful women intimidate them, while cute women help them feel in control. This overall preference for cute women is immediately obvious in Japanese commercials, animation, pornography, and the attire of Japanese prostitutes (McVeigh, 2000b; Sinclair, 2006). Some women who want to be sexually attractive try to make themselves “ero-kawaii,” or “cute sexy.” They wear cute clothing but combine them with more sophisticated styles by following tips distributed by talk shows, Exploring Kawaii 15 women’s journals, magazines, and books. However, males are alert to burikko (“fake child”) who attempt to act innocent when in reality they are mature. Males perceive these women as manipulative, while naturally cute women are seen as very desirable (McVeigh, 2000b). Gender Roles The kawaii phenomenon has strong implications for young women’s gender roles in Japan. Masculinity and femininity are considered the two components of gender role. Masculinity is comprised of traits which are considered desired in males, while femininity is comprised of traits desired in females. Masculinity and femininity are not considered to be mutually exclusive, but rather two-dimensional (Marsh & Byrne, 1991). It is possible for a person to be high in both masculine and feminine traits. Masculinity has also been called instrumentality, traits which reflect a desire and ability to get things done quickly such as independence, assertiveness, and dominance. Femininity has been called expressiveness, traits which reflect caring and empathy for others. In 1974 Bem created the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) to assess the levels of masculine and feminine traits in individuals (Bem, 1974). In the U.S., males tend to score higher on masculinity than females, while females score higher on femininity (Bem, 1974). However, the results are very different in Japan. Sugihara and Katsurada (1999) administered a Japanese translation of the BSRI to 265 college students. They found that males and females did not differ from one another in levels of masculinity and femininity, but that both were higher in femininity and masculinity. In a later study, Sugihara and Katsurada (2000) used a modified version of the BSRI and found that Japanese males and females scored equally on femininity while males scored higher on masculinity. Again, males and females both scored higher on femininity than on masculinity. Exploring Kawaii 16 The high levels of femininity among Japanese males may be attributable to cultural differences. The BSRI was created in the United States, an individualistic culture where instrumental traits are highly regarded. In contrast, Japan is a collectivist culture. Feminine expressive traits which lead to group harmony are often more desirable than masculine instrumental traits. This explains why expressiveness might be high among both males and females. Sugihara and Katsurada (2000) used a modified version of the BSRI in which traits from the masculinity scale which were not highly regarded in Japanese culture, such as “dominant” and “self-reliant,” were removed. This may explain why males in this study scored higher on masculinity than the males in Sugihara’s and Katsurada’s 1999 study. With these traits removed, males scored higher on a scale of masculinity more consistent with their collectivist culture (Sugihara & Katsurada, 2000). Since the invention of the BSRI, there has been much controversy over which traits are more beneficial to members of each gender. Over the years several different theories about sex roles and self-concepts have risen and fallen in popularity (Marsh & Byrne, 1991). These are: 1. Sex-typed mode. Masculine males and feminine females have the best outcomes. 2. Additive androgyny model. Masculinity and femininity both contribute in different but positive ways. The main effects of masculinity and femininity will be statistically significant. 3. Masculinity model. Masculinity is related to positive outcomes while femininity either has a negative main effect or no main effect. 4. Androgyny model. There is a significant interaction between masculinity and femininity. This means that being high in both types of traits leads to better outcomes than masculinity or femininity alone. There may or may not be main effects of masculinity and femininity. Exploring Kawaii 17 Much of the research supports the masculinity model. In Western nations, masculinity has been correlated to mental health (Bassoff & Glass, 1982), self-esteem (Antill & Cunningham, 1979, 1980; Feather, 1985; Whitley, 1983), low levels of depression (Elpern & Karp, 1984), problem-solving skills (Nezu & Nezu, 1987), and creative accomplishment (Hittner & Daniels, 2002). In Japan, masculinity has been correlated with career self-efficacy (Matsui, Kakuyama, Konishi, Tsuzuki, & Onglatco, 1999; Matsui & Onglatco, 1991), vocational maturity (Ono & Sakayanagi, 1986), and career commitment (Matsui, Ohsawa, & Onglatco, 1991). The androgyny model also has some support. Androgyny refers to individuals high in both masculinity and femininity, and it has been correlated with successfully being accepted for jobs one has applied for (Francesco & Hakel, 1981), attractiveness (Green & Kenrick, 1994), and emotional intelligence (Guastello & Guastello, 2003). Specifically in Japan, androgyny has been linked with communication skills (Hirokawa, Yamada, Dohi, & Miyata, 2001), successful adaptation across the lifespan (Shimonaka, Nakazato, Kawaai, & Sato, 1997), and vocational maturity (Ono & Sakayanagi, 1986). However, criticism about inappropriate statistical practices has cast doubt on androgyny research (Taylor & Hall, 1982). Furthermore, some studies have found that androgynous individuals and masculine individuals reap nearly identical benefits, implying that masculinity is the only factor of importance (Nezu & Nezu, 1987; Whitley, 1983). Overall, the masculinity model has received the most support. From these results, it is reasonable to assume that Japanese individuals with high masculinity will benefit more, especially if they want to pursue a career. Statement of the Problem This literature review explores kawaii’s characteristics and uses among young Japanese women. However, most of this literature comes from essays and newspaper articles. There have Exploring Kawaii 18 been few methodical studies on kawaii, and even fewer from a psychological standpoint. Additionally, there are still many unanswered questions about the meaning and implications of kawaii. The present study focuses on two hypotheses and several research questions. The first hypothesis is that there will be a significant negative correlation between masculinity and selfrated kawaii. The definition of kawaii from the literature generally excludes many masculine traits such as independence, assertiveness, and self-sufficiency. This supports the idea that there may be a significant negative correlation between masculinity and self-rated kawaii. Because masculine traits are generally associated with success, this implies that self-rated kawaii women may be at a disadvantage in life. The second hypothesis is that there will be a significant positive correlation between femininity and self-rated kawaii. The definition of kawaii generally includes feminine traits such as yielding, childlike, and affectionate. It would make sense for self-rated kawaii women to embody all of these traits. The research questions are as follows (a) Is wanting to be kawaii the norm among young women?; (b) Is there more than one style of kawaii?; (c) What are the most important characteristics of being kawaii, and how are they defined?; (d) How important is it look kawaii versus act kawaii?; and (e) Is there a relationship between self-rated kawaii and priorities for the future? This is an exploratory mixed-methods study which uses a triangulation design (Creswell, 2003). The exploratory study broadly examines the concept of kawaii to young women in order to better understand the concept, rather than narrowly focusing on any given meaning of kawaii. As a mixed-methods study, it will combine quantitative data and qualitative data. The Exploring Kawaii 19 quantitative data are participant results from the Kawaii Survey I designed and the Bem Sex Role Inventory. This data will be analyzed through Pearson’s correlations and a Friedman test. The qualitative data are codes from semi-structured interviews. This data will be analyzed through constant comparison analysis. In accordance with its triangulation design, the two types of data will be analyzed separately, but then compared in the results and discussion session, each with equal weight. The hope is that combining these two methods will provide a more complete understanding of kawaii than qualitative or quantitative methods could alone. Method Study 1 This quantitative study tested the hypotheses and explored all four research questions. Participants were given a survey which tested for the importance of looking and acting kawaii, the importance of different characteristics of kawaii, how appropriate kawaii is in different situations, and the importance of having a full-time career and having children. Participants were also given a survey which tested for masculine and feminine personality traits. Participants Japanese female students living at Akita International University, a small liberal arts school that conducts its classes in English, received flyers in their mailboxes inviting them to participate in a voluntary confidential online survey. Posters advertising the survey were hung in the cafeteria, a dorm hallway, the library, and the student hall. All advertisements indicated that students had two weeks to participate before the survey closed. In exchange for participation, students were entered into a raffle for a 3000 yen ($33) VISA gift card. Due to low participation rates (54 students) after two weeks, flyers were again distributed to mailboxes and posters were put up to advertise that the deadline had been extended Exploring Kawaii 20 by three weeks. When participation rates were still low one week later (67 students), I also utilized snowball sampling methods by approaching students in computer lab courses, the Japanese Language Friends Club, and the Tea Ceremony Club. Four days before the final deadline, female students again received flyers in their mailboxes, this time to remind them that the deadline was soon. The final number of participants was 86. However, 22 of these were not included in the final analysis because they did not answer at least 5.5% of the questions. Students who lived off campus did not receive flyers, but may have seen the posters on campus. Additionally, the 25% of the student population that was studying abroad were not included because I was not able to obtain permission from the campus administration to send them electronic mail to advertise the survey. The final sample included 64 female Japanese students at Akita International University in Japan between the ages of 18 and 24. All students knew and spoke English frequently. For their majors, 27% studied global business, 42% studied global studies, 27% were undecided, 3% studied English for Academic Purposes, and 2% studied Japanese teaching. Forty-one percent were first years, 23% were second years, 13% were third years, 20% were fourth years, and 3% were fifth years. 48% came from rural areas, 36% came from suburban areas, and 14% came from urban areas. Two percent stated that their family income per year was less than 300,000 yen ($33,000), 23% said 300,001-600,000 yen ($33,001-$66,000), 20% said 600,001-900,000 yen ($66,001-$99,000), 47% said more than 900,000 yen ($99,001), and 8% did not answer. Materials Kawaii Scale. I created the Kawaii Scale, as currently there are no other available scales in English which assess opinions on different aspects of kawaii. It is based on the extensive literature review, as well as measurement theory written by Leary (2004). It consists of 50 Exploring Kawaii 21 questions. Topics cover self-rated kawaii, the importance of looking and acting kawaii, kawaii characteristics, and priorities for the future. Self-rated kawaii is based on the answer to the statement “Overall I am kawaii.” Importance of looking kawaii is based on the statement “It is very important to me to look kawaii.” Importance of acting kawaii is based on the statement “It is very important to me to act kawaii.” Reponses were formatted on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (always true). Kawaii characteristics refer to how important certain items-- voice, mannerisms and behaviors, makeup, saying kawaii things, clothing, hair, accessories, shoes, and belongings—are in order for a person to be kawaii. No examples for the characteristics were provided so that participants could be asked about their own definitions of these characteristics during the interviews. Responses were again formatted on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not important at all) to 7 (very important). During the follow-up interviews, participants were asked what they would change about the survey if they wanted to use it to conduct their own study of kawaii. Out of 18 interviewed students, 44% wanted to remove food, 33% students wanted to remove thoughts, and 33% wanted to remove smell. These items were not used in the final analysis. Additionally, 56% of the interviewed participants indicated that they were confused about how mannerisms and behaviors were different from one another. Thus, the mannerisms and behaviors scores were combined and averaged for the final analysis. Importance of having children is indicated by “My highest priority for the future is to have children,” and importance of career is indicated by “My highest priority for the future is to Exploring Kawaii 22 work full-time in a career.” Responses are again formatted on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not true at all) to 7 (always true). The Kawaii Scale has face validity, as demonstrated by the extensive literature review. Additionally, the changes to the kawaii characteristics sub-scale after conducting the interviews reflects an effort to increase the face validity of the measure based on participant responses. With these changes, Crononbach’s alpha (an estimate of inter-item reliability) for the kawaii characteristics sub-scale was .89. The reliability for the other sub-scales was not measured because they were either only 1-item, or were designed so that the responses could be compared with one another rather than summed to measure a certain construct. Modified Bem Sex Role Inventory. The Bem Sex Role Inventory measures masculinity and femininity based on self-reports of traits which are stereotyped as either masculine or feminine in society. It consists of 60 questions with responses formatted on a 10-point scale ranging from 1 (never or almost never true) to 10 (always or almost always true). It is divided into three subscales: masculine characteristics, feminine characteristics, and neutral characteristics. Masculine traits include athletic, analytical, and ambitious. Feminine traits include childlike, compassionate, and shy. Neutral traits include adaptable, conceited, and conscientious. Neutral traits are not calculated into either the masculinity or femininity score, and are used as filler. For this study, the coding of the BSRI was modified slightly to be more valid in a Japanese sample. These changes were based on the suggestions made by Katsurada and Sugihara (1999) in their study on the validity of the BSRI in Japanese samples. Using a sample of 256 Japanese college students, they found that some traits which had been considered neutral were scored as significantly more desirable for Japanese men or women than traits which had been Exploring Kawaii 23 considered feminine or masculine. Thus, the neutral traits “warm,” “happy,” and “tactful,” were changed to be coded as feminine traits. The feminine traits “sensitive to the needs of others,” “understanding,” and “loyal,” were changed to be coded as neutral. The neutral trait “individualistic” was changed to be coded as masculine, and the masculine trait “reliable” was changed to be coded as neutral. On the measure itself, the traits switched actual positions within the question order. Crononbach’s alpha was .87 for masculinity, and .82 for femininity. There were no studies which used the suggested changes to the BSRI to which these scores could be compared. However these reliabilities were higher than the .79 reliability for masculinity and .76 for femininity in Kutsarada and Sugihara’s 1999 study which used the unaltered BSRI. Procedure Prior to participation, participants were given an online consent form which presented the nature and purpose of the study to the students. Participants were then asked whether they gave informed consent. This indicated that they understood the purpose of the study, that they could stop filling out the survey at any time, and that they would receive contact information for any questions or concerns. The participating students were then asked two questions to make sure that they were Japanese females. Next, they answered several demographic questions including age, religion, family income, class status, and major. Students then completed the Kawaii Scale and the Bem Sex Role Inventory. After they finished the surveys, participants were provided with a debriefing statement that thanked them for their participation and gave them my contact information. Students were asked to provide their student ID number so that they could be entered into a raffle for a 3000 yen VISA gift card in exchange for their participation. Students were further asked if they would be willing to be interviewed in exchange for a chance to win Exploring Kawaii 24 another 3000 yen prepaid gift card. After they indicated yes or no, they were again thanked for their participation. Study 2 This was a qualitative study which used semi-structured interviews to explore what kawaii meant to the participants. Items included follow-up questions to the participants’ answers on the survey. This study also explored whether kawaii was useful to the participants in ways predicted by previous research. Participants Students who indicated on the survey that they would be willing to be interviewed were contacted via email and asked what time would be best for them to meet. Twenty-one (33%) of the participants from Study 1 responded and were interviewed. However, one recorded interview was lost due to data corruption, one student answered only half of the questions, and one chose not to be recorded. These interviews were excluded from the final analysis, making the final sample size number 18 (28% of the sample from Study 1). Only 28% of participants from this sample were first years, as compared with 41% in Study 1. This may be because upperclassmen at this school have more experience speaking in English than first years and felt more comfortable being interviewed. Additionally, 61% came from rural areas, compared to 48% from Study 1. Aside from these differences, this sub-sample closely resembled the overall sample from Study 1. Materials The interview questions were developed as a complement to the survey questions (See Appendix B). Some questions were made independent of the survey, such as “Do you think there are different styles of kawaii?” Others were direct follow-ups to survey questions. For example Exploring Kawaii 25 “On the survey, you were given the statement ‘It is very important for me to act kawaii.’ You chose (number between 1-7). Can you please elaborate on why you chose that answer?” For the kawaii characteristics sub-scale, participants were asked what they might change or keep the same about the sub-scale, as well as for examples of the different characteristics. Procedure Most interviews took place in a private library meeting room, but one took place in the participant’s dormitory room. Each student was interviewed individually. Prior to participation, participants were given a consent form which presented the nature and purpose of the study to the students. I then summarized the consent form for the participant, emphasizing that there were no right or wrong answers and that they could choose not to answer any question that they did not feel comfortable answering. Participants were then given time to look over the form. They were given two places to sign: one if they consented to be interviewed, and one if they consented to be recorded. After signing, participants were interviewed for thirty minutes to three hours, with most interviews ending after one hour. The participants who were interviewed for longer than an hour were asked whether they wanted to continue the interview, schedule another time to finish the interview, or end the interview. After the interview was over, participants were debriefed and asked whether they had any remaining questions. In exchange for participation, students received a chance to win a second 3000 yen ($33) VISA gift card. Design and Analysis To analyze the interviews, I used constant comparison analysis (Leech & Onwuegbuzie, 2007). Constant comparison analysis refers to categorizing the data into meaningful sections, and labeling each section with a label called a “code.” The codes are then compared with one another so that they can be grouped together or combined. These codes can either be predetermined Exploring Kawaii 26 (deductive) or emerge from the data (inductive). Finally, the codes are organized under broader themes. The interviews are then examined to see whether or not each code applies to them, and then tallied up to see how many interviews are matched with each code. The codes for this study were both deductively and inductively determined. For example, many of the characteristics codes were obtained deductively directly from the survey, such as voice, mannerisms and behavior, hair, clothing, makeup, accessories, shoes, belongings, and saying kawaii things. However, the sub-codes were determined inductively from the data. For example, I coded sections from each interview related to voice and then compiled them. After several careful readings new codes, such as “natural,” were developed when an idea was expressed in at least 2 of the 18 interviews. Additionally, some kawaii characteristics, such as body traits, did not belong in any of the categories from the survey, and so became completely new codes. Another inductive method was writing notes at the end of each interview about the overall themes presented by the participant. Some of these were later transformed into parts of the coding scheme if they seemed common across the different interviews. Finally, some codes were deductively obtained from the interview questions. For example the codes “It is possible to be feminine without being kawaii,” and “It is not possible to be feminine without being kawaii” were taken directly from the interview question “Do you think that it is possible to be feminine without being kawaii?” However, these were also supplemented by an inductive code-- “All women are kawaii in some way.” I then coded each interview by checking whether or not each code applied to the interview (see tables 1 and 2). For each code, the interview could be coded as “yes” meaning that the code applies to the interview, or “no” meaning that the code does not apply to the interview. Exploring Kawaii 27 For example, if a high-pitched voice was mentioned in the interview, then that interview could be coded “yes” for “high-pitched voice.” To test for inter-coder reliability, I hired a secondary-coder. First, I trained the secondarycoder by giving her the literature review, discussing Japanese culture, defining Japanese words commonly used in the interviews, and explaining how to use the coding scheme. The secondarycoder and I then independently coded two practice interviews. When we came back together, we looked for differences between how we had coded the interviews. We then discussed each difference, determined why we had disagreed, and modified the coding scheme for clarity. Afterwards, we independently coded two more practice interviews, discussed their differences, and revised the coding scheme. These practice interviews served to improve the definitions of the codes and increase inter-coder reliability. Finally, we coded 5 interviews (28% of the total number of interviews) to calculate inter-coder reliability. Coding a percentage of interviews greater than 10% is an acceptable way to calculate reliability (Lombard, Snyder-Duch, & Bracken, 2002). The Cohen’s kappa for these five interviews was .868 and the percent agreement was 93%. Results The hypothesis that there would be a negative correlation between masculinity and selfrated kawaii was not supported. A Pearson’s correlation for the data revealed that the relationship between self-rated kawaii (M = 3.68, SD = .82) and masculinity (M = 4.34, SD = .68) was not significant, r = .119, n = 64, p = .357 (two-tailed). The hypothesis that there would be a positive correlation between femininity and selfrated kawaii was also not supported. A Pearson’s correlation also found that the relationship between self-rated kawaii and femininity (M = 2.75, SD = 1.57) was not significant, r = .186, n Exploring Kawaii 28 = 64, p = .147 (two-tailed). This indicates that kawaii and feminine personality traits are not related. The qualitative data partially supports this finding. When asked “Is it possible to be feminine without being kawaii?” 55.6% of women said yes, while only 11.1% said no (see table 1). Participants who thought that women could be feminine without being kawaii stated that women could have feminine non-kawaii traits, such as being able to sit in “seiza,” which is method of sitting on one’s knees that requires practice to be able to do for long periods of time. Others said that women could be feminine by being “kireii (beautiful),” “kakkoii (cool),” or “sexy.” The concepts of “kireii” and “kakoii” are explored more in the next section. However, 22.2% of interviewed participants were not able to decide whether it was possible to be feminine without being kawaii because they could not imagine a woman who was not kawaii. They believed that all women are kawaii in at least some aspect, including in personality. Kawaii as a Norm When explicitly asked, 100% of interviewed participants stated that most young women want to be kawaii (see table 1). Additionally, even though there was no corresponding interview question, 77.9% of participants stated that kawaii meant being attractive or liked by others, and 61.6% thought that being called kawaii by someone else was a compliment. These results indicate that kawaii is positive and highly desired. However, at the same time, when explicitly asked, 72.2% of interviewed participants stated that it is not strange to not want to be kawaii, while only 11.1% stated that it is strange to not want to be kawaii. These results indicate that even though most women thought that being kawaii is desirable, they did not show any malice towards those who chose not to be kawaii. The percentage of women who believed that not wanting to be kawaii is not strange probably might Exploring Kawaii 29 have been even higher if I had explicitly asked every participant. However, several participants indicated that they did not want to be kawaii, and I avoided the question about strangeness so that these participants would not feel awkward. Two thirds of interviewed participants discussed socially acceptable alternatives to being kawaii, such as “kakkoii” (cool) and “kireii” (beautiful). Participants described kakkoii as looking more “boyish” and “cool” with “short hair.” They were also described as being “like a man,” “opinionated,” and “smart.” Participants described kireii as being more like “a woman” whereas kawaii was “like a girl.” Examples for kireii extended to appearance “not so much makeup,” “tall,” “matured,” “straight hair,” “gorgeous,” longer skirts than kawaii skirts, and neutral colors like “black,” “white,” and “brown.” Examples outside of appearance included “independent,” “smart,” “sexy,” “reliable,” “good at everything,” “skillful,” “grown-up,” and “respectable.” At times, kawaii, kireii, and kakkoii were described as mutually exclusive, but at other times they were described being able to coincide. One participant stated “I think that it’s easier to be looked as a kakkoii and kireii at the same time more than [it is to be] kawaii and kakkoii [at the same time].” Additionally, distinctions were sometimes made between appearance and personality. For example, one person said that it was possible to act kakkoii, but not possible to act kireii. Interestingly, 66.7% expressed that being kawaii is less common, important, or appropriate in middle aged women than in younger women. Some stated that kawaii is only appropriate up to a certain age, and then women should be kakkoii or kireii. “Until 23 kawaii is an advantage but maybe after 24 maybe females should be kireii.” “Kawaii sounds like they are not very reliable or independent, but [women in their] 30s, 40s, 50s are adult…so being kawaii in Exploring Kawaii 30 those ages…sounds like they are not adult yet…it’s more kireii.” This indicates that the normality of kawaii is related to age. Styles of Kawaii Most interviewed participants (72.2%) believed that there was more than one “kawaii style” (see table 2). Fifty percent stated that each person has her own style, 38.8% named kawaii styles with labels, and 27.8% stated that what is considered kawaii changes with the trend. Labeled kawaii styles included “boyish,” “girly,” “natural,” “garu,” “Lolita,” “morigiyaru,” “tsukematsuge,” and “office lady” (see table 2). None of these were coded because no style was described by more than one participant. It was unclear whether morigiyaru was the same or different from giyaru. Some stated that clothing from different styles could be mixed in an outfit, while others said that they could not. Likewise, some said that girls who wear different styles spend time together, while others said that girls who wear certain styles spend more time with each other. For “trend,” some participants asserted that the meaning of kawaii changes often. For example “this year short skirts or short pants were in trend, were in fashion…the [trend] goes away when the season ends.” Only 16.7% thought that there was only one kawaii style. Friedman’s test ranked the important of each characteristic for each surveyed participant, then averaged all of the ranks (see table 3). Because there were nine characteristics, the highest rank attainable would have been a nine and lowest would have been a one. Clothing, hair, accessories, and shoes all ranked above a 5, which made them most important. Makeup, belongings, voice, and saying kawaii things were ranked below a 5, which made them the least important. Mannerisms and behaviors were ranked 5.02, which made them more neutral. Exploring Kawaii 31 Table 4 indicates that most examples of characteristics could be categorized using the nine characteristics on the survey, but two more categories (“body” and “other”) were created for those that could not be easily categorized. Saying Kawaii Things Saying kawaii things received the lowest rank of those tested on Friedman’s test. Only 22.2% of interviewed participants gave an example. There was no overlap between their examples. Examples included “positive words,” saying “I want to be with you” to a male, making surprised noises like “kya,” using certain slang, and saying “neh” at the end of the sentence. In Japanese, saying “neh” and the end of a sentence is comparable to saying “right?” in English. It is a way to question one’s own statement and ask for agreement. This participant stated that it makes the statement “not strong.” Voice Kawaii voice received the second lowest rank on Friedman’s test. Voice was qualified as high-pitched during the analysis of the interviews because 100% of participants mentioned voice and described it as high pitched. Of these, 55.6% described intentionally high pitched voices, 44.4% described naturally high-pitched voices, and 16.7% compared the sound of the voice to a character on TV such as on anime. Most participants who discussed intentional voices cast them in a negative light, often providing them as an example of “acting” kawaii. Belongings Kawaii belongings received the third lowest rank on Friedman’s test. Though 44.4% of interviewed participants gave examples of belongings, the only common example was having characters, such as Minnie Mouse or Hello Kitty, on the belongings (16.7%). For example, “There are certain characters that they are selling and one of them is for example um the one Exploring Kawaii 32 from that movie, Marie, the cat (from the Aristocats). And then another one is Kitty-chan (Hello Kitty). And uh there are a little more for example Stitch (Lilo and Stitch). People think it’s kawaii and then people who like Stitch want to collect only Stitch memos and notes or pants and stuff or the stickers for example.” Other examples were “purse or wallet,” “pink,” “color and pictures, a pattern or flower.” Makeup Kawaii makeup received the fourth lowest rank on Friedman’s test. Of interviewed participants, 66.7% gave examples of kawaii makeup. Only pink (44.4%) and eyelashes (27.8%) were common descriptions. Pink (44.4%) sometimes referred to makeup in general “pink makeup,” and sometimes specific parts of the face, “Pink cheeks,” “pink lips,” “Eyelashes (27.8%) refer to a variety of descriptions, such as “curling eyelash,” “fake eyelash,” “expensive mascara,” “long eyelash,” “pink eye shadow.” Mannerisms and Behaviors Kawaii mannerisms and behaviors received the fifth lowest and highest rank on Friedman’s test, and 83.3% of interviewed participants gave examples. The first example was weak or dependent. Descriptions included “act a bit spoiled,” “if the cute girls are having heavy bags, they…pretend that ‘oh it is too heavy’ so that boys can help,” will not “kill the bug” by themselves, “they pretend like they can’t…open their cans and drinks…when they are in front of men,” and they “pretend to look a little stupid.” One participant gave the very poignant example “One of my friends, she’s really good at math…but with a guy if he knows a difficult answer, she can say “‘Segoi (awesome)! I didn’t know that! I didn’t know the answer, it’s really segoi!’” These responses indicated that being weak or dependent was a behavior rather than a personality trait. Exploring Kawaii 33 The second example was mistakes or “gap” between appearance and actions (22.2%). Some examples were simply mistakes: “For example, if you make a mistake with your Japanese, kawaii.” Others specifically mentioned that a mistake had to be unusual for the individual: “if there is someone who like usually works hard and does everything perfectly, sometimes like forgets a little something or makes some like mistakes we might call that kawaii.” The third example was “tenen” (22.2%). Tenen was described by several girls as a “word for the person who makes sometimes silly or stupid mistakes.” When asked for an example, one participant said “Maybe in like the extreme example, when we’re talking about studying…and the next girl says…‘What should we eat for dinner?’ or something. It’s completely off the topic. And we’ll say it’s tenen” Another person clarified that though tenen could be kawaii, it could also not be kawaii. A kawaii example would be “We were just talking about how final would be so hard and then one of my friends said ‘I want to eat cake.’” A not kawaii example would be “One of my friends wants to use bathroom during class and she suddenly stood up and asked the teacher in Japanese even though she was American “May I go to the bathroom?” I thought it was tenen but not kawaii.” The fourth example was walking toes pointed in or small steps (16.7%). For this one, all participants who mentioned this example physically demonstrated it for me. The fourth example was leaning on hands, with both elbows planted into the desk, arms bent towards the ceiling, both hands folded together, and the head resting on the hands (11.1%). The fifth example was bending the head down and looking up (11.1%): “It’s kind of hard if the girls is taller, but if the girls is shorter, instead of facing the guy straight, kind of look down and then only move your eyes.” The sixth example was cooking. “I think girls who can cook are kawaii.” Exploring Kawaii 34 Other examples included “some people cover their mouth with…one hand,” “the way a normal person would run is…they bend their elbows…but kawaii girls don’t bend them,” “touch[ing] the boy,” and putting the hair behind the ear with the opposite hand. Shoes Kawaii shoes received the fourth to highest rank on Friedman’s test. Though 61.1% of women gave an example of shoes, heels (33.3%) were the only commonality. Others included “pain[ful],” “bright color,” “furry,” and “round” toe area. Accessories Kawaii accessories received the third to highest rank on Friedman’s test. Though 44.4% of women gave an example of accessories, “small” was the only commonality and it was shared by only 11.1% of participants. Other descriptions included “color,” “these days the accessories look like sweets,” “hats,” and “no hats.” Hair Kawaii hair received the second to highest rank on Friedman’s test. Though 55.6% of participants gave examples of hair, long (22.2%) and curly (22.2%) were not agreed upon by everybody. For example, one person said “women always straight or curl,” implying that either straight or curly could be kawaii. Other examples included “bangs,” “dye,” the latest hairstyle worn by “Kimura Kaira,” and “perm.” One person said that it depended on the place in Japan: “people from Nagoya…there is a hair style called Nagoya roll.” Clothing Kawaii clothing received the highest rank on Friedman’s test. All but one person did not give an example of kawaii clothing. There were many common examples listed for kawaii clothing (skirt, pink, frill, lace, ribbon, one-piece, layers), but there were also many examples Exploring Kawaii 35 unique to each interview. These included the styles and trends described earlier. Descriptions which did not fall under socially accepted styles or trends included containing “small details” rather than “simple,” “childish elements,” “colorful,” “jacket,” “fluffy skirts.” Body Body was not a characteristic included in the survey, as the literature did not indicate that aspects about the body would be related to kawaii. However, throughout the coding process it became apparent that many participants (55.6%) were giving examples of natural aspects about the body excluding voice. These included big eyes (22.2%) and short height (27.8%). Other examples included “small hands, small feet,” “long nose,” “small face,” long legs,” “slender body,” “long eye-lashes,” and a “round-shaped” face. Other Three characteristics could not be categorized under any of the other headings. For example, smiling (50%) was often characterized as an important aspect of kawaii, but it was unclear whether this was a behavior or body characteristic. Some stated that it was important for the person to smile “a lot” or “always be smiling.” This characterizes it as a behavior. However, others said the person should have a “cute smile.” It was unclear whether the person naturally has a cute smile, or is intentionally trying to smile in a cute way. This ambiguity led to smile not being categorized as either a behavior or a body characteristic. Nails also were not part of the original characteristics survey, but 22.2% of participants mentioned them when asked what else they would add to the list. Examples included “pink,” “long,” “jewelry,” “paint.” Finally, Westerners were mentioned by two participants (11.1%). One stated that she associated Western girls with kawaii and then expanded “We Asians tend to think that white Exploring Kawaii 36 people are attractive. I don’t know why but we do.” The other stated that kawaii “has a little bit harmful aspects because many Japanese people want to be…not Asian…’cause Japanese people want white skin and light colored hair and bigger eyes so some women or girls took artificial plastic surgery.” Looking Versus Acting Kawaii As for whether looking or acting kawaii is more important, the Friedman’s test indicated that looking kawaii may be more important. Appearance characteristics such as clothing, hair, accessories, and shoes, were ranked as being more important than action characteristics such as saying kawaii things and mannerisms and behaviors. However, this was not consistent throughout the rankings; kawaii belongings and makeup were both ranked lower than mannerisms and behaviors. The interviews made it clear that “acting” kawaii can have a negative connotation. All but one thought that acting kawaii could be strange, fake, or negative (see table 1) as expressed by the statement “to act kawaii seems like pretend to be kawaii and it’s so annoying.” Some used the word “burikko,” meaning fake child. Additionally, when participants discussed kawaii behavior, they expressed the most annoyance with voice and weakness or dependence. Other kawaii behaviors, such as cooking, did not seem to evoke negative feelings. Self-Rated Kawaii and Priorities for the Future A Pearson’s correlation found that the relationship between self-rated kawaii and importance of having children (M = 3.41, SD= 1.97) was not significant, r = .059, n = 64, p = .645 (two-tailed). A second Pearson’s correlation also found that the relationship between selfrated kawaii and importance of having a career (M = 3.97, SD = 1.69) was not significant, r = Exploring Kawaii 37 .039, n = 64, p = .761 (two-tailed). These results indicate that self-rated kawaii has no significant relationship with these specific goals for the future. Discussion The most interesting finding from this study was that the hypotheses were not supported. There was no significant relationship between self-rated kawaii and masculinity or femininity. These results are puzzling because the definition of kawaii appears to exclude masculine personality traits. For example, the masculine traits “aggressive,” “athletic,” and “independent” were specifically mentioned as not being kawaii traits by interviewed participants. Additionally, some participants stated that they did not want to be kawaii because kawaii women could not be “reliable,” and “independent,” which are masculine traits. If it is truly the case that self-rated kawaii and masculine traits are not related, then this indicates that women who rate themselves as more kawaii are not at a disadvantage to women who rate themselves as less kawaii even though masculine traits are associated with success, especially in careers e.g., Matsui and Onglatco (1991) and Nezu and Nezu (1987). Additionally, there was no relationship between self-rated kawaii and femininity. Again, this is surprising because the definition of kawaii appears to include many feminine traits, including cheerful, childlike, gullible, yielding, and happy. Indeed, several of these traits were mentioned in the kawaii characteristics, such as “weak or dependent” and “smiles often or cutely.” However, interview results also indicated that more than half of women believed that it was possible to be feminine without being kawaii. Kireii, in particular, was described as being a more mature but still feminine alternative to being kawaii. Perhaps self-rated kawaii was not significantly correlated with feminine traits because kawaii alternatives also included feminine traits. Exploring Kawaii 38 Another possible reason for the discrepancies between the quantitative and qualitative results is that kawaii may be related more to appearance than to personality. By wearing kawaii fashion and by using kawaii mannerisms and behaviors, perhaps a woman can appear kawaii even though she is just as masculine or feminine as less kawaii women. For example, one participant mentioned that her friend, who is quite good at math, would pretend that she did not know answers to math questions when she was with males. Thus, she would appear to be yielding and dependent. If appearance is the most important determinant of self-rated kawaii, then self-rated kawaii may not indicate much about personality. An alternative explanation for the discrepancies is that the sample size for the survey was only 64 students, which is relatively small and makes it difficult to find relationships between variables. The effect size for the relationship between self-rated kawaii and gender was .119 for masculinity and .147 for femininity according to Pearson’s r, both of which are considered below the recommended minimum effect size of .2 (Ferguson, 2009). However, Spearman’s r revealed an effect size of .226 for femininity, which is above the recommended minimum effect size. These results indicate that additional research with a larger sample size may find a significant nonlinear relationship between self-rated kawaii and femininity as was hypothesized. However, the Spearman’s r effect size for masculinity was only a .052, indicating that even with a larger sample size it is unlikely that there would be a relationship between self-rated kawaii and masculinity. Kawaii and Priorities for the Future The fact that there was no relationship between self-rated kawaii and importance of having children or a career is consistent with the lack of relationship between self-rated kawaii and gender roles. Having children is considered a duty for females in Japan, while having a Exploring Kawaii 39 career is more unusual (Zielenziger, 2006). Even though kawaii is often characterized as being “girly” or “feminine,” it is not positively correlated with the more feminine goal of having children, or negatively with the more masculine goal of having a career. Thus, it is to the women’s benefit that masculinity, which is associated with variables related to career success, is not related to kawaii. Despite the lack of a relationship, perhaps kawaii women have an easier time pursuing the goal of having children than less kawaii women considering that kawaii makes women more attractive to males (McVeigh, 2000a). This is especially true in Japanese culture because couples rarely form unless both partners are very committed to the relationship (Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, Miyake, & Weisz, 2000), and society preaches that women’s main goal in life should be to raise children (Zielenziger, 2006). This is an idea which future research should explore. Mannerisms and Behaviors The most interesting finding related to characteristics was that, aside from voice and clothing, mannerisms and behaviors were the characteristics most often described by interviewed participants (see table 4). Weakness or dependence was by far the most common example, and was always described as a behavior rather than a personality trait. Many of the examples seemed to be dependent on whether or not there were males present. This makes intuitive sense, because Japanese males have expressed a preference for kawaii women over kireii women (McVeigh, 2000b; Sinclair, 2006). Kireii women intimidate them, whereas kawaii women help them feel in control. By acting weak or dependent, kawaii women can help men feel more in control, which makes the men more likely to like the kawaii women. The concept of weakness or dependence matches with the idea of kawaii in the literature as “inability to deal with every day life” (Kinsella, 1995, p. 238-239). Some participants described weakness and dependence as positive, Exploring Kawaii 40 while others described it as negative. It is possible to argue either way. It can be positive because women get men’s attention. It can be negative because it perpetuates stereotypes of women being weak and dependent, which could result in women receiving less responsibility. Examples of mistakes were more complex. Some participants discussed that simply making mistakes was kawaii, which ties in with being weak or dependent. However, others stated that it had to be unusual for the individual to make a mistake in order for the mistake to be kawaii. This is more complicated because it requires a high amount of ability (which is not a common kawaii characteristic) in combination with an individual mistake (which seems consistent with kawaii). This shows that even women who are normally capable and independent may be described as kawaii at certain moments. From these results, it was unclear from this study how to answer the research question “How important is it look kawaii versus act kawaii?” Clearly, Japanese women believe that acting kawaii can be negative, which adds a new dimension to previous research. Though the literature stated that males dislike women who are intentionally kawaii (McVeigh, 2000b), it did not say anything about women’s attitudes towards these women. Despite these negative views, engaging in kawaii behaviors and mannerisms may still be an important aspect of kawaii if done without intent. This is reflected by the fact that, even though many participants interpreted the question “How important is it to you to act kawaii?” negatively, mannerisms and behaviors were still ranked 5 out of 9 by the Friedman’s test. It is unclear how many women naturally engage in kawaii behaviors, and what the attitudes are towards these women. It is also unclear whether other men and women can always perceive when another woman is pretending to be kawaii. Exploring Kawaii 41 Body Characteristic and Western Features Another interesting finding was that even though aspects of the body were not described in the literature about kawaii, over half the interviewed participants gave examples of body characteristics (see table 4). Big eyes and short height were the most common. Both of these are consistent with the idea that kawaii is child-like because very young children are short and have eyes which are large in proportion to the rest of their face. These examples are interesting in that nobody can control their height or how large their eyes are naturally, and so having a kawaii body is predetermined by genetics. However, one participant specifically mentioned that some Japanese people get plastic surgery to be more kawaii. This was surprising because she also explicitly stated that kawaii could be harmful in that “Japanese people want to be…not Asian,” and that they want white skin and large eyes. Another participant word-associated Westerners with kawaii and stated that “Asians tend to think that white people are attractive.” The fact that some Western characteristics and kawaii features overlap, and that some Japanese get plastic surgery to attain those features, is disturbing. Though western perceptions of beauty have negatively impacted Japanese perceptions of beauty in the past, the literature has never directly connected kawaii with Western beauty. The first double eyelid surgery to make a Japanese woman look more western was documented in 1896 (Shirakabe, Kinugasa, Kawata, Kishimoto, & Shirakabe, 1985). It then thrived in popularity through much of the 20th century, but has been linked to negative effects such as the appearance of aging quickly and cultural backlash. It is possible, though unclear, that this is the type of surgery that the interviewed participant was referring to. As for other links to kawaii, extreme thinness has also become a defining characteristic of female beauty as a result of western influence (Kiriike, Nagata, Sirata, & Yamamoto, 1998). This Exploring Kawaii 42 has had the negative effect of a spike of eating disorders (Suematsu, Ishikawa, Kuboki, & Ito, 1985) and girls thinking of themselves as “fat” or “too fat” (Kaneko, et al., 1999). Also, traditionally kimonos had been designed specifically to flatten breasts and bring special attention to the nape of the neck (Miller, 2006). However, after World War II large breasts were considered beautiful. As a result of these changes, small almost prepubescent bodies with large breasts were celebrated in mainstream media rather than medium bodies with straight figures (Miller, 2006). Though this body-type was not explicitly mentioned during the interviews, kawaii relates to being child-like, which is illustrated by the small thin bodies, and attractive to others, which is embodied by the large breasts. Thus, this represents one possible way that western influence may be related to kawaii. Clothing and Styles Clothing was ranked the most important characteristic of kawaii. Most interviewed participants believed that are many different styles of kawaii. However, the most common descriptors of kawaii clothing were skirt, pink, frill, lace, ribbon, one-piece (like a dress), and layers. Additionally, only 27.8% of participants mentioned that what is considered kawaii changes with the trend. Literature about kawaii fashion focuses heavily on how quickly trends change, and how different these trends are from one another (Drake, 2001; Kinsella, 1995; “The tribes of Tokyo,” 2006; Zaun, 2003). Perhaps pink, frill, lace, etc. are simply the latest kawaii trend. However, these same characteristics have appeared in kawaii fashion at other points in time (Kinsella, 1995; “The tribes of Tokyo,” 2006). Alternatively, perhaps there are certain characteristics of kawaii fashion that never stop being considered kawaii or that most people consider kawaii, even while other types of fashion change or receive mixed reviews. This would explain why some participants thought that there was only one kawaii style, while others named Exploring Kawaii 43 kawaii styles with specific labels, such as garu and Lolita, and why. Another 50% of participants stated that each person has his or her own style, which indicates that kawaii has something to do with personal taste. All of these results indicate that different people have different conceptions about kawaii style. One person wrapped several of these ideas together, stating “When I think about the ‘cute’ fashion, if I don’t have to think about like if I’m going to wear it or not, my image of cute fashion would be like pink, kind of girlish clothes…so there’s this image [but] the clothes that I would like to wear…[are] simple clothes.” When asked if she thought the simple clothes were kawaii, she said yes. Her image of kawaii and her own style of kawaii were different, indicating that perceptions of kawaii fashion can be complex even within each individual. Other Characteristics Almost 75% of the interviewed participants mentioned or consulted magazines during the interviews, indicating that many ideas about kawaii characteristics may come from magazines. This is supported by the literature, which claims that high school girls consult at least five fashion magazines every month (Drake, 2001). The current results indicate that even when girls reach college, they continue to read fashion magazines. Saying kawaii things received a relatively low ranking compared with the other characteristics. Speaking with onomatopoeias, abbreviated expression, and slang such as “I like English xx2” instead of “I like English very much,” was observed by one professor from 1989 to 1998 (Akita, 2005). None of these descriptions mentioned during the interviews, and only 22.2% of interviewed participants could think of any example at all. Perhaps these examples of kawaii things were limited to the region where this professor taught, or became out of style after 1998. Exploring Kawaii 44 Voice received the next lowest ranking, but all participants gave the example of the highpitched voice as being part of kawaii. This is consistent with the literature (Bremner, 2002). However, many of these participants discussed high-pitched voice as a way of “acting” kawaii and cast it in a negative light. It is surprising that even though it was deemed less important than almost every other characteristic, all 18 participants thought of the same example and mentioned it. Perhaps the high pitched voice is a symbol of the burikko, fake child. Only 16.7% of participants mentioned cartoon characters in relation to belongings, which is surprising considering the amount of attention characters such as Hello Kitty have received in the literature (Katz & Sugiyama, 2006; Kinsella, 1995; B. McVeigh, 2000a). There are several possible reasons. First, perhaps these characters have received so much attention in Western magazines and essays because they are not as popular in Western culture. Second, perhaps people like to own kawaii belongings with characters on them, but these items are not essential in order for a person to be kawaii. However, characters were the only common description of belongings given by participants, which shows that characters still matter even if the importance of characters on belongings has been exaggerated in the literature. Descriptions of makeup and hair were more consistent in the interviews than they were in the literature. For example, one article describes girls with completely different types of makeup depending on what kawaii fashion the girl embraces ("The tribes of Tokyo," 2006). In contrast, some common elements in the interviews for makeup were pink and paying attention to the eyelashes. Common elements for the hair included long and curly. Thus kawaii hair and kawaii makeup did show some more codes in common than was expected, but the butterfly clips and barrettes mentioned in the literature were never mentioned by the participants. Perhaps they were part of a passing fad. Exploring Kawaii 45 Shoes and accessories were ranked relatively highly, but there were not many common descriptions for either. Additionally, some participants said that they would have combined shoes, accessories, and clothing into one category on the survey. In future surveys, perhaps shoes and accessories should be considered in conjunction with clothing. Smiles and nails were both mentioned as other kawaii characteristics, which was surprising because they were not mentioned in the literature. In the future, the difference between intentionally smiling and naturally having a kawaii smile should be explored. Nails should be considered a new kawaii characteristic. Kawaii as a Norm The interviews indicate that being kawaii is generally a positive and desired attribute, but that young women who do not want to be kawaii are not generally perceived as abnormal. Furthermore, young women can choose between alternatives to kawaii such as kireii and kakkoii. Thus, kawaii symbolizes childhood while kireii and kakkoii symbolize adulthood. Many participants stated that as the female enters adulthood kawaii may become less appropriate. This is supported by the literature (McVeigh, 2000a). However, unlike the literature the participants mentioned kireii and kakkoii as traits that older women should embody. However, the literature indicates that Japanese men prefer kawaii women over kireii women because they find kawaii women less intimidating (Sinclair, 2006). This may put kireii women at a disadvantage when trying to find romantic partners. Further research should be conducted on how to define kireii and kakkoii, and how broadly women who describe themselves with these words are accepted by differently aged men and women. Limitations and Confounds Exploring Kawaii 46 One limitation for this study was the small sample size and the methods used to recruit participants. Only 64 students completed the surveys. Additionally, due to initial low response rates, I used snowball sampling methods to recruit participants from classes in computer labs and certain clubs. These methods represent another potential confound because the sample was not completely random. However, it should be noted that at least 18% of the approximately 450 female Japanese degree-seeking students at AIU at the time completed the survey, and up to 25% attempted it. Unfortunately, approximately 110 of the upperclassmen were studying abroad and could not be contacted. Considering the fact that several participants stated that kawaii is less appropriate after a certain age, the large number of absent upper classmen may have impacted the ratings of the importance of kawaii and the opinions of kawaii expressed during the interviews. Another limitation was the language barrier. All of the literature reviewed was written in English. Even though some of these articles were written by Japanese citizens, many Japanese articles were likely excluded from review, which hindered the understanding of kawaii from a Japanese perspective. During the actual study, 22 students who started the survey did not fully complete it, possibly due to the difficulty of filling out a survey in English. Additionally, during the interviews some participants expressed that they did not understand certain words in English, but this was resolved through the use of Japanese-English dictionaries. Each participant had a different level of fluency in English, and the material that they were able to cover in an hour varied greatly by individual. As mentioned earlier, I did not ask all of the participants the questions “Is it strange not to want to be kawaii?” and “Is it possible to be feminine without being kawaii?” because some Exploring Kawaii 47 participants had indicated that they did not want to be kawaii and I did not want to make them feel awkward. However, this later made coding the interviews more difficult. Cultural bias may also have been a confound. Interpretivist qualitative researchers accept that reality is subjective (Ulin, Robinson, & Tolley, 2005). This is what motivates them to use qualitative methods to get a better understanding of other people’s realities and circumstances for their behaviors. Thus, they accept that researchers are not immune from having their own subjective realities. As such, I took extra care to be objective with the knowledge that I was doing research in a culture different from my own. However, my own experiences as an American woman may have shaped my views of kawaii to some extent. For example, conveying weakness was described as valuable by some participants, while strength is more admired in the United States. It was especially important throughout the course of this study not to assign positive or negative value to what the participants shared so that I could be open to a richer understanding of their perspectives. Strengths, Implications, and Future Research This was a mixed-methods study. The combination of quantitative and qualitative methods created a more cohesive understanding of kawaii as a whole. As an exploratory study, it also uncovered many potential directions for future research. The lack of a relationship of self-rated kawaii with gender roles and priorities for the future is the strongest argument against assuming that kawaii women are truly weak or less ambitious than other women. This finding was supported by the participants’ belief that many women pretend to be weak or dependent. This is the first study to search for a statistical relationship between self-rated kawaii and other variables. It adds important information to incorporate into theories about kawaii’s social implications for women. Future research could Exploring Kawaii 48 compare self-rated kawaii to variables such as school-performance or happiness. It would also be interesting to find out whether other women can always perceive when another woman is “pretending” to be kawaii, and whether there are women who naturally engage in kawaii behaviors. To my knowledge, this was also the first scientific attempt to create a scale about the characteristics of kawaii. As such, it had a relatively high reliability of .89 according to Crononbach’s alpha. Following up with interviews showed that some kawaii characteristics mentioned in the literature, such as food and thoughts, were not immediately understandable to college women. Others, such as short height and nails, were not in the literature but were mentioned often by participants. The interviews also helped define kawaii characteristics according to college women rather than according to the professors and reporters represented in the literature. Additionally, it was helpful to obtain current data on kawaii because the definition of kawaii changes quickly and much of the literature was dated. Future studies could improve this scale by adding new characteristics related to such as body, nails, and smile. They could also divide the scale into subscales such as appearance versus behavior, translating it into Japanese, and considering the impact of time on the scale’s validity. This study also added the concepts of kireii and kakkoii to research about kawaii. Although there have been many articles written about kawaii, few of these articles compare kawaii to kireii and kakkoii even though participants often mentioned these words during the interviews. A study which specifically compares the three different concepts could explore appearance and personality. Some participants differentiated between the two by stating that a person could have one type of appearance, such as a kawaii appearance, but another type of personality, such as a kireii personality. The study could also compare how gender roles relate to Exploring Kawaii 49 these concepts. For example, it would make sense that kakkoii women would embody more masculine traits than kawaii traits based on the descriptions of kakkoii as being more independent. Finally, research should be conducted into the various possible uses of kawaii which were described in the literature review. These were to escape adulthood (Kinsella, 1995), to manipulate relationships with authority figures (McVeigh, 2000b), to conform while expressing individuality (McVeigh, 2000a), and to attract men (McVeigh, 2000b). The implications of these uses for self-rated kawaii women, for women who do not want to be kawaii, and for women as a whole should be considered. Exploring Kawaii 50 Table 1 Themes and Codes for the Interviewed Sample (N = 18) Themes and codes Percent Kawaii and femininity Possible to be feminine without being kawaii All women are kawaii in some way Not possible to be feminine without being kawaii Kawaii as a norm Most young women want to be kawaii Not strange to not want to be kawaii Socially accepted kawaii alternatives Many things are called kawaii Strange to not want to be kawaii Being kawaii Acting kawaii can be strange, fake, or negative Being attractive or liked by others Mentions or consults magazines Less common, important, or appropriate in middle age women Compliment Kawaii styles More than one Each person has her own style Different kawaii styles with names attached Trends Only one 55.6 22.2 11.1 100 72.2 66.7 27.8 11.1 94.4 77.8 72.2 66.7 61.1 72.2 50 38.9 27.8 16.7 Table 2 Socially accepted styles and examples Style Boyish Girly Natural Garu Lolita Morgiyaru Giyaru Tsukematsuge Office Lady Example “jeans and jerseys or…backpacks...t-shirts…baseball cap” “pastel color or bright color and skirts or one-piece dress” “brown or white” “bright colors,” and “short bottoms” “showy clothing,” “frill,” and “bright colors,” avoid “pants” “laces,” “layers,” “long skirts” “sexy,” “similar to “Gothic Lolita” “fake eyelashes” “working lady” Exploring Kawaii 51 Table 3 Friedman ranked importance of kawaii characteristics (N = 64) Characteristic Mean SD Saying things Voice Belongings Makeup Mannerisms and behaviors Shoes Accessories Hair Clothing 3.80 3.98 4.60 4.55 4.75 4.92 5.00 5.27 5.44 1.83 1.70 1.58 1.89 1.46 1.63 1.61 1.58 1.44 Friedman Rank 3.37 3.42 4.65 4.77 5.02 5.42 5.72 6.15 6.48 Exploring Kawaii 52 Table 4 Characteristics of Kawaii Described by Interviewed Sample (N = 18) Themes and codes Percent Saying kawaii things High pitched voice Intentional Natural Like character on TV Belongings Characters Makeup Pink Eyelash Mannerisms and behaviors Weakness or dependence Mistakes or “gap” between appearance and action Tenen Walking with toes pointed in or small steps Leaning on hands Bend head down and look up with eyes Cooking Shoes Heels Accessories Small Hair Long Curl Clothing Skirt Pink Frill Lace Ribbon One-piece Layers Body (natural) Big eyes Short height Other Smile Nails Westerners 22.2 100 55.6 44.4 16.7 44.4 22.2 66.7 44.4 27.8 83.3 66.7 22.2 22.2 16.7 11.1 11.1 11.1 61.1 33.3 44.4 11.1 55.6 22.2 22.2 94.4 72.2 55.6 38.9 27.8 22.2 16.7 11.1 55.6 22.2 27.8 50 22.2 11.1 Exploring Kawaii 53 References Akita, K. (2005). Cuteness: The sexual commodification of women in the Japanese media. In T. Cirilli & J. Campbell (Eds.), Women and the media: Diverse perspectives (pp. 44-58). New York: University Press of America Inc. Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1979). Self-esteem as a function of masculinity in both sexes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(4), 783-785. Antill, J. K., & Cunningham, J. D. (1980). The relationship of masculinity, femininity, and androgyny to self-esteem. Australian Journal of Psychology, 32(3), 195-207. Bassoff, E. S., & Glass, G. V. (1982). The relationship between sex roles and mental health: A meta-analysis of twenty-six studies. The Counseling Psychologist, 10(4), 105-112. Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. Bland, T. (2007). Hello Kitty started out as an image for greeting cards and became much more. Ezine Retrieved June 18, 2009, from http://ezinearticles.com/?Hello-Kitty-Started-OutAs-an-Image-For-Greeting-Cards-and-Became-Much-More&id=744808 Bremner, B. (2002). In Japan, Cute Conquers All. Business Week Online. Retrieved June 12, 2009 from http://www.businessweek.com/bwdaily/dnflash/jun2002/nf20020625_7574.htm Central Intelligence Agency (2009). Japan. The World Factbook. Retrieved June 12, 2009 from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/geos/ja.html Drake, K. (2001). Kwest for Kawaii. Time Magazine. Retrieved June 12, 2009 from http://www.time.com/time/arts/article/0,8599,131022,00.html. Exploring Kawaii 54 Elpern, S., & Karp, S. A. (1984). Sex-role orientation and depressive symptomatology. Sex Roles, 10(11), 987-992. Fackler, M. (2007). Career women in Japan find a blocked path, despite equal opportunity law. New York Times (Late Edition East Coast).Retrieved June 12, 2009 from http://proquest.umi.com/pqdlink?did=1315462601&sid=1&Fmt=3&clientId=41159&RQ T=309&VName=PQD Feather, N. T. (1985). Masculinity, femininity, self-esteem, and subclinical depression. Sex Roles, 12(5), 491-500. Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532-538. Francesco, A. M., & Hakel, M. D. (1981). Gender and sex as determinants of hireability of applicants for gender-typed jobs. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5(5), 747-757. From the cradle to the toiletry market: Young women go gaga for baby products (November 27, 1995). The Nikkei Weekly. Retrieved on June 12, 2009 from http://www.lexisnexis.com/us/lnacademic/search/homesubmitForm.do Frühstück, S. (2007). Uneasy warriors: Gender, memory, and popular culture in the Japanese army. Los Angeles: University of California Press. Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115(3), 257-263. Green, B. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (1994). The attractiveness of gender-typed traits at different relationship levels: Androgynous characteristics may be desirable after all. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(3), 244-253. Exploring Kawaii 55 Guastello, D. D., & Guastello, S. J. (2003). Androgyny, gender role behavior, and emotional intelligence among college students and their parents. Sex Roles, 49(11), 663-673. Hirokawa, K., Yamada, F., Dohi, I., & Miyata, Y. (2001). Effect of gender-types on interpersonal stress measured by blink rate and questionnaires: Focusing on stereotypically sex-typed and androgynous types. Social Behavior and Personality, 29(4), 375-384. Hittner, J. B., & Daniels, J. R. (2002). Gender-role orientation, creative accomplishments and cognitive styles. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(1), 62-75. Ministry of Internal Affairs and Communications. Japan Statistical Yearbook 2009 (2009). Retrieved June 12, 2009 from http://www.stat.go.jp/english/data/nenkan/index.htm. Kaneko, K., Kiriike, N., Ikenaga, K., Miyawaki, D., Yamagami, S., & Kaneko, K. (1999). Weight and shape concerns and dieting behaviours among pre-adolescents and adolescents in Japan, Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences: Blackwell Publishing Limited. Katz, J. E., & Sugiyama, S. (2006). Mobile phones as fashion statements: Evidence from student surveys in the US and Japan. New Media & Society, 8(2), 321-337. Kinsella, S. (1995). Cuties in Japan. In L. Scov & B. Moeran (Eds.), Women, media, and consumption in Japan (pp. 220-254). Honolulu: The University of Hawaii Press. Kiriike, N., Nagata, T., Sirata, K., & Yamamoto, N. (1998). Are young women in Japan at high risk for eating disorders?: Decreased BMI in young females from 1960 to 1995. Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 52, 279-281. Krauss, M. (2001). Tech, teens challenging marketers in new ways. Marketing News, 35(9), 1010. Exploring Kawaii 56 Leech, N. L., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2007). An array of qualitative data analysis tools: A call for data analysis triangulation. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(4), 557-584. Lombard, M., Snyder-Duch, J., & Bracken, C. C. (2002). Content analysis in mass communication: Assessment and reporting of intercoder reliability. Human Communication Research, 28(4), 587-604. Marsh, H. W., & Byrne, B. M. (1991). Differentiated additive androgyny model: Relations between masculinity, femininity, and multiple dimensions of self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(5), 811-828. Matsui, T., Kakuyama, T., Konishi, H., Tsuzuki, Y., & Onglatco, M.-L. (1999). The influence of cognitive variables on the relationship between instrumentality and career orientation among Japanese college women. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54(3), 416-428. Matsui, T., Ohsawa, T., & Onglatco, M. L. (1991). Personality and career commitment among Japanese female clerical employees. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 38(3), 351-360. Matsui, T., & Onglatco, M. L. (1991). Instrumentality, expressiveness, and self-efficacy in career activities among Japanese working women. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 39(2), 241250. McVeigh, B. (2000). How Hello Kitty commodifies the cute, cool and camp: 'Consumutopia' versus 'control' in Japan. Journal of Material Culture, 5(2), 225-246. McVeigh, B. (2000). Wearing ideology and the "cult of cuteness." Wearing Ideology: State, Schooling, and Self-Presentation in Japan (pp. 135-156). New York: Oxford International Publishers Ltd. Miller, L. (2006). Beauty up: Exploring contemporary Japanese body aesthetics. Los Angeles: University of California Press. Exploring Kawaii 57 Nelson, L. (2006, August 11). A failing grade in cute. Metropolis: Japan's English Magazine. Retrieved June 12, 2009 from http://metropolis.co.jp/tokyo/646/lastword.asp Nezu, A. M., & Nezu, C. M. (1987). Psychological distress, problem solving, and coping reactions: Sex role differences. Sex Roles, 16(3), 205-214. Ono, A., & Sakayanagi, T. (1986). Influence of sex role self-concept and life fulfillment sentiment on vocational maturity. Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 45(1), 132-147. Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000). The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71(5), 1121-1142. Schoemer, K., & Chang, Y. (1995). The cult of cute. Newsweek, 126(9), 54. Shimonaka, Y., Nakazato, K., Kawaai, C., & Sato, S. (1997). Androgyny and successful adaptation across the life span among Japanese adults. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158(4), 389-400. Shirakabe, Y., Kinugasa, T., Kawata, M., Kishimoto, T., & Shirakabe, T. (1985). The doubleeyelid operation in Japan: Its evolution as related to cultural changes. Annals of Plastic Surgery(15), 224-241. Sinclair, J. (2006). Pink box: Inside Japan's sex clubs. New York: Abrams. Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 240-261. Suematsu, H., Ishikawa, H., Kuboki, T., & Ito, T. (1985). Statistical studies on anorexia nervosa in Japan: detailed clinical data on 1,011 patients. Psychotherapy And Psychosomatics, 43(2), 96-103. Exploring Kawaii 58 Sugihara, Y., & Katsurada, E. (2000). Gender-role personality traits in Japanese culture. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24(4), 309-318. Sugihara, Y., & Katsurada, E. (2002). Gender role development in Japanese culture: Diminishing gender role differences in a contemporary society. Sex Roles, 47(9/10), 443-452. Taylor, M. C., & Hall, J. A. (1982). Psychological androgyny: Theories, methods, and conclusions. Psychological Bulletin, 92(2), 347-366. The tribes of Tokyo (2006). WWD: Women's Wear Daily, 191(128), 36-37. Ulin, P. R., Robinson, E. T., & Tolley, E. E. (2005). Qualitative methods in public health: A field guide for applied research (First ed.). San Francisco: CA John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Whitley, B. E. (1983). Sex role orientation and self-esteem: A critical meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(4), 765-778. Zaun, T. (2003, August 5). For Japanese girls, uniforms are now too cool for school. Wall Street Journal (Eastern Edition)., p. A1. Zielenziger, M. (2006). Shutting out the sun: How Japan created its own lost generation. New York: Vintage Books. Exploring Kawaii 59 Appendix A Statement of Informed Consent You are invited to participate in the study entitled “Kawaii and Gender Roles.” We are performing research to get a better understanding of kawaii and personality traits. You will be asked to fill out two short surveys related to kawaii and personality traits. Your total participation time will be approximately 30 minutes. We do not foresee any risks to participating, although you may be uncomfortable answering some of the questions. You may refuse to answer any question that you do not wish to answer. You will receive the chance to win a ¥3000 VISA prepaid card for your participation in this research and your data will help me understand how kawaii and personality traits are related. Please make sure to give your student ID number at the end so that you can win the prize. Because only the investigator will have access to these surveys and your ID number, your answers to the survey questions will be confidential. No other person will ever connect your name or ID number with your answers. If you are interested in discussing the research further please contact Stephanie Klapper at [email protected]. If you have any questions regarding your rights as a participant in this study please contact the chair of the institutional review board at St. Mary’s College of Maryland, Roger Stanton at [email protected], or Department of Psychology, 18952 E. Fisher Rd., St. Mary’s City, MD 20686, United States of America. This project has been approved by the institutional review board. Your participation in this research is voluntary and you may choose not to participate at any time. Do you give your informed consent to participate in this study? Yes No Exploring Kawaii 60 Kawaii Scale Only complete this survey if you are female. There are no right or wrong answers. Are you Japanese? ☐ Yes ☐ No Are you a female? ☐ Yes ☐ No What is your age in years? _________ What is your religious affiliation? __________ How much income does your family earn every year? ☐ Less than ¥300,000 ☐ ¥300,001- ¥600,000 ☐ ¥600,001- ¥900,000 ☐ More than ¥ 900,000 What type of area are you from originally? ☐ Rural ☐ Suburban ☐ Urban What is your year in college? ☐ First-year ☐ Second-year ☐ Third-year ☐ Fourth-year College Major: ☐ Global business ☐ Global Studies- North America ☐ Global Studies- Mixed concentration ☐ Other ☐ Global Studies- East Asia ☐ Undecided ☐ Other Exploring Kawaii 61 On a scale of 1 to 7, rate how true the following statements are. 1 2 Not true at all 3 4 5 6 7 Always true It is very important to me to look kawaii. It is very important to be to act kawaii. Overall, I am kawaii. My friends are kawaii. Most college women want to be kawaii. I choose some times to be kawaii and other times not to be kawaii. I believe that males find me attractive. I believe that females find me attractive. My highest priority for the future is to have children. My highest priority for the future is work full-time in a career. After college, I would rather have a full-time job than a part-time job. I am feminine. I am masculine. I am excited to take on adult roles. On a scale of 1 to 7, rate the following characteristics on how important you think they are in order to be kawaii. Please answer according to your own definition of kawaii. It does not matter whether you actually want to be kawaii or not. 1 2 Not important at all Kawaii voice Kawaii mannerisms Kawaii makeup Eating kawaii food Saying kawaii things Kawaii clothing Kawaii hair Kawaii accessories Kawaii shoes Kawaii behaviors Kawaii belongings Kawaii smell Kawaii thoughts 3 4 5 6 7 Very important Exploring Kawaii 62 On a scale of 1 to 7, rate how well the following aspects of being kawaii describe you. 1 Not well at all 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very well Kawaii voice Kawaii mannerisms Kawaii makeup Eating kawaii food Saying kawaii things Kawaii clothing Kawaii hair Kawaii accessories Kawaii shoes Kawaii behaviors Kawaii belongings Kawaii smell Kawaii thoughts On a scale of 1 to 7, how appropriate is it to be kawaii in the following situations? 1 2 Never appropriate 3 4 5 6 7 Always appropriate Class Work Home with family With female friends With male friends In a group of male and female friends mixed together How much money do you spend on kawaii clothing every month? ☐ ¥0-3000 ☐ ¥9001-12000 ☐ Greater than ¥18000 ☐ ¥3001-6000 ☐ ¥12001-15000 ☐ ¥6001-9000 ☐ ¥15001-18000 How much do you spend on kawaii items that are not clothing every month (stationary, decorations, etc)? ☐ ¥0-3000 ☐ ¥9001-12000 ☐ Greater than ¥18000 ☐ ¥3001-6000 ☐ ¥12001-15000 ☐ ¥6001-9000 ☐ ¥15001-18000 Exploring Kawaii 63 How much money do you spend in total every month? _________ Are there other important aspects of being kawaii that are not mentioned here? _____ How comfortable do you feel reading and answering questions in English? 1 2 Not comfortable 3 4 5 6 7 Very comfortable Exploring Kawaii 64 Bem Sex Role Inventory On the next 6 pages, you will find listed a number of personality characteristics. On a scale from 1 to 7, please rate how well these characteristics describe yourself. Please indicate, on a scale from 1 to 7, how true of you each of these characteristics is. Please do not leave any characteristic unmarked. 1 Never or almost never true 2 Usually not true 3 4 Sometimes Occasionally but true infrequently true 1. Defend my own beliefs 2. Affectionate 3. Conscientious 4. Independent 5. Sympathetic 6. Moody 7. Assertive 8. Warm 9. Individualistic 10. Strong personality 11. Happy 12. Jealous 13. Forceful 14. Compassionate 15. Truthful 16. Have leadership abilities 17. Eager to soother hurt feelings 18. Secretive 19. Willing to take risks 20. Sensitive to the needs of others 21. Adaptable 22. Dominant 23. Tender 24. Conceited 25. Willing to take a stand 26. Love children 27. Loyal 28. Aggressive 29. Gentle 30. Conventional 31. Self-reliant 32. Yielding 5 Often true 6 Usually true 7 Always or almost always true 33. Helpful 34. Athletic 35. Cheerful 36. Unsystematic 37. Analytical 38. Shy 39. Inefficient 40. Make decisions easily 41. Flatterable 42. Theatrical 43. Self-sufficient 44. Tactful 45. Understanding 46. Reliable 47. Soft-spoken 48. Unpredictable 49. Masculine 50. Gullible 51. Solemn 52. Competitive 53. Childlike 54. Likable 55. Ambitious 56. Do not use harsh language 57. Sincere 58. Act as a leader 59. Feminine 60. Friendly Exploring Kawaii 65 DEBRIEF FORM You will receive the chance to win a ¥3000 yen prepaid VISA gift card for your participation in this survey. If you would like to enter the raffle, plase enter your student ID here. Thank you for participating in the study entitled “Kawaii and Gender Roles.” This study was created to get a better understanding of what “kawaii” means to women at Akita International University. This research will help determine what the most important components of kawaii are, in what situations it is appropriate to be kawaii, and how much money college women spend on being kawaii. It will also contribute to the research about the relationship between self-rated kawaii and gender roles. The next step of this project is a personal interview. If you choose to participate, you will receive the chance to win a second ¥3000 gift card to Aeon, with a higher probability of winning. You will be asked about kawaii in general and how kawaii relates to you. Your total participation time will be approximately 30 to 60 minutes. Because only the investigator will be with you at the interview, your answers to the interview questions will be confidential. No other person will ever connect your name with your answers. You may refuse to answer any question that you do not wish to answer. Participation in this study is voluntary. Are you willing to participate in a personal interview? ☐ Yes ☐ No Exploring Kawaii 66 Thank you If you have any questions about this research, contact Stephanie Klapper ([email protected]). If you have any questions regarding your rights as a participant in this study please contact the chair of the Institutional Review Board, Roger Stanton at [email protected]. Exploring Kawaii 67 Appendix B Interview Protocol Before the interview: Hello. Thank you for meeting me tonight/this afternoon/this morning. I’m really interested in the topic of kawaii and really appreciate that you are taking time out of your day to talk to me about it. This is a form which basically outlines how this interview will go. I will ask you about your opinions on kawaii and how kawaii relates to you. There are no right or wrong answers. I don’t have an opinion on kawaii so I will appreciate anything that you have to say on the topic. You can decide not to answer any question that you do not feel comfortable asking. You can stop participating at any time. The interview should last about 30 to 60 minutes. If you participate in the interview, you will receive the chance to win a second 3000 yen prepaid VISA gift card. With your permission, I will record this interview so that I can use your responses in the most effective way possible. Your identity will remain completely confidential and nobody but me will have access to the recording. Your name and the recording will never be connected. You can opt not to be recorded if you choose. I will also take some notes, but only to help me remember the interview better. I’ll give you time now to read the consent form and sign it if you agree to these conditions. Please let me know if you have any questions. *sign, sign* Here is your copy of this consent form. After the interview: Thank you for your participation in this study. *hand over debrief sheet* Do you have any questions about this research? If you have any additional questions, you can email me at my email address listed on the consent form. Exploring Kawaii 68 Consent Form You are invited to participate in the study entitled “Kawaii and Gender Roles.” We are performing research to get a better understanding of kawaii and personality traits. You will be asked about kawaii in general and how kawaii relates to you. Your total participation time will be approximately 30 to 60 minutes. We do not foresee any risks to participating, although you may be uncomfortable answering some of the questions. You may refuse to answer any question that you do not wish to answer. You will receive a chance to win a second ¥3000 Prepaid VISA gift card for your participation in this research and your data will help me have a better understanding of kawaii. With your permission, the interview will be digitally recorded. This is so that the interviewer will be able to use your responses in the most effective way possible. Your identity will remain completely confidential and nobody but the investigator will have access to the recording. You may opt not to have the interview recorded. If you are interested in discussing the research further please contact Stephanie Klapper at [email protected]. If you have any questions regarding your rights as a participant in this study please contact the chair of the institutional review board at St. Mary’s College of Maryland, Roger Stanton at [email protected], or Department of Psychology, 18952 E. Fisher Rd., St. Mary’s City, MD 20686, United States of America Participation in this study is voluntary. If you do not feel comfortable with the interview, you have the right to refuse participation or discontinue participation at any time without penalty. I agree to be interviewed: ________________________________ Participant’s Signature ______________ Date I agree to have this interview recorded: ________________________________ Participant’s Signature ________________________________ Investigator’s Signature ______________ Date _____________ Date Exploring Kawaii 69 Interview Questions What does “kawaii” mean to you?” How would you define it? What are five or ten words that you associate with the word “kawaii?” On the survey, you were given the statement “It is very important for me to look kawaii.” On a scale of 1-7 with 1 meaning not true at all and 7 meaning mostly true, you chose 6. Can you please elaborate on why you chose that answer? o What does it mean to you to “look” kawaii? On the survey, you were given the statement “It is very important for me to act kawaii.” You chose 4. Can you please elaborate on why you chose that answer? o What does it mean to you to “act” kawaii? Do you think that most young women want to be kawaii? Why/why not? o Is it strange to not want to be kawaii? o If a woman is not kawaii, can she still be feminine? Why/why not? What was your overall impression of this section [point at characteristics section] of the survey? What would you change or keep the same? Can you give examples for each of these components? ” I choose some times to be kawaii, and other times not to be kawaii.” You put a 7. Can you please elaborate on that? o Do you think that kawaii is a personality trait or a state of being? o On the survey, you rated how appropriate is it to be kawaii in certain situations. Can you elaborate on why you picked these answers? Are there different types of kawaii styles? o Are there strict guidelines for each style of kawaii, or is it possible to express some individuality in kawaii style? o Tell me about ero-kawaii or sexy kawaii. How is it different/similar to regular kawaii? What role does kawaii play in forming relationships with people in positions of authority, such as professors or bosses? o Overall, do you think that the idea of kawaii is more beneficial or more hurtful you as an individual? Why? How is it beneficial? How is it hurtful? o Is it more beneficial or more hurtful for all Japanese women as a group? How is it beneficial? How is it hurtful? Exploring Kawaii 70 What are your plans for life after college? o Do you want a temporary job or to start a life-long career? o Do you want to get married? How do you feel about adulthood/life after college? o Do you think of adulthood as a positive or a negative thing? o What role, if any, does kawaii play in adulthood? Extra questions if there is time: Do you think that kawaii style is different in different places? Urban versus Rural areas Akita versus other colleges Can males be kawaii? Exploring Kawaii 71 Debrief Thank you for participating in the study entitled “Kawaii and Gender Roles.” This study was created to get a better understanding of what “kawaii” means to women at Akita International University. This research will help define the most important components of kawaii and the appropriateness of kawaii in different situations. It will also contribute to research about the relationship between self-rated kawaii and gender roles.
© Copyright 2024