Estimating the Impact of Sales Representatives On the Average Revenue Per Customer∗ Alon Eizenberg Department of Economics, the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, and CEPR April 2015 Abstract Companies make extensive use of sales representatives with the aim of improving their ability to capture value from customers. Little empirical evidence, however, is available with regard to the impact of sales representatives on customer willingness-to-pay (WTP) and the firm’s revenues. This paper uses a unique transaction-level dataset from a company in the High Technology sector to shed light on this issue by studying the impact of interaction with sales representatives on the Average Revenue Per User (ARPU). A major challenge is that the interaction may be correlated with the customer’s unobserved, inherent willingness-to-pay for the product. The data, however, provide a natural exclusion restriction, motivating an instrumental variable strategy. Descriptive analysis indicates that interaction with a representative results in a higher incidence of longer subscription periods, consistent with institutional details confirmed by the company. Furthermore, naive regression results imply that, conditional on purchase, interaction with representatives substantially increases the mean revenue. These findings, however, are not robust to endogeneity concerns, and, indeed, instrumental variable estimates indicate that interaction actually lowers the mean revenue. An additional finding is that interaction with a sales representative has a negative effect on the probability of repeat purchase, conditional on an initial transaction taking place. These results indicate that sales representatives draw low-WTP customers into the customer pool. Sales representatives, therefore, have nontrivial effects on the distribution of preferences among the company’s customers, thus improving its ability to price discriminate. Keywords: Resource Allocation, Marketing, Sales force. ∗ Contact: [email protected]. I am grateful to a company in the High Technology sector for making their data available, and for their helpful feedback. I thank Saul Lach for helpful comments. Orry Kaz provided excellent research assistance. This work was supported by the Israeli Science Foundation (ISF) Grant #1338. 1 Introduction Consider a firm that offers a product (or a menu of differentiated products) for sale using two parallel channels. The first channel operates online: the firm’s products are available for direct purchase on its website by filling out a simple online form. This channel does not involve interaction with a sales representative. In the second channel, in contrast, the customer interacts with a sales representative (e.g., by phone, e-mail or an online chat) in order to learn more about the company’s offerings, and, potentially, make a purchase. This paper asks the following question: what is the impact of such direct communication on customers’ willingness-to-pay (hereafter WTP) and the company’s revenues? And, to what extent does such communication enhance the company’s ability to effectively price-discriminate? While many companies make substantial investments in call centers and in the training of sales representatives, empirical evidence on the effectiveness of this investment is scarce and mixed, and this paper wishes to make a modest step in the direction of filling that gap. Similarly as with advertising, direct communication (hereafter DC) between sales representatives and customers may have two main roles: informative (i.e., informing consumers of important aspects of the product), and persuasive (i.e., convincing the customer to make a purchase, or to choose a more advanced and expensive version of the product). This paper does not attempt to separate out the two effects, but rather aims at documenting the existence and magnitude of the total effect of DC, leaving its interpretation for future work. An important aspect of DC is that it creates a bi-directional information flow between the customer and the company. While the customer learns about the company’s offerings, the sales representative can use the communication to learn about the customer’s preferences and needs. Such information could be used to offer the customer a product that best fits their needs, or to effectively price-discriminate. For instance, the sales representative may tailor a discount or promotional offer based on her perception of the customer’s price sensitivity. The literature in economics and marketing has long recognized the important role played by such information flows (Jayachandran et al. 2006). Firms have also been increasingly aware of this issue, as evident by the large number of businesses that invest in Customer Relationship Management (CRM) technology, i.e., software that facilitates the process of collecting and analyzing customer-specific information. Such information allows the firm to identify the heterogeneous characteristics of its clients and to discriminate among them (Peppers et al. 1999). Implementing such an approach is often a complex and costly process, and evidence on its success are mixed: according to a study by the Gartner Group, 55% of all CRM projects fail to produce results (Rigby et al. 2002). An empirical analysis of the effectiveness of direct communication with customers may help us obtain a better understanding of the impact of such investments. This paper measures the effectiveness of direct communication with customers by utilizing 2 a unique dataset from a company that operates in the High Technology sector (hereafter “the Company”). The Company sells SaaS (software as a service) solutions to business customers. It targets two main customer segments: large (”Enterprise”) customers, and Small and Medium Business (”SMB”) customers. The empirical analysis in this paper focuses on SMB transactions.1 SMB customers visiting the Company’s website observed a menu of vertically-differentiated product versions. These customers could then choose their preferred product, and to complete the transaction online, without communicating with a representative. Alternatively, customers could choose to communicate with a sales representative. This paper offers an econometric analysis of the effect of DC on such transactions. The data allow me to track an individual customer over time, and, indeed, repeated customer purchases are an important aspect of the data. I therefore define the individual customer, and not the individual transaction, as the unit of analysis throughout most of the paper. I conduct regression analysis that investigates the impact of DC on the conditional expectation of consumer spending. This approach effectively focuses the analysis on the impact of DC on the Average Revenue Per User (ARPU). The ARPU statistic is widely used by practitioners (e.g., analysts) to evaluate company performance. It is, therefore, of interest to investigate the manner with which it is affected by interacting with a sales representative. Descriptive analysis reveals that DC is associated with a higher incidence of long subscription periods, consistent with institutional details confirmed by the Company: specifically, sales representatives received incentives to stir customers into longer subscription periods, and were allowed to use targeted promotional discounts for that purpose. Furthermore, a naive regression analysis demonstrates that DC has a positive, statistically as well as economically significant effect on the revenue extracted from the customer over the sample period. While these findings are instructive, they cannot credibly reveal a causal effect of DC on transaction values and revenue. The reason is that DC interaction is inherently endogenous: potential customers had the freedom to choose whether or not to engage in DC. If customers who were a-priori willing to pay higher prices for the Company’s product also displayed an above-average tendency to engage in DC (say, as a reflection of their heightened interest in the product), the positive relationship could be spurious rather than causal, leading us to overestimate the effectiveness of DC in extracting consumer surplus. Of note, this issue suggests an interesting parallel with the traditional endogeneity problem encountered in the study of the impact of advertising on sales (e.g. Schmalensee 1972, Berndt 1991, Bagwell 2003, Elberse and Anand 2005). In that literature, advertising is endogenous since it is correlated with unobserved demand shifters at the product level. In the current context, 1 “Enterprise” customers are handled via a specialized sales team, and tend to purchase rather expensive (and sometimes tailor-made) versions of the Company’s product. Of note, despite the labeling of the online channel as targeting “SMB” clients, in practice there was no effective mechanism that limited the access of large customers to online purchasing via this channel. 3 the interaction with a sales representative is endogenous since it is correlated with unobserved characteristics at the level of the individual customer. This paper contributes to the literature in two ways: first, it uses a unique dataset that allows one to observe, regarding each transaction, whether or not it involved communication with a sales representative. Such information is quite often not available to researchers (e.g., typical data from a retailer would often not report whether or not a customer shopping for clothes was assisted by a representative). Second, within the dataset available in this paper, an instrumental variable strategy is suggested to overcome the endogeneity problem. The identifying strategy is based on the fact that transactions are characterized by a “marketing channel” that is observed in the data. The marketing channel captures the manner by which the customer arrived at the website (for example, whether via a search engine, by clicking on a paid link, or via an affiliate website). The identifying assumption is that this technical information is not correlated with the customer’s unobserved willingness-to-pay. Probit regressions indicate that this variable does have a significant effect on the probability of engaging in interaction with a sales representative, a finding which is consistent with institutional details: in some cases, the interaction was initiated by a sales representative in response to the customer’s visit at the website (e.g., a download of a free version of the product) that caused the customers’ details to be recorded. This creates a correlation between the observed marketing channel and the endogenous DC communication variable, making the marketing channel an effective instrumental variable. In contrast to the naive regression results, the instrumental variable estimates suggest a negative and significant impact of DC on the revenue extracted from a customer, and this result is found to be robust across various specifications and sensitivity analyses. Interaction with a sales representative, therefore, decreases the average revenue per-customer, conditional on a purchase taking place. The unconditional impact of DC on extracted revenue, however, is not identified, since the data only informs us about transactions that actually took place. I interpret this finding as follows: sales representatives have two systematic effects on customer purchase decisions. First, conditional on purchase, they may increase the extracted revenue by inducing customers to purchase more advanced versions of the product, to engage in repeat purchases, or to purchase longer subscription periods. This positive effect on revenues is mitigated by the use of promotional discounts by the representatives, but may still be considered positive nonetheless. Second, sales representatives may systematically draw into the customer pool customers who would otherwise refrain from purchasing any of the firm’s offerings. Since these customers are likely to a-priori display a low willingness to pay for the product (e.g., they may have simply downloaded a free version without a real intent of making a purchase), they are likely to purchase cheap versions of the product, even after their interaction with the sales representative. The instrumental variable results indicate that the second effect is strong enough to offset 4 the first, creating an overall negative effect of DC on the average revenue. The naive regression that does not correct for endogeneity delivers the opposite result since it fails to account for the fact that some customers may choose to talk to a representative because of an unobserved high valuation for the product, which may exaggerate the first effect, causing an upward bias in the estimated coefficient on DC interaction. These findings indicate a nuanced effect of sales representatives on firm performance: while they can expand the company’s customer base, this expansion results in a lower ARPU measure. Since both statistics (customer base and ARPU) are closely tracked by analysts and are often referred to in discussions of a firm’s market value, these findings shed light on the impact of sales representatives on firm performance: it likely expands the customer base at the cost of reducing the per-customer average revenue. This insight may be important for managerial decisions aimed at improving specific aspects of firm performance. Another implication of this finding is that sales representatives increase the dispersion of willingness-to-pay in the firm’s customer population, reflecting expanded opportunities for price discrimination across various consumer segments. This nuanced view is further reinforced by an additional finding from the empirical analysis: conditional on an initial transaction taking place, interaction with a sales representative lowers the probability of repeat purchase by the customer. Once again, this result can be interpreted in light of the likely role of sales representatives in wooing low-WTP costumers to perform a transaction. The rest of the paper is organized as follows. Following a brief literature review, Section 2 describes the data used in this research, and institutional details that pertain to the Company. Section 3 presents estimation results, and Section 4 offers concluding remarks. Literature. A large body of literature addresses the evaluation of salesforce effectiveness and of its contribution to the organization’s success. Rich et al. (1999) tie this interest to the “...obvious link between sales performance and overall corporate revenue,” and explain that studying the effectiveness of the sales force can be a useful “shortcut” to understand many aspects of corporate success. One of the main practical tasks for this literature is to identify optimal sales force compensation schemes. Jackson et al. (1995) note that managers use a variety of strategies to evaluate salesforce performance, reporting that managers display a “continued reliance on qualitative measures” (such as the salesperson’s attitude or professional knowledge) alongside more quantitative indicators at the salesperson level, involving both output measures (number of accounts, sales and profits) and input measures such as the number of calls made (see also K¨ uster and Canales 2008). As Rich et al. further describe, consistently with the mix of measures used by managers, the sales management literature has also measured salesforce performance using a variety of research strategies, with roughly half the studies using subjective evaluations from managers, and the other half using objective data on indices such as sales volume, sales commissions or percent of 5 quota.2 The current paper contributes to this vast literature by offering a unique research design: first, I focus on transactions at the level of the individual customer, rather than at the level of an individual sales person. Second, I compare two parallel channels through which customers may make purchases, one being an “anonymous” online channel, and the second involving direct communication with a sales person. Finally, I offer an identification strategy that overcomes the endogeneity of communicating with a sales person. To the best of my knowledge, these features of the research design differentiate this paper from previous work in this area. This paper is also related to a strand of the empirical literature in economics that attempts to measure customer heterogeneity, reflected in preferences and willingness-to-pay (e.g., Berry, Carnall, and Spiller 1996). It is also related to a vast literature on price discrimination (e.g., Verboven 1996, Clerides 2002, Clerides 2004, Leslie (2004), Cohen 2008, Borzekowski, Thomadsen and Taragin 2009). 2 Data and institutional details The data cover the universe of 12,218 SMB transactions performed during eight months, January through August 2012. For each such transaction, we observe the package chosen (out of three vertically-differentiated packages), the length of the subscription (ranging from one month to twelve months), and the total amount paid. Crucially, we also observe whether the transaction involved direct communication with a sales team, or, alternatively, was performed online. DC occurs in 19.9% of all transactions.3 Each of the 12,218 observed transactions can be characterized along two dimensions. The first is the quality of the package chosen: the Company offered three levels of service, to which I refer as A, B and C (with A offering the highest value). More advanced packages are characterized by increased functionality of the offered services. The second dimension is the duration of the subscription to the services: one, three, six or twelve months.4 An important aspect of the data are repeated transactions by individual clients. Since each transaction is associated with a client ID code, it is possible to track an individual customer’s transactions over the eight sample months. The 12,218 transactions were generated by 3,550 unique clients, implying that an average 3.44 transactions per customer during the studied period. The data do not allow us to observe transactions that took place before or after the eight sample months (though subscriptions purchased prior to the sample period could clearly still be valid during it, and, similarly, observed subscriptions purchased during the sample period could be 2 This observation derives from meta-analysis in Churchill et al. (1985). A caveat is that, if a customer conversed with a sales team, but did not complete a transaction, and later performed the transaction online creating a new ID, the analysis is likely to miss the fact that DC occurred. Unfortunately, the data do not allow me to address this issue. 4 The data also contain very few transactions that do not fall into this classification. Six transactions are missing the plan description information, and an additional 268 transactions belong in two other small plans. 3 6 valid beyond it). This aspect of the data motivates the focus on the total amount of a client’s transactions during the sample period as the dependent variable of interest. The mean of these values across customers is the company’s ARPU over the eight months (from SMB transactions), and this study aims at identifying how this mean varies conditional on contact with a sales representative. Also motivating this focus is the fact that “DC status” (i.e., whether communication with a representative takes place or not) appears to be remarkably stable over different transactions of an individual customer. Of the 3,550 unique customers, only 35 (i.e., about 1%) of customers displayed a switch in the sense of performing transactions both with and without DC. All other customers either always communicate with a representative, or never do so. This fact makes it reasonable to treat DC status as fixed at the level of the individual customer. For internal consistency, the 35 customers who switched were removed from the analysis. Plans and pricing. Each combination of quality and duration (e.g., A3 which stands for a three-month subscription to the “A” package) is observed to be sold at different prices, reflecting promotions, and, possibly, the ability of the sales team to tailor a transaction to the WTP of a specific customer with whom they communicate. Table 1 describes the “modal” (in the sense of being most often observed) price for each quality-duration pair: Table 1: ”Modal Pricing,” US$ Quality/duration A B C 1 3 6 12 990 290 99 2,000 600 240 4,950 1,450 495 7,900 2,600 890 Negligible plans omitted. The nonlinearity of the modal price in the subscription length is evident: a three-month subscription, for example, costs less than three times as much as a one-month subscription in either the A, B or C packages. This is consistent with second-degree price discrimination. “Non-modal” prices are present in some 766 transactions, or 6.3% of the total 12,218 transactions. In 92 such transactions, price is lower than the modal price due to reimbursement for a previous subscription that has not been fully exploited. This was verified by calculating the discount, subtracting it from the modal price, and checking that this matches the actual price paid. For example, when we observe that a C1 transaction with the same client took place 27 days prior to purchasing another subscription, the refund for the four non-utilized days should be (4/31)*99.5 Some 184 additional “non-modal” cases are clear promotional discounts that are recurring (e.g., a 20$ discount over the modal price). 5 Since the transaction time within the day is not recorded, I considered the bounds of (3/31)*99 and (5/31)*99 in such cases. The 92 cases alluded to above are those in which the discount fell in between the bounds. 7 This leaves 766-92-184=490 cases where no clear explanation is available for the non-modal price. Some of these cases likely involve refunds for previous transactions that we do not observe (since they occurred prior to our sample period). Other cases may involve various promotions, and, finally, some of these values may be mistakes. The non-modal pricing is not more common in transactions that involve DC, such transactions comprising 46% of the total non-modal cases (with the remaining 54% cases pertaining to online transactions). Such prices cannot, therefore, be systematically attributed to negotiations with sales representatives over discounts. Following discussions with the Company, I determined that certain transaction prices are clear mistakes, and so I removed them from the regressions reported in the results section below (as explained in detail there). Non-modal prices are more frequent with regard to expensive transactions (e.g., purchases of the A package, or of 12-month subscriptions). For sensitivity checks, I also performed regressions in which all observations involving non-modal prices were removed from the sample, delivering similar results. Insights into the research question can be obtained by examining the distribution of transactions over the 12 plans, conditioning on whether the transaction involved contact with a sales team, or not. Table 2 reports the frequency of each plan within transactions involving such communication (top panel), and transactions not involving it (bottom panel), using the full sample of 12,218 transactions (again excluding certain negligible plans). The table conveys first that, regardless of DC status, plans B1 and C1 are by far the most popular. It further indicates a likely role for the sales team in directing customers into more expensive plans, and, in particular, ones involving a longer subscription period. Specifically, within a plan type (i.e., A, B or C) the fraction of transactions characterized by a one-month subscription is substantially higher for transactions not involving direct communication with a sales representative. Concretely, as the table shows, this fraction is 87.3%, 95.2% and 97.2% for A, B and C plans purchased without communication, respectively, whereas in transactions involving communication, these fractions are 34.2%, 72.2% and 88.9%, respectively. These descriptive patterns are consistent with institutional details. As confirmed by the Company, sales representatives were provided incentives to try and divert potential customers into longer subscription periods, and were able to use promotional discounts to achieve this goal. This fact bears several implications for the role played by the sales representatives. The mere fact that transactions involving DC resulted in longer subscriptions does not, in itself, imply a causal effect. Part of this effect could be spurious, in the sense that customers who, to begin with, were more likely to purchase longer subscriptions may have also been more prone to contact a representative. The formal regression analysis provided in the section below addresses this endogeneity problem via an instrumental variable strategy, allowing me to identify a causal effect. Second, to the extent that representatives are able to stir customers toward longer subscriptions, they may be achieving this goal via promotional discounts, thus offsetting some of the positive 8 effect that interaction with representatives has on the extracted revenue. The formal analysis below considers the overall revenue from a customer as a dependent variable in order to take this issue into account. Table 2: Plans purchased conditioning on contact with sales team Transactions with DC=1 Quality/duration A B C 1 3 6 12 Total % one-month 26 854 1,024 13 172 68 16 53 27 21 104 33 76 1,183 1,152 34.2 72.2 88.9 Transactions with DC=0 Quality/duration A B C 1 3 6 12 Total % one-month 89 3,067 6,037 0 25 33 8 77 80 5 53 59 102 3,222 6,209 87.3 95.2 97.2 Negligible plans omitted. Marketing channels and the exclusion restriction. Also observed in the data are variables that capture different marketing channels that describe the way in which the customer arrived at the company’s website at the point of initial contact. Those include “Paid” (customer arrived at the company’s website via a paid link or ad), “Direct” (customer arrived directly at website), “Affiliate” / “Referral” (customer was referred from another website via an affiliation agreement), “Organic” (customer arrived via a search engine) and “Campaigns.” Of note, this information is not available for about half of the transactions. Table 3 presents the number of transactions in each of these channels and indicates that the two most prominent channels are “Direct” and “Organic.” Importantly, these marketing channel variables pertain to the first value recorded for the customer. Therefore, they remain stable over the various transactions performed by an individual customer. The data also report a “latest marketing” variable which is typically, but not always, identical to the “first marketing” variable that is used in the analysis and discussed above. The table indicates that about half of the 12,218 transactions have a missing value for the marketing channel variable. Among those transactions with a non-missing channel, “Direct” and “Organic” are the most heavily represented channels. The average transaction values in these channels, as well as in “Referral” and in the “Missing” category, appear similar, suggesting no systematic effect of the marketing channel on transaction values. Several smaller channels, specifically, “Campaigns,” “Affiliate” and “Paid,” reflect higher mean transaction values. 9 Table 3: Marketing Channels Marketing Channel Affiliate Campaigns Direct Organic Paid Referral Missing # transactions Mean transaction value ($US) 211 45 3213 1852 247 518 6132 361.66 608.90 256.70 250.64 305.47 258.11 262.75 The identifying assumption, according to which the marketing channel can be excluded from an estimation equation that explains revenues, is therefore largely supported by the raw data. Just the same, I report below robustness checks in which dummy variables for the three channels “Campaigns,” “Affiliate” and “Paid” are not used as instrumental variables, to guard against the possibility that they are correlated with underlying consumer valuations. As shown below, the results are qualitatively robust to this issue. To serve as effective instruments for direct communication with a sales representative, the marketing channel should also be correlated with the endogenous variable, the DC dummy variable. Simple descriptive evidence is consistent with this hypothesis: 27% of observations with a non-missing marketing channel involve DC, compared to 12% of observations with a missing channel. This regularity also holds within each type of plan, as shown in Table 4 below: Table 4: Marketing channel and DC Communication by Plan Transaction type C1 C3 C6 C12 B1 B3 B6 B12 A1 A3 A6 A12 % DC With Channel % DC Without Channel 21.4% 74.6% 43.9% 63.0% 26.9% 89.3% 46.6% 70.4% 46.2% 100.0% 70.0% 85.7% 8.6% 50.0% 13.6% 24.6% 14.6% 78.9% 28.6% 62.8% 10.5% 0.0% 64.3% 78.9% Negligible plans (described above) omitted. The notion that having a recorded marketing channel increases the probability of DC is consistent with institutional details reviewed above. Specifically, a recorded channel is associated with 10 a greater ability and motivation of the sales team to track the customer and pursue interaction (say, following a free download from the company’s website). These results support the use of these channels as effective instruments for DC interaction. To provide an initial, descriptive outlook on this relationship, I perform a Probit analysis which results are provided in Table 5. The dependent variable is the DC dummy variable (standard errors in parentheses, *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1). This regression includes all 12,218 transactions. The table clearly shows that each of the dummy variables for the various marketing channels has a positive and significant effect on the dependent variable, i.e., they positively affect the probability of DC interaction. The constant term captures the omitted effect of a missing marketing channel, which is negative. In the formal regressions analysis offered below, the unit of observation would be the individual customer rather than the individual transaction, and the revenue garnered from the customer would be regressed on the DC variable via Two-Stage Least-Squares, using the marketing channels as instruments.6 Table 5: Probit Analysis Campaign 0.967*** (0.189) 0.579*** (0.0874) 0.587*** (0.0313) 0.793*** (0.0908) 0.524*** (0.0376) 0.504*** (0.0631) -1.164*** (0.0206) Paid Direct Affiliate Organic Referral Constant Observations 12,218 See text. 6 As reported there, a TSLS analysis is used with a linear first-stage, as opposed to the nonlinear first stage reflected in the Probit analysis, presented here for descriptive purposes only. This avoids the “forbidden regression” issue raised by Angrist and Pischke (2008). 11 3 Estimation results 3.1 Sales representatives’ effect on ARPU The empirical analysis is conducted at the level of the individual customer. The dependent variable is the total value ($US) of transactions signed with the client during the sample period. We shall refer to this value as the total revenue extracted from the customer during the sample period.7 Prior to computing this measure, I omit from the sample eight observations (five C1 transactions with values greater than 102$, and three A1 transactions with values in excess of 1,100$) that represent clear data errors. In addition, I also remove transactions by the 35 customers who “switched” between online transactions, and transactions that involve a sales representative due to the difficult-to-interpret status of DC for such customers, and the possibility that these switches are actually capturing data errors (see the discussion in Section 2 above). Following this removal of observations, I am left with 3,514 observations (relative to a total of 3,550 unique customers in the original dataset). This elimination of observations has no material impact on the results. The main explanatory variable is the dummy variable “DC”, taking the value 1 if the customer engaged in communication with a sales team, and 0 otherwise (also referred to as the “DC dummy variable”). The coefficient on this variable captures the effect of DC on the mean revenue extracted per customer, i.e., the ARPU. An additional explanatory variable of interest is a dummy variable taking the value 1 if the customer paid by wire transfer. Such transactions typically involve relatively large amounts, and customers who performed them always speak to a sales representative. In order to avoid confounding this issue with the impact of DC interaction, we control for this variable via an explanatory variable denoted wire max, taking the value 1 if the customer performed at least one transaction via wire transfer. Consistent with the discussion of endogeneity in the sections above, each regression is performed twice: via simple OLS, and using linear Two-Stage Least-Squares (TSLS) with the dummy variables for marketing channels serving as instruments for the DC variable. The results of this analysis are offered in Table 6. As columns (3) and (4) show, customers paying via wire transfer tend to conduct larger transactions. Irrespective of whether we control for this issue or not, the effect of the DC variable on ARPU is found to be positive in a simple OLS regression, but negative in the IV analysis (as shown by comparing column (1) to column (2), and column (3) to column (4)). Focusing on the third and fourth columns, the OLS estimate suggests that interacting with a sales team increases ARPU by 117.2$, but the IV results show quite the opposite, reporting that such interaction reduces ARPU by a sizable 764.2$. 7 In transactions involving multiple months, it is possible that payment is received in installments over time, and some of these payments could, therefore, only be received by the Company after the sample period. For simplicity, we ignore this issue and consider the total amount of transactions signed during the sample period as the total revenue obtained from that customer over the eight studied months. 12 Table 6: Regression analysis DC (1) (2) (3) (4) OLS1 TSLS1 OLS2 TSLS2 392.4*** (43.11) -1,508*** (288.0) 786.8*** (22.71) 1,314*** (83.46) 117.2*** (42.01) 2,484*** (109.0) 786.8*** (21.20) -764.2*** (189.7) 3,141*** (179.6) 1,011*** (52.05) 3,514 0.023 3,514 3,514 0.149 3,514 0.042 wire max Constant Observations R-squared Dependent variable is total revenue from transactions with the customer. Instrumental variables in the TSLS regressions are dummy variables for marketing channels. Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. We next consider the robustness of the qualitative pattern found in Table 6 to the choice of instrumental variables. Specifically, we wish to consider combinations of marketing channel dummy variables that exclude the channels which exclusion from the main regression could raise some concern following the discussion in Section 2 (namely, “Campaigns,” “Paid” and “Affiliate”). The results of this analysis are presented in Table 7. The negative impact of sales team interaction on ARPU is maintained, implying that the results are robust to issues associated with the choice of instrumental variables.8 To summarize, the regression analysis delivers the robust result that interaction with the Company’s sales team decreases the mean per-customer revenue. As discussed above, since this analysis is conditioned on the customer making purchases (otherwise we would not observe the customer), this result does not indicate that sales representatives do not perform well. Rather, it highlights a nuanced view of their contribution: sales representatives may be especially effective in drawing customers with a low inherent willingness-to-pay for the product into the customer pool. These customers tend to perform small transactions, and this offsets a likely positive effect that sales representatives have on the extracted value from customers with higher spending potential. 8 Consistent with Section 2, another robustness check performed involved estimating the model without the “non-modal” transactions, delivering similar results. Additional details on these sensitivity checks are available from the author upon request. 13 Table 7: Robustness to choice of IVs Direct Organic Referral Direct Referral Direct Organic DC wire max Constant Observations R-squared Organic Referral -1,004*** (217.9) 3,320*** (198.7) 1,072*** (58.98) -756.2*** (267.5) 3,135*** (228.0) 1,009*** (70.79) -989.8*** (241.1) 3,309*** (212.9) 1,069*** (64.55) -1,669*** (619.5) 3,816*** (479.4) 1,241*** (159.3) 3,514 3,514 0.044 3,514 3,514 Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. 3.2 Sales representatives’ effect on repeat purchase The results above motivate some additional exploration of the sales team’s impact. Specifically, conditional on an initial transaction taking place, does interaction with the sales team increase the probability of repeat purchase? The raw data indicate a plausible negative relationship between the extent of repeated transactions and DC communication. In particular, the fraction of transactions involving DC is decreasing with the total number of transactions, as conveyed by Table 8 below: Table 8: Repeated Transactions and DC Number of transactions DC=0 DC=1 DC=1 fraction 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 729 376 257 184 163 150 486 189 4 1 471 181 104 58 54 29 54 22 2 0 0.39 0.32 0.29 0.24 0.25 0.16 0.10 0.10 0.33 0.00 This descriptive evidence, however, is only suggestive of a negative relationship. First, it does not address endogeneity concerns. Second, it is derived from a sample which may be somewhat inefficient for the relevant purposes: for instance, a customer whose first transaction involved, say, a 12-month subscription cannot possibly make a repeat purchase during our observed sample period of eight months. Some restriction on the sample is, therefore, desirable. 14 I therefore proceed with formal regression analysis in which I restrict attention to 2,840 customers (of a total of 3,550, or 3,514 following omissions described above) whose first transaction involved a one-month subscription, i.e., it involved purchase of A1, B1 or a C1 package. The approach of focusing attention on customers whose first transaction involves a one-month subscription offers a practical, albeit imperfect, way of avoiding biases stemming from sample length. A limitation of this approach is that we cannot tell with certainty whether the customer’s observed first transaction is really the first transaction—this would not be the case had the customer performed a previous purchase prior to the observed sample period. Despite this limitation, restricting the sample in this way allows us to focus on the key question of interest. The results of this analysis are provided in Table 9. Columns (1)-(3) report regressions that use the total number of transactions with the customer as the dependent variable. This analysis is performed using OLS, Poisson regression and a TSLS regression with the same instrumental variables as in the ARPU analysis (i.e., marketing channel dummy variables). The use of the Poisson regression is appropriate since the dependent variable is a count variable, while the TSLS approach allows us to correct the results for endogeneity issues involving the explanatory variable, the DC dummy variable. Using all three different specifications, the effect of sales team interaction is found to be negative and statistically significant. That is, conditional on an initial (one-month) transaction taking place, the total number of transactions with a customer is lower when the customer interacts with a sales representative. This result holds with and without accounting for the endogeneity of that interaction. This result is consistent with the earlier ARPU results from Section 3.1 above, and suggests yet another aspect of the interaction with a sales representative. The likely interpretation is, again, that sales representatives are mostly effective in drawing lowwillingness-to-pay customers into the customer pool, and these customers are less likely to engage in repeat purchases over time relative to higher willingness-to-pay customers. The results in columns (1)-(3) are not entirely easy to interpret, however, for the following reason: imagine that, having performed an initial purchase of a one-month subscription, a customer is convinced by a sales representative to purchase an expensive 12-month subscription. Since the dependent variable merely counts transactions, it will take the value of 2, whereas for another customer, who made five consecutive purchases of one-month subscriptions, the dependent variable would take the value of 5, despite the fact that this is, in some sense, a less desirable outcome for the firm. Columns (4)-(5) of the table attempt to overcome this issue by considering a binary dependent variable, taking the value 1 if the costumer performed more than one transaction, and zero otherwise. This approach allows a clean analysis of a simple question: what is the effect of interacting with a sales representative on the probability of there being any repeat purchase by the customer? As the results in these columns indicate, this effect is, once again, negative, reaffirming the message delivered by the results in columns (1)-(3) and consistent with 15 Table 9: Repeat purchase analysis DC Constant (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) # transactions OLS -0.880*** (0.113) 4.098*** (0.0519) # transactions Poisson -0.242*** (0.0252) 1.411*** (0.0105) # transactions IV -8.954*** (0.996) 5.799*** (0.224) Multiple transactions OLS -0.0739*** (0.0196) 0.778*** (0.00901) Multiple transactions IV -0.808*** (0.126) 0.933*** (0.0283) 2,811 0.021 2,811 2,811 2,811 0.005 2,811 Observations R-squared Dependent variable in columns (1)-(3) is the total number of transactions with the client during the sample period. In columns (4)-(5), it is a dummy variable taking the value 1 if the customer performed more than one transaction, and zero otherwise. Standard errors in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. our overall analysis. 4 Concluding Remarks This paper studies an issue on which little empirical evidence exists to date: the impact of communicating with a sales representative on the revenue extracted from customers. The paper utilizes a unique dataset that records the incidence of such communication, allowing me to investigate its impact on the history of customer transactions relative to an alternative channel involving online transactions. The existence of two parallel channels, and the availability of transaction level data indicating whether communication with a representative occurred or not, therefore provides a unique opportunity to shed light on this issue. Another unique aspect of the data is the availability of a natural exclusion restriction, allowing me to address the difficult endogeneity problem embedded in the statistical exercise. The picture that emerges from the analysis is quite consistent across different empirical specifications: communication with a sales representative is associated with lower average revenue, and with lower client retention. My interpretation of this finding is that it does not reflect poor performance by the sales team. Quite to the contrary, it suggests that a major aspect of the sales team is its ability to attract customers with a low a-priori willingness to pay for the product. This extensive margin effect appears to dominate an intensive margin effect (i.e., increasing the willingness to pay of customers conditional on purchase) on both average revenues and repeat purchase behavior. Given the large investments in Customer Relationship Management throughout firms and sectors in the modern economy, it is important to provide evidence on the effectiveness and 16 consequences of sales teams. The extant literature offers mixed evidence on this issue, often based on surveys of managers. This suggests that many managers are skeptical about the returns on such investments. By providing statistical evidence on the actual impact of interaction with sales representatives, I hope to make a modest contribution to a better understanding of this important topic. A clear limitation of this work is its reliance on data from a single firm. Nonetheless, it is this specificity that allows me to take an in-depth view of transaction level data, and to develop an identification strategy that overcomes an important endogeneity problem. Future research should hopefully continue to explore this issue using additional data sources. References [1] Angrist, J., and J.S. Pischke, (2008), “Mostly Harmless Econometrics,” Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. [2] Bagwell, K, (2003), “The Economic Analysis of Advertising,” In Mark Armstrong and Rob Porter (eds.), Handbook of Industrial Organization, North-Holland: Amsterdam [3] Berndt E. R. (1991), “The Practice of Econometrics: Classic and Contemporary,” Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley [4] Berry, T.S., Carnall, M., and P.T. Spiller (1996), “Airline Hubs: Costs, Markups and the Implications of Customer Heterogeneity,” NBER Working Paper 5561, , National Bureau of Economic Research [5] Borzekowski, R., R. Thomadsen, and C. Taragin (2009), “Competition and Price Discrimination in the Market for Mailing Lists,” Quantitative Marketing and Economics, 7, 147-79 [6] Churchill, G.A., Ford, N.M., Hartley, S.W. and Walker, O.C. (1985): “The determinants of salesperson performance: a meta-analysis,” Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. XXII, May, pp. 103-18. [7] Clerides, S. (2002), “Book value: intertemporal pricing and quality discrimination in the US market for books,” Journal of Industrial Organization , 20, 1385-408 [8] Clerides, S. (2004), “Price discrimination with differentiated products: definition and identification,” Economic Inquiry, 42, 402-12 [9] Cohen, A. (2008), “Package size and price discrimination in the paper towel market,” International Journal of Industrial Organization, 26, 502-16 [10] Elberse, A., and B. Anand (2005): “Advertising and Expectations: The Effectiveness of PreRelease Advertising for Motion Pictures,” Harvard Business School Working Paper Series,, No. 05-060 [11] Jackson, D.W. Jr., Schlacter J.L., and W.G. Wolfe (1995): “Examining the Bases Utilized for Evaluating Salespeoples’ Performance,” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Manage17 [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] [17] [18] [19] ment, 15(4) pp. 57-65 Jayachandran, S., Sharma, S., Kaufman, P., and P. Raman(2005): “The Role of Relational Information Processes and Technology Use in Customer Relationship Management,” Journal of Marketing, 69(4): 177-192 K¨ uster, I. and P. Canales, (2008): “Some determinants of salesforce effectiveness,” Team Performance Management: An International Journal, 74(7/8). 296-326 Leslie, P. (2004), “Price Discrimination in Broadway Theatre,” Rand Journal of Economics, 35 (3), 520-41 Peppers, D., Rogers, M., and b. Dorf (1999), “Is Your Company Ready for One-to-One Marketing,” Harvard Business Review, 77 (1), 151-161 Rich, G.A., Bommer, W.H., MacKenzie, S.B., Podsakoff, P.M. and J.L. Johnson (1999): “Apples and Apples or Apples and Oranges? A Meta-Analysis of Objective and Subjective Measures of Salesperson Performance,” 19(4) pp. 41-52 Rigby, D.K., Reichheld, F., and P. Schefter (2002), “Avoid the Four Perils of CRM,” Harvard Business Review, 80 (2), 101-109 Schmalensee R. (1972), “The Economics of Advertising,” Amsterdam: North-Holland Verboven, F. (1996), “International price discrimination in the European car market,” Rand Journal of Economics, 27, 240-68 18
© Copyright 2024