Measuring subgoals of the sexual behavioral system: What is sex good for? Gurit E. Birnbaum Bar-Ilan University Omri Gillath University of California, Davis ABSTRACT In five studies, we created and validated a self-report measure of individual differences in sex behaviors and beliefs related to the pursuit of various subgoals of the sexual system. In a pilot study, we created an initial version, in Hebrew, of the Sexual Behavioral System Subgoals (SBSS) scale and determined its 4-factor structure. Study 1 confirmed the 4-factor structure of the SBSS: Relationship initiation; relationship maintenance; promoting frequent sexual activity; and a negative factor reflecting interference with the sexual system. Studies 2 and 3 provided additional evidence for the reliability and validity of the SBSS. In Study 4, American students completed an English translation of the SBSS. Confirmatory factor analysis provided cross-national evidence for the structure of the SBSS. KEY WORDS: attachment • evolutionary psychology • gender differences • romantic relationships • sex From an evolutionary perspective, the major function of the sexual behavioral system is to pass one’s genes to the next generation. However, impregnation is not sufficient for the survival of human offspring, who are vulnerable throughout an exceptionally prolonged development. Hence, it The authors wish to thank Phillip Shaver and Mario Mikulincer for their helpful comments. All correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gurit E. Birnbaum, PhD, Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, 52900, Israel [e-mail: [email protected]]. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships Copyright © 2006 SAGE Publications (www.sagepublications.com), Vol. 23(5): 675–701. DOI: 10.1177/0265407506065992 676 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) is generally beneficial for sexual partners to stay together long enough to maintain the pregnancy and care for offspring, thereby increasing the children’s chances of survival and eventual reproduction (H. E. Fisher, 1998; Hazan & Zeifman, 1994; Mellen, 1981). Accordingly, in the course of human evolution, selection pressures produced subordinate functional behaviors and psychological mechanisms regulated by the higher-order goal of reproduction (Buss & Kenrick, 1998). In line with Buss and Kenrick’s (1998) conceptualization of functionally adaptive psychological mechanisms, the subordinate goals of the sexual behavioral system should motivate functional emotions, cognitions, and behaviors, such as those that foster commitment between sexual partners. Research and theory in evolutionary psychology have focused mainly on species-typical adaptations, which have become an integral part of the neural hardware of all humans, but investigators have largely ignored individual differences in the emphasis placed on particular sexual-system subgoals and their associated psychological mechanisms (Buss, 2004). Still, individual differences in adaptive mechanisms and their manifestations can emerge from a variety of heritable and environmental sources (e.g., viable genetic subtypes whose relative frequency is maintained by dynamic equilibrium within a gene pool and cultural variations, within-family norms, and unique developmental experiences; Bailey, 1998; Buss & Greiling, 1999). Specifically, individual differences in the subgoals of the sexual behavioral system, which can stem from a combination of heritable and nonheritable sources, might be translated into corresponding individual differences in behavioral intentions and actual sexual behavior. The primary goal of these investigations is to devise a measurement of these individual differences. Most previous studies of individual differences in sexuality have examined one of four domains: Differences in sexual attitudes, such as the tendency to respond to sexual stimuli along a negative (erotophobia) to positive (erotophilia) dimension of affect and evaluation (e.g., Fisher, Byrne, White, & Kelley, 1988); differences in physiological aspects of sexuality, such as individual differences in sexual arousal (e.g., Chambless & Lifshitz, 1984); differences in sexual behavior, such as individual differences in the willingness to engage in sexual intercourse without relational commitment (e.g., Simpson & Gangestad, 1991); and differences in cognitive representations of sexuality, such as individual differences in sexual selfconcept (e.g., Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen, Cyranowski, & Espindle, 1999) or conceptions of sexual intercourse and its implications (Birnbaum, 2003; Birnbaum, Glaubman, & Mikulincer, 2001; Birnbaum & Laser-Brandt, 2002). Although measures of these domains are reliable, valid, and predict diverse sexual phenomena, they do not tap an evolutionarily based functional analysis of sexual goals and subgoals that may have evolved to solve specific problems of survival and reproduction. Birnbaum and Reis (in press) developed a self-report scale measuring women’s mental representations of the sexual aspect of romantic relationships. This scale was based on a functional-evolutionary analysis of the sexual behavioral system and included questions about the sexual Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 677 experiences, feelings, expectations, and beliefs about the self, the sexual partner, and sexual activity with the partner. Although this scale was designed to tap theoretically distinct components of women’s mental representations of the sexual domain, it is not suitable for men and does not include an essential motivational aspect of these representations – a person’s beliefs about the possible impact of sexual activity on personal and interpersonal well being. Specifically, the original scale assesses beliefs about the partner’s responsiveness to one’s sexual needs; level of excitement and arousal during sexual activities; one’s cognitive, emotional, and behavioral tendencies during these activities (e.g., sex-related shame, distancing from a partner); and beliefs about the extent to which sex contributes to relationship maintenance. Even though this last factor assesses the perceived relational impact of sexual activities, there are other potential personal and interpersonal consequences of sexual activities that are not assessed by the scale. Measuring a person’s beliefs about these rewards of sexual activities may be extremely important for understanding ambiguous findings in the literature concerning the link between sexual activities and relationship satisfaction (e.g., Hassebrauck & Fehr, 2002; Henderson-King & Veroff, 1994) and specifying the place occupied by sexuality in a person’s goal system. For example, a person who believes that sex contributes to relationship maintenance will be more satisfied in his or her relationship following sexual activity with a romantic partner than a person who believes that sex is irrelevant for relationship maintenance. The present research was designed to provide a research tool, suitable for both men and women, that assesses a person’s beliefs about the various personal and interpersonal rewards and costs of sexual activity and related behaviors from an evolutionary perspective. Based on a comprehensive review of the literature on the sexual behavioral system, we propose to assess four major subgoals of the sexual behavior system that may play a role in both men’s and women’s reproductive success in the context of long-term adult romantic relationships. The first subgoal is to bring partners together and establish a relationship by means of sexual activity. Sexual attraction presumably evolved to motivate individuals to have sex with available partners. Although sexual relations may occur without affectional bonding (Buss, 2004; Diamond, 2003; Fisher, Aron, Mashek, Li, & Brown, 2002), sexual activity may be one means of establishing a relationship which, if extended in time, might have benefits for both partners and, most importantly, for the survival of their offspring (Hazan & Zeifman, 1994). That is, successful human mating often depends, and depended during human evolution, on relationship formation, because without a relationship between the parents, their offspring might be much less likely to survive to reproductive age. Thus, one goal of sexual activity can be to create a relationship with someone that extends beyond a single sexual episode. The second subgoal of the sexual system is to keep partners attached to each other for an extended period of time. Several unusual characteristics of human reproductive physiology, anatomy, and behavior imply that the 678 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) sexual system fosters not only the formation of an emotional bond between sexual partners but also its maintenance. For example, female ovulation is concealed, whereas most mammalian species confine mating to a brief estrous period around the advertised time of ovulation. Humans may have sex on any day of the menstrual cycle and they generally do not know when the likelihood of pregnancy is greatest. A widely accepted explanation for the evolution of concealed ovulation (but not the only feasible one) is the ‘keep daddy-at-home’ hypothesis (Alexander & Noonan, 1979). This hypothesis argues that concealed ovulation increases the probability of offspring survival by inducing the male partner to stay ‘at home’ for at least two reasons: To have frequent sex with his partner, thereby increasing the chances of fertilization, and to guard against her impregnation by other males. In addition, the vaginal angle in human females has shifted in such a way as to make penetration easier in the mutually ventral position (Ford & Beach, 1951), which allows belly-to-belly contact and mutual gaze, as well as increasing the chances of female orgasm (Short, 1979). This most prevalent and preferred copulatory position, along with humans’ tendencies to have sex in private and to sleep together after intercourse (Ford & Beach, 1951), may have evolved because they also contributed to emotional bonding between sexual partners. This is consistent with empirical work showing an increase in serum oxytocin levels in both men and women during sexual intercourse and in the moments preceding orgasm. The hormone oxytocin, which is also secreted during lactation, was found to enhance maternal bonding in animal models, and is assumed to promote tactile contact, which itself contributes to attachment, as well as intensifying the pleasure of sexual arousal and orgasm (Carter, 1998). A third subgoal of the sexual system is to promote frequent sexual activity through increasing positive affect and sexual motivation. Sexual desire is among the strongest forces that motivate human behavior, and sexual gratification is one of the greatest human pleasures (Shaver, Hazan, & Bradshaw, 1988). This contention raises a key question: Why might sex have such potential for joy and pleasure? The likely explanation is that repeated sexual intercourse is often required for reproduction, and frequent intercourse is much more likely if it is highly pleasurable. That is, in a species with no overt cues that signal ovulation, neuroendocrine capacity enabling hedonic pleasure and sexual desire presumably evolved to reinforce and motivate sexual intercourse, thereby maximizing the probability of conception (Diamond, 1997; Gallup, 1986). The three subgoals of the sexual system mentioned earlier focus on the possible personal and interpersonal rewards of sexual activity that contribute to one’s reproductive success through the formation and/or maintenance of an emotional bond between sexual partners. Nevertheless, along with the potential for gratification and delight, human sexuality may sometimes be a source of negative feelings (e.g., Goldenberg, Pyszczynski, McCoy, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999), manifested in the form of erotophobic attitudes (Fisher et al., 1988) or negative themes in both men’s and women’s sexual experience (Birnbaum & Laser-Brandt, 2002). Why might Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 679 sex, with its key role in reproduction, sometimes create distress that may hamper sexual functioning? Aversive sexuality can be a dysfunctional consequence of negative sexual experience associating sexual thoughts and behavior with negative feelings such as pain, fear, and guilt. In addition, the potential for aversive sexuality may be an integral part of humans’ sexual behavioral system that has been formed over human evolutionary history by selection pressures geared to solve specific problems of survival and reproduction. The existence of pervasive cultural and religious norms, mores, restrictions, and taboos that surround sexuality, implies that regulated sexuality may be functionally adaptive, promoting humans’ reproductive success. We suggest that aversive sexuality may be functional as long as it regulates sexuality in the sense that it minimizes the negative consequences of unrestricted sexual expressions. Accordingly, the fourth subgoal of the sexual system is to restrict the expressions of sexuality when sexual activity is viewed as aversive and incompatible with other important personal and interpersonal goals. Perceiving sexuality as aversive may be linked to individual differences in mating strategies: Some individuals prefer to engage in uncommitted sexual relations, whereas others adopt a more monogamous sexual strategy (Rowe, 1995; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991). This evolved variability is retained by dynamic equilibrium among the various mating strategies, in which the relative costs and benefits of each strategy are stabilized (Buss & Kenrick, 1998; Dawkins, 1976), depending upon the nature of the local environments in which individuals are raised (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000). Perceiving sexuality in negative terms might have been evolved to maximize reproductive benefits of the monogamous sexual strategy through restraining sexual desire and its behavioral manifestations (Goldenberg et al., 1999), and signifying a faithful, trustworthy, and valuable longterm mate in the eyes of potential partners (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Hence, for some people experiencing sexuality aversively may be their optimal means of attracting and retaining desirable and valuable mates (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000). Alternatively, distress and other negative emotions (e.g., frustration, pain, indifference, and boredom) may be provoked when a potential partner violates sexual goals and desires. This follows from the definition of emotion as a function of environmental events that have consequences for personal well being (Mandler, 1975, 1984). Indeed, sexual distress and difficulties are an important index of disrupted relational harmony (e.g., Kaplan, 1974; Snyder & Berg, 1983). Hence, perceiving sexuality as aversive may alert individuals to interference with their sexual and relational goals, thereby guiding attention to the source of distress and motivating them to prevent similar future interference (Buss, 1989). 680 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) The current research The current studies describe the construction of a measure capable of assessing individual differences in behaviors and beliefs related to the pursuit of various subgoals of the sexual behavioral system as they function in the context of long-term adult romantic relationships. In constructing this measure, we also take into account sex differences in sexual desires, attitudes, and behaviors (e.g., Buss & Schmitt, 1993; DeLamater, 1987; Gagnon & Simon, 1973). Hence, although the sexual system subgoals might be applicable to both men and women, we investigate whether gender differences exist along its dimensions in the context of romantic relationships. In a pilot study, we developed a self-report questionnaire of the Sexual Behavioral System Subgoals (SBSS). Study 1 evaluated the structure of this measure, and provided initial evidence for its convergent and incremental validity. Studies 2 and 3 provided additional evidence for the reliability and validity of the scale. In Study 4, we present cross-national evidence for the generalizability of the measure’s underlying factor structure. Pilot study Method Participants. Participants were 398 Israeli undergraduate university students (219 women and 179 men ranging from 18 to 50 years of age, Mdn = 25) who volunteered for the study without compensation. Of the participants, 62.5% were single/never married and 37.5% were married or cohabiting. All participants had experienced heterosexual intercourse, either in a current or past relationship. Measures and procedure. We identified four major subgoals of the sexual behavior system that are relevant to both men’s and women’s reproductive success within long-term romantic relationships: (1) Bringing partners together in a relationship; (2) keeping them attached to each other; (3) promoting frequent sexual activity; and (4) interference with the sexual system – viewing sex as aversive and incompatible with other goals. We then developed 26 questionnaire items from three sources. Some items were selected from the Experience of Heterosexual Intercourse Scale (EHIS) (Birnbaum & Laser-Brandt, 2002), an atheoretical comprehensive profile of the emotional, cognitive, and motivational aspects of men’s and women’s sexual experience. Additional items were written by the investigators and focused on unrepresented aspects. Finally, 8 graduate psychology students were asked to comment on each item’s relevance to the proposed dimensions and to suggest other items. In all, 68 items were generated for the initial item pool. We then eliminated 42 items judged to be unrepresentative (or ambiguous) of the proposed dimension. Participants were approached individually by the authors and were told that the study dealt with feelings, expectations, and beliefs about sexual activity with a partner. After agreeing to participate in the study (more than 90% of the Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 681 approached students agreed to participate in the study), participants were asked to ‘indicate the extent to which the following items describe your feelings, expectations, and beliefs about sexual activity with a partner.’ Participants responded to each item using a 9-point scale, ranging from ‘not at all characteristic’ (1) to ‘very characteristic’ (9). The 26 items (see sample items in Table 1) were presented in random order (the same order for all participants). Completion took most participants about 10 minutes. TABLE 1 Factor structure of the SBSS Item Relationship initiation I have sometimes initiated romantic relationships by having sex I have gotten involved in relationships in order to have sex Having sex is a way to begin romantic relationships I’ve had no difficulty using sex to establish romantic relationships In my experience, sexual intercourse has been a good foundation on which to build a relationship I II III .94 .77 .49 .41 .36 Negative reactions Having sex often leaves me frustrated, both physically and emotionally I feel indifferent or apathetic during sexual activity It’s difficult for me to enjoy sexual intercourse I feel bored while having sex .89 .73 .71 .67 Maintaining the bond Sexual intercourse maintains partners’ interest in their relationship Having sex helps me feel secure in a relationship Having sex helps me maintain a romantic relationship Without adequate sex, a romantic relationship is likely to deteriorate Sexual attraction is a necessity for maintaining a long-term relationship .75 .67 .62 .49 .36 Sexual pleasure and motivation Sex is a major source of pleasure in my life Many people feel intense sexual need or desire almost all the time Having sex can bring me to a state of ecstasy Sexual intercourse is great fun Cronbach’s alpha IV .75 .57 .56 .50 .82 .84 .76 .68 682 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) Results and discussion We examined the underlying structure of the 26 items through a series of exploratory factor analyses with Varimax rotation. Six items that cross-loaded (> .40) on more than one factor were eliminated, as were 2 items with loadings smaller than .50 on the proposed factor, resulting in an 18-item scale. A final factor analysis yielded 4 factors, corresponding to the dimensions described earlier, accounting for 54.0% of the total variance (23.1%, 16.8%, 8.3%, 5.8%, respectively). We also examined the underlying structure of the SBSS items through a series of factor analyses with oblimin rotation. Comparison with the results of the Varimax rotation revealed a very similar structure and pattern of loadings. Cronbach alpha coefficients for the four SBSS subscales ranged from .78 to .85, implying adequate internal consistency. On this basis, scale scores were computed for each participant by averaging the items loading on each factor. The magnitude of the correlations among these four scores was small to moderate, ranging from .06 to .40, and indicating that the SBSS dimensions assess relatively unique information. To insure that the underlying structure of the SBSS was the same for both men and women, we performed a factor analysis on each sex. Comparison of the factor analyses revealed that the internal structure and the loadings patterns of the SBSS were very similar in the men’s and women’s sample (the 4 factors accounted for 61.9% of the total variance for men and for 58.0% of the total variance for women; Cronbach alpha coefficients for the 4 SBSS subscales ranged from .74 to .85 for men and from .78 to .87 for women). Study 1 Study 1 was designed to evaluate the structure, reliability, and validity of the measure developed in the pilot study. We administered a questionnaire packet that included the revised version of the SBSS to a confirmatory sample of 128 participants. Method Participants. Participants were 128 Israeli undergraduate university students (66 women and 62 men ranging from 19 to 50 years of age, Mdn = 24) who volunteered for the study without compensation. Of the participants, 57.8% were single/never married and 42.2% were married or cohabiting. All participants had experienced heterosexual intercourse, either in a current or past relationship. Measures and procedure. We administered a questionnaire packet, which included the revised 18-item version of the SBSS (see Table 1). The instructions and rating scale were identical to those described above. Participants also completed several additional measures. The Israeli Sexual Behavior Inventory (ISBI; Kravetz, Drory, & Shaked, 1999) was used and participants answered a 13-item version of ISBI tapping four areas of sexual functioning: Sexual satisfaction (3 items, e.g., ‘I feel satisfied with my sexual life’), sexual arousal (2 items, e.g., ‘I feel aroused during Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 683 sexual intercourse’), orgasmic responsivity (3 items, e.g., ‘how frequently your sexual activities with your partner resulted in orgasm’), and intimacy during sexual intercourse (5 items, e.g., ‘My partner and I display signs of affection during sexual intercourse’). Participants rated the extent to which they agreed with each item on a 5-point scale, ranging from ‘not at all’ (1) to ‘very much’ (5). Coefficient alpha reliabilities and inter-item correlations were adequate for the four categories of sexual functioning (.59 for sexual satisfaction items, .77 for orgasmic responsivity items, .62 for intimacy items, and r = .30, p < .001 for the two sexual arousal items). On this basis, we computed four scores by averaging items in each category. The Expanded Sexual Arousability Index (SAI-E; Chambless & Lifshitz, 1984), translated into Hebrew by Birnbaum and Reis (in press), was used to measure perceived sexual anxiety. Items describe 28 sexual experiences and situations, which are rated along a 7-point Likert scale according to how anxious the respondent feels, or would feel, when engaged in the described activity (e.g., ‘when a loved one undresses you’). Response options range from 0 ‘pleasant/relaxing’ to 6 ‘extremely anxiety provoking.’ The SAI-E obtained an internal consistency estimate of .91 (Cronbach’s alpha). Higher scores represented greater sexual anxiety. Participants also completed the Hebrew version of the Women’s Sexual Selfschema Scale (Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994) which was translated into Hebrew by Birnbaum and Reis (in press), while a team of four bilingual psychologists translated the Men’s Sexual Self-schema Scale (Andersen et al., 1999) into Hebrew, using the back-translation technique. Participants rated 50 trait adjectives for women (26 scored and 24 fillers) and 45 trait adjectives for men (27 scored and 18 fillers), respectively, reflecting a range of cognitive representations of sexual self-schema (e.g., ‘romantic,’ ‘embarrassed,’ or ‘direct’). Participants rated each item on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all descriptive of me’ (0) to ‘very much descriptive of me’ (6). Both the men’s and women’s scales are composed of three factors: The women’s scale contains two positive aspects (romantic/passionate and open/direct self-views) including 10 and 9 items, respectively, and a negative aspect (embarrassment/conservatism) including 7 items (Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994). For the men’s scale, passionate/loving included 10 items, powerful/aggressive 13 items, and openminded/liberal traits 4 items (Andersen et al., 1999). In the present study, factor analysis with Varimax rotation replicated Andersen and her colleagues’ (Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen et al., 1999) three factors for both men and women, excluding several items that loaded less than .40, and were dropped from subsequent analyses. Cronbach alphas indicated an adequate internal consistency for the three factors for both men and women (for women, .82 for the romantic/passionate; .75 for the open/direct; .66 for the embarrassment/conservatism, and .63 for the total scale; for men, .81 for the passionate/loving, .71 for the powerful/aggressive, and .58 for the open-minded/liberal traits, and .74 for the total scale). Following Andersen and her colleagues’ suggestion, we calculated a single total sexual self-schema score by summing items for the two positive factors and subtracting values for the negative factor items for women, and by summing items for the three factors for men. Higher scores reflected more positive sexual selfschema. A version of the Sexual Opinion Survey (SOS; Fisher et al., 1988), translated into Hebrew by W. A. Fisher (1998), was also used. This is a 21-item self-report 684 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) measure of erotophobia–erotophilia (evaluation of sexual cues along a negative–positive dimension). Participants rated their affective responses to a variety of autosexual (e.g., ‘masturbation can be an exciting experience’), heterosexual (e.g., ‘I personally find that thinking about engaging in sexual intercourse is arousing’), and homosexual situations (e.g., ‘If I found out that a close friend of mine was a homosexual, it would annoy me’) on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from (1) ‘strongly disagree’ to (7) ‘strongly agree.’ The reliability of the SOS was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha = .85). Higher scores represented a greater erotophilic tendency. A version of the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991), translated into Hebrew by Birnbaum and Reis (in press), is a self-report scale that measures the willingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relations. Items assess both overt sexual behavior (e.g., ‘with how many different partners have you had sex on one and only one occasion?’), and attitudes toward casual, uncommitted sexual relations (e.g., ‘I can imagine myself being comfortable and enjoying “casual” sex with different partners’). Attitudinal items were rated on a 9-point scale ranging from (1) ‘strongly disagree’ to (9) ‘strongly agree.’ To create a unit-weighted index, responses to each item were standardized through z-scores. The SOI is factorally unidimensional, and obtained an adequate Cronbach’s alpha of .86 in the current sample. Higher scores reflect a greater likelihood of engaging in casual, uncommitted sexual relations. Demographic questions tapped age, current relationship status, and length of current relationship. Results and discussion Confirmatory factor analysis, using the AMOS program (Arbuckle, 1999), determined the fit between the 18-item version of the SBSS and our theoretical model. The chi-square test of goodness-of-fit was significant, χ2(117) = 159.76, p < .01, indicating significant deviation of the data from the proposed model. This is likely attributable to the study’s sample size. Other model fit statistics less sensitive to sample size yielded acceptably high values (Bentler, 1995): GFI = .90 and CFI = .95, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was .05. Therefore, we viewed the model as an acceptably good fit to the data. All items loaded significantly on their respective factors, p < .05 (see Table 1). We tested and compared the hypothesized four-factor model to two alternative models. In the first model, we treated the 18 items as unidimensional, and in the second, we treated the items as loading either a positivity or negativity dimension. Neither alternative models provided an adequate fit to the data (detailed information is available from the first author). Furthermore, all indices indicated that the four-factor model was a substantial improvement in fit over both the single- and two-factor models. To insure that the underlying structure of the SBSS was the same for both men and women, we split our sample by gender, and performed an independent confirmatory factor analysis on each sample. Once again the overall chisquare test was significant for both men and women, χ2(114) = 151.33, p < .05 and χ2(117) = 160.97, p < .01, respectively, likely reflecting the sample size. Model fit indices that adjust for sample size indicated good fit (GFI = .90 and CFI = .96 for men and GFI = .92 and CFI = .96 for women) and low RMSEA of .05 for men and of .04 for women. As these values are within recommended Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 685 levels (Bentler, 1995), the model represents good fit to the data for both men and women. These analyses support our claim that the SBSS consists of four dimensions concerning one’s beliefs about the various personal and interpersonal rewards and costs of sexual activity from an evolutionary perspective. Cronbach alpha coefficients for the four SBSS subscales ranged from .68 to .84, implying adequate internal consistency. On this basis, scores were computed for each participant by averaging the items loading on each factor. The magnitude of the correlations among these four scores was small to moderate, ranging from .04 to .46, indicating that the SBSS dimensions assess relatively unique information. Correlations relevant to the convergent validity of the SBSS are presented in Table 2. The sexuality measures manifested somewhat different patterns of associations across the SBSS factors, indicating that the four factors represent unique aspects of the perceived contribution of the sexual behavioral system. Importantly, although the SBSS factors were significantly associated with attitudinal, emotional, and behavioral aspects of sexuality, the strength of the correlations is mostly small to moderate, implying considerable unshared variance. Interestingly, men and women showed a similar pattern of associations between the SBSS factors and sexuality measures, with the exception of the associations between Pleasure and Motivation and the willingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relations (i.e., the SOI), and between Negative Reaction and sexual anxiety (i.e., SAI-E), which were significant only for men, as well as the association between Pleasure and Motivation and orgasmic responsivity, which was significant only for women. These sex-specific findings imply that men’s sex-related affect and cognitions may be a manifestation of short-term mating strategy preference (Buss & Schmitt, 1993), combined with self-enhancing motivation (e.g., prestige among peers, sexual ‘performance’), whereas women’s sex-related affect and cognitions may be more complex and less affected by relationship-irrelevant motives (e.g., Birnbaum & LaserBrandt, 2002). In addition, smaller variability in men’s orgasmic responsivity TABLE 2 Correlations between SBSS factors and other sexual measures (N = 128) All SBSS factors Relationship initiation Sexual measure M W SAI-E –.13 –.15 SOI .57*** .56*** SOS .37** .40*** Sexual self-schema .39** .32** Orgasmic responsivity .20 –.02 Sexual intimacy .08 .03 Sexual arousal .23 .16 Sexual satisfaction –.17 .06 *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. Negative reactions Maintain the bond M M W .36** .16 .14 .00 –.07 –.04 –.07 –.11 –.25* –.53*** –.28* –.27* –.18 –.12 –.25* –.29* W –.15 –.19 .13 .11 .24* .14 .24* .19 .18 .23* –.00 .14 .09 .19 –.09 .07 Pleasure and motivation M W –.30* –.32* .48*** .08 .51*** .30* .14 .17 .17 .32** .02 .19 .29* .46*** .05 .21 686 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) (e.g., Spector & Carey, 1990) may explain the relative disconnection between Pleasure and Motivation and orgasmic responsivity among men. To assess the incremental validity of the SBSS over existing sexual measures, we conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses. Specifically, we examined four areas of sexual functioning (orgasmic responsivity, intimacy during sexual intercourse, sexual arousal, and sexual satisfaction), as tapped by the ISBI (Kravetz et al., 1999). In each analysis, the sexual measures (SAI-E, SOI, SOS, Sexual Self-schema) were entered as the first set of predictor variables, followed by the set of four SBSS scores. As shown in Table 3, the SBSS predicted three sexual functioning scales, over and above the other sexuality measures; that is, the series of hierarchical regression analyses demonstrated that the SBSS exhibited significant incremental validity in predicting orgasmic responsivity, sexual arousal, and sexual satisfaction. Negative Reactions (β = –.27, p < .001) and Sexual Pleasure and Motivation (β = .16, p < .05) were associated with orgasmic responsivity; Sexual Pleasure and Motivation (β = .33, p < .001) was associated with sexual arousal; and Negative Reactions (β = –.25, p < .01) was associated with sexual satisfaction. In summary, these analyses supported the four-dimensional conceptualization of the various personal and interpersonal rewards and costs of sexual activity from an evolutionary perspective. Findings showed that the SBSS was TABLE 3 Regression of ISBI on SBSS factors and sexual measures scores (SAI-E, SOI, SOS, Sexual Self-schema) R2 change F change Orgasmic responsivity Step 1: Sexual measures .47*** Step 2: SBSS .08*** 27.23*** (4, 121) 5.53*** (4, 117) Sexual intimacy Step 1: Sexual measures .09* Step 2: SBSS .04 Sexual arousal Step 1: Sexual measures .22*** Step 2: SBSS .09** Sexual satisfaction Step 1: Sexual measures .05 Step 2: SBSS .08* *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. 2.92* (4, 121) 1.44 (4, 117) 8.51*** (4, 121) 3.58** (4, 117) 1.71 (4, 121) 2.57* (4, 117) Adjusted R2 of full equation .46*** .53*** .05* .07* .19*** .26*** .02 .07* F 27.23*** (4, 121) 18.42*** (8, 117) 2.92* (4, 121) 2.20* (8, 117) 8.51*** (4, 121) 6.41*** (8, 117) 1.71 (4, 121) 2.18* (8, 117) Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 687 a reliable and valid instrument, which applied equally well to both men and women, even though each of the scales is deliberately short. The SBSS also demonstrated convergent validity with attitudinal, emotional, and behavioral aspects of sexuality and incremental validity in predicting sexual functioning beyond variance explained by existing sexual measures. Finally, to discover whether gender differences existed along the SBSS dimensions, a one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for gender was performed on the four factors of the SBSS. This MANOVA yielded a significant gender difference F(4,123) = 23.52, p < .001, η2 = .14. Univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) indicated that this effect was significant for two of the four factors: Relationship Initiation, F(1,126) = 16.58, p < .001, η2 = .12, and Pleasure and Motivation, F(1,126) = 4.98, p < .05, η2 = .04, with men scoring higher on both factors (M = 4.42, SD = 2.15 and M = 6.59, SD = 1.37, respectively) than women (M = 3.03, SD = 1.68 and M = 6.03, SD = 1.43, respectively). Overall, it seems that men, in comparison with women, reported greater usage of sex as a means of initiating romantic relationships. Furthermore, men were more likely to perceive sex as a strong motivator and as a source of pleasure and joy than women. Taken together, Study 1’s findings are in line with theoretical approaches to human sexuality and empirical evidence suggesting that women adopt a more emotional-interpersonal orientation to sexuality and place a greater emphasis on interpersonal aspects during sexual intercourse. Men, on the other hand, develop a more recreational orientation toward their sexuality and place a greater emphasis on expressing their sexual needs (e.g., Buss & Schmitt, 1993; DeLamater, 1987; Gagnon & Simon, 1973) and on taking the initiative and dominant role of courting (e.g., Byers & Heinlein, 1989; O’Sullivan & Byers, 1992). Study 2 Study 2 was designed to examine the associations between the SBSS and measures of theoretically related constructs. We also sought to distinguish this measure from a potentially relevant, nonsexual domain, personality. Finally, we sought to determine whether the sexual behavioral system accounted for variance in sexual outcomes beyond that explained by the attachment behavioral system. Method Participants. Participants were 130 Israeli undergraduate university students (80 women and 50 men ranging from 18 to 64 years of age, Mdn = 24) who volunteered for the study without compensation. Of the participants, 59.4% were single/never married, and 40.6% were married or cohabiting. All participants had experienced heterosexual intercourse, either in a current or past relationship. Measures and procedure. Participants individually completed the final 18-item version of the SBSS. Instructions and rating scales were identical to those 688 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) described in the pilot study. Cronbach alpha coefficients for the four SBSS subscales ranged from .75 to .85. Factor intercorrelations ranged from .00 to .48. In addition, participants were given the following measures: (i) Response bias (social desirability; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964) and personality domains which have been associated with sexuality measures – self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1979), neuroticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1967), extraversion, and openness to experience (Costa & McCrae, 1985); and (ii) relational measures. This consisted of converging measures of relationship satisfaction (Hendrick, 1988) and adult attachment (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). The measures were presented in random order. A Hebrew version of Rosenberg’s (1979) Self-esteem Scale (SES) was used, consisting of 10 statements responded to on a scale ranging from (1) ‘strongly disagree’ to (4) ‘strongly agree.’ The scale measures general selfesteem (e.g., ‘I feel I have a number of good qualities’). We obtained an internal consistency estimate of .86 (Cronbach’s alpha). Higher scores reflected greater self-esteem. A Hebrew version of the 33-item Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964) was used. Participants rated whether or not each item was self-descriptive. Cronbach’s alpha was .78. Higher scores represented more socially desirable responding. A brief Hebrew version of the Neuroticism Subscale of the Eysenck Personality Inventory (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1967) was used. This 12-item scale measures tendencies to experience anxiety, distress, and emotional lability (e.g., ‘I am easily bothered by things’). Participants rated items on a 5-point scale, ranging from ‘not at all’ (1) to ‘very much’ (5). Cronbach’s alpha was .91. Higher scores indicated greater neuroticism. A Hebrew version of the Extraversion Subscale from the NEO Personality Inventory – Revised (Costa & McCrae, 1985) was used. This scale measures sociability and the tendency to seek out stimulation as well as tendencies to react impulsively without deliberation (e.g., ‘I know how to captivate people’). Participants rated whether or not each of the items was self-descriptive. Cronbach’s alpha for the 23 items was .85. Higher scores reflected greater extraversion. The Hebrew version of the Openness to Experience Subscale from the NEO Personality Inventory – Revised (Costa & McCrae, 1985) measures open-mindedness and interest in culture (e.g., ‘I am interested in abstract ideas’). Participants rated the extent to which each of 48 items were self-descriptive on a 5-point scale, ranging from ‘not at all’ (1) to ‘very much’ (5). Cronbach’s alpha was .78. Higher scores indicated greater openness to experience. A Hebrew version of the Relationship Assessment Scale (RAS; Hendrick, 1988) was used to measure relationship satisfaction only for those participants who were currently involved in a romantic relationship. This scale consists of 7 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (e.g., ‘In general, how satisfied are you with your relationship?’). The RAS is unidimensional, and we obtained an internal consistency estimate of .89 (Cronbach’s alpha). Higher scores represented greater relationship satisfaction. A Hebrew version of the Experience in Close Relationships Scale (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998) translated into Hebrew by Mikulincer and Florian (2000), assesses attachment orientations. This 36-item scale taps dimensions of attachment anxiety and avoidance. Participants rated the extent to which each item was descriptive of their feelings in close relationships on a 7-point scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ (1) to ‘strongly agree’ (7). Eighteen items tapped Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 689 attachment anxiety (e.g., ‘I worry about being abandoned’), and 18 items tapped attachment avoidance (e.g., ‘I get uncomfortable when a romantic partner wants to be very close’). Cronbach’s alphas were high for the anxiety (.91) and avoidance dimensions (.88). Higher scores indicated greater attachment-related avoidance or anxiety. A series of demographic questions included age, current relationship status, length of current relationship, and number of lifetime sexual partners. In addition, participants rated their general view of their sexuality (‘Rate yourself as a sexual man/woman’) on a 5-point scale, with 1 being ‘not at all’ and 5 being ‘extremely.’ Results and discussion Data relevant to the convergent and discriminant validity of the SBSS are presented in Table 4. Social desirability and the potentially relevant, nonsexual, personality domains were chosen as discriminant measures because they represent variance associated with volunteer bias in sex research (Catania, Gibson, Chitwood, & Coates, 1990). The correlations between the SBSS and social desirability response set (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964) were not significantly different from zero. In addition, the precise pattern of associations between the SBSS factors and personality variables varied slightly between the sexes: ‘Relationship initiation’ and ‘Maintaining the bond’ are relatively uncorrelated with personality domains. ‘Negative reactions’ correlates negatively with selfesteem, extraversion, and openness to experience (although the last two correlations were significant only for women) and correlates positively with neuroticism (mainly among men). ‘Sexual pleasure and motivation’ exhibits small to moderate relationships with extraversion, self-esteem, and openness to experience but only among women. Overall, this pattern of associations between personality domains and negative and positive sex-related affect is in line with Andersen and Cyranowski’s (1995) contention that the role of personality in women’s sexuality TABLE 4 Correlations between SBSS factors and measures used for assessment of response bias, convergent, and discriminant validity (N = 130) All Dimension Measure RAS Anxiety Avoidance Self-esteem Social desirability Neuroticism Extraversion Openness SBSS factors Relationship initiation M W –.10 .22 –.21 –.06 .08 .09 .30* .14 –.13 .17 –.21 .06 .11 .19 .08 .28* *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. Negative reactions Maintain the bond M M W M W –.02 .09 –.04 –.01 .10 .01 .04 –.08 –.07 .09 –.21 .13 –.06 .13 .05 .28* –.25 .08 –.10 .03 .20 –.01 .24 .15 .09 –.09 –.24* .41*** –.19 –.08 .40*** .41*** W –.03 –.33* .45*** .28* .21 .38** –.35* –.46*** .18 .12 .41** .19 –.10 –.36** .15 –.27* Pleasure and motivation 690 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) may operate differently depending upon the developmental stage in a woman’s sexual life. Given that our sample consisted mostly of young men and women, it is quite likely that characteristics of self esteem, extraversion, and openness to experience, which are associated with excitement seeking, dominance, and higher levels of sexual information and sexual activity (Costa & McCrae, 1985), will be relatively sexual facilitative. In contrast, scoring low on these personality domains may be relatively sexually inhibitory, particularly as women begin sexual interactions with others and are more expected to restrict their sexual expressions, compared with men (e.g., Buss & Schmitt, 1993; DeLamater, 1987; Gagnon & Simon, 1973). Importantly, although two of the SBSS factors are significantly associated with the relevant personality dimensions, the strength of the significant correlations is mostly small to moderate, implying that they cannot be equated with these measures. Hence, SBSS factors are differentiated from these personality measures. The somewhat higher, though nonoverlapping, relationships of ‘Negative reactions’ and ‘Sexual pleasure and motivation’ with the personality measures are consistent with prior findings showing that neuroticism is associated with low sexual satisfaction as well as high degrees of worry and guilt feelings about sex, and seeing sexual behavior as disgusting (Eysenck, 1971). In addition, extraversion is associated with intensified sexual behavior (e.g., Schenk & Pfrang, 1986). Because of the hypothesized contribution of the sexual behavioral system to the formation and maintenance of intimate romantic relationships, we expected the SBSS factors to be correlated with various more general relationship qualities. The tendency to experience negative reactions in sexuality was positively correlated with attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety and negatively associated with relationship satisfaction, although the last correlation was significant only for women. Importantly, although the SBSS factors were significantly associated with romantic relationship qualities, these correlations do not suggest that the scales measure the same domain. To test how much of the variance in one’s general view of his/her sexuality, and sexual behavior, as indicated by the number of lifetime sexual partners, could be explained by SBSS over and above other personality constructs and relational measures, two hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. In each of the analyses, the personality constructs, relational measures, and social desirability score were entered in Step 1, followed by SBSS scores in Step 2. SBSS scores accounted for significant increments in explained variance beyond personality, relational, and social desirability scores in predicting global rating of self as a sexual man/woman (22.3%, p < .001), and the number of lifetime sexual partners, 36.8%, p < .001. Study 3 Study 3 was designed to provide a test-retest reliability estimate for the SBSS. In addition, we sought to determine whether the SBSS accounted for variance in sexual functioning beyond that explained by the attachment behavioral system. Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 691 Method Participants. Participants were 58 Israeli undergraduate university students (40 women and 18 men ranging from 19 to 40 years of age, Mdn = 25) who volunteered for the study without compensation. Of the participants, 94.8% were currently involved in a romantic relationship and 5.2% were not currently involved in a romantic relationship. All participants had experienced heterosexual intercourse, either in a current or past relationship. Measures and procedure. Participants individually completed the final version of the SBSS consisting of 18 items twice, with an 8-week interval. Instructions and rating scales were identical to those described in the pilot study. In addition, participants were given a questionnaire packet which included: (i) The Israeli Sexual Behavior Inventory (ISBI; Kravetz et al., 1999); and Hebrew versions of the following measures: (ii) Neuroticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1967); (iii) relationship satisfaction (RAS; Hendrick, 1988); and (iv) adult attachment (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998), which were described in Studies 1 and 2. The measures were presented in random order. Results and discussion Test–retest reliability of the SBSS was obtained for an 8-week interval. Coefficients for each factor were Relationship Initiation, .92; Negative Reactions, .84; Maintaining the Bond, .83; and Sexual Pleasure and Motivation, .93. These test–retest reliabilities are high and suggest that the SBSS is stable over this particular period of time. To further assess the incremental validity of the SBSS over models of attachment, we conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses. Specifically, we examined four areas of sexual functioning (orgasmic responsivity, intimacy during sexual intercourse, sexual arousal, and sexual satisfaction), as tapped by the ISBI (Kravetz et al., 1999). In each analysis, we entered attachment (avoidance and anxiety) scores and RAS score (Hendrick, 1988; to rule out a sentiment override explanation) in Step 1, followed by neuroticism (Eysenck, 1971) in Step 2. Incremental validity of the SBSS was then established by entering all four subscales at the next step. The contribution of the sexual behavioral system accounted for significant increments in explained variance beyond neuroticism, attachment, and RAS scores in predicting orgasmic responsivity, sexual arousal, and sexual satisfaction (see Table 5). Relationship Initiation (β = .36, p < .05) and Negative Reactions (β = –.59, p < .001) were associated with orgasmic responsivity, Negative Reactions (β = –.44, p < .01) and Sexual Pleasure and Motivation (β = .29, p < .06) were associated with sexual arousal, and Negative Reactions (β = –.73, p < .001) was associated with sexual satisfaction. The SBSS scales did not add significant incremental variance for intimacy during sexual intercourse. Study 4 Study 4 was designed to provide cross-national evidence for the generalizability of the underlying factor structure of the SBSS in another western culture. That is, in Study 4, we administered this 18-item scale to a sample 692 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) TABLE 5 Regression of ISBI on SBSS, neuroticism, RAS, and attachment scores All R2 change Orgasmic responsivity Step 1: Attachment and RAS .13 Step 2: Neuroticism .00 Step 3: SBSS scores .31*** Sexual intimacy Step 1: Attachment and RAS .13 Step 2: Neuroticism .00 Step 3: SBSS scores .07 Sexual arousal Step 1: Attachment and RAS .07 Step 2: Neuroticism .00 Step 3: SBSS scores .36*** Sexual satisfaction Step 1: Attachment and RAS .26** Step 2: Neuroticism .03 Step 3: SBSS scores .38*** F change Adjusted R2 of full equation 2.32 (3, 48) .14 (1, 47) 6.05*** (4, 43) .07 2.37 (3, 48) .09 (1, 47) .87 (4, 43) .08 .06 .34*** .06 .05 1.21 (3, 48) .10 (1, 47) 6.68*** (4, 43) .01 5.60** (3, 48) 1.80 (1, 47) 11.99*** (4, 43) .21** .00 .32*** .23** .60*** F 2.32 (3, 48) 1.75 (4, 47) 4.27*** (8, 43) 2.37 (3, 48) 1.77 (4, 47) 1.31 (8, 43) 1.21 (3, 48) .91 (4, 47) 4.02*** (8, 43) 5.60** (3, 48) 4.72** (4, 47) 10.56*** (8, 43) **p < .01; ***p < .001. of 476 undergraduate students from the University of California, Davis (USA), and their responses were submitted to confirmatory factor analysis. A cross-national replication of the structure of the SBSS with a sample speaking a different language would be a robust test of the stability of the four-factorial structure. Method Participants. Participants were 476 undergraduate students (319 women and 157 men ranging from 17 to 35 years of age, Mdn = 19) from the University of California, Davis, who received Introductory Psychology course credit for their participation. The sample was ethnically diverse with 49.2% Caucasians, 3.6% Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 693 African Americans, 25.2% Asian American, 6.9% Hispanics, and 13% who chose ‘other.’ Measures and procedure. Participants individually completed an English version of the SBSS as part of an Internet survey about ‘attitudes and feelings regarding close relationships.’ The survey was posted on the Internet with the title ‘Experience in Close Relationships and Relational Satisfaction Survey’. Links to the online survey were located on the Department of Psychology’s experiments website. The survey was programmed with SPSS software (mrInterview) such that IP addresses were read, and surveys submitted from the same address for the second time were automatically rejected. The survey was introduced as follows: Thank you for taking part in this study. In the following pages we will ask you about your attitudes and feelings regarding close relationships. All the information gathered in this survey will be kept confidential, so please try to respond as honestly as possible. The data collected will help us to learn more about the function of relationships. Please make sure that you have enough time available now to complete the survey free of interruptions. For the present study, a team of four bilingual psychologists translated the full 18-item SBSS into English, using the back-translation technique. Instructions and rating scales were identical to those described in the pilot study. Results and discussion Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS; Arbuckle, 1999) was again used to conduct a confirmatory factor analysis of the factor structure of the SBSS. Once again the overall chi-square test was significant, χ2(117) = 428.07, p < .001, likely reflecting the large sample size. Model fit indices that adjust for sample size indicated good fit (GFI = .91 and CFI = .92) and low RMSEA of .07. As these values are within recommended levels (Bentler, 1995), the model represents good fit to the data. To insure that the underlying structure of the SBSS was the same for both men and women, we split our sample by gender, and performed an independent confirmatory factor analysis for men and women. Once again the overall chi-square test was significant for both men and women (χ2 (113) = 174.48, p < .001 and χ2 (115) = 237.91, p < .001, respectively). Model fit indices that adjust for sample size indicated good fit (GFI = .90 and CFI = .95 for men and GFI = .92 and CFI = .95 for women) and low RMSEA of .05 for both men and women. As these values are within recommended levels (Bentler, 1995), the model represents good fit to the data for both men and women. Cronbach alpha coefficients indicated adequate internal consistency for the four SBSS subscales (from .73 to .85). In the present sample, the correlations between these factors were mostly small to moderate (ranging from –.05 to .42), indicating that nearly all of the SBSS scores were quite independent of each other. However, the correlations between Maintaining the Bond and Relationship Initiation, as well as Maintaining the Bond and Sexual Pleasure and Motivation were rather high (.64 and .58, respectively). It is important to note that the current sample was younger than the Israeli samples, t(788) = –16.05, p < .01, and these age differences may account for the different pattern of correlations revealed in the current sample. The various personal- and interpersonal-related functions of the sexual behavioral system may be more differentiated as people gain more experience in understanding the differential 694 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) contribution of sexuality to distinct phases of relationship development. That is, younger persons might not differentiate sharply between the role played by sexuality in relationship initiation and maintenance, because they may be in the early stages of relationships in which both are prominent. Another plausible explanation for the distinctive pattern of correlations revealed in this study might be the different language and nationality characterizing the current sample. General discussion Across five studies, we developed a self-report measure of individual differences in behaviors and beliefs concerning the pursuit of subgoals of the sexual behavioral system. These subgoals are based on a functional-evolutionary analysis of the sexual behavioral system in the context of long-term adult romantic relationships. Results indicate that the SBSS was a reliable and valid instrument that applied equally well to men and women. The pilot study as well as Studies 1 and 2 revealed that the SBSS incorporated welldifferentiated dimensions of behaviors and cognitive representations of the functionality of sexual activity in the context of an adult romantic relationship. Studies 1 and 2 also indicated that our theoretical model of the dimensions underlying men’s and women’s sexual behavior represented good fit to the obtained data and that the SBSS displayed convergent validity with other measures of cognitive, behavioral, and affective aspects of sexuality and romantic relationships. Additionally, Studies 1, 2, and 3 established that the SBSS was unrelated to social desirability and self-esteem. These studies showed that this scale also demonstrated incremental validity in predicting sexual functioning, as well as emotions and beliefs related to it, beyond variance associated with attachment dimensions, relationship satisfaction, and key individual differences. Finally, Study 4 provided cross-national evidence for the generalizability of the underlying factor structure in the US. The sexual behavioral system subgoals integrate functional behaviors and their cognitive substrate and evolved to solve particular adaptive problems of reproduction and to regulate the expressions of sexuality in romantic relationships. Based on a functional analysis of the importance of adult human sexuality, we identified four dimensions of the personal- and interpersonal-related functions of the sexual behavioral system that are related to both men’s and women’s reproductive success in the context of long-term romantic relationships: (i) Bringing partners together and establishing a relationship by means of sexual activity; (ii) keeping partners attached to each other for an extended period of time; (iii) promoting frequent sexual activity through increasing positive affect and sexual motivation; (iv) restricting the expressions of sexuality when sexual activity is viewed as aversive and incompatible with other important personal and interpersonal goals. Our analyses broadly supported this theoretical proposal. Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 695 The first dimension, ‘Relationship Initiation,’ reflects the beliefs that sexual intercourse is a means of initiating romantic relationships as well as the foundation on which to build them. This dimension was positively associated with the willingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relations, erotophilic tendency, and positive sexual self-schema. Sex drive motivates individuals to pursue either short-term or long-term mating with a potential partner (Buss, 2004; Fisher et al., 2002). Consequently, sexual activity is one strategy that may be used to initiate romantic relationships, although it is perceived by many people as more acceptable in later (when compared with earlier) stages of relational progression (see Sprecher & McKinney, 1993). Our findings are consistent with prior studies showing that people with unrestricted sexuality, who are more likely to engage in uncommitted sexual relations, rated the desire for possible sex as more important reasons for initiating opposite-sex friendships (Bleske-Rechek & Buss, 2001). It seems that this constellation of positive sexual self-view and attitudes, combined with less restricted sexuality, is likely to facilitate the use of sexuality to successfully initiate both short-term and long-term relationships. Indeed, people who had a positive sexual self-view and lacked behavioral inhibition about their sexuality had more sexual and romantic partners and broader sexual experience, compared with people with negative sexual selfview (e.g., Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen et al., 1999). Still, although both unrestricted individuals and individuals with positive sexual self-view use sex to initiate romantic relationships, they may be motivated by different relational goals: Unrestricted individuals may be more likely to use sex to facilitate short-term relationships (see Simpson, Wilson, & Winterheld, 2004), whereas individuals with positive sexual self-view may engage in sex to form and maintain long-term committed relationships (Andersen & Cyranowski, 1994; Andersen et al., 1999; Cyranowski & Andersen, 1998). The second dimension, ‘Negative Reactions,’ taps feelings of frustration, difficulties in experiencing sexual pleasure, indifference, and boredom during sexual activity. This dimension was positively associated with neuroticism, attachment-related avoidance and anxiety, as well as sexual anxiety, and negatively associated with relationship satisfaction (only among women), self-esteem, extraversion, and most sexual functioning measures. These findings fit Kaplan’s (1974) assertion that the various remote psychological antecedents of sexual dysfunction (e.g., early history factors, distressed relationships) may be diverted into the ‘experiential surface’ in the form of negative affectivity and cognitive obstacles to erotic pleasure. Distressed sexuality then may have diverse personal and interpersonal consequences. Specifically, perceiving sex as aversive may regulate sexual expression, imply a loyal and valuable long-term mate in the eyes of potential partners (Buss & Schmitt, 1993), and thereby maximize reproductive benefits of the monogamous sexual strategy (Goldenberg et al., 1999). Nevertheless, because this dimension was not significantly correlated with sexual restrictiveness (the unwillingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relations; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991), it seems more likely that 696 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) experiencing sexuality as frustrating and boring may signify disrupted relational harmony (e.g., Kaplan, 1974; Snyder & Berg, 1983) and motivate individuals to prevent similar interference to their relational and sexual needs and goals in the future (Buss, 1989). The implication of the motivation inflicted by distressed sexuality for dyadic relationships may depend upon one’s relational goals and the importance he or she attributes to sexual activity in the context of romantic relationships: Some individuals may dismiss the implication of the disturbed sexuality for their relationship, whereas others may be motivated to resolve dyadic conflicts and improve current sexual and relational well being, or possibly dissolve their relationship and look for a more appropriate partner. Future research should examine the contribution of mating strategies, relational goals, and the perceived importance of sexuality, as it functions in adult romantic relationships, to the understanding of the consequences of distressed sexual intercourse for personal and interpersonal motivation and well being. The third dimension, ‘Maintaining the Bond,’ reflects the beliefs that sexual intercourse and sexual attraction maintain romantic relationships as well as the interest of romantic partners in them, and that without adequate sex, a romantic relationship is likely to deteriorate. This dimension was positively associated with erotophilic tendency, positive sexual self-schema, and orgasmic responsivity. Because infants and children were more likely to survive and thrive if reared by two parents, human reproductive success was enhanced when sexual partners stayed with each other during their offspring’s exceptionally prolonged and vulnerable period development (H. E. Fisher, 1998; Hazan & Zeifman, 1994). Consequently, couples must engage in strategies that help them maintain their more developed relationships. In later phases of relationship development, processes related to the provision of mutual support, warmth, responsiveness, and interdependence may make a greater contribution to relationship maintenance than sexual desire does (Kotler, 1985; Reedy, Birren, & Schaie, 1981; Sternberg, 1986). However, sexual expression may still be a common maintenance strategy (e.g., Bell, Daly, & Gonzalez, 1987), mainly among erotophilic individuals with positive sexual schema. These individuals experience their sexuality positively and therefore may use it to express, or achieve, affection, emotional intimacy, and love, which in turn, also may foster the emotional bonding with their partner. These findings raise some related questions regarding the operation of the sexual behavioral system in later phases of ongoing romantic relationships: What are the differential relational outcomes of dysfunctional, infrequent sexual activity, or the total loss of sexual activity among individuals who believe that sex contributes to relationship maintenance, compared with individuals who believe that sex is irrelevant for relationship maintenance? Will individuals who believe that sex contributes to relationship maintenance increase their sexual intercourse frequency after relationship threat, conflict, or temporally perceived relational insecurity? Further studies are needed to determine how perceiving sexual activity as contributing to relationship maintenance moderates the sex–relationship linkage. Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 697 The fourth dimension, ‘Sexual Pleasure and Motivation,’ which reflects the beliefs that sex is a strong motivator as well as a source of pleasure and joy, was positively correlated with extraversion, openness, self-esteem (mainly among women), as well as with most of the cognitive, affective and behavioral sexual measures and negatively correlated with sexual anxiety. Surprisingly, although sexual desire and pleasure enhance emotional bonding between sexual partners (Hazan & Zeifman, 1994), our findings revealed that their contribution to relationship satisfaction was rather marginal in comparison to perceiving sexual activity as aversive. Indeed, sexual activity may be less important to relationship satisfaction than other relational expressions (e.g., affection, emotional intimacy, caring), particularly in later stages of relationships (Greenblat, 1983; Reedy et al., 1981). However, the contribution of distressed sexual interactions to relationship satisfaction may be more pronounced than that of harmonious sexual interactions, because of its potential detrimental consequences for both personal and relational well being. This finding is in line with the ‘bad is stronger than good’ argument (see Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Voys, 2001, for a review). Thus, the dimensions ‘Sexual Pleasure and Motivation’ and ‘Negative Reactions’ may represent independent, complementary psychological mechanisms: Whereas ‘Negative Reactions’ is designed to attune individuals to potential threat to their relational goals, sexual desire and pleasure evolved to motivate sexual behavior, which may promote attachment formation, as well as effectively pursuing short-term mating strategies. Implications and limitations The current findings revealed different patterns of associations between the pursuit of various subgoals of the sexual behavioral system and relational and sexual measures across the four perceived functional dimensions. Our findings affirmed that each dimension represented a unique functional aspect of the sexual behavioral system with distinctive contributions to one’s close relationships. The SBSS will allow future research to address questions about the specific mechanisms whereby the sexual behavioral system contributes to the initiation, maintenance, and dissolution of romantic relationships: In what ways are different strategies and relationship goals reflected in the operation of the sexual system through different stages of relationship development and in different kind of sexual-romantic relationships? Are sexual functioning and relational well being reciprocal, unrelated, or does sexual functioning compensate for relational threat and insecurity? More specifically, is the sex–relationship linkage moderated by the unique combination of the perceived functions of the sexual behavioral system? What are the sexual and relational outcomes of conflicting perceived functions of the sexual behavioral system, such as in the case where a person believes that sex contributes to relationship maintenance and experiences sex in negative terms? What are the relational sequels of couples’ clashing functions of the sexual behavioral system? For example, what are the relational outcomes of dysfunctional sexuality or reduced 698 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) sexual frequency when one partner believes that sex contributes to relationship maintenance and the other believes that sex is irrelevant for relationship maintenance? And vice versa, how will disharmonious relationships influence sexual functioning among couples with discrepancies in their perceived functionality of the sexual behavioral system? Further research is needed to explore the tangled role played by the sexual behavioral system in the broader context of close relationships. Several caveats of the present research should be highlighted. First, our samples consisted mostly of young men and women, thus the generalizability of the patterns of associations between perceived functions of the sexual behavioral system and relational and sexual measures to older individuals, who have gained more experience in understanding the differential contribution of sexuality to distinct phases of relationship development, is less certain. Next, it should be noted that the data in this study were correlational and prevent us from depicting the causal direction of the association between perceived personal- and interpersonal-related functions of the sexual behavioral system and relational and sexual measures. For example, perceiving sex as aversive may be one of the immediate causes contributing to distressed relationships. Nevertheless, one can easily argue the reverse, namely that distressed relationships result in experiencing sexual intercourse as aversive, not the other way round. Finally, our research focused on relationship satisfaction, a rather narrow aspect of relationship quality, which encompasses many other relational components, such as emotional intimacy, perceived support, agreement, responsiveness, and interdependence, as well as a variety of determinants and consequences that may change as a relationship develops. On the whole, in spite of its possible limitations, the present research should be viewed as an important step in the development of a reliable and valid instrument tapping sex behaviors and beliefs related to the pursuit of various subgoals of the sexual behavioral system. We hope this measure will promote better understanding of the role of functional and dysfunctional sexuality in relationship initiation, maintenance, and dissolution. Future studies are thus encouraged to adopt a longitudinal prospective design, administering the SBSS in multiple waves to couples involved in steady relationships, and assessing whether age and relationship duration, along with changes in relationship quality, major life transitions, and accumulative sexual experiences, have differential effects on the pursuit of various subgoals of the sexual behavioral system, and vice versa. REFERENCES Alexander, R. D., & Noonan, K. M. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care, and human social evolution. In N. A. Chagnon & W. G. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: An anthropological perspective (pp. 436–453). North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press. Andersen, B. L., & Cyranowski, J. M. (1994). Women’s sexual self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1079–1100. Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 699 Andersen, B. L., & Cyranowski, J. M. (1995). Women’s sexuality: Behaviors, responses, and individual differences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 891–906. Andersen, B. L., Cyranowski, J. M., & Espindle, D. (1999). Men’s sexual self-schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 645–661. Arbuckle, J. L. (1999). Amos 4.0 user’s guide. Chicago: SmallWaters Corporation. Bailey, J. M. (1998). Can behavior genetics contribute to evolutionary behavioral science? In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 221–234). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Voys, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. Bell, R. A., Daly, J. A., & Gonzalez, C. (1987). Affinity-maintenance in marriage and its relationship to women’s marital satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 445–454. Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software. Birnbaum, G. E. (2003). The meaning of heterosexual intercourse among women with female orgasmic disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 61–71. Birnbaum, G. E., Glaubman, H., & Mikulincer, M. (2001). Women’s experience of heterosexual intercourse–scale construction, factor structure, and relations to orgasmic disorder. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 191–204. Birnbaum, G. E., & Laser-Brandt, D. (2002). Gender differences in the experience of heterosexual intercourse. The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 11(3–4), 143–158. Birnbaum, G. E., & Reis, H. T. (in press). Women’s sexual working models: An evolutionaryattachment perspective. The Journal of Sex Research. Bleske-Rechek, A. L., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Opposite-sex friendship: Sex differences and similarities in initiation, selection, and dissolution. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1310–1323. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford. Buss, D. M. (1989). Conflict between the sexes: Strategic interference and the evocation of anger and upset. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 735–747. Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual differences. Journal of Personality, 67, 209–243. Buss, D. M., & Kenrick, D. T. (1998). Evolutionary social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 982–1026). New York: McGraw-Hill. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–232. Byers, E. S., & Heinlein, L. (1989). Predicting initiations and refusals of sexual activities in married and cohabiting heterosexual couples. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 210–231. Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818. Catania, J. A., Gibson, D. R., Chitwood, D. D., & Coates, T. J. (1990). Methodological problems in AIDS behavioral research: Influences on measurement error and participation bias in studies of sexual behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 339–362. Chambless, D. L., & Lifshitz, J. L. (1984). Self-reported sexual anxiety and arousal: The Expanded Sexual Arousability Inventory. Journal of Sex Research, 20, 241–254. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence. New York: Wiley. 700 Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23(5) Cyranowski, J. M., & Andersen, B. L. (1998). Schemas, sexuality, and romantic attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1364–1379. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DeLamater, A. (1987). Gender differences in sexual scenarios. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Females, males, and sexuality (pp. 127–140). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Diamond, J. (1997). Why is sex fun? The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Basic Books. Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A biobehavioral model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. Psychological Review, 110, 173–192. Eysenck, H. J. (1971). Personality and sexual adjustment. British Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 593–608. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1967). Personality structure and measurement. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Fisher, H. E. (1998). Lust, attraction, and attachment in mammalian reproduction. Human Nature, 9, 23–52. Fisher, H. E., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2002). Defining the brain systems of lust, romantic attraction, and attachment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 31, 413–419. Fisher, W. A. (1998). The Sexual Opinion Survey. In C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, R. Bauserman, G. Schreer, & S. L. David (Eds.), Handbook of Sexuality Related Measures (pp. 218–223). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Fisher, W. A., Byrne, D., White, L. A., & Kelley, K. (1988). Erotophobia-erotophilia as a dimension of personality. The Journal of Sex Research, 25, 123–151. Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of sexual behavior. New York: Harper & Row. Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (1973). Sexual conduct: The social sources of human sexuality. Chicago: Aldine. Gallup, G. G. (1986). Unique features of human sexuality in the context of evolution. In D. Byrne & K. Kelley (Eds.), Alternative approaches to the study of sexual behavior (pp. 13–42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–587. Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., McCoy, S. K., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). Death, sex, love, and neuroticism: Why is sex such a problem? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1173–1187. Greenblat, C. S. (1983). The salience of sexuality in the early years of marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 289–299. Hassebrauck, M., & Fehr, B. (2002). Dimensions of relationship quality. Personal Relationships, 9, 253–270. Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological tether. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 151–177). London: Jessica Kingsley. Henderson-King, D. H., & Veroff, J. (1994). Sexual satisfaction and marital well-being in the first years of marriage. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 509–534. Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 93–98. Kaplan, H. S. (1974). The new sex therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Kotler, T. (1985). Security and autonomy within marriage. Human Relations, 38, 299–321. Kravetz, S., Drory, Y., & Shaked, A. (1999). The Israeli Sexual Behavior Inventory (ISBI): Scale construction and preliminary validation. Sexuality and Disability, 17, 115–128. Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley. Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton. Mellen, S. L. W. (1981). The evolution of love. Oxford: Freeman. Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2000). Exploring individual differences in reactions to mortality salience: Does attachment style regulate terror management mechanisms? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 260–273. Birnbaum & Gillath: Subgoals of the sexual behavioral system 701 O’Sullivan, L. F., & Byers, E. S. (1992). College students’ incorporation of initiator and restrictor roles in sexual dating interactions. Journal of Sex Research, 29, 435–446. Reedy, M. N., Birren, J. E., & Schaie, K. W. (1981). Age and sex differences in satisfying love relationships across adult lifespan. Human Development, 24, 52–66. Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books. Rowe, D. C. (1995). Evolution, mating effort, and crime. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 573–574. Schenk, J., & Pfrang, H. (1986). Extraversion, neuroticism, and sexual behavior: Interrelationships in a sample of young men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15, 449–455. Shaver, P. R., Hazan, C., & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The integration of three behavioral systems. In R. J. Sternberg & M. Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68–99). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Short, R. V. (1979). Sexual selection and its component parts, somatic and genital selection, as illustrated in man and the great apes. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 9, 131–155. Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 870–883. Simpson, J. A., Wilson, C. L., & Winterheld, H. A. (2004). Sociosexuality and romantic relationships. In J. H. Harvey, A. Wenzel, & S. Sprecher (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality in close relationships (pp. 87–112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Snyder, D. K., & Berg, P. (1983). Determinants of sexual dissatisfaction in sexually distressed couples. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 12, 237–246. Spector, I. P., & Carey, M. P. (1990). Incidence and prevalence of the sexual dysfunctions: A critical review of the empirical literature. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 389–408. Sprecher, S., & McKinney, K. (1993). Sexuality. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications. Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93, 119–135.
© Copyright 2024