COMPARING THE DEVELOPMENT OF A SAMPLE OF SOUTH AFRICAN PRE-SCHOOL BOYS AND GIRLS UTILIZING THE GRIFFITHS MENTAL DEVELOPMENT SCALES–EXTENDED REVISED TAMARIN ASHLEA JAKINS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MAGISTER ARTIUM IN COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY in the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences at the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University January 2009 Supervisor: Doctor Louise Stroud Co-Supervisor: Professor Cheryl Foxcroft ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The completion of this treatise would not have been possible without the involvement, expertise, guidance, support and encouragement of many individuals. I would therefore like to express my heartfelt gratitude and appreciation to the following people: My supervisor, Dr. Louise Stroud – thank you for sharing your invaluable assistance, expertise and knowledge with me throughout the course of this research. Your constructive feedback and insight paved each step of the way and you taught me to expand my thinking not only on this topic, but also more broadly around the field of child development and developmental assessment. Thank you for your guidance, encouragement and support. You believed in me and it made the difference. To my co-supervisor, Prof. Cheryl Foxcroft - your invaluable guidance and expertise provided throughout the duration of this study is greatly appreciated. Thank you for your guidance especially with the methodology and statistical analysis. I have learnt a great deal in the process. To Ms. Dalray de la Harpe - thank you for conducting the statistical analysis and for your patience in fielding the many questions in this regard. To the testers who assisted me with the data collection for this study - thank you for your professionalism, enthusiasm and dedication towards the assessment of the children. To the National Research Foundation (NRF) - thank you for the financial assistance provided towards this study. Opinions expressed and conclusions arrived at are those of the researcher and are not attributed to the NRF. To Mr. Risdon Harding, Marketing Director of Pearson Education South Africa - my sincerest appreciation for your generosity in donating the school readiness workbooks and parent guides for the children classified within the low socio-economic status group in this study. To the principals of the pre-schools - thank you for allowing the children from your preschool to participate in this study during school time. Your co-operation greatly facilitated the completion of the data collection process and I sincerely appreciate it. iii To the boys and girls who participated in the study - thank you for your co-operation and willingness to be assessed and most importantly for making a study such as this worthwhile. I dedicate this work to you. To the parents of the children – without your consent, this study would not have been possible. Thank you for trusting us with the assessment of your ‘little treasures’. To Dr. André de Jager and colleagues - thank you for your support, understanding and encouragement. To my parents - thank you for the continual love, empathy, support, and comfort you provided throughout the research process. Most importantly thank you for the sacrifices you have made to enable me to see this study through to fruition. I love you. To my friends - thank you for your support, patience and humour. To my personal Saviour, Lord Jesus Christ, with you all things are possible. Thank you for being my pillar of strength, for never leaving nor forsaking me and for guiding me throughout the course of this study. iv CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ii TABLE OF CONTENTS iv LIST OF TABLES ix LIST OF FIGURES x DECLARATION xi ABSTRACT xii CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION 1 1.1 Gender 1 1.2 Child development 2 1.3 Child developmental assessment and the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) 4 1.4 Problem formulation and aims 7 1.5 Chapters of the study 8 CHAPTER TWO: GENDER 10 2.1 Introduction 10 2.2 Clarification of the terms ‘sex’ and ‘gender’ 10 2.3 Historical influences on the research of children’s gender development 11 2.3.1 Meta-analysis 2.4 Gender differences 13 14 2.4.1 Gender differences within language development and verbal skills 16 2.4.2 Gender differences within quantitative ability 18 2.4.3 Gender differences within visual-spatial ability 19 2.4.4 Gender differences in aggression 21 2.5 Gender similarities 22 2.5.1 Gender similarities within language development and verbal ability 23 2.5.2 Gender similarities within quantitative ability 24 2.5.3 Gender similarities within visual-spatial ability 25 2.5.4 The influence of gender on motor development and motor skills 26 2.6 Influence of gender on the performance of South African children on the Griffiths Scales 27 v 2.7 Towards a contemporary cognitive view of understanding gender performance 29 2.8 Chapter overview 31 CHAPTER THREE: CHILD DEVELOPMENT 32 3.1 Introduction 32 3.2 Child development 32 3.3 The impact of early development on subsequent development 33 3.4 Concept of intelligence 35 3.4.1 The psychometric approach to intelligence 37 3.5 Griffiths’ theoretical view of child development 38 3.6 Brief review of developmental theories 40 3.7 Piaget’s theory of cognitive development 43 3.8 Demetriou’s developmental model of information processing 45 3.8.1 Demetriou’s architecture of the developing mind 45 3.8.2 Demetriou’s model of working memory 49 3.8.3 Specialised capacity systems (SCSs) 51 3.9 3.8.3.1 The system of categorical thought 51 3.8.3.2 The system of quantitative thought 52 3.8.3.3 The system of causal thought 53 3.8.3.4 The system of spatial thought 54 3.8.3.5 The system of verbal-propositional thought 55 3.8.3.6 The system of social-interpersonal thought 56 3.8.3.7 The drawing-pictographic system 57 The nature of development 57 3.10 Continuity-discontinuity debate 58 3.11 Chapter overview 59 CHAPTER FOUR: CHILD DEVELOPMENTAL ASSESSMENT AND THE GRIFFITHS MENTAL DEVELOPMENT SCALES-EXTENDED REVISED (GMDS-ER) 61 4.1 Introduction 61 4.2 Child developmental assessment 61 4.3 Child developmental assessment within a multi-cultural context 63 4.4 Psychological assessment measures utilized with infants and young children in South Africa 65 vi 4.5 Developmental assessment measures utilized in South Africa 66 4.5.1 The Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS) 66 4.5.2 Grover-Counter Scale of Cognitive Development 66 4.5.3 Educationally-focused developmental screening measures 67 4.5.3.1 School-readiness Evaluation by Trained Testers (SETT) 67 4.5.3.2 School-entry Group Screening Measure (SGSM) 68 4.5.3.3 Aptitude Test for School Beginners (ASB) 68 4.6 International developmental assessment measures adapted for use in South Africa 69 4.6.1 The Wecshler Scales 69 4.6.2 McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (McCarthy Scales) 71 4.6.3 Bayley Scales of Infant Development-Second Edition (BSID-II) 71 4.6.4 Griffiths Scales of Mental Development (GMSD) 72 4.7 Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) 72 4.7.1 The development of the GMDS-ER: A historical journey 73 4.7.2 Description of the GMDS-ER Subscales 76 4.7.2.1 Subscale A: Locomotor 77 4.7.2.2 Subscale B: Personal-Social 77 4.7.2.3 Subscale C: Language 77 4.7.2.4 Subscale D: Eye and Hand Co-ordination 78 4.7.2.5 Subscale E: Performance 78 4.7.2.6 Subscale F: Practical Reasoning 78 4.7.3 Scoring and administration of the GMDS-ER 79 4.7.4 Reliability 80 4.7.5 Validity 82 4.7.6 The need for South African research on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) 84 4.7.7 Research studies conducted on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales and Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) 85 4.7.7.1 Clinical studies 86 4.7.7.2 Technical studies 88 4.8 Chapter overview 90 CHAPTER FIVE: RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 92 5.1 Introduction 92 5.2 Problem formulation and aims of the study 92 vii 5.3 Research method 93 5.4 Sampling procedure 99 5.5 Participants 100 5.5.1 Matched-pairs procedure 101 5.5.2 Description of the sample 102 5.6 Assessment measures 106 5.6.1 The Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) 106 5.6.2 Biographical Questionnaire 107 5.7 Research procedure 108 5.8 Ethical considerations 109 5.8.1 Informed consent 109 5.8.2 Confidentiality and privacy 110 5.8.3 Fairness 110 5.8.4 Inclusion criteria 110 5.8.5 Investigator competence 111 5.8.6 Feedback 111 5.9 Data analysis 111 5.9.1 The first aim of the study 112 5.9.2 The second aim of the study 113 5.10 Chapter overview 113 CHAPTER SIX: RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 114 6.1 Introduction 114 6.2 Comparison of gender groups in terms of age 115 6.3 Descriptive analysis of the sample’s performance on the GMDS-ER (Aim 1) 116 6.3.1 Mean developmental profile of the whole sample 116 6.3.2 Mean developmental profiles of the 5-year-old boys and girls 119 6.3.3 Mean developmental profiles of the 6-year-old boys and girls 121 6.4 Comparisons of the sample’s performance on the GMDS-ER (Aim 2) 6.4.1 Comparison of overall performance of the gender groups 6.5 Comparison of the Subscale profiles on the GMDS-ER (Aim 2) 123 123 125 6.5.1 Locomotor Subscale 127 6.5.2 Personal-Social Subscale 128 6.5.3 Language Subscale 130 6.5.4 Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale 131 viii 6.5.5 Performance Subscale 132 6.5.6 Practical Reasoning Subscale 133 6.6 Chapter overview 134 CHAPTER SEVEN: CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH 137 7.1 Introduction 137 7.2 Conclusions 137 7.3 Limitations 139 7.3.1 Limitations of the research method 139 7.3.2 Limitations of the sampling procedure 139 7.3.3 Limitations regarding various test administrators utilized in the study 140 7.3.4 Limitations regarding the lack of norms for the South African population on the GMDS-ER 140 7.4 Recommendations for future research 141 7.5 Chapter overview 142 REFERENCE LIST 144 APPENDIXES Appendix A: Information and Informed Consent Form 169 Appendix B: Covering letter to principals of pre-primary schools 174 Appendix C: Covering letter to parents of participants 176 Appendix D: Biographical Questionnaire 178 Appendix E: Report template 182 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Description of the GMDS-ER Subscales 6 Table 2 Classification of breadwinner’s education 97 Table 3 Classification of breadwinner’s occupation 98 Table 4 Classification of socio-economic status 98 Table 5 Sample breakdown in terms of age, gender, cultural group and socio-economic status Table 6 Independent sample t-test comparing the mean chronological age of the boys and girls in the whole sample Table 7 Table 9 115 Independent sample t-test comparing the 5-year-old boys and girls on mean chronological age Table 8 103 115 Independent sample t-test comparing the 6-year-old boys and girls on mean chronological age 116 Overall mean and Subscale performance of the whole sample 116 Table 10 Mean developmental profile for the 5-year-old boys 119 Table 11 Mean developmental profile for the 5-year-old girls 119 Table 12 Mean developmental profile for the 6-year-old boys 121 Table 13 Mean developmental profile for the 6-year-old girls 121 Table 14 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the whole sample 123 Table 15 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the 5-year-old age group 124 Table 16 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the 6-year-old age group 124 Table 17 Summary of the sample’s mean performance in terms of GQ and sub-quotients across the 5- and 6-year-old age groups 126 x LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Griffiths’ basic avenues of learning 38 Figure 2 Demetriou’s general model of the architecture of the developing mind 46 Figure 3 Demetriou’s model of working memory 49 Figure 4 Demetriou’s model of working memory (Adapted) 50 Figure 5 Sample breakdown in terms of age categories 104 Figure 6 Sample breakdown in terms of cultural group 105 Figure 7 Sample breakdown in terms of socio-economic status 106 Figure 8 Developmental mean performance of the whole sample on the GMDS-ER 117 Figure 9 Mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 5-year-old sample 120 Figure 10 Mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 6-year-old sample 122 Figure 11 The comparative Subscale performances of the 5- and 6-year-old boys and girls 126 xi DECLARATION BY STUDENT FULL NAME: TAMARIN ASHLEA JAKINS STUDENT NUMBER: 201346117 QUALIFICATION: MAGISTER ARTIUM IN COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY DECLARATION: In accordance with Rule G4.6.3, I hereby declare that the above-mentioned treatise is my own work and that it has not previously been submitted for assessment to another University or for another qualification. SIGNATURE: ________________ DATE: 27 MARCH 2009 xii ABSTRACT Both children and adults share a common, culturally distinct view of what it means to be male or female. These gender stereotypes are pervasive in society and daily social interactions, and influence all aspects of gender development (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). The inherent physical differences of boys and girls may have triggered the speculation and accompanying myths surrounding the existence of gender differences in childhood development. Many people believe that boys and girls follow a different developmental path that lead to gender differences in intellectual ability. An up-to-date and integrative review of theory and research on gender indicates two opposing perspectives on the topic, with substantial information supporting each view. However, this is the first study to examine and compare a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls from a truly holistic developmental perspective on the recently released Griffiths Mental Development ScalesExtended Revised (GMDS-ER). Developmental theorists have emphasized the profound impact early childhood development has on subsequent development, as it shapes the course of an individual’s life. Contemporary cognitive developmental theories, such as Demetriou’s (2000; 2004) developmental model of cognitive development, advance that child development does not progress in a fixed and predictable manner. Instead, Demetriou proposes that development occurs in a wave-like fashion, where the processes and functions of the various levels of the mind may be at differing points in their cycle of development. When one function progresses to a higher level, it unlocks the possibility for another function to advance to a different point in its cycle (Demetriou et al., 2002). As the significance of understanding the process of early childhood development more fully increases, so does the need to establish with more confidence the value and role of developmental assessment in the early identification of problems. The overall purpose of this study was to generate comparative information regarding the general development of a sample of 5- and 6-year-old South African pre-school boys and girls. Specifically, the study aimed to explore and describe the developmental profiles of pre-school boys and girls within the abovementioned age group with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as their performance on the six Subscales. Then, the mean General Quotients (GQ) and developmental profiles of the pre-school boys and girls obtained on the GMDS-ER were compared. An exploratory-descriptive quantitative research method was used. The sample (N = 64) was selected through a combination of non-probability, purposive and convenience xiii sampling. Within this framework, a between-subjects design in which matching was used to control extraneous variables, was employed. Data was analysed using descriptive statistics and independent sample t-tests to compare the GQs of the gender groups. A Hotellings T2 was used to compare the Subscale profiles. No significant gender differences were found when comparing the overall developmental and Subscale profiles of the boys and girls on the GMDS-ER. However, certain interesting trends did emerge from a review of the findings when compared to the literature review and previous studies. The information generated from this study has contributed to our knowledge base of the performance of South African children on the recently released GMDS-ER. Key concepts: Gender, Child Development, Child Developmental Assessment, Griffiths Mental Development Scales, Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised, Demetriou’s developmental model of cognitive development. 1 CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION The present study focuses on comparing the developmental profiles of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls between the ages of 5- and 6-years-old on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). Thus, this study falls within the broad field of child development as it essentially explores whether the gender groups in this sample differ developmentally. This chapter aims to contextualise the present study by providing the reader with a review of the concepts of gender and child development, followed by a discussion of a holistic and multi-cultural perspective of child developmental assessment in the South African context. Particular attention is paid to the GMDS-ER, as it is the assessment measure utilized for the present study. The problem formulation and aims of the study are provided, followed by an outline of the chapters of the study. 1.1 Gender Starting at the life-changing announcement at birth of the child’s gender, boys and girls are expected to differ from each other in all kinds of ways (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). Research has suggested that knowing a person’s gender causes assumptions and predictions to be made about a whole host of other characteristics about that person, and that very early in development, children themselves learn culturally prescribed gender stereotypes (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). The majority of the research conducted on gender development occurred around the 1960s and 1970s. During that time, the psychometric conceptions of intelligence held a substantial influence on psychometric measurement for the next three generations, and focused narrowly on verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative abilities (Luiz et al., 2006a). The existence of gender stereotypes prompted several researchers to investigate whether gender differences existed within these intellectual domains, generating a substantial amount of research. However, it is important to note that historically many psychologists were opposed to gender comparisons of any nature, fearing that when differences were found, the data would be interpreted and misused in ways that supported a misogynist agenda (Halpern, 1997). As a result, research conducted on gender differences in intelligence has been the topic of much controversy and sensitivity, reflected by the contradictory evidence documented in chapter two of this study. 2 Research findings on children’s gender development have been categorized according to those findings that reveal gender differences and those findings that reveal gender similarities. When intelligence or IQ tests were originally constructed, they were intentionally designed to avoid producing any overall differences in male and female performance (Hyde & McKinley, 1997; Snow & Weinstock, 1990). Careful analyses have concluded that there is no difference in the overall performance between males and females in intelligence tests (Flynn, 1998; Halpern & La May, 2000). However, a pattern of differences have been found indicating that boys are better at some tasks and skills, whereas girls are better at other tasks and skills (Arceneaux, Cheramie & Smith, 1996; Blakemore, Berenbaum & Liben, 2009; Born, Bleichrodt & Van Der Vlier, 1987; Halpern, 2000). In 1974, Maccoby and Jacklin reviewed the results of more than 1,600 studies that compared males and females on some behaviour or psychological characteristic. They were as interested in similarities as they were in differences between the genders, and found that gender differences were well-established in areas of verbal, visual-spatial, quantitative ability and aggression. In chapter two, literature and research studies that have recorded gender differences and similarities between boys and girls will be described according to these areas. The influence of gender on the performance of South African boys and girls on the Griffiths Scales has not received considerable interest. A review of the research conducted on the different versions of the Griffiths Scales indicate that there are more studies reflecting a pattern of gender differences, as opposed to those that have not. Interestingly, a literature survey as well as a conclusive NEXUS search revealed that to date there are no recorded studies in South Africa comparing the development of boys and girls in their early childhood years on the recently released GMDS-ER. 1.2 Child development The growth and development that pre-school children must cover between the ages of two and six in order to develop the thought processes necessary for them to begin school is vast (Fraiberg, 1959; Craig, 1996). Along this cognitive journey, children may encounter several obstacles and challenges, which could hinder their developmental progression. Therefore, one of the goals of studying development is to furnish the practitioner with the theoretical information needed to identify developmental delays as early as possible. Other goals include understanding changes that appear to be universal regardless of culture, explaining individual differences, and understanding how children’s behaviour is influenced by the environmental context or situation (Newcombe, 1996). The growth and development 3 that occurs during the early years of development has an impact on subsequent development. Therefore, the earlier developmental problems are identified and the earlier the intervention can be implemented, the greater the child’s chances are in overcoming their developmental difficulties. Sadly, if developmental problems are not detected in early childhood, the future development of the child can be significantly stunted thus resulting in a lifetime of lowered, untapped potential (Schröder, 2004). Research conducted on gender development attempts to understand whether boys and girls undergo the same pattern of developmental growth. Dr. Griffiths was bestowed the title of the “architect of the most carefully constructed infant scales for her development of the Griffiths Scales” (Luiz, 1994, p.5). She believed that the assessment of mental development should involve a comprehensive investigation of a child’s abilities, including motor, social and cognitive abilities by direct observation, testing and reports from caregivers (Luiz et al., 2006a). Griffiths’ theoretical view of child development contrasted with the psychometric conceptions of intelligence that emerged during the time that the original Griffiths Scales was being produced. She was mindful of the importance of interactions between the various learning avenues and developed a broad-based approach to the developmental assessment of children (Luiz et al., 2006a). The learning avenues of development were operationalised by developing corresponding Subscales. Whilst Griffiths’ theoretical approach may appear somewhat dated, it remains fundamentally sound, and its value is further reflected by the fact that her systemic view is consistent with current theory of child development (Stewart, 2005). A controversy that has existed for several years among developmental theorists is whether development occurs gradually in small increments or dramatically in large periodic steps followed by periods of levelling out. Many of the older developmental theories subscribe to a continuous view of development characterised by stages, whereas contemporary developmental theories are advancing that child development does not progress in such a fixed and predictable manner. Demetriou’s (2000; 2004) developmental model of cognitive development originates from the information-processing perspective, and subscribes to development occurring in a wave-like fashion thereby reflecting a modern systemic approach to child development. Stewart (personal communication, June 30, 2008) considers this modern developmental theory to be the most appropriate theoretical update to the Griffiths Scales. Therefore, this theory serves as the theoretical basis for this study. 4 1.3 Child developmental assessment and the Griffiths Mental Development ScalesExtended Revised (GMDS-ER) A holistic view of child development is emphasized in chapter three and similarly a holistic perspective to the developmental assessment of South African children is vital in view of the poor social conditions the majority of children in South Africa have experienced (Luiz, 1994). Brooks-Gunn (1990) stressed that the measurement of the well-being of a child should include the assessment of the physical, cognitive, social and emotional developmental arenas. Therefore, a comprehensive developmental assessment should include these four aspects of functioning, which are not mutually exclusive (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). “If one accepts the definition of child well-being offered by Brooks-Gunn as the goal we wish South African children to attain, by implication a comprehensive measure of development is required” (Luiz, 1994, p.9). The GMDS-ER plays a key role in this regard as it paints a complete picture of the child’s developmental abilities. Developmental measures are valuable tools practitioners use to guide therapeutic and remedial interventions. Developmental measures can be categorized as either screening or diagnostic measures (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Screening measures usually provide an overall view of the child’s development, rather than information relating to specific areas. Diagnostic measures are more comprehensive in that they provide numerical scores and age equivalents for both overall performance as well as for each specific area assessed (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). In review of the state of the developmental measures in South Africa, Foxcroft and Roodt (2001; 2006) have found that during the last decade, there has been a concerted effort made by researchers to attend to the need for more reliable and valid developmental measures for use with South African children. Researchers have focused on the construction of new culture-reduced tests and adapting, revising and norming assessment measures that have been extensively used in other countries and thus proved to be reliable and valid (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). According to Jansen (1991) there is no such thing as a ‘culture-free’ test as psychological tests are samples of behaviour, which are affected by the cultural milieu in which the individual is reared. Thus, a more realistic approach to developing a culture-free test is to develop a test with content that is based on experiences which are common across cultures and thus proves to be ‘culture-fair’ (Baker, 2005). The concept of play, whilst perhaps in different forms across cultures, is a universal behaviour, and the Griffiths Scales were developed by observing children in their natural environments, and whilst engaged in natural activities (Allan, Luiz & Foxcroft, 1992). Furthermore, several studies conducted 5 within different parts of the world have demonstrated the Griffiths Scales applicability to diverse population groups as they tap into experiences that are common to different cultures (Luiz, Collier, Stewart, Barnard & Kotras, 2000). The GMDS-ER was selected as the psychometric measure to generate the developmental profiles of the boys and girls in the present study. The Griffiths Scales were originally developed to assess child development across five Subscales, from birth to two years of age. In the 1960s, the Scales were revised and extended to cover the period from birth to eight years four months. A sixth Subscale, named Practical Reasoning, was added to the Extended Griffiths Scales. Whilst there is an extensive amount of support for the Griffiths Scales, several comprehensive reviews in the 1980s and 1990s identified areas in which they could be improved (Allan, 1988; 1992; Bhamjee, 1991; Hanson, 1982; 1983; Hanson & Aldridge Smith, 1982; 1987; Hanson, Aldridge-Smith & Humes, 1985; Luiz et al., 1995). These studies found that various items of the Scales were outdated and several items were culturally biased and ambiguous (Kotras, 2003). Therefore, the original Griffiths Scales were no longer providing the practitioner with reliable and valid results on the current evaluations of the child. This provided the rationale for the revision of the Scales. The revision process began in March 1994 and proceeded over a ten-year period. The GMDS-ER was launched in the United Kingdom in 2004. The GMDS-ER assesses the following Subscales, namely: Locomotor (Subscale A); Personal-Social (Subscale B); Language (Subscale C), Eye and Hand Co-ordination (Subscale D); Performance (Subscale E); and Practical Reasoning (Subscale F). A description of these six Subscales, including examples of tasks, is provided in Table 1. These six Subscales constitute the General Quotient (GQ) and produce a Mental Age (MA), which is the child’s developmental age equivalent as measured by the GMDS-ER. 6 Table 1: Description of the GMDS-ER Subscales Subscales Example of tasks Description Locomotor This scale assesses gross motor The ability to run fast (Subscale A) skills including the ability to outdoors, bounce and catch a balance, co-ordinate and control ball, hop on one foot etc. movements. Personal-Social This scale assesses proficiency in The ability to give a home (Subscale B) the activities of daily living, address, to fasten a shoe buckle independence and an ability to and to eat with a knife and fork interact with other children. etc. Language This scale assesses verbal ability, Naming colours, relating a (Subscale C) the understanding of the meaning story about a given picture and of words and the ability to use repeating 6-16 syllable language effectively (receptive and sentences etc. expressive language). Eye and Hand This scale assesses fine-motor Drawing, writing, threading Co-ordination skills and relates to visual abilities beads and cutting with scissors (Subscale D) with handwork. etc. Performance This scale assesses manipulation Working with form boards of (Subscale E) skills including speed of working 4, 6 and 11 shapes, building and precision. blocks to resemble given patterns etc. Practical This scale assesses problem-solving Memory recall and repetition Reasoning ability, basic mathematical of 1 to 5 digits, picture (Subscale F) comprehension and moral arrangements of cards to tell a reasoning. It indicates the child’s story, visual memory etc. ability to benefit from formal schooling. (Note: This table is based on Luiz et al., 2006a, p.3) 7 Research on the Griffiths Scales has primarily been conducted in two areas, namely clinical and technical studies. Since the revision of the Scales, several studies have focused on the performance of clinical and normal populations on the original Griffiths Scales and on the GMSD-ER. In addition, technical studies have been conducted on the Subscales of the GMDS-ER, its psychometric properties and its validity and reliability as a developmental assessment. Research relating to the clinical utility of the Scales has provided evidence that the Scales are useful in the clinical assessment and diagnosis of children from normal as well as diverse population groups. Stewart (2005) has agreed that the value of the Griffiths Scales is illustrated in the fact that they can be administered across all clinical populations and “have proved to be a most effective and efficient tool in the assessment of young children, in a diversity of cultural and social contexts” (p.25). Since the introduction of the GMDS-ER, this updated measure has significantly contributed to our growing knowledge base of child development within clinical samples. Clinical studies utilizing the GMDS-ER have focused on: autistic children (Gowar, 2003), HIV positive/AIDS infants (Kotras, 2001; Sandison, 2005), hearing impaired children (Schröder, 2004), children with cochlear implants (Makowem, 2005), children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Baker, 2005), and a comparison of first and second born twins (Davidson, 2008). Research relating to technical studies has shown that the GMDS-ER is a reliable and valid assessment measure (Beail, 1985; Griffiths, 1984; Luiz, 1988c; Mothuloe, 1990; Stewart, 1997; Worsfold, 1993). The recent revision and restandardisation of the GMDS-ER has necessitated investigations into its psychometric properties. This need for technical studies has resulted in numerous research findings based on studies conducted on the Subscales of the GMDS-ER (Barnard, 2000; 2004; Knoesen, 2005; Kotras, 2003; Moosajee, 2007; Povey, 2008). Other studies have focused on comparing a sample of normal South African children with a sample of British children (Van Heerden, 2007; Van Rooyen, 2005). The present study is a technical study as it assessed a normal sample of South African preschool boys and girls on the GMDS-ER. This study aims to further contribute to the credible use of the GMDS-ER, by forming part of a greater project that intends to generate norms for the South African population. 1.4 Problem formulation and aims To date, no research has focused on comparing the developmental profiles of South African pre-school boys and girls utilizing the GMDS-ER. Therefore, it is to this much- 8 needed area of research that the present study devoted itself. The research question that was formulated to guide the study focused on whether the boys and girls within this sample would perform differently on this assessment measure. Against this backdrop, this study aimed to explore and compare the developmental profiles of a sample of normal South African preschool boys and girls between the ages of 5 and 6 years utilizing the GMDS-ER. In order to achieve this, the following specific aims were explored: 1. To explore and describe the developmental profiles of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group), with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as their performance on the six Subscales. 2. To compare the General Quotient (GQ) and the developmental profiles across the six Subscales of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group) on the GMDS-ER. 1.5 Chapters of the study Chapter two focuses on gender. An overview is provided of literature and studies conducted on gender differences and similarities in children. The reader is then briefly introduced to a proposed taxonomy of underlying cognitive processes envisaged to contextualise findings from gender-related studies. Chapter three focuses on child development. A definition of child development is provided, followed by a discussion of the importance early childhood has on subsequent development. The concept of intelligence will be explored in this chapter, with particular attention being placed on the psychometric view of intelligence as the Griffiths Scales fall within this category. The reader is then introduced to the theoretical tenets underpinning Ruth Griffiths’ view of child development. Demetriou’s (2000; 2004) developmental model of cognitive development is discussed in detail as this developmental theory serves as the theoretical basis for this study. Chapter four focuses on child developmental assessment and the GMDS-ER, with emphasis placed on the need for a holistic and multi-cultural perspective in the developmental assessment of South African children. An overview is provided of developmental assessment measures utilized with infants and young children in South Africa, followed by a description of selected international developmental measures that have been adapted for use in South Africa. The reader’s focus is guided to the introduction and description of the GMDS-ER, as it is the developmental assessment utilized in the present study. 9 Chapter five presents the problem formulation of the present study and the primary and specific aims that guided the research process. The methodology employed in conducting the study is delineated, including the research design, the participants, the sampling procedure and relevant inclusion criteria, as well as the assessment measures utilized and the data collection procedure. Lastly, a description of the statistical analysis is provided, followed by a discussion on the ethical considerations relevant to the present study. Chapter six presents the results and a discussion of the findings according to the specific aims of the study. Chapter seven provides the conclusions of the study as well as a critical evaluation of the study, addressing its limitations, and recommendations for future research. 10 CHAPTER TWO GENDER Gender is not simply something that is imposed on children; at all points of development, children are actively constructing for themselves what it means to be male or female (Golombok, & Fivush, 1994, p.111) 2.1 Introduction The physical differences between boys and girls, men and women are obvious and universal. Literature on gender development indicates that children begin to consistently label themselves as male or female around the age of two years, and that very soon after this, they begin to associate particular behaviours and traits with one gender or the other (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). It is understandable that the existence of gender stereotypes has prompted several researchers in the past to question and investigate whether gender differences exist specifically within cognitive and intellectual ability. “Research is the only way in which psychologists can distinguish between those stereotypes that have a basis in fact (i.e. that are statistically associated with one group more than another) and those that do not” (Halpern, 1997, p.1091). As a result of the myths surrounding gender differences, the value of a study such as this, which aims to explore the developmental similarities and differences between a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls, is realised. In this chapter, the terminology issues in gender-related research will be clarified, followed by an overview of literature and studies conducted on gender differences and similarities in children. Thereafter, South African gender-related findings obtained on the Griffiths Scales are discussed. A description is then provided of a proposed taxonomy of underlying cognitive processes envisaged to contextualise findings on gender differences and similarities. 2.2 Clarification of the terms ‘sex’ and ‘gender’ Until the 1970s, the term ‘sex’ was the commonly used term to refer to boys and girls and men and women, and ‘sex roles’ was the most commonly used term to refer to adopting cultural definitions of masculinity and femininity. More recently the term ‘gender’ has been used to refer to the same things (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.2). One common perspective among many psychologists would reserve the term ‘sex’ for references to biological aspects (hormones, chromosomes, genitals) of being male or female and the term ‘gender’ for references to social characteristics defined by cultural norms and beliefs (Blakemore et al., 2009; Ding & Littleton, 2005; Unger, 1979; Winstead, Derlega & 11 Unger, 1999). However, Maccoby (1988) has argued that by assuming biological causes for some characteristics and social causes for certain others is problematic: “uncovering the biological and social connections to behaviour is a major research objective, not something to be assumed at the outset through the choice of terminology” (p.755). In agreement, Blakemore et al. (2009) emphasize that “it is not always easy to know what is biological and what is learned, and many behaviours may be influenced by several different factors” (p.3). It seems likely that biological and social factors may interact with each other, and therefore in the light of this view many researchers choose to use sex and gender interchangeably (Ding & Littleton, 2005). According to Blakemore et al. (2009), “there is no convention for the use of these terms that is accepted by all scholars of sex and gender, even within a single discipline like psychology” (p.3). For the purpose of ensuring consistency throughout this work, the researcher has opted to use the term ‘gender’. However, it should be pointed out that in this context it is intended to be inclusive of both biological and psychological aspects. Historically, research conducted on gender differences and similarities in intelligence has been the topic of much controversy, which is reflected in the contradictory evidence documented in this literature review. Regardless of the position taken on the many questions that pertain to gender-related similarities and differences, most psychologists would agree that gender is a fundamental component of identity; it is the primary way of classifying humans into groups, a fact that can be seen in all of the artefacts of popular culture (Halpern, 1997, p.1091). The majority of the research conducted on gender occurred around the 1960s and 1970s, when “psychological research had become increasingly methodologically sophisticated, theoretical models were more prevalent, and the second wave of the feminist movement arose on the scene” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.33). During that time, the psychometric conceptions of intelligence held a substantial influence on psychometric measurement for the next three generations, and focused narrowly on verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative abilities (Luiz et al., 2006a). As a result, a large body of research on gender was conducted within these particular domains. 2.3 Historical influences on the research of children’s gender development According to Blakemore et al. (2009), three major works were published in the 1960s and 1970s that had a significant impact on the understanding of children’s gender development. They were Eleanor Maccoby’s book entitled The Development of Sex 12 Differences published in 1966, Money and Ehrhardt’s book entitled, Man and Woman, Boy and Girl, published in 1972; and Maccoby and Jacklin’s book entitled The Psychology of Sex Differences published in 1974 (Blakemore et al., 2009). Eleanor Maccoby has been considered to be one of the most influential developmental psychologists of the 20th century, and her publication in 1966 was regarded as a “major turning point in the study of children’s gender development” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.33). This book focused on understanding the possible reasons for gender differences and not just merely documenting the differences themselves. Research in gender development in the early part of the 20th century focused on the examination of gender differences with little systematic investigation into the roots of these differences and had no clear theoretical foundation on which to base the findings (Blakemore et al., 2009). Mischel’s (1966) social learning theory and Kohlberg’s (1966) cognitive developmental theory was introduced in this text and to date remains among the major theoretical models that guide research into children’s gender development (Blakemore et al., 2009). John Money and Anke Ehrhardt’s publication in 1972 focused on the topic of formation of gender identity and gender role, and they argued that “it was outmoded to ask questions about biological versus environmental influences on gender identity and gender role”, as they asserted that, “development unfolds with a series of interacting influences” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.35). Money has been criticised for focusing so narrowly on biological factors in his views about gender, as well as for not being biological enough (Diamond & Sigmundson, 1997; Rogers & Walsh, 1982). Blakemore et al. (2009) disagree and believe that Money and Ehrhardt emphasized both factors. In Maccoby and Jacklin’s (1974) book, results of more than 1,600 studies that compared males and females on some behaviour or psychological characteristic were reviewed. Compared to previous works on the topic of gender development, Maccoby and Jacklin were as interested in similarities as they were in differences, which set their book apart from previous reviews on the topic (Blakemore et al., 2009). They pointed out a serious flaw in research on gender in that when differences were not found, information about the lack of difference was usually not published. Therefore if a handful of studies on some topic found a difference between males and females, and published such a difference, the finding would be repeated in textbooks and other sources for years, yet there might be many more studies that did not find such a difference that did not enter published scholarship (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.35). 13 This book was organised into three sections, namely a) intellect and achievement, b) social behaviour, and c) origins of sex differences (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Within intellect and achievement, Maccoby and Jacklin discussed research on perception, learning, memory, achievement and ability testing, and achievement motivation. They found that the basic processes of perception, learning and memory were very similar in males and females. Specifically, they concluded that girls had better verbal skills and boys had better spatial and mathematical skills, but found that consistent differences only emerged during adolescence (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). They also concluded that there may be greater male variability in spatial or mathematical skills, but not verbal skills and that despite females obtaining higher grades at school, their achievement was less than males after leaving school (Blakemore et al., 2009). Maccoby and Jacklin found that it was much more difficult to examine social behaviour in children than in the cognitive domain, nonetheless a large number of social behaviours were examined. They found that boys were more aggressive with their rough-andtumble play and fighting than girls, especially in terms of direct, physical aggression. This book has shaped research on gender development for the next generation as researchers are still actively citing it 34 years since it was first published (Blakemore et al., 2009). Research findings on children’s gender development have been naturally placed within two categories, namely those findings that show gender differences and those findings that show similarities. Over the years, these two categories have been given different names, such as the alpha bias or the gender differences hypothesis and beta bias or the gender similarities hypothesis (Blakemore et al., 2009; Hyde, 2005). Due to the vastness of information within this area, research findings documented in this chapter are presented within these two categories. Before this is done, it would be of assistance to the reader to first be provided with an overview of the statistical methods utilized in research on gender differences to enable them to make sense of the findings reviewed in this chapter. To achieve this purpose, the statistical methods of meta-analysis, narrative review and effect size will be briefly described in the following section. 2.3.1 Meta-analysis The question of how to integrate scientific knowledge had become an important one due to the fact that empirical research on gender differences in cognition had accumulated (Caplan, Crawford, Hyde & Richardson, 1997). For most of the 20th century, findings on gender differences were summarised using narrative reviews, in which they read and organised the research and tried to make sense of the pattern of findings (Blakemore et al., 14 2009). The reviews included in Maccoby and Jacklin’s (1974) research is illustrative of this approach. “Although narrative reviews are still being done, another important technique was developed in the 1970s called meta-analysis” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.74). Meta-analytic reviews of gender research emerged in the 1980s and have continued in contemporary work (Hyde, 1986; 1994; Johnson & Eagly, 2000; Kimball, 2000). Caplan et al. (1997) have defined meta-analysis as a “systematic, quantitative technique for aggregating results across different studies, thus reducing some of the subjectivity involved in narrative review” (Caplan et al., 1997, p.31). Blakemore et al. (2009) explain the meta-analysis method in the following edited description. After locating all the studies that include data relevant to that topic, the researcher calculates an effect-size for each study, usually d, and then calculates an average of all effect sizes. The researcher then calculates whether all the effect sizes are homogenous….According to Kimball (2001), most of the meta-analyses on gender differences have data with effect sizes that are not homogenous….The researcher then tries to determine what factors might have led to the different effect sizes and divides the full set of studies into subsets that have similar effect sizes and then attempts to see what differentiates the groups of studies that produce large versus small effects (p.74). Specifically, the concept of effect size measures the magnitude of gender differences, and the most commonly used index is d, which is the difference between the means of the two groups expressed in terms of standard units by dividing the pooled within-group standard deviation (Hyde, 2005). In analyses of gender differences, d represents the ratio of the difference between the means obtained by the male and female participants to the withingender variability (Caplan et al., 1997; Hyde, 2005). Whether the effect size is positive or negative indicates the direction of the difference: a positive value of d indicates higher scores for males, and a negative value of d indicates higher scores for females. Once the value of d is computed for each study, a weighted mean effect size is computed across all the studies (Caplan et al., 1997). According to Cohen (1969), an effect size of 0.20 is small, an effect size of 0.50 is medium, and an effect size of 0.80 is large. Throughout this chapter, the researcher will describe effect sizes, narrative reviews and meta-analyses as findings of research on gender differences are discussed. 2.4 Gender differences When intelligence or IQ tests were originally constructed, they were intentionally designed to avoid producing any overall differences in male and female performance (Hyde & McKinley, 1997; Snow & Weinstock, 1990). Despite the difficulty in looking for differences in intellectual ability in tests designed not to have them, careful analyses have concluded that 15 there is no difference in overall performance of intelligence tests between males and females (Flynn, 1998; Halpern & La May, 2000). However, a pattern of differences has been found, indicating that boys are better at some tasks and skills, such as spatial tasks and mathematical problem solving, whereas girls are better at other tasks and skills, such as verbal fluency, writing ability and perceptual speed (Arcenaux, Cheramie & Smith, 1996; Blakemore et al., 2009; Born, Bleichrodt & Van Der Vlier, 1987; Halpern, 2000). For example, Camarata and Woodcock (2006) examined the performance of more than 4,000 participants ranging in age from 2 to 90 years who took part in the standardisation studies of the Woodcock-Johnson (W-J) series of cognitive and achievement batteries during the 1970s and 1980s. The W-J measures general intelligence, as well as verbal ability, visual-spatial thinking, auditoryprocessing, fluid reasoning, long-term retrieval, processing speed, and short-term memory, as well as subsets of each of those broader domains (Blakemore et al., 2009). As expected, no difference in general intelligence was found. However, Blakemore et al. (2009) emphasized a very significant finding that showed that females had faster processing speeds across almost all of the timed tests and at most ages, although the difference was particularly noticeable during adolescence. “This report of females having faster processing speed has not been widely reported before now, and thus additional research will be needed before it will be possible to judge whether this finding is replicable” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.80). Substantial research has been undertaken to examine differences such as these in intellectual abilities. According to Hyde (2005), the differences hypothesis dominated the popular media with “the belief that males and females, boys and girls, are psychologically different” (p.581). Another common perspective, termed ‘alpha bias’, also emphasizes differences and contends that males and females are very different (Blakemore et al., 2009; Hare-Mustin & Marecek, 1988). Understandably, the examination of gender differences in achievement and cognitive ability has resulted in an ample amount of literature on the topic (Deaux, 1984; Fennema & Sherman, 1977; Geary, 1989; 1994; 1996; Halpern, 1986; 1989; 1997; Linn & Peterson, 1985; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001; Voyer, Voyer & Bryden, 1995). However, it is important to note that historically many psychologists were opposed to gender comparisons of any nature, fearing that when differences were found, the data would be interpreted and misused in ways that supported a misogynist agenda (Halpern, 1997). “In the past, people who maximised gender differences often argued that males had an inborn superiority” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.69). On the other side of the coin, others have argued that the biological, cognitive and social variables that vary as a function of one’s gender are a critically important area of psychological inquiry, and, for this reason, 16 comparisons between females and males should become a routine part of scientific reports (Eagly, 1990; 1994). Thus, extensive studies were carried out to investigate the authenticity of the differences hypothesis, which provided significant contributions to the field of psychology. Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) found that gender differences were well-established in the areas of verbal, visual-spatial, quantitative ability, and aggression. There are fewer studies investigating gender differences in the behavioural areas, as it is a difficult construct to measure. One study investigated possible differences in emotional and behavioural problems between the genders and reported some differences manifested in boys versus girls as rated by their teachers (Butler, Marsh, Sheppard & Sheppard, 1985; Hollis, 1995; Horacek, Ramey, Campbell, Hoffman & Fletcher, 1987; Pianta & McCoy, 1997; Ramey & Campbell, 1991; Reynolds, 1991; 1994). In this chapter, relevant literature and studies that have recorded gender differences between boys and girls will be described below within these domains. In addition, a brief outline of gender differences within aggression will be provided. However, before proceeding to the following section, two common assumptions about gender differences need to be brought to the reader’s attention when reviewing the literature. The first is the assumption that a gender difference in some skill, ability or behaviour means that it is true for all males and females, and the second assumption is that the finding of a gender difference implies that it is biologically based, and therefore results in an unchangeable ability or behaviour (Blakemore et al., 2009; Eagly, 1995; Halpern et al., 2007). Blakemore et al. (2009) cautions, “both of these assumptions are very common and very wrong” (p.70). 2.4.1 Gender differences within language development and verbal skills When investigating whether there are gender differences in the verbal domain, the most obvious place to start looking is in early language development. The examination should be guided by questions such as, ‘Do girls or boys learn to talk earlier?’, ‘Is the language use of young boys and girls different?’ and ‘Does one gender have more language-related problems such as stuttering or dyslexia?’ (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.80). Several studies have found gender differences favouring females from infancy through to the pre-school years. McCarthy (1954) compiled a comprehensive report on children’s early language development, in which girls were reported to be slightly ahead of boys on several measures of language development including age at first word, first combinations, average sentence length at various ages, and grammatical correctness. 17 Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) reported that girls learned to talk slightly earlier than boys, yet the differences were so slight that they were not statistically significant. More recent studies have found that girls are slightly ahead of boys on some measures, particularly vocabulary growth (Berglund, Eriksson & Westerlund, 2005; Fenson et al., 1994; Galsworthy, Dionne, Dale & Plomin, 2000; Morriset, Barnard & Booth, 1995; Rome-Flanders & Cronk, 1995). One study reported that, on average, girls knew 13 more words than boys at 16 months of age, 51 more words at 20 months, and 115 more words at 24 months (Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer & Lyons, 1991). Bornstein, Hahn and Haynes (2004) examined children between 1 and 7 years of age on several different language development measures, such as maternal and teacher reports, standardised measures of language skill, and analysis of transcripts of spontaneous speech. They found that girls were consistently ahead of boys on almost all of these measures between the ages of 2 and 6, but not before or after. “This suggests that girls’ language development is advanced in early childhood, but that boys eventually catch up” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.81). Gender differences within verbal ability between older boys and girls also show a female advantage. Some studies have indicated that girls are superior with executive aspects of language such as reading, writing, spelling, and rote memory (Buffery & Gray, 1972; Hutt, 1972). Hurlock (1981) also found that boys, as a group, lag behind girls, as a group, in learning to talk. She stated that at “every age, boys’ sentences are shorter and less grammatically correct, their vocabularies are smaller, and their pronunciations are less accurate than girls” (p.171). According to Neisser et al. (1996) some verbal tasks show substantial mean differences favouring females, which include synonym generation and verbal fluency. However, there are findings in verbal ability that show a male advantage, for instance McCarthy and Kirk (1963) found a consistent trend across all age levels, that boys were better at visual coding, such as pointing at named objects when the stimulus was visual. Buffery and Gray (1972) have attempted to provide an explanation for the female advantage in verbal ability and suggested an innate neural mechanism for speech perception that is more developed in girls. They found that this is related to the lateralisation of linguistic functions to the left hemisphere. Girls have fewer speech disorders, such as stuttering and dyslexia, than boys (Halpern, 2000; Hyde & McKinley, 1997). According to Blakemore et al. (2009), stuttering is three to four times more likely to be found in boys, and dyslexia, which is a disturbance in the processing of the sounds of language leading to difficulties with reading, is found two to ten 18 times more frequently in boys. “The more serious the dyslexia is, the more likely it is to be found in boys” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.81). Most of the research conducted in this area has focused on linguistic parameters or cognitive content, whereas much less has been done to investigate the functional aspects of language use, particularly as related to gender differences. Work done by Cook, Fritz, McCornack and Visperas (1985) aimed to investigate the functions of interpersonal speech in a sample of young pre-school children (N = 32), and to ascertain how early these differences are apparent if it is found that functional differences do exist. The findings of this study indicated that males talked more to their male peers than did females. The results also showed that males made significantly greater use of statements that expressed their personal desires and statements that asserted leadership, assuming a teacher role, greater competency, and imitation of an activity. According to the researchers, these findings were consistent with other literature on gender differences in social interaction and with the literature on adult males’ conversational style (Cook et al., 1985). 2.4.2 Gender differences within quantitative ability There are many different kinds of quantitative or mathematical abilities and skills. There are more routine abilities, such as counting, and the simple operations of arithmetic such as addition or subtraction; and different areas of mathematics such as algebra and geometry. There is a difference between computation and problem solving, where in the former the correct answer is calculated without making errors and in the latter, one determines the correct procedure to use, and then the procedure is used to find a solution to a problem that is often stated in words (Blakemore et al., 2009). Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) concluded that gender differences are well-established in mathematical ability. According to Hoyenga and Hoyenga (1979) “males show a mathematical superiority” (p.235). The age at which quantitative differences between the genders begins to emerge is unclear. For instance, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) asserted that gender differences in mathematics performance were small or non-existent in childhood and that the male advantage appeared beginning around the time of puberty. However, in a study conducted by Robinson, Abbott, Berninger and Busse (1996) it was concluded that the male advantage in mathematical giftedness could be seen as early as pre-school. Other studies (Hyde, Fennema & Lamon, 1990; Neisser et al., 1996) have focused on the early years of school and have shown that females have a clear advantage on quantitative tasks, but that this reverses sometime before puberty where males then maintain their superior performance into old age. 19 Several studies have arrived at the same conclusion and have found a female advantage in the early primary school years, when mathematics consists of computational knowledge and speed; then little or no gender difference occurs throughout the rest of the primary school years. In addition, they have found a male advantage when the mathematical concepts require more reasoning and are more spatial in nature, in the context of solving problems in geometry and calculus (Geary, 1996; Halpern et al., 2007; Hyde et al., 1990). Males also score consistently higher on tests of proportional and mechanical reasoning (Meehan, 1984; Stanley, Benbow, Brody, Dauber & Lupkowski, 1992). Research has also examined whether boys and girls solve mathematical problems differently, and if different strategies are found whether they could account for differences in standardized test scores (Blakemore et al., 2009). On standardized tests, girls are more likely to do well on problems that are familiar and well-defined and more likely to use strategies that have been taught to them by their teachers (Gallagher & De Lisi, 1994; Gallagher et al., 2000; Stumpf, 1995). Boys are found to perform better on problems that require a less well-defined solution strategy, and more on figuring out the problem at the time, or on using novel strategies (Blakemore et al., 2009). Halpern et al. (2007) cautioned that when interpreting gender differences it is important to examine the distribution of the sample, and emphasized the fact that differences depend on the portion of the distribution that is examined. As males are more variable in quantitative and visual-spatial abilities, they explained that there are more males at both high- and low-ability ends. For instance, researchers have identified mathematically talented pre-schoolers, scoring one or two standard deviations above the norm, and have concluded that males are over represented in this group even at this young age (Robinson et al., 1996). 2.4.3 Gender differences within visual-spatial ability Spatial skill is defined as “the ability to encode, generate, retrieve, or mentally manipulate visual images in some way” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.83). The kinds of items that are generally included are mazes, paper-folding tasks, embedded figures, determination of vertical and horizontal, and imagining the rotation of objects in two or three dimensions (Newcombe, Mathason & Terlecki, 2002). A large body of research conducted over the last 25 years has revealed substantial gender differences for some, but not all, of the measures that reflect visual-spatial information processing (Halpern et al., 2007). Researchers have found that some differences in visual-spatial ability emerge during the infant years. According to Reinisch and Sanders (1992), females have a perceptual advantage seen in early infancy, with 20 fluency differences in the toddler years. The male advantage in transforming information in visual-spatial short term memory is seen as early as it can be tested at around age three and in mathematical giftedness as early as pre-school (Robinson et al., 1996). Levine, Huttenlocher, Taylor and Langrock (1999) reviewed the literature on gender differences in pre-schoolers’ spatial skills and found that, on average, pre-school boys are more accurate than girls at spatial tasks that measure accuracy of spatial transformations and score higher on the Mazes subtest of the Wecshler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence. They concluded that gender differences in favour of boys are present on spatial tasks by age 4 years 5 months. Similarly, Halpern (1993) also found reliable gender differences in visual-spatial tasks by age 4 years 5 months, prior to pre-school. It is interesting to note that some studies, in which female superiority during the pre-school years has been documented, have shown that in subsequent years the gender difference disappears and that the genders are equal until adolescence (Berry, 1966; Coates, 1974). Males then begin doing better than females (Wolf, 1971), and this gender difference may last well into old age (Cohen, 1977). Townes, Trupin and Fay (1980) found that boys are more advanced in spatial and motor skills than girls. Dorfman (1977) found that at all ages between 6- and 19-years, males do better at moving a spot of light over which they have control to intersect a moving target on a screen. Keogh (1971) also found that when 9- and 10-year-old boys and girls were asked to reproduce a visual pattern by walking it out on the floor, boys did better in all conditions. Among tenyear-olds, boys are better than girls at discriminating between various two-dimensional shapes (Etaugh & Turton, 1977). It is logical that an individual’s visual-spatial competency in later years is dependent on the earlier development of these abilities. Cohen (1977) has argued that spatial ability increases with age up to late adolescence, and that at any given age a person’s ability is highly correlated with his or her relative ability at other ages. It has been documented that in tests of visual-spatial ability, post-pubescent males scored higher than females, in fact only about 25 percent of the females scored above the median of the males (Bieri et al., 1958; Hakstian & Cattell, 1975; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; McGee, 1976; Sherman, 1967; Vaught, 1965). Several researchers have wondered whether there is a relationship between math performance and spatial skills, that is whether spatial skills are part of the reason for the gender difference in mathematics (Blakemore et al., 2009; Halpern, 2000; Linn & Petersen, 1986; Shea, Lubinski & Benbow, 2001). Several studies have found that spatial skills are related to math performance, even after the impact of general intelligence is statistically controlled (Casey, Nuttall & Pezaris, 1997; Casy, Nuttall, Pezaris & Benbow, 1995; 21 Friedman, 1995; Geary, Saults, Liu & Hoard, 2000). Spatial ability has found to be highly correlated with performance IQ, which refers to the ability to apply information supplied in a question in both younger and older children; and spatial ability is correlated with verbal IQ, but only for older children (Crandall & Sinkeldam, 1964). South African studies on gender differences within cognitive ability are limited, however some have also supported the above findings. Kruger (1983) found girls to be more advanced in both their school readiness and cognitive, motoric, and language ability than boys (Edwards, 1975; Landman, 1978). An overview of the literature on gender development points to another area in which extensive studies have also been done to investigate whether gender differences exist and that is in children’s display of aggression. As the area of behavioural gender differences is not within the focus of this study, the purpose of its brief exploration in the following section is to provide the reader with an understanding of the nature of the studies conducted on gender differences. 2.4.4 Gender differences in aggression Aggression is typically defined as behaviour that is intended to hurt or harm another, or an action that is perceived by the victim as hurtful (Coie & Dodge, 1998). Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) concluded that aggression was the most consistently found social behaviour that differed between males and females. In addition, they concluded that the difference was found as soon as children were capable of behaving aggressively, and that although aggression itself declined with age, the gender difference continued into adulthood (Blakemore et al., 2009). In their review, they identified different types of aggression, namely physical, verbal, or fantasy (pretending to harm another or thinking about it) aggression. There is also a form of aggression termed social aggression, in which people harm others by manipulating their social relationship, which damage another’s self-esteem, social standing or social relationships (Crick & Rose, 2000; Underwood, Galen & Paquette, 2001). Blurton-Jones (1972) investigated a sample of 25 atypical children to determine which gender displayed more aggression. They found a higher level of aggression in boys than in girls. It has also been documented that behavioural gender differences are found across a variety of cultures. For example, the cross-cultural work reported by Whiting and Pope (1988) involved time-sampled behaviour observations in seven cultures. Two age cohorts were observed in each culture: children from 3 to 6, and from 6 to 10. In all the cultures studied, direct physical assault of one child upon another was rare, but what they did find was that boys engaged in more rough-and-tumble play, exchanged more insults, and a boy was more 22 likely than a girl to counterattack if aggressed against in either verbal or physical form (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Whiting & Pope, 1988). A more recent meta-analysis of gender differences in aggression was careful to control several characteristics of the studies used in the meta-analysis, such as observational versus self-report (Knight, Fabes & Higgins, 1996). They found that there was a very stable gender difference in aggression (d = 0.5 – 0.6), with the largest difference in physical aggression observed directly (d = 0.8 – 0.9). A vast amount of research has found contradictory evidence for the above notion that developmental differences exist between the genders. Proponents of this view advance the gender similarities hypothesis (Epstein, 1988; Hyde, 1985; 2005; Hyde & Plant, 1995; Kimball, 1995). Golombok and Fivush (1994) have highlighted irregularities in research findings that have reported gender differences. Although there is widespread belief that males are superior in mathematical abilities and females are superior in verbal abilities, in fact the research has shown very small differences in these areas. Careful statistical analyses across hundreds of studies have demonstrated that gender differences in ability in math and language are so small as to be virtually non-existent for all practical purposes (p.177). 2.5 Gender similarities According to Hyde (2005), the gender similarities hypothesis states that “males and females are similar on most, but not all, psychological variables. Men and women, as well as boys and girls, are more alike than they are different” (p.581). This perspective has also been referred to as the ‘beta bias’, as it attempts to minimize gender differences (Blakemore et al., 2009). People who minimize differences argue that the differences are small, inconsistent, and artifactual (Hyde, 2005). In Maccoby and Jacklin’s 1974 review of gender research, they found that not only were consistent differences rather thin on the ground, but the few differences that were identifiable were usually of a small to moderate magnitude (Ding & Littleton, 2005). Maccoby and Jacklin dismissed as unfounded many popular beliefs in psychological gender differences, including beliefs that girls are more ‘social’ than boys; that girls are more suggestible; that girls have lower self-esteem; that girls are better at rote learning and simple tasks; whereas boys are better at higher level cognitive processing and that girls lack achievement motivation (Hyde, 2005). Hyde (2005) conducted a review of 46 meta-analyses which addressed the classic gender differences questions – that is, areas in which gender differences were reputed to be reliable, such as mathematics performance, verbal ability, visual-spatial ability and aggressive behaviour. She found that 30% of the effect sizes were in the close-to-zero range, and an 23 additional 48% were in the small range. Therefore, 78% of gender differences are small or close-to-zero. This result is similar to that of Hyde and Plant (1995), who found that 60% of effect sizes for gender differences were in the small or close-to-zero range. According to Hyde (2005), the “results from the review of 46 meta-analyses support the gender similarities hypothesis” (p.581). In this regard, Ding and Littleton (2005) have stressed that it is important to note that a larger number of myths exist regarding the gender differences between boys and girls than the actual differences that have been consistently found in scientific research. Hyde (2005) concluded that due to the fact that Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) only found wellestablished differences within the four areas of verbal ability, visual-spatial ability, quantitative ability and aggression; that overall then “they found much evidence for gender similarities” (p.581). Hyde (2005) and colleagues have criticised secondary reports of Maccoby and Jacklin’s findings in textbooks and other sources, for having focused exclusively on their conclusions about gender differences (Glietman, 1981; Lefrançois, 1990). The literature and research findings demonstrating gender similarities are presented in the following section. 2.5.1 Gender similarities within language development and verbal ability Hyde reported the first meta-analysis of verbal gender differences in 1981, and re- analysed the findings of studies reviewed by Maccoby and Jacklin. Twenty-seven studies of verbal ability included in the original Maccoby and Jacklin review was analysed. Hyde (1981) reported an effect size of 0.24 and concluded that the gender of the child alone accounted for approximately one percent of the variance in verbal performance. The findings from this meta-analysis indicate that although statistically significant, the results are small in absolute magnitude (Bjorklund, 1995). Since the publication of Hyde’s report, several other metaanalytic studies of gender differences in verbal ability have been published, and they generally conclude that whilst the difference in verbal abilities favouring females continue to be found, the effect size is small in an absolute sense, and perhaps even getting smaller over the decades (Bjorklund, 1995; Feingold, 1988; Hyde & Linn, 1988; Marsh, 1989; Plomin & Foch, 1981). Hyde and Linn (1988) conducted a meta-analysis on 165 studies that reported data on gender differences in verbal ability, with the aim to reassess the belief that gender differences exist. Averaged across all of these samples, the mean effect size was –0.11. This indicated a slight female superiority in performance, but the researchers argued that the difference was so small that it indicated that there are not any gender differences in verbal ability. More detailed 24 analysis of various types of verbal ability (e.g., vocabulary, reading comprehension, and analogies) similarly yielded no evidence of a substantial gender difference (Hyde & Linn, 1988). Hedges and Nowell’s (1995) meta-analysis provided similar results and also found that the differences were insignificant. Despite the criticism Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) have received regarding their statistical methods, they too have found similarities between the genders in verbal ability and stated that “for large unselected populations the situation seems to be one of very little gender difference in verbal skills from about three to eleven, with a new phase of differentiation occurring at adolescence” (p.85). Based on the recent metaanalyses it can be concluded that the effect size determines whether the differences obtained are large enough to be considered significant. It appears that there are differences favouring females in verbal abilities, but they are so small on average that knowing the gender of a child would not help one predict how verbal he or she would be. As Bjorklund (1995) has stated, “even if these differences are based in biology, the two genders are biologically more alike with respect to verbal abilities than they are different” (p.348). 2.5.2 Gender similarities within quantitative ability According to Hyde et al. (1990), reviewers have consistently concluded that males perform better than females in quantitative and mathematical ability. The earliest measures of some aspect of quantitative ability begin at about age three with measures of number conservation, soon followed by enumeration (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Several studies have been conducted on pre-school children to determine if the gender similarities hypothesis is accurate. “There appear to be no gender differences in performance on quantitative and mathematical tasks during the pre-school years, or in mastery of numerical operations and concepts during the early school years, except in disadvantaged populations” (Hyde et al., 1990, p.85). Similarly, Farnham-Diggory (1970) found no gender differences in a sample of 282 children between the ages of 4- and 9-years-old with regards to mathematical synthesis tasks. Studies conducted on children in their middle childhood years have also concluded that gender differences do not exist. For instance, in a large study on a sample of 5,020 children between the ages of 7- and 13-years-old, Parsley et al. (1963) found no significant gender difference on tests of arithmetic fundamentals and reasoning tests. Siegel (1968) found no significant gender differences in a sample of 192 children aged 7- to 11-years-old on digitprocessing tasks. Hyde et al. (1990) conducted a meta-analysis of gender differences with regard to performance in mathematics. This was based on 100 studies that provided effect sizes for 254 25 separate samples and represented the testing of over three million people. According to Caplan et al. (1997) the findings were surprising given the long-held belief in a male superiority in mathematics. This meta-analysis provided little support for the global conclusions that “boys excel in mathematical ability” (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974, p.352) or “the finding that males outperform females in tests of quantitative or mathematical ability is robust” (Halpern, 1986, p.57). The overall gender difference is small at most (d = 0.15 for all samples or –0.05 for general samples) (Hyde et al., 1990). As a result, Hyde et al. (1990) concluded that there are no gender differences in understanding of mathematical concepts and in problem solving in the early childhood years. 2.5.3 Gender similarities within visual-spatial ability Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) have located several studies in which visual-spatial ability has been studied in children. They reviewed studies conducted on the spatial subtests of the Differential Aptitudes Test and the Primary Mental Abilities Test, including mazes, formboards and block counting. On the whole, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) found no gender differences in these measures in the early childhood and pre-school years. Shipman (1971) studied a large sample of 1,411 children between the ages of 3- and 4-years-old and found that there were no significant gender differences with regards to identifying and matching geometric shapes. Similarly, Kraynak and Raskin (1971) investigated a sample of 64 3- and 4year-old children and found no significant gender differences with regards to matching two and three-dimensional geometric stimuli. Strayer and Ames (1972) found no significant differences when they investigated the performance of a sample of 40 4- and 5-year-old children with regards to the number of errors they made when completing a formboard and when coping geometric shapes. Brainerd and Vanden Huevel (1974) found no significant differences when they studied a sample of 120 children between the ages of 5- and 6-yearsold in which they had to choose two-dimensional drawings to represent three-dimensional objects. Mumbauer and Miller (1970) conducted research on a sample of 64 children between the ages of 4- and 5-years-old on the Children’s Embedded Figures Test (CEFT) and found no significant gender differences. Schubert and Croploy (1972) conducted research on a sample of 211 children between the ages of 6 and 15 on the Block Design subtest of the WISC and found no significant difference. Recently, Hyde (2005) collected the major meta-analyses that have been conducted on psychological gender differences and included studies conducted on the visual-spatial abilities with children on the areas of spatial relations, spatial perception, 26 mental rotation, spatial visualizations, and the progressive matrices. The effect sizes from these studies ranged between the close-to-zero to small range. South African studies have also found contradictory evidence for the notion that there are differences between the genders. Kroukamp (1991) found that gender did not significantly influence the prediction of scholastic performance. Furthermore, Stevenson, Parker, Wilkinson, Hegion and Fish (1976) found little evidence to support the differences between boys’ and girls’ basic cognitive processes such as learning and memory. In recent years, research has turned to another area of development to examine whether gender differences exist in motor development and skill. A brief overview is provided in the following section on the influence of gender on motor skills. 2.5.4 The influence of gender on motor development and motor skills Motor skills form an important part of a young child’s developmental progression. In the first year of life, babies learn to roll over, sit up, crawl and walk. In the second year these skills become much more fine tuned, and by the age of two children can walk efficiently and do such fine-motor tasks as open drawers and build towers three or four blocks high (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.75). There are no gender differences in children in these early developmental accomplishments (Blakemore et al., 2009). There are no gender differences in the motor decisions young children make, in their ability to undertake risky motor activities, or in the degree of risks they take in these physical arenas as infants and toddlers (Mondschein, Adolph & Tamis-LeMonda, 2000). After the age of two years, gender differences begin to appear in the development of physical skills. Eye-motor co-ordination is accomplished at a younger age in girls, as it is not reliant on muscular strength (Karapetsas & Vlachos, 1997; Pollatou, Karadimou & Gerodimos, 2005; Thomas & French, 1985). Studies on neuromotor development, involving tasks such as repetitive finger movements, side-to-side jumping, walking on toes or heels, or maintaining balance, was completed on 600 Swiss children between 5 and 18 years of age (Largo et al., 2001a; 2001b). Very small gender differences were found, with girls developing fine-motor skills and upper body tasks sooner and boys did better on tasks requiring rapid movement. However, none reached statistical significance. On tasks related to muscular strength, pre-school boys perform significantly better than girls, such as ball throwing speed and throwing distance (Blakemore et al., 2009). In conclusion, differences were found in some motor and physical skills. “When skills reflect underlying neurological maturity or measure fine-motor skills, girls do slightly better. 27 When skills depend on substantial muscle strength, boys perform slightly better” (Blakemore et al., 2009, p.77). Thomas and French (1985) attributed these differences to experiences and gender role socialisation than to differences in the physical capacities of boys and girls. An overview of the abovementioned literature and research studies demonstrates the inconsistencies that exist regarding the possibility of gender differences. The emergence of the gender differences hypothesis and the gender similarities hypothesis indicates the two opposing sides on this controversial psychological topic. South African research studies investigating the influence of gender on performance is limited to findings obtained on the original Griffiths Scales of Mental Development (GMDS). The influence of gender on performance on the recently released GMDS-ER has not been explored as yet. 2.6 Influence of gender on the performance of South African children on the Griffiths Scales The influence of gender on the performance of South African boys and girls on the Griffiths Scales has not received considerable interest, which is reflected in the limited findings documented in this section. A review of the existing Griffiths research indicates that there are more studies in which gender differences have been found, as opposed to those that have not. In a longitudinal study of language and intelligence, Moore (1967) compared the Griffiths General Quotient (GQ) and Speech and Hearing Quotient of boys and girls at the ages of 6 and 18 months. He found no significant differences at the age of 6 months, but at 18 months the speech quotient was significantly higher for girls. Mothuloe (1990) investigated the use of the original Griffiths Scales as a measure of development for black Setswanaspeaking children, in which he adapted a number of the items on the Scales, as they appeared culturally biased. His sample consisted of 45 children in the age range of 5 years 9 months to 7 years 3 months. Mothuloe (1990) found that girls obtained significantly higher scores than boys in respect of the Locomotor Scale. Bhamjee (1991) using the original Griffiths Scales conducted a study of 360 South African Indian children in the age range of 3- to 8-years-old. She investigated the extent to which the variables of age, gender and socio-economic status influenced the performance of her sample on the Scales. Bhamjee (1991) found significant gender differences within the GQ and Personal-Social Subscale, with girls obtaining a higher quotient and subquotient respectively. Knoesen (2003) using the Revised Griffiths Scales explored the relationship between the Scales and Grade One scholastic development. Her sample consisted of 93 children within the age range of 5-years to 6-years-11-months. The results indicated that boys and girls 28 performed differently on each of the subscales. Unfortunately, Knoesen (2003) cannot comment on the significance of this observed difference, as it was not statistically tested. However, the results do prove useful for the purposes of descriptive observations and necessitate the need for further investigation. Knoesen found the girls in the sample obtained slightly higher subquotients than the boys for each Subscale, with the exception of the Locomotor Scale. The girls obtained an above average GQ, whilst the boys’ GQ fell just short of the above average cut off point of 110. The boys in the study obtained above average performances on Subscales A (Locomotor), B (Personal-Social) and F (Practical Reasoning), while the girls obtained above average performances on Scales A, B, C (Language), D (Eye and Hand Co-ordination), and F. Both genders performed within the average range on the Performance Subscale (Subscale E). Van Rooyen (2005) explored and compared the performance of a sample of normal South African children between the ages of 4- to 7-years-old with the performance of a sample of normal British children on the GMDS-ER. The sample consisted of 129 South African children and 161 British children. Significant differences were found favouring girls in the South African sample on the Personal-Social, Eye and Hand Co-ordination, Performance and Practical Reasoning Subscales. It is interesting to note that significant differences were found on all the subscales within the British sample where the girls performed significantly better than the boys. However, no significant gender differences were found in the performance of boys and girls on the original Griffiths Scales (Allan, 1988; Tukulu, 1996; Ward, 1997). Allan (1988) compared the performance of a sample of 60 normal pre-school South African and British children aged 5 years on the Griffiths Scales and found no significant difference between boys and girls in their performance across the Subscales. Tukulu (1996) investigated the relationship between the Denver II Scales and the Griffiths Scales using a correlational method. Her sample consisted of 60 Black, Xhosa-speaking children between the ages of 3- to 6-years. Tukulu (1996) found that girls performed better than boys on both the Denver II and the Griffiths Scales, although the differences were not statistically significant. Ward (1997) conducted a similar study to Tukulu, in which she also investigated the use of the Denver II Scales and the Griffiths Scales using a correlational method but with a pre-school Coloured sample of 50 children, ranging in age from 3- to 6-years-old. Ward (1997) found differences within the performance of her sample on particular Subscales, with few differences reaching the required level of statistical significance. Ward (1997) found that the boys in her sample performed better than the girls on the GQ, as well as on the Locomotor, Personal-Social and 29 Practical Reasoning Subscales. The results also indicated that the girls in Ward’s study performed better on the Language, Eye-Hand Co-ordination and Performance Subscales. An overview of the abovementioned literature and research studies reflects the inconsistencies that exist regarding the possibility of gender differences. Although the literature review indicates that numerous studies on gender differences have been embarked upon, a literature survey as well as a conclusive NEXUS search revealed that to date there are no recorded studies in South Africa comparing the development of boys and girls in their early childhood years on the recently released GMDS-ER. In recent years, the tendency to conceptualise the results of research findings within the context of verbal, quantitative, and visual-spatial abilities has been heavily criticised (Halpern, 1997; Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001). This has led to a taxonomy of underlying cognitive processes proposed to contextualise findings of gender differences and similarities. 2.7 Towards a contemporary cognitive view of understanding gender performance According to Naglieri and Rojahn (2001), understanding gender differences in performance is partially based on the interpretations researchers give to the assessment measures they utilize. They believe that some researchers make such inferences complicated because they do not provide clear definitions of their constructs, the tasks are of a complex nature, and a variety of tests may be used to measure the same construct. For example, quantitative ability is typically measured on the basis of mathematical achievement. As a result, some items are included such as basic maths operations, long division, word problems, oral arithmetic problems, algebra, and the like whereas some topics (e.g., trigonometry) will be excluded in assessment measures aimed at young children (Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001). Similarly, studies that compare girls and boys in verbal ability could include a variety of tasks such as vocabulary, verbal fluency, and verbal analogies that, although all verbal, may have different cognitive demands and lead to inconsistency when measuring the verbal ability construct (Halpern, 1997; Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001). There has been a paradigm shift in views on intelligence that has now incorporated underlying constructs across several domains that have not been recognised in standard intelligence measures and as such cannot be documented. This led Halpern (1997) to propose a taxonomy with the aim of unifying cognitive constructs for interpretive purposes. Halpern (1997) rejected the verbal, quantitative and visual-spatial taxonomy domains. This conceptualisation may have been used so often because it has been a superstructure for group and individual tests of ability since the early part of the 19th century (Kaufman & 30 Lichtenberger, 1999). Although the schema has been popular in psychology and education for about 100 years, its weakness is apparent both in the study of gender-related differences and in intelligence testing (Naglieri, 1999). To replace this organizational system, Halpern (1997) suggested a taxonomy based on “underlying cognitive processes, which offers a more finegrained analysis of how information is retrieved from memory and what participants are doing when they are working on a cognitive task” (p.1092). The idea that working memory can be separated into different functions is also consistent with recent work in cognitive psychology (Shah & Miyake, 1996), and the separation of cognitive tasks into component processing subsystems such as phonological and meaning subsystems is in accord with recent findings with brain-imaging techniques (Halpern, 1997; Posner & Raichle, 1994). From this perspective, Halpern (2001) summarised some of her major findings as follows: Girls outperform boys on tests of verbal fluency, foreign language, fine-motor skills, speech articulation, reading and writing, and math calculation, and they typically earn higher grades in school in all or most subjects. Boys have been found to do better on tasks such as mental rotation, mechanical reasoning, math and science knowledge, and verbal analogies (p.430). According to Naglieri and Rojahn (2001), these abovementioned findings were organised into eight areas that appear to reflect the underlying cognitive processes described by Halpern (1997) as areas in which girls and boys differ. Furthermore, Halpern (1993) illustrated the value of a new taxonomy as follows: In general, those tasks in which females tend to excel and exhibit large differences involve generating synonyms, producing language fluently, and computing and solving anagrams. The underlying cognitive processes for these tasks seem to involve rapid access to and retrieval of information in memory. By contrast, the tasks in which the literature shows that males excel are verbal analogies- the mapping of meaning in relationships, mathematical problem solving, mental rotation and spatial perception and using dynamic visual displays. Here the underlying cognitive processes involve manipulating and maintaining a mental representation (pp.96-103). Halpern’s (1993; 1997) efforts to provide alternative descriptions of the underlying cognitive processes which she used to organise a variety of tasks and her suggested taxonomy, is an important recognition of the need for a carefully articulated perspective, or theory, from which differences between girls and boys can be understood (Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001). McHough, Koeske and Frieze (1986) argued that gender differences could not be understood adequately unless girls and boys are compared according to a theoretical model of cognitive function. Geary (1989) further emphasized that conceptual models of cognitive 31 differences between the genders should provide an integration of the neurological and sociocultural components that influence the development of cognitive processes. 2.8 Chapter overview At the outset it was decided to use the term ‘gender’ throughout this work to maintain consistency as complexity can result when sex and gender are used interchangeably. It was noted that for the purposes of this study, the term gender should encompass both the biological and psychological aspects of child development, and not merely the social aspects as some have suggested. It was explained that the psychometric conceptions of intelligence held a substantial influence on psychometric measurement during the 1960s and onwards for the next three generations, and that psychometric measurement focused narrowly on verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative abilities (Luiz et al., 2006a). As a result, a large body of research on gender differences and similarities focused on these particular domains. The reader was then provided with an overview of the literature and relevant findings within the two differing perspectives, namely the gender similarities and gender differences hypothesis according to the verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative domains. To provide the reader with a comprehensive understanding of the nature of studies conducted on gender, a brief description was allocated to the area of differences in children’s display of aggression. South African research studies investigating the influence of gender on performance on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales (GMDS) was discussed. Furthermore, it was highlighted that studies in this regard in the local context is severely limited which is also reflected in the fact that no studies have embarked upon exploring gender performance on the recently released extended revised GMDS-ER. In conclusion, an overview was provided of a relatively new taxonomy of underlying cognitive processes in which findings of gender similarities and differences can be contextualised. The following chapter will explore the field of child development, with special emphasis placed on the developmental model of information processing theory, which will serve as a theoretical basis to guide and understand the results of the study. 32 CHAPTER THREE CHILD DEVELOPMENT 3.1 Introduction In this chapter, the focus will shift to the area of child development where a definition of child development is provided, followed by a discussion of the importance early childhood has on subsequent development. The concept of intelligence has played an instrumental role in the understanding of how children’s cognitive abilities develop and for this reason will be explored in this section, with particular attention placed on the psychometric view of intelligence as the Griffiths Scales fall within this category. Thereafter, the theoretical tenets underpinning Ruth Griffiths’ view of child development will be described, followed by a brief overview of specific developmental theories that have had a valuable and lasting influence in child development. Compared to these specific theories, which view development from a more psychosocial perspective, Piaget’s theory of cognitive development will be described in more detail as it paves the way for the introduction of the developmental theory used in this study. Demetriou et al.’s (2002) developmental model of cognitive development, which originated from the information-processing perspective, will be unpacked as this developmental theory serves as the theoretical basis for this study as the results are guided and understood in accordance with the theory. It is important to acknowledge the debate surrounding the nature of development as it has continued for many years and focuses on whether development progresses in a continuous versus discontinuous manner. 3.2 Child development The growth and development that pre-school children must cover between the ages of two and six in order to develop the thought processes necessary for them to begin school is vast. During this crucial period young children can make things appear by turning their heads or disappear by closing their eyes, to concept-forming, linguistically competent realists (Fraiberg, 1959; Craig, 1996). Development is a broad term that refers to the “orderly and relatively enduring changes over time in physical and neurological structures, in thought processes, in emotions, in forms of social interaction, and in many other behaviours” (Newcombe, 1996, p.4). The goal of studying development is three-fold, namely to understand changes that appear to be universal regardless of culture, to explain individual differences, and to understand how children’s behaviour is influenced by the environmental context or situation (Newcombe, 1996). Another important reason to study development is the 33 early identification of possible developmental delays (Kotras, 2001; Newcombe, 1996; Schröder, 2004). It is imperative that child development is viewed holistically. This is reflected in the interdisciplinary nature of information that exists about child development. Psychologists, sociologists, anthropologists and biologists have joined forces with professionals from fields such as education, medicine and social services in the search for solutions to problems faced by children on a daily basis (Stewart, 2005). Consequently, the field of child development has practical relevance for several disciplines resulting in a growing body of knowledge reflecting developmental changes that are systemic in nature, and as such need to be studied holistically. Griffiths emphasized the need for a broad-based conception of development, and defined it as “the processes and rates at which growth and maturation of a child’s attributes and abilities takes place” (Luiz et al., 2006a, p.1). Griffiths believed that the assessment of mental development should involve a comprehensive investigation of a child’s abilities including motor, social and cognitive abilities by direct observation, testing and reports from caregivers. In line with this holistic approach, Kail and Cavanaugh (2000) posit four forces fundamental to successful development, namely: 1) biological (genetic and health factors), 2) psychological (perceptual, cognitive, emotional and personality factors), 3) socio-cultural (interpersonal, societal, social and ethnic factors), and 4) life-cycle factors (similar events affect individuals differently). Many psychologists investigated to what extent a child’s development will negatively be affected if these fundamental forces are absent. 3.3 The impact of early development on subsequent development Long before scientific studies of children were carried out, it was an accepted fact that the early years are critical in the child’s development. This was expressed in the old Chinese proverb, “As the twig is bent, so the tree’s inclined”. In a more poetic way, Milton expressed the same fact when he wrote, “the childhood shows the man, as morning shows the day” (Hurlock, 1978, p.25). The growth and development that occurs during these early years of childhood has a significant impact on later development (Luiz, 1997). In studies conducted in several areas of development, the persistence of early patterns of behaviour has been demonstrated. For instance, it has been found that in attitudes and values, individuals change little as life progresses even when marked cultural changes are taking place (Bronson, 1966; Brooks & Elliott, 1971). Likewise, studies of personality have revealed that early patterns persist relatively unchanged as time goes on (Hurlock, 1978). The first five years of the child’s school experience, for example, has been called the ‘critical period’ in the 34 development of the achievement drive. The reason for this, Sontag and Kagan (1963) have pointed out, is that high levels of achievement behaviour at that age are highly correlated with achievement behaviour in adulthood. Results of such studies have justified this conclusion by Bijou (1978): Most child psychologists have said that the pre-school years, from about ages two to five, are among the most important, if not the most important, of all the stages of development, and a functional analysis of that stage strongly points to the same conclusion. It is unquestionably the period during which the foundations are laid for the complex behavioural structures that are built in a child’s lifetime (p.26). Hurlock (1978) believes that as evidence accumulates to show that early foundations tend to be persistent and to influence the child’s attitudes and behaviour throughout life, it becomes increasingly apparent why early development is important. Hurlock (1978, p.27) provides the following explanation of four important factors, in substantiation of this claim, namely: 1. Since learning and experience play increasingly dominant roles in development, as children grow older, they can be directed into channels that will lead to good adjustment. The family handles this task, though the larger social group can provide a culture in which children can fulfil their potentials. Guidance is most needed in the early stages of learning to place the child on the right track, which will result in them being less likely to get on the wrong track later; 2. Due to the fact that early foundations quickly develop into habitual patterns, they will have a lifelong influence on the children’s personal and social adjustments; 3. Contrary to popular belief, children do not outgrow undesirable traits as they grow older. Instead, patterns of attitudes and behaviour, established early in life, tend to persist regardless of whether they are good or bad, beneficial or harmful to the child’s adjustments; and 4. As it is sometimes desirable to make changes in what has been learned, the sooner the changes are made, the easier it is for children and the more co-operative they are in making changes. Against this backdrop, most professionals are of the opinion that the earlier developmental problems are identified and the earlier the intervention can be implemented, the greater the child’s chances are in overcoming their developmental difficulties. Sadly, if 35 developmental problems are not detected in early childhood, the future development of the child can be significantly stunted thus resulting in a lifetime of lowered, untapped potential (Schröder, 2004). McGraw (1935) noted that a measure of a child’s abilities should encompass the child’s physical, mental, motor, emotional and personality development. The GMDS-ER has proven to be effective in holistically assessing a child’s abilities across the several developmental domains listed, and serves as a valuable tool in identifying developmental lags. According to Stewart (2005), it is imperative that “any description of child development must include in the discussion a review of the concept of intelligence, because a major aspect of child development is the development of intelligence” (p.97). Therefore, in the following section the concept of intelligence will be reviewed, with emphasis being placed on the psychometric approach to intelligence. 3.4 Concept of intelligence There are multiple views on the concept of intelligence and about what behaviours constitute intelligence. Initially, intelligence was defined as a general competence - an ability that could be displayed across a wide variety of tasks. However, before long this view was challenged by those who believed that different intellectual skills do not necessarily occur together (Newcombe, 1996). Although many experts agree that intelligence is multidimensional and includes several abilities, they do not always agree on the nature of these abilities. Some divide abilities according to the content of the task - for example, verbal comprehension, number comprehension, spatial relations, social skills, and musical ability (Anastasi, 1987). Others emphasize cognitive processes such as memory, inference, and problem solving (Newcombe, 1996). Although considerable clarity has been achieved in some areas, no such conceptualisation has yet answered all the important questions and none commands universal assent (Neisser et al., 1996). Indeed, when two-dozen prominent theorists were asked to define intelligence, they gave two-dozen somewhat different definitions (Sternberg & Detterman, 1986). Neisser et al. (1996) noted that there are many individual differences to be taken into consideration that could further complicate the attainment of a concept of intelligence that has universal assent. 36 Neisser et al. (1996) noted that: Individuals differ from one another in their ability to understand complex ideas, to adapt effectively to the environment, to learn from experience, to engage in various forms of reasoning, to overcome obstacles by taking thought. Although these individual differences can be substantial, they are never entirely consistent: a given person’s intellectual performance will vary on different occasions, in different domains, as judged by different criteria (p.77). “Defining intelligence in children is more complicated and challenging because the traits that may characterise intelligence in a two-year-old have changed considerably when the intelligence of a seven year old is examined” (Stewart, 2005, p.99). Behaviours that reflect intelligence change with age until a person reaches maturity (Beck, 1994). According to Neisser et al. (1996), “concepts of intelligence are attempts to clarify and organize this complex set of phenomena” (p.77). Although no conceptualisation has been accepted universally as yet, Stewart (2005) believes that “theories guide our understanding” in this regard (p.100). Theories guide research into areas which might not otherwise attract interest or that might otherwise seem too complicated to handle. Like a prospector’s map of a secret treasure, they lead us to expect substantial yields in areas that would otherwise seem to have little promise (Liebert et al., 1978, p.4). Theories are vital tools in all scientific investigations; they provide an organizing framework for observations of children, and theories with a strong empirical base provide a sound starting point for practical action (Beck, 1994; Stewart, 2005). Theories systematise observations into an organised pattern, and also develop a rational explanation of how or why the observed phenomenon occurs (Craig, 1996). The advent of developmental psychology has resulted in various theories, which provide valuable insight into the development of a child. However, as Stewart (2005) has pointed out, “the study of child development does not provide definitive answers as investigators often interpret behaviours in different ways” (p.100). Each developmental theory has positive and negative aspects, but none is likely to be the theory of development. This occurs because of the complexity of developmental processes, different theories address discrete aspects of the process (Craig, 1996). In accordance with the holistic view of development, a child develops across several domains namely, physical, cognitive, emotional and social and therefore theories need to be able to explain such domain-specific development. “In addition, arguments about how to conceptualise human intelligence, as comprised of either a single or multiple factors, have been heard in the field of mental testing for the best part of a hundred years and continue 37 unabated” (Stewart, 2005, p.100; Wood, 1998). Assessment has been the focus of this study and it is imperative that practice is grounded within theory. As the development of intelligence constitutes a major portion of the theory of child development it is necessary to keep abreast of current thinking in this particular area. Furthermore, “while intelligence is only one aspect of development, it is frequently emphasized and it is the main theoretical construct upon which developmental assessment is based” (Stewart, 2005, p.98). In this regard, the following section will briefly look at the psychometric approach to intelligence. 3.4.1 The psychometric approach to intelligence Ever since Alfred Binet’s great success in devising tests to distinguish mentally retarded children from those with behaviour problems, psychometric instruments have gained significant importance in the field of psychology. Tests are used for many purposes, such as selection, diagnosis, and evaluation (Neisser et al., 1996). Despite its apparent usefulness, there has also been a debate regarding what constructs intelligence tests measure and how the results should be interpreted (Stewart, 2005). This stems from the multiple views of intelligence discussed in the previous section. As a result, “some believe that intelligence tests measure a single, underlying general intelligence quotient (IQ), g, while others believe there are up to 120 different abilities” (Stewart, 2005, p.101). According to Neisser et al. (1996, p.78), “one common view today envisages something like a hierarchy of factors with g at the apex, but there is no full agreement on what g actually measures: it has been described as a mere statistical regularity (Thomson, 1939), a kind of mental energy (Spearman, 1927), a generalized abstract reasoning ability (Gustafsson, 1984), or an index measure of neural processing speed (Reed & Jensen, 1992)”. It is important to note that there have been many disputes over the utility of IQ and g, where some theorists are critical of the entire psychometric approach, while others regard it as firmly established (Neisser et al., 1996). Griffiths (1954; 1970; 1984) developed the General Quotient (GQ) of the Griffiths Scales in accordance with the principle of a general factor, and developed six distinct and important avenues of development (Stewart, 2005). The psychometric conceptions of intelligence emerged at the time that Griffiths was producing her original work. Griffiths was mindful of the importance of interactions between the various learning avenues and developed a broad-based approach to developmental assessment of children (Luiz et al., 2006a). Griffiths operationalised her learning avenues of development by developing corresponding subscales. Some learning avenues are more cognitive in content, namely Language, Performance and Practical Reasoning Subscales (Stewart, 2005). Griffiths also ascribed equal 38 value to the other avenues of learning, such as those related to motor development, namely the Locomotor and Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscales, whilst the Personal-Social Subscale addresses the adaptive aspects of development (Stewart, 2005). Griffiths’s broadbased approach conceptualises the holistic view of the child, and as Stewart (2005) succinctly states “its value is reflected in the enduring status of the Griffiths Scales” (p.101). 3.5 Griffiths’ theoretical view of child development Griffiths (1954; 1970; 1984) developed the Extended Scales based on the same theoretical foundation as the Baby Scales. Griffiths’ (1954) view of child development is reflected in her philosophy based on the “the basic avenues of learning” (p.28). Griffiths’ (1954) thinking is in line with modern systemic approaches, as illustrated by Figure 1. Figure 1: Griffiths’ basic avenues of learning (Source: Griffiths, 1954, p.29) 39 The following quotation provides a brief overview of Griffiths’ (1954) description of her basic avenues of learning. The large circle represents the social background in which the child is situated. The social factor encircles the child from the beginning, modifying and influencing all his experiences. The next circle represents the physiological functions and organic movements, awareness of which appears basic to experience. Superimposed on this physiological substrate, certain weak physical attempts to move the body in various ways… lead on to locomotor development. Arm and hand movements, at first vague and poorly directed…develop later into more complicated manipulative acts. For successful manipulative development, both hand and eye must co-operate. The two develop together in manipulative performances of growing complexity. Almost from birth, the normal baby makes vague sounds…listens intently to sounds and to the voices of those around him. Hearing and voice together result in vocalisation and babble, and the development of this is finally speech. All this development takes place in time and space. Performance and Speech are the two main aspects of intellectual development, and together form the basis ultimately of formal education both practical and verbal. A more advanced stage is reached when the older child learns to read and write, for then all four main avenues of learning, eye and hand together with voice and hearing, all co-operate in the acquisition of this complex ability of understanding and reproducing written language….All these abilities form, as they develop, a complex and unified whole (pp.30-31). Griffiths (1954) developed the Baby Scales at a time when the psychometric view of intelligence played a dominant role in test construction and focused solely on the verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative domains as indicators of intellect. Her holistic view of a developing child was in stark contrast to her peers (Luiz et al., 2004a). In constructing the Baby Scales, Griffiths acknowledged this psychometric view and stated that in construction of the Griffiths Scales, she had to “cast a wide net to include a large number of different specific abilities, so that g or general intelligence could be measured in as many as possible of its manifestations” (Griffiths, 1954, p.31). The object of Griffiths’s work was to develop a measuring instrument that would be “reasonably exact, well-balanced and comprehensive so that those activities that later go to make an efficient person, or as many as possible of those skills that are measured in assessing intelligence in older subjects would be represented” (Griffiths, 1954, p.32). The Extended Scales, which measured development in children from two to eight years of age, included a new subscale, the Practical Reasoning Subscale, which records the child’s numerical reasoning ability. According to Stewart (2005), Griffiths’s thinking was considerably advanced for that time period as it is in line with current contemporary developmental theories. 40 Stewart (2005) described Griffiths’ contribution to child development in the following paragraph. Griffiths’ theoretical approach may appear somewhat dated, but it remains fundamentally sound. Most of its features are consistent with current theory of child development, as Griffiths acknowledged the child within his social systems; she recognised the physiological aspect of child development; and she attributed equal importance to the psychological aspects of the child. She identified six domains of development, which she stated provided a thorough view of development. She maintained that the domains measured separate abilities, but emphasized that there is also considerable overlap between them. All these aspects are consistent with current thinking on child development (p.97). 3.6 Brief review of developmental theories “A theory of child development can be likened to a lens through which we view children and their growth. The theory filters out certain facts and gives a particular pattern to those it lets in” (Thomas, 2005, p.4). This quote aptly describes the importance of developmental theories, and illustrates the valuable insight they provide into child development. Theorists agree that children are actively engaged in their own development. Children try to make sense of the world around them, and in doing so, construct ideas and hypotheses about how the world works. Children’s understanding of the world depends on their ability to understand information, to make logical inferences, and to draw conclusions, and these skills develop throughout childhood (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). A brief overview will be provided on selected theories, followed by a detailed examination of a cognitive developmental model of information processing, which will be used to frame the results of this study. Sigmund Freud was the first psychologist to focus on developmental issues and particularly on very early development as an important psychological period (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). His prominent theory was also the driving force of the psychoanalytic tradition. Freud’s psychoanalytic theory describes the development of an individual as a succession of stages determined by maturation and that progression from one stage to the next is seen as the result of changes in the source of sexual energy (Meyer, Moore & Viljoen, 2002). Freud produced a model of child development that featured a series of growth stages. He labelled these growth steps psychosexual phases because he believed the development of the personality-the psyche-was critically influenced by the manner in which the child learned to expend sexual energy (libido) from one period of life to the next (Thomas, 2005). The psychosexual stages that form the cornerstone of his development theory are: 1) oral (within 41 the first year), 2) anal (within the second year), 3) phallic (within the third and fifth year), 4) latent (within the fifth and sixth year), and 5) genital (seventh year onwards). The name of each stage represents the most important physiological source of sexual energy at that stage. Freud believed that personalities are divided into three parts, namely the id, ego and superego. In addition, he delineated that there are three levels of consciousness, which are the conscious, preconscious and the subconscious (Louw & Edwards, 1997). The participants of this study fall within the latent stage of Freud’s theory. For the purposes of this study, it is interesting to note that five and six year-olds are mainly concerned with learning their gender role, and this period has often been called the gang age because children seldom voluntarily form groups that include the opposite sex (Thomas, 2005). During this stage, Freud believes that children play mainly with friends of their own sex because they want to consolidate their acquisition of appropriate sex-role behaviours (Meyer et al., 2002). Erik Erikson was a German-born psychoanalyst whose self-imposed mission was to extend and refine Freud’s notions of personality development, with particular attention to child development. Erikson’s theory of child development was guided by his adherence to the epigenetic principle, which refers to the belief that “everything that grows is governed by a present construction plan” (Thomas, 2005, p.88). Erikson acknowledged cultural variations that could exist in the developmental process, which he believed may influence the types of interactions children would have as they would be varied from one culture to another. However, he held that “personality growth follows a sequence of inner laws that set the potentialities for the kinds of significant interactions children will achieve with the people and institutions they meet in their particular culture” (Thomas, 2005, p.88). Erikson’s ego psychological developmental theory is based on stages. Erikson believed that the interaction of individuals with their social environment produces a series of eight major psychosocial crises the individual must work through in order to achieve eventual ego identity and psychological health. At each stage, synthesis of a preferred choice and the developmental crisis should be achieved in order to advance to the next stage. The eight stages are: 1) basic trust versus basic mistrust (within the 1st year), 2) autonomy versus shame and doubt (2nd to 3rd year), 3) initiative versus guilt (3rd to 6th year), 4) industry versus inferiority (6th to 12th year), 5) identity versus identity diffusion (12th to 18th year), 6) intimacy versus isolation (18th to 25th year), 7) generativity versus self-absorption or stagnation (25th to 65th year), and 8) integrity versus despair (60 years and onwards) (Davidson, 2008; Thomas, 2005). According to Erikson the participants in this study fall within the initiative versus guilt and industry versus inferiority stages. Children in the initiative versus guilt stage gain more 42 skill in using language, in moving about, and in handling things. The psychosocial modalities of intrusion and inclusion emerge during this stage, and children are able to act on their initiative and may experience guilt about their behaviour in which they feel conflicted between their abilities to intrude into other people’s lives and their new-found realisation of moral rules, which are encouraged by their identification with the parent of the same-sex (Meyer et al., 2002). This stage is important in the development of the conscience and the danger of this stage is that the conscience will develop too strictly or in a moralistic way. Therefore, the ideal resolution of the crisis lies in finding a balance between the childlike enthusiasm for doing and making things and the tendency to be too strict in self-judgement. The ego strength that emerges in this stage is that of purpose (Meyer et al., 2002; Thomas, 2005). Children in the industry versus inferiority stage develop a sense of industry, learn to handle the tools of their culture and become “keen collaborators in any productive process” (Meyer et al., 2002, p.200). Children want to earn recognition by producing something and to gain the satisfaction of completing work by perseverance. Children will develop a sound sense of industry if they are provided with learning tasks and subsequent guidance by society that they can accomplish and recognize as being worthy of their efforts. However, if the child is not provided with such learning opportunities and thus not adequately prepared for school life, a feeling of inadequacy and inferiority can develop (Thomas, 2005). Alfred Adler (1930) viewed the first five years of life as being important rather than demarcating developmental stages. Adler’s individual psychology developmental theory held that within the first five years of life a prototype is formed. Adler emphasized certain problems confronting the individual at specific age levels, which can be grouped into three categories, namely occupational, social and sexual. Furthermore, he believed that there are three childhood situations, namely inferiority, pampering and neglect. Adler proposed that two types of factors have a significant effect on development, which is constitutional and social environmental factors, where he considered influences being physical and family respectively (Davidson, 2008). Compared to the abovementioned theories, which view development from a more psychosocial perspective, the Piagetian approach to development is based in cognitive theory. “Piaget’s theory of cognitive development has provided an invaluable contribution which has had a lasting influence on the field of developmental assessment” (Luiz, 1994, p.4). The theorist who most advanced our understanding of cognitive development and provided an enduring influence on our thinking of child development is Piaget (Stewart, 2005). Modern cognitive theory has built on aspects of 43 Piaget’s developmental theory and his profound observations, and it is for this reason that his theory is examined in more detail below. 3.7 Piaget’s theory of cognitive development Many regard Piaget to be the cognitive developmental psychologist of the twentieth century (Moseley et al., 2005). Drawing upon biology, sociology and philosophy, his psychological theorising and methodologies revolutionised a field that was dominated by the contrasting perspectives of environmentalism and biological determinism (Moseley et al., 2005). Piaget’s ‘clinical method’ of observing children in their natural environments, as well as interviews with children engaged in various intellectual tasks, originated in the 1920s (Moseley et al., 2005). Piaget held a similar philosophy to Griffiths in that he too valued seeing and understanding the world from the child’s perspective. The central tenet of his theory of cognitive development was that the child passed through a set of ordered, qualitatively different stages. In essence, Piaget’s theory is described as a stage theory (Moseley et al., 2005). Accordingly, he viewed development as unfolding through a series of stages, characterising an invariant developmental sequence. The child must progress through each of the stages in exactly the same order and no stage can be missed out. The stages are associated with characteristic age periods although considerable individual differences can be observed (Craig, 1996; Moseley et al., 2005). There are four stages, namely: 1) sensorimotor stage (0-2 years), 2) pre-operational stage (2 to 7 years), 3) concrete operational stage (7 to 11 years), and 4) formal operational thought (11 years onwards) (Craig, 1996). According to Piaget, the participants in this study would fall within the pre-operational stage. During this stage, the child begins to communicate by using symbols and language. Piaget considered the ability to grasp the logic of relations and classes as underpinning intelligence and argued that children at this stage tend to focus upon one aspect of an object or a situation at a time. Through a number of experiments, Piaget concluded that pre-operational children experience difficulty in solving problems involving class-inclusion, conservation and transitive reasoning. At this age children continue to display egocentrism, thus experiencing difficulty in recognising that their own thoughts and perceptions may differ from those of others (Craig, 1996; Moseley et al., 2005). Piaget believed that the impetus towards further cognitive development and growth occurs when cognitive structures are incapable of reconciling conflict between existing understandings and current experience. Development is thus facilitated through the processes of assimilation, disequilibration and accommodation (Craig, 1996). Assimilation is the interpretation of events using existing cognitive structures, 44 whereas accommodation describes the process whereby existing representations are modified to encompass new experiences that cannot be assimilated. Cognitive restructuring involving the development of more sophisticated schemata is the natural outcome. “This process has been likened to the process of a filing system in which assimilation involves filing material into existing categories, and adaptation involving modification of the existing system because new material does not fit these” (Moseley et al., 2005, pp.191-192). Piaget’s theory had a substantial influence on various schools of thought studying intellectual development in childhood. According to Weinert and Weinert (2005), “Piaget’s work ultimately caused a revolution in developmental theory with such concepts as activity, adaptation, self-regulation, construction, and cognitive structures occurring in a universal sequence of qualitatively different developmental stages” (p.192). However, there have been many theoretical and methodological criticisms of Piaget’s theory. These criticisms have been documented by Moseley et al. (2005) and will be described briefly. They found that investigations that have attempted to replicate Piaget’s findings have yielded different results. Such studies have demonstrated that children are capable of performing many cognitive tasks and operations at an earlier age than Piaget outlined. Subsequently, it is now widely accepted that Piaget underestimated the importance of social meaning and context of his experiments, and the importance of children’s linguistic facility in understanding and responding to his questions. Therefore, as a result many children failed to demonstrate their true capability in the experiments. Studies have also indicated that individual’s operate at different levels in different domains, which challenges Piaget’s notion that developmental stages had overarching structures that operated across multiple domains (Bidell & Fischer, 1992). Lastly, Piaget’s stepwise view of development is outdated with the latest findings in cognitive development. There appears to be a considerable amount of overlap between stages, and therefore it is more appropriate to regard development as a continuous process (Moseley et al., 2005). In light of these criticisms, Piaget’s ideas were subsequently harnessed along cognitive psychology’s information-processing paradigm resulting in the neo-Piagetian theories of writers such as Pascual-Leone (1970), Fischer (1980), Case (1985) and Demetriou (1998). This has resulted in a substantial body of more recent and contemporary literature on cognitive development from the information-processing perspective. The informationprocessing theorists have constructed their theory on the foundation of Piaget’s cognitive theory, using empirical methods to contribute to the existing knowledge base of cognitive development. They have developed a groundbreaking theory that subscribes to the notion of development neither being continuous nor discontinuous but instead occurring in a wave-like 45 fashion reflecting a more modern systemic approach to child development. This developmental theory serves as the theoretical basis for this study as the results presented in chapter six are guided and understood in accordance with the theory. 3.8 Demetriou’s developmental model of information processing Demetriou and his colleagues set out to validate through empirical research an integrated developmental model of the mind, first outlined in 1985 by Demetriou and Efklides, and further developed by Demetriou (1993) and Demetriou, Efklides and Platisdou (1993). In addition to a series of cross-sectional studies (Demetriou & Kazi, 2001), they carried out a longitudinal study in which specially devised assessments were regularly administered over a three-year period (Demetriou, Christou, Spanoudis & Platisdou, 2002). Demetriou’s overall aim is to build and validate an overarching theoretical model, thereby “laying the ground for integrating the study of intelligence and cognitive functioning with the study of personality and self” (Demetriou & Kazi, 2001, p.218). Since the end of the nineteenth century, three fields of psychology have attempted to understand the human mind: cognitive, differential and developmental psychology. Demetriou (2000; 2004) believes that the three fields must be integrated if a general theory of intelligence and mind is to become possible. Demetriou et al. (2002) view the developing intellect as a multi-level and multisystem structure. This structure includes four basic constellations of processes and abilities: processing efficiency, working memory, thinking and problem solving, and self-awareness and self-regulation. They believe that these are likely to be the components of g, the general factor of intelligence. According to this theory, the architecture of the human mind includes two basic hierarchical levels of knowing (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004). The first level involves systems oriented to the environment, and the second level involves systemoriented constructs. At the intersection of these two levels, there is a functional system that defines the activation and the interaction between the two knowing levels. It also needs to be noted that the processes and structures in each of the levels are themselves hierarchically organised (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004). 3.8.1 Demetriou’s architecture of the developing mind Demetriou et al.’s (2002) general model of the mind is based on the three concentric circles shown in Figure 2, which represents seven specialised capacity systems or spheres (SCSs), processing capacities and the hypercognitive system. 46 Figure 2: Demetriou’s general model of the architecture of the developing mind Note: This figure is based on Demetriou et al., 2002, p.5 (Source: Moseley et al., 2005, p.226) The first knowing level involves systems addressed to the environment. The input coming to this level is information coming from the environment and its output are actions, overt or covert, directed to the environment (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004). Through empirical studies, these environment-oriented systems have been delineated into seven systems of thought that specialise in the representation and processing of different domains of relations in the environment (Demetriou et al., 2002). These systems are called specialised capacity systems (SCSs) because each of them includes a characteristic set of operations and processes which are appropriate for thinking and problem solving within its domain of application (Kargopoulos & Demetriou, 1998). In addition, each of the seven systems is biased to a different symbol system, the one most appropriate for that specialised capacity 47 system’s representational and computational needs, such as language for the propositional system, mental images for the spatial system, and mathematical symbols for the quantitative system (Demetriou, Efklides & Platsidou, 1993). Development of thinking and problem solving within each SCS is influenced through the combined influence of constitutional (maturational), socio-cultural and experiential factors and inconsistent performance at the transition zones between levels in Figure 2 is very common. Equally, Demetriou et al. (2002) found that development is very often uneven across developmental domains (Demetriou, 2004; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004; Moseley et al., 2005). The four developmental stages identified in Figure 2 are essentially those of Piaget: 1) sensorimotor, 2) pre-operational, 3) concrete operational, and 4) formal operational (Moseley et al., 2005). The seven SCSs will be examined in more detail later in this section and its applicability to the Griffiths Scales. The second level of knowing which involves system-oriented constructs refers to the hypercognitive system. Hypercognition refers to the supervision and co-ordination of cognition (Demetriou et al., 2002). The input to the hypercognitive system is information coming from the first level (SCSs). This information is organised into the maps or models of mental functions, which are used to guide the control of the functioning of the SCSs and the processing potentials available (Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The hypercognitive system is conceived as being an interface between mind and reality, between aspects of cognition, and between processing capacities and the SCSs (Demetriou et al., 2002; Moseley et al., 2005). The hypercognitive system is hierarchically organised and is described as having an active self-knowing component (working hypercognition) and a self-descriptive component (long-term hypercognition) (Demetriou et al., 2002). Working hypercognition’s efficiency is dependent on the individual’s processing capacities. The function of working hypercognition is concerned with organising, monitoring and evaluating the responses and performances of the self and of others, while long-term hypercognition incorporates a model of the mind, a general model of intelligence and self-concept (Demetriou et al., 2002). Working hypercognition “is responsible for the management of the processing system” and “carries over to it, so to speak, both the person’s personhood and the person’s more general views about the mind” (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999, pp.328-329). The functions of the working and long-term hypercognitive systems are summarised in Figure 3. At the intersection of the environment-oriented level (SCSs) and the self-oriented level (hypercognitive system), is the processing system or capacities. The processing capacities, which consists of three dimensions, namely: speed of processing, attentional 48 control of processing and working memory are present in all thinking and have a major influence on general problem solving (or psychometric g) (Demetriou et al., 2002). Speed of processing refers to the maximum speed at which a given mental act may be efficiently executed, thereby conserving mental resources. More specifically, it indicates the time needed by the system to record and give meaning to information (Demetriou et al., 2002). Control of processing refers to the processes that enable the thinker to stay focused on the information of interest while filtering out interfering and goal-irrelevant information (Macleod, 1991; Posner & Raicle, 1997; Sternberg, 1975). Working memory refers to the processes enabling a person to hold information in an active state while integrating it with other information until the current problem is solved (Demetriou et al., 2002). The optimal level of development within the processing system is when an individual achieves processing efficiency (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004; Demetriou et al., 2002). Processing efficiency can be described as the capacity of the thinker to focus on goalrelevant information, and to make use of available mental resources as efficiently as possible (Demetriou et al., 2002). The processing system is a dynamic field that is always occupied by elements coming from the other hierarchical levels, in proportions that vary from moment to moment. Specifically, the input to this system is environment relevant information, skills, and processes, which pertain to a SCS, and monitoring, regulation, and evaluation processes, which pertain to the hypercognitive system (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004). The hypercognitive system is responsible for effecting the orchestration and the processing of the SCSs and for the evaluation of the outcome of processing in relation to the goal of processing. Working hypercognition specifically is the management system, which is responsible for the management of the processing system (Demetriou et al., 1993). As shown in Figure 2, the SCSs, the hypercognitive system, and the processing system are at one and the same distinct and dynamically intertwined (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004). That is, the SCSs are formulated in response to the structure of the environment and become operative via the processing system and known via the hypercognitive system (Demetriou, 2004; Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The processing system is void if not fed by the SCSs and undirected if not controlled by the hypercognitive system. Therefore, whilst they are recognised as different operational and functional systems, there are still important generic influences at work, involving processing efficiency, working memory, self-awareness and self-regulation (Demetriou et al., 2002). The processing capacities and hypercognitive system direct and manage each domain of thought (SCS) resulting in its efficient application and 49 functioning. Furthermore, the domains would not be specialised if the functions were not directed to the correct environmental stimulus or input. Figure 3: Demetriou’s model of working memory (Source: Demetriou et al., 2002, p.8) 3.8.2 Demetriou’s model of working memory Demetriou et al.’s (2002) model of working memory incorporates and extends that of Baddeley and Hitch (1974). Baddeley’s (1990; 1993) model, which has received extensive empirical and theoretical scrutiny in the past ten years, is widely regarded as a good approximation of the architecture of working memory (Caplan & Waters, 1999; Engle, 2002; Kemps, De Rammelaere & Desmet, 2000; Morra, 2000; Ribaupierre & Bailleux, 2000; Scheider, in press; Swanson & Sachse-Lee, 2001). As shown in Figure 3, Demetriou incorporated the SCSs, processing capacities and long-term hypercognitive system to his model of working memory to demonstrate the interrelatedness of the various components (Demetriou, 2000; 2004; Demetriou et al., 2002). In Demetriou’s model of working memory, the working hypercognitive system is responsible for managing the operations of the specialised storage system, the SCSs and the hypercognitive system. The specialised storage system receives information from the 50 environment. The visual, phonological and other specialised storage units draw on different environmental sources. For example, the phonological storage unit only stores incoming verbal information, whereas the visual storage unit only stores visual-spatial information. The information is then sent to the appropriate SCS to be processed. For example, the working hypercognitive system sends the information from the phonological storage unit to the verbalpropositional SCS. The output from the SCS is then stored in the working hypercognitive system and co-ordinated with the existing knowledge filed in the long-term hypercognitive system. The functioning of these interrelated components is dependent on the individual’s processing capacities (Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The processing capacities determine how effectively the information is channelled through the various components and retained in working memory. The role of processing capacities is reflected in Figure 4. Well-developed processing capacities ensure that less time is needed to record and process the information thereby increasing the individual’s working and long-term memory capacity (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Figure 4: Demetriou’s model of working memory (Adapted) (Source: Demetriou et al., 2002, p.8 [Adapted]) According to Demetriou et al. (2002) the role of working memory in the functioning of thinking and problem solving remains strong. They believe that it is one of the main factors underlying individual differences in thinking and problem solving. In other words, that 51 working memory is a mechanism for the implementation of the processing potential of a given age into actual thinking and problem solving skills and abilities. Thus, differences between individuals in the condition or use of this mechanism results in differences in how fully they can transform their processing potentials into actual thinking and problem solving skills and abilities (Demetriou et al., 2002). It is, however, recognised that individual differences depend on social and cultural opportunities, such as education, the complexity of the social environment in which the child develops and so forth (Demetriou et al., 2002). 3.8.3 Specialised capacity systems (SCSs) As previously mentioned, Demetriou et al. (2002) categorized the environment- oriented level into seven domains of thought. These systems of thought were induced on the basis of evidence generated by a large number of studies, such as factorial studies looking for underlying factors standing for different abilities and found the SCSs always coming out as independent factors (Demetriou et al., 2002). These domains specialise in the representation and processing of their corresponding relations in the environment. They are specialised because each domain requires a particular method of processing information, and uses different symbol systems. Due to the differing processes that occur within each SCS and accompanying symbol system, the development of each system may proceed through partially different types of developmental sequences and at different rates of progression (Demetriou & Efklides, 1987; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 1999; 2004; Demetriou, Raftapoulos & Kargopoulos, 1999; Demetriou & Valanides, 1998; Demetriou et al., 2002). The seven SCSs developed by Demetriou et al. (2002) are the systems of categorical thought, quantitative thought, causal thought, spatial thought, verbal-propositional thought, social-interpersonal thought and the drawing-pictographic system. Within each SCS consists a hierarchy of three elements, namely 1) core elements, 2) operations, rules, skill processes and problem solving, and 3) knowledge and beliefs (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The domain and composition of each of these seven systems is presented below. 3.8.3.1 The system of categorical thought The primary function of categorical reasoning is to enable the child to identify information that is important for the task at hand and reduce unnecessary complexity so as to facilitate future information selection needs. Therefore, this domain specialises in the handling of similarities and differences between objects (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Five- and six-year-old children pre-select a criterion for categorization and apply this criterion 52 to specify the identity of objects and their similarity relations (e.g., colour, shape etc.) Children then use this criterion to classify things on the basis of having or not having this attribute (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Browsing, scanning, and comparison, which enable the child to identify the properties related to a categorization task, are examples of the operations involved in this system (Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Classification and rule-induction strategies, which primarily involve organisation and reduction of information into more general mental schemes, are examples of processes aimed at the simplification of complexity for the sake of mental economy (Demetriou et al., 2002). One of the items in the GMDS-ER within the Performance Subscale assesses a child’s ability to assemble brick-boxes by colour at the age of four-years-old. The test administrator places the lids in front of the incorrect box and asks the child to put the bricks back in their boxes with the lids on. The four-year-old child selects the classification strategy of colour to categorize the different objects, and assembles the brick-boxes with the corresponding colour bricks and lids to complete the task. The child was not instructed to match the same colour box with its lid; the child did this instinctively. This criterion is an example of one of the core elements involved in this domain, that of categorical perception according to colour (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The formboard items within the Performance Subscale require the child to use the core element of categorical perception according to shape, and then match the similar insets with the corresponding place on the board. Without this classification strategy this can prove to be quite a complex mental activity for a child. Fiveand six-year-olds are able to understand class inclusion relations based on natural kinds as well (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). For instance, pre-school children understand that dogs are members of the same species and cats are members of another species. In other words, the core element within the system of categorical thought is that of categorical perception and action (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The second level consists of the operations that enable the child to spot and process similarities and differences between objects. The third level, the knowledge and beliefs within that SCS refer to a person’s conceptions and misconceptions about the world, such as concepts people have about physical phenomena or different types of people for instance (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). 3.8.3.2 The system of quantitative thought According to Demetriou et al. (2002) all elements of reality can potentially undergo quantitative transformations. They believe that things aggregate or separate so that they increase, decrease, split or multiply in space or time for many different reasons. Demetriou 53 (2004) identified subitization as the core process involved in this system. Subitization is defined as “the ability to specify the numerosity of small sets (smaller than three or four elements) by simply looking at them” (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004, p.30). One of the items within the Practical Reasoning Subscale on the GMDS-ER assesses a four-year-old’s ability to count four bricks correctly. However, it has been qualitatively observed that several children that have been assessed on the GMDS-ER knew that there were four bricks without needing to count. The second level of this domain involves operations such as counting, pointing, bringing in and removing and sharing, and their internalised mental counterparts, that is the four arithmetic operations (Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Demetriou (2004) has found that five- and six-year-old children develop the ‘increasedecrease scheme’, which is co-ordinated with the basic principles of counting and that this generates a first understanding of number conservation. The Practical Reasoning Subscale of the GMDS-ER is only introduced to children over the age of two years, and assesses the most primitive indications of arithmetic comprehension and the realisation of the most practical problems. Pre-schoolers are required to count 15 bricks, to compare two lines for length and point to the longest one, to remove the middle brick from a row of five bricks and to know the number of fingers on each hand as well as altogether. The third level of organisation within this SCS involves all kinds of factual knowledge about the quantitative aspects of the world. Examples include knowledge about reading the time, money values and rules underlying everyday transactions, and numerical knowledge such as multiplication tables (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). On the original Griffiths Scales, children were asked to name the monetary values of coins and to tell the time using a clock-face on a board. However, these items were removed during the ten-year revision process as a facet analysis of these items found them to be of lesser value than other more relevant items on the Scales. 3.8.3.3 The system of causal thought According to Demetriou (2000; 2004), objects and people are very often dynamically related, sometimes functioning as the cause of changes and other times as the recipients of causal effects. Causal reasoning enables the grasp of dynamic interactions between objects or persons. Perception of fundamental causal relationships, such as when there is direct transfer of energy from one object to another (e.g., we push something to move it) are examples of the core processes involved in this system (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). At the second level of organisation, causal reasoning involves operations enabling the thinker to manipulate and represent causal relations (Demetriou, 2004). The most prominent operation utilized for this 54 purpose is trial-and-error manipulations aiming to uncover the causal role of objects. Demetriou (2004) found that five- and six-year-olds develop causal schemes to enable them to accurately describe causal sequences. An example of this is when a child discovers that a toy no longer works. The child may search for the cause of this event by testing (usually persistently and inflexibly) some idea directly suggested by the apparent aspects of the event (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The picture arrangement items within the Practical Reasoning Subscale of the GMDS-ER measure causal reasoning. Picture cards are placed in front of the child on the table, from left to right, in the incorrect order. The child is told that the cards are in the wrong place and instructed to re-arrange the cards so that they tell a story. The first picture arrangement series shows a boy kicking a ball to a girl, with arrows drawn on the card to show the direction the ball is travelling in. This is a practice example and if the child answers incorrectly, the test administrator explains the correct method until the child conveys that he or she understands. Children whose causal reasoning is well-developed proceed to more difficult picture arrangements, which are for older children. Causal reasoning is not only related to cognitive ability purely but also to the psychological and social world of the child. For instance, the immaterial effects exerted on human behaviour by desires, ambitions, and feelings are examples of masked causal relations (Demetriou et al., 2002). Young toddlers’ test boundaries set by their parents and soon learn that if they do not listen to their parents, they will be punished. Another scenario could be that the child may get his or her own way if their parents fail to establish solid boundaries for acceptable and unacceptable behaviour. Psychologically, young children start grappling with cause-and-effect relations before they reach pre-school (Demetriou et al., 1993). Their homes are their first classroom and their parents assume the teaching role. Knowledge related to the ‘why’ and ‘how’ of things pertains to the third level of the organisation of the system (Demetriou et al., 1993). 3.8.3.4 The system of spatial thought This system is directed to the representation and processing of two aspects of reality, orientation and movement in space and situations or scenes, which can be visualised mentally as integral wholes and processed as such (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Therefore, anything that can be perceived and then somehow preserved as a mental image can become the object of the activity of this system (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Therefore, in this domain, spatial relations within (the composition and structure of objects) and between objects (relative distances, directions, and orientations) acquire prominence because they are crucial in the representations of objects themselves, their location in space, and of the space 55 that surrounds them (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Formation of mental images and processes, such as perception of size, depth, and orientation of objects, constitutes the foundations or core processes of this domain (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). To be able to represent them, operate on them, and move between them, the thinker needs operations, such as mental rotation, addition, and removal, which honour these relations, thereby enabling the thinker to visualise the objects and space from the perspective needed so as to be able to recognise and locate them and efficiently move between or to them (Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Mental images, mental maps and scripts about objects, locations, scenes or layouts stored in memory belong to the third level of the organisation of this system (Kosslyn, 1983). Demetriou et al. (2002) found that five- and six-year-olds are able to analyse pictures in their component parts, as well as able to construct spatial dimensions and spatial operations, such as mental rotation. For example, upon assessing a sixyear-old boy on the GMDS-ER recently, the concept of mental rotation was very well demonstrated. He had built a model of a bridge, and as the table that the boy and Tester were sitting at was large the Tester moved closer and sat to his right. Out of his own, he started building the model again and this time constructed it from his angle facing the Tester. He was able to mentally rotate the model to visualise how the Tester would see it. Spatial reasoning plays a vital role in the successful completion of several items that span across the Subscales of the GMDS-ER. The Locomotor Subscale focuses on the assessment of gross-motor skills, perceptual-motor abilities, visual acuity, figure-ground perception and depth perception, balance, and flexibility etc. The Eye and Hand Coordination Subscale assesses visual-perceptual skills, perceptual-motor and sensorimotor ability. The Performance Subscale requires a synthesis and analysis of skills, as it looks at spatial logic and reasoning, visual-spatial and visual-motor skills, and spatial organisation. If a child has spatial reasoning and perceptual problems, his or her performance on several subscales of the GMDS-ER will be affected. 3.8.3.5 The system of verbal-propositional thought Verbal reasoning facilitates interaction between persons, and it is used as a guide to action. Core processes in this system underlie the ability, which is present from infancy, to use the grammatical and syntactical structures of language in order to infer the relations between the events or situations mentioned in a sequence of sentences (Braine, 1990; Demetriou, 1998). At the second level of organisation, operations and processes in this domain are primarily directed to the truth and validity relations between verbal statements so 56 that person may judge the accuracy of the information received and decipher deception etc. (Demetriou et al., 1993; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Two types of skills are used to attain these aims. First, there are grammatical and syntactical skills enabling the individual to interpret and interrelate the components in verbal statements so that information may be abstracted in goal-relevant, meaningful, and coherent ways. Second, there are skills enabling one to differentiate the contextual from the formal elements in a series of statements and operate on the latter (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). For example, focusing on such verbs as ‘is’ or ‘belongs to’, or connectives such as ‘and’, ‘if’, and ‘or’ directs thinking to the relationships between the statements, rather than simply the statements themselves. These processes, the second in particular, enable one to grasp the basic logical relations (‘and’) and disjunction (‘either…or’), implication (‘if…then’), etc. (Efklides, Demetriou & Metallidou, 1994). Explicit knowledge about grammar and syntax and explicit knowledge about logical reasoning belong to the third level of the organisation of this system (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Demetriou (2004) found that five- and six-year-olds are able to distinguish propositions from each other and organise them in order to suggest a given conclusion. In addition, they are also able to grasp and understand permission rules. For example, if you do X then you can do Y. The Language Subscale of the GMDS-ER assesses young children’s language ability. More specifically, the emphasis of this subscale is on expressive language. However, it does measure a child’s receptive language abilities, verbal and semantic reasoning, verbal comprehension, as well as general and applied linguistic knowledge. 3.8.3.6 The system of social-interpersonal thought This system deals with the understanding of social relationships and interactions (Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). This system involves operations and processes that enable understanding and manipulation of the forces underlying verbal and non-verbal social interactions, such as motives and intentions (Demetriou & Kazi, 2001; Demetriou, Kazi & Georgiou, 1999). Demetriou et al. (2002) found that social thought is related to crystallised intelligence underlying knowledge about the world. One of the areas that the Personal-Social Subscale of the GMDS-ER assesses is that of the child’s ability to interact with other children as well as his or her level of independence (Luiz et al., 2004a). Griffiths (1954) believed that social interaction such as co-operative play with peers is important. Furthermore, whilst emotional factors are known to affect performance on all of the subscales of the GMDS-ER, it is believed that they impact this subscale in particular (Baker, 2005). 57 3.8.3.7 The drawing-pictographic system This system involves multiple skills and operations that integrate many of the systems above into an idiosyncratic whole that is particular to humans (Demetriou et al., 2002). This system makes use of imaginal and kinetic processes and abilities for the sake of the pictorial representation of the relations associated with any of the other systems (Demetriou et al., 2002). Through this system, children can represent their environment or their thoughts themselves by the production of drawings or any other kinds of signs (Demetriou & Kazi, 2001; Demetriou et al., 2002). Within the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale of the GMDS-ER are drawing items. Children are specifically asked to draw a person and a house, whilst the other items require them to copy shapes and objects. Children’s drawings reveal substantial information about their intellectual and emotional maturity. Psychological projective measures utilize children’s drawings as they provide the psychologist with an indication of the child’s self-concept, their view of their family and/or friends. Therefore, the fact that a developmental theory of the mind is recognising the importance of the pictographic system in child development is valuable. The nature of development as postulated by Demetriou et al. (2002) is discussed in the following section. 3.9 The nature of development Thinking and problem solving processes are organised in domain specific systems, which are differentiated on the types of relations they represent, and on the process and the symbol systems to which they are biased (Demetriou et al., 2002). In other words, the processing system and hypercognitive system processes exist within the SCSs but take on specific functions to enable that domain to efficiently operate on that symbol system (Demetriou et al., 2002). To reiterate, it was mentioned earlier that the development of thinking and problem solving within each SCS is influenced through the combined influence of constitutional (maturational), socio-cultural and experiential factors. Due to the differing processes that occur within each SCS and accompanying symbol system, the development of each system may proceed through partially different types of developmental sequences and at different rates of progression (Demetriou et al., 2002). Demetriou et al. (2002) view the nature of development as “a series of overlapping waves or cycles of change” (p.131). In this series, the processes and functions of the various levels and systems of the mind may be at different points in their own cycle of development. However, when one of them reaches a certain point in its own cycle of development (e.g., processing efficiency), it opens the possibility for 58 another process or function to advance to a different point in its cycle (e.g., working memory or problem solving) (Demetriou et al., 2002). There is a reciprocal growth relationship in that the later processes may bounce back and alter the movement of the first process in its cycle (Demetriou et al., 2002). It is important to add that the wavelength or rate of change is not uniform across successive phases of development. It is logistic as anticipated by dynamic systems theory (Fischer & Bidell, 1998; van Geert, 1994). According to the basic assumption of dynamic systems theory, any kind of change (e.g., learning) is logistic when it occurs under limited resources (Demetrou & Raftapoulos, 2004). The phases of logistic development of the various abilities do not coincide. For example, processing efficiency approaches its peak before the various domains of thinking and problem solving. These differences in the development of the various processes reflect their functional relations as revealed by both the structural equations and dynamic systems model (Demetriou et al., 2002). A review of child development theory is not complete without recognising the continuity-discontinuity debate that has existed amongst psychological theories for decades. 3.10 Continuity-discontinuity debate A controversy has existed for several years among developmental theorists around the issue of whether development occurs gradually in small increments or dramatically in large periodic steps followed by periods of levelling out. An example of a theory that proposed that development occurs in increments is Skinner’s behaviourism theory, in which he pictured development as occurring in “imperceptibly small increments” (Thomas, 2005, p.39). In contrast, Piaget proposed steps and sub-steps of cognitive development, whereas Freud developed major psychosexual stages of growth. Havighurst proposed that children at differing points in their life are confronted with specific developmental tasks and that these form a succession of stages in development. Thomas (2005) found that the essence of many theories is found in the sorts of stages postulated and in the way a child’s movement from one level to the next is viewed. Everyone agrees that development from one day to the next is gradual, and in this sense growth is continuous and occurs in small increments. The real question is then whether the child’s structure and behaviour periodically display symptoms that warrant the label of a new stage of development? (Thomas, 2005, p.39). Thomas (2005) has emphasized that parents, teachers, paediatricians and children themselves are all interested in the matter of whether children are growing up ‘normally’, resulting in the understandable tendency to question what a theory expects of developmental 59 stages. More specifically, how are levels or stages related to a child’s chronological age? Are the stages universal and invariant for children in all cultures? (Thomas, 2005). These questions are especially pertinent in the South African context, in which it is imperative to have a view of child development that does justice to the different cultures. In answering the first question about how stages are related to age, theorists such as Piaget, Freud and Kohlberg have developed stages that are somewhat loosely tied to specific ages. The second question about the universal and invariant attributes of a developmental theory is important to consider as theories are used as a yardstick to determine a child’s normalcy in relation to his or her peers (Thomas, 2005). It is interesting to note that many theorists view their stages as universal and common among children in all cultures and their sequence invariant. Thomas (2005) defines invariant as meaning that “not only are the stages found in all cultures for which we have reports, but the nature of development is such that no other sequence in the pattern of growth stages is possible” (p.40). Piaget, Erikson and Kohlberg assume invariance, which has been questioned by critics such as Phillips and Kelley (1975). In addition, decisive questions have centered on whether children progress through developmental stages at the same pace, as well as whether they can display typical characteristics of more than one stage at the same time. Terms such as retarded, normal, advanced, and gifted have traditionally been used to describe children who are developing at particular rates compared to their peers, regardless of whether development is viewed as continuous or stepwise (Thomas, 2005). Piaget demonstrated in his theory about children’s ideas of causality that children are able to operate on two stage levels simultaneously (Piaget, 1930). In conclusion, many of the older developmental theories subscribe to a discontinuous view of development characterised by stages, whereas contemporary developmental theories are advancing that child development does not progress in such a fixed and predictable manner. 3.11 Chapter overview This chapter focused on child development. Awareness was raised on the goal of studying child development and it was identified that an important reason to study development is the early identification of possible developmental delays. The section highlighted that Griffiths (1954) argued for a broad-based conception of development, which was in contrast to the psychometric view of intelligence at the time that Griffiths’ was producing the Griffiths Scales. Emphasis was placed on the impact early development can have on a child’s subsequent development and that the earlier developmental problems are identified and intervention implemented, the greater the child’s chances of overcoming 60 developmental difficulties. The value of the Griffiths Scales was highlighted as it has proven to be effective in holistically assessing a child’s abilities across the several developmental domains listed, as well as serving as a valuable tool in identifying developmental lags. As intelligence is the main theoretical construct upon which developmental assessment is based, the concept and psychometric view of intelligence was discussed. It was noted that whilst Griffiths’ theoretical approach may appear somewhat dated, it still remains fundamentally sound and that most of its features are consistent with current theory of child development (Stewart, 2005). More specifically, Griffiths acknowledged the child within his social systems; she recognised the physiological aspect of child development; and she attributed equal importance to the psychological aspects of the child (Stewart, 2005). Once a brief overview of selected developmental theories was provided, more attention was paid to the Piagetian approach to development. Thereafter, Demetriou’s (2000; 2004) developmental model of information processing was presented in some detail and selected as the theory to guide and understand the results of this study. The reader was then provided with a brief overview on the continuity versus discontinuity debate of development to inform his or her own thinking on the issue. Childhood development has a profound impact on the course of an individual’s life. As the significance of understanding the process of early childhood development more fully increases, so does the need to establish with more confidence the value and role of developmental assessment in the early identification of problems. The following chapter explores child developmental assessment, focusing on a thorough description of the GMDSER as this study has utilized this assessment measure to explore and compare the development of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls. 61 CHAPTER FOUR CHILD DEVELOPMENTAL ASSESSMENT AND THE GRIFFITHS MENTAL DEVELOPMENT SCALES-EXTENDED REVISED 4.1 Introduction Children are the touchstone of a healthy and sustainable society. How a culture or society treats its youngest members has a significant impact on how they will grow, prosper and be viewed by others. Unfortunately, not all children are born healthy; not all children have access to good nutrition, adequate health care, and acceptable housing; not all children are raised by parents who can comfort, nurture, and challenge them appropriately; and not all children are born free of disabilities or other biological vulnerabilities. It is the mission of early childhood intervention to help young children and their families thrive (Shonkoff & Meisels, 2000). It is against this backdrop that this chapter provides an overview of child developmental assessment, with an emphasis being placed on the need for a holistic perspective when considering the developmental assessment of South African children. Child developmental assessment within a multi-cultural context will be discussed including the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised’s (GMDS-ER) applicability to diverse population groups. An overview will be provided of the developmental assessment measures utilized with infants and young children in South Africa, followed by a description of selected international developmental measures that have been adapted for use in South Africa. As the GMDS-ER is the assessment measure employed within the current study, it will be discussed in detail and the focus will shift to the history of the Scales, including a description of the Subscales, the scoring and administration procedure and its reliability and validity as a diagnostic measure. A brief discussion on the need for South African research on the GMDS-ER will be provided, concluded by clinical and technical research studies conducted on the GMDS-ER to date. 4.2 Child developmental assessment Since the early 1970s, the need for the early identification of children with difficulties has been recognised (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Considering the South African history and current context, further awareness needs to be created around the benefit of early identification of developmental problems. In South Africa’s diverse background, there are many children who do not have access to adequate resources and learning opportunities and thus fall within the disadvantaged bracket. Without some form of assessment, these children 62 may go unnoticed and their developmental difficulties only be detected later when they fail to master the developmental milestones. It may prove more challenging to intervene in the older child’s life and sadly the window of opportunity may be missed. Thus, “the rationale for assessing a child’s development at an early age is simple: the sooner a child’s difficulties can be identified, the sooner an intervention can be implemented and, hence, the sooner a child can be assisted” (Luiz & Jansen, 2001, p.155). The benefits of identifying problems early is that intervention can be implemented which will assist the child to catch up developmentally and cope successfully as opposed to those who are identified much later (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Knoesen (2003) asserts that the early identification of the child’s areas of developmental weaknesses will enable the practitioner to provide remedial intervention to maximally develop those areas of developmental delay. Van Rooyen (2005) further highlighted the need for developmental assessment within the South African context, and explained that due to the substantial number of at risk children in South Africa, there is a great need for intervention programmes that focus on the improvement of the development of vulnerable children. Luiz and Jansen (2001) provide the example of how detecting a cognitive developmental delay early in a child, having an accompanying lack of social and emotional stimulation, can lead to a certain amount of remediation. They assert that if this cognitive delay is detected at an early enough age and the child provided with intensive stimulation, the cognitive deficit may disappear to a certain extent after a period of time. Therefore, it can be said that early childhood intervention can also serve as a measure of hope. It can assist in remedying deficits that exist due to unequal opportunities in education for instance. Therefore, the goal of early childhood assessment within the South African context should be to “acquire information and understanding that will facilitate the child’s development and functional abilities within the family and community” (Shonkoff & Meisels, 2000, p.232). Greenspan and Meisels (1996) define developmental assessment as: “a process designed to deepen understanding of a child’s competencies and resources and of the caregiving and learning environments most likely to help a child make fullest use of his or her developmental potential” (p.11). The holistic view of child development was emphasized in the previous chapter, and by implication a holistic perspective to the developmental assessment of South African children is vital in view of the poor social conditions the majority of South African children have experienced (Luiz, 1994). Brooks-Gunn (1990) stressed that the measurement of the well-being of a child should include the assessment of the physical, cognitive, social, and emotional developmental arenas. 63 Therefore, a comprehensive developmental assessment should include these four aspects of functioning, which are not mutually exclusive, meaning that a problem in one area may have an effect on another area (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). If one accepts the definition of child wellbeing offered by Brooks-Gunn as the goal we wish South African children to attain, by implication a comprehensive measure of development is required (Davidson, 2008). Luiz (1994) asserts that the Griffiths Scales play a “pivotal role in this process” (p.9). Developmental measures can be categorized as either screening or diagnostic measures (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Developmental screening involves a brief, formal evaluation of developmental skills, which attempts to identify children who may be potentially at risk for developmental difficulties (Squires, Nickel & Eisert, 1996). Nonspecialists, such as parents, educators and clinic nurses, can administer screening measures as long as they have been trained to use them (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). In comparison to screening measures, diagnostic measures can be described as “in-depth, comprehensive, individual holistic measures used by trained professionals and the aim of diagnostic measures is to identify the existence, nature, and severity of the problem” (Luiz & Jansen, 2001, p.155). Both screening and diagnostic measures assess a child holistically, in which they examine areas such as fine and gross motor co-ordination, memory of visual sequences, verbal expression, language comprehension, social-emotional status and so forth. Screening measures usually provide an overall view of the child’s development, rather than information relating to specific areas. Diagnostic measures are more comprehensive in that they provide numerical scores and/or age equivalents for both overall performance as well as for each specific area assessed (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). 4.3 Child developmental assessment within a multi-cultural context The family may be the most important context for the child, but the culture and community provide the contexts for the family. Families are cultural mediators, and if we are to understand the child, we need to understand the sources of influence. Furthermore, if we are to offer services to the child and family, we need to be sensitive to these factors (Meller, Ohr & Marcus, 2001; Mosley-Howard, 1995; Okagaki & Diamond, 2000; Roopnarine & Carter, 1992). Culture is a complex construct that is often intertwined with the concepts of ethnicity and race. Betancourt and Lop´ez (1993) regard culture as a distinct system of meaning or a cognitive schema that is shared by a group of people or an identifiable segment of the population. Culture has been defined as “historically transmitted patterns of meaning” (Mosley-Howard, 2005, p.337). In contrast, ethnicity is typically used to describe a group 64 defined by a common nationality, culture or language. Therefore, ethnicity refers to a broad grouping to which members have a subjective sense of belonging (Garcia Coll & Magnuson, 2000). In more recent conceptual models of development, socio-cultural influences on infant and child development have increasingly been proposed (Garcia Coll & Magnuson, 2000). “This shift has been driven by a compelling combination of scholarly and clinical contributions that have indicated that universal assumptions about development do not equally explain all processes and pathways of development for all populations” (Garcia Coll & Magnuson, 2000, p.94). It has consistently been found that an individual’s cultural group has an influence on test performance (Allan, 1992; Heimes, 1983; Mothuloe, 1990; Tukulu, 1996). Foxcroft and Roodt (2001) assert that “although there may be cultural differences in test scores, there is no decisive evidence that culture influences competence rather than performance” (p.328). In other words, although “socio-cultural factors may affect manifestations of underlying cognitive and emotional processes, it is unlikely that cultural differences exist in the basic component cognitive processes” (Miller, 2001, p.328). Mwamwenda (1995) stated that empirical studies have shown that the pattern and sequence of acquisition of Piagetian stages of cognitive development is universal, however the rate of acquisition may vary among different cultures. Therefore, it is imperative that cultural differences are taken into account when test performance is interpreted (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). According to Jansen (1991) there is no such thing as a ‘culture-free’ test as psychological tests are samples of behaviour, which are affected by the cultural milieu in which the individual is reared. Thus, a more realistic approach to developing a culture-free test is to develop a test with content that is based on experiences which are common across cultures and thus proves to be ‘culture-fair’ (Baker, 2005). The concept of play, whilst perhaps in different forms across cultures, is a universal behaviour, and the Griffiths Scales were developed by observing children in their natural environments, and whilst engaged in natural activities (Allan, Luiz & Foxcroft, 1992). Furthermore, several studies conducted within different parts of the world have demonstrated the Griffiths Scales applicability to diverse population groups as they tap into experiences that are common to different cultures (Luiz, Collier, Stewart, Barnard & Kotras, 2000). In addition, research conducted on the Griffiths Scales have demonstrated that they can be applied in the evaluation and treatment of children from a variety of cultures and from first as well as third world countries (Allan, 1988; Bhamjee, 1991; Mothuloe, 1990). Luiz (1994) confirmed that the Griffiths Scales could be considered to be ‘culture-fair’ as they utilize experiences that are common to different 65 cultures, an attribute which is obviously very significant within the South African context. Another positive reason for using the Griffiths Scales in a multi-cultural context is that the tester demonstrates a number of the items, making the test process more understandable to children with limited experience with psychometric assessment. Furthermore, the language component of the Griffiths Scales is smaller than that of some other psychometric measures, thus enabling the tester to assess aspects of a child’s development without having to rely on proficient verbal skills (Baker, 2005). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development is one of the few measures that can be used across various South African populations with confidence (Bhamjee, 1991; Baker, 2005; Knoesen, 2003; Kotras, 2001; Schröder, 2004; Van Heerden, 2007). The Association for Research in Infant and Child Development (ARICD) based in the United Kingdom, London, is highly supportive of projects in third world countries such as Asia and South Africa, which aim to increase Griffiths research in these continents. Such research aims to facilitate the development of a set of norms specifically applicable to developing populations such as those found in these abovementioned countries (L. Stroud, personal communication, December 3, 2007). In conclusion, Luiz (2000) highlighted the following as needing to be addressed and focused on when considering developmental assessment within a multi-cultural South African context: 1. The absolute need for holistic developments; 2. Screening measures that are culture-reduced, valid and reliable from our diverse South African population; 3. The value and necessity for para-professionals and parents to increase their knowledge about normal developmental milestones and to form part of the screening process; and 4. The importance of establishing a community network system to facilitate communication, referrals and the sharing of knowledge amongst the various professionals, lay-professionals, parents and caregivers who are involved in the lives of South African infants and children. 4.4 Psychological assessment measures utilized with infants and young children in South Africa In review of the state of the developmental measures in South Africa, Foxcroft and Roodt (2001; 2006) have found that during the last decade, there has been a concerted effort made by researchers to attend to the need for more reliable and valid developmental measures 66 for use with South African children. Researchers have focused on the construction of new culture-reduced tests and adapting, revising and norming assessment measures that have been extensively used in other countries and thus proved to be reliable and valid (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). A brief description of developmental measures used in South Africa will be presented, followed by a description of selected international developmental measures that have been adapted for use in South Africa. 4.5 4.5.1 Developmental assessment measures utilized in South Africa The Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS) The Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS) were developed in 1979 and initially designed for use with White South African children between the ages of 3 and 7 years 11 months (Madge, 1981). There are two main aims of this assessment battery, namely 1) to establish a child’s general intellectual level, and 2) to evaluate a child’s functional strengths and weaknesses (Madge, 1981). Van Rooyen (2005) described the JSAIS, as a comprehensive measure that provides a profile of abilities, yet does not assess gross motor and personalsocial development. It is interesting that the JSAIS was adapted and standardised for Asian children, and norms established for Coloured Children between the age group of 6 years to 8 years and 11 months (Robinson, 1989; Swart, 1987). Furthermore, the measure was further standardised for English and Afrikaans-speaking children who have received formal schooling in these languages (Van Rooyen, 2005). However, this assessment measure has not been normed for black children, and Van den Berg (1987) has stated that this can only be achieved once parallel forms of the test have been developed for the South African black language groups. In view of the need for a holistic view of development to be adopted within the South African context and advanced to our developmental measures, it can be said that the major limitation of the JSAIS is its inability to be used in our diverse population. Further criticisms include its limited focus on the different domains of development as envisaged by Griffiths’ learning avenues, and its ignorance of contextual factors in the assessment of children. 4.5.2 Grover-Counter Scale of Cognitive Development The Grover-Counter Scale was based on Piagetian theory and developed in accordance with Piaget’s stages of cognitive development. It measures five domains, with each domain measuring a stage in a person’s development, called Sections. Section A consists 67 of simple recognition of shapes and colours, Section B evaluates the ability to reconstruct patterns from memory, Section C evaluates the ability to copy a model made by the tester, Section D evaluates the ability to reconstruct a model from memory, and Section E evaluates the ability to produce and complete patterns from a design card within a time limit (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). Each section provides an indication of the child’s stage of current development, based on factors that look at procedural elements of how the child goes about performing the tasks (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). Therefore, it is rather apparent that there are several similarities between the items found on the Griffiths Scales and the Grover-Counter Scale. Dickman (1994) has found that there is a growing body of research developing on the Scale that has proved its clinical use in South Africa. 4.5.3 Educationally-focused developmental screening measures Due to South Africa’s socio-political history and rich diversity of people, it is important that attempts are made to instil equality in every sphere. Formal schooling equips children with the vital tools of learning to further their thinking and hands them the key with which they can open new doors to success for not only themselves but for their community. Therefore, the attempt to instil equality should start with ensuring that all children have access to proper education. In this regard, “educationally focused screening measures assess the extent to which young children are developing the necessary underpinning skills that are the essential building blocks for being able to successfully progress in the early school years” (Luiz & Jansen, 2001, p.158). Such assessments are vitally important in detecting any early developmental difficulties in children so that they can be rectified and thus allow the children to develop to their full potential. The following assessment measures have been developed in South Africa and assess “whether a pre-school child or Grade One child is at risk for developing scholastic difficulties” (Luiz & Jansen, 2001, p.158). 4.5.3.1 School-readiness Evaluation by Trained Testers (SETT) The SETT was developed in 1984, and measures whether a child is ready for school or not. It is administered by setting up a learning station, similar to a classroom. The SETT evaluates a child’s development in the following areas, namely: language, general or intellectual development, physical and motor development, and emotional and social development (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). According to Knoesen (2003), the SETT was developed for use with urban White, Coloured and Asian children. No black children were included in the normative sample. An investigation into the predictive validity of the SETT for White, 68 Coloured and Asian children was conducted by Bender (1990) and in analysis of the results, found that the SETT is able to predict the primary school progress of these population groups. 4.5.3.2 School-entry Group Screening Measure (SGSM) The SGSM is a non-verbal cognitive group-screening measure developed in South Africa for five- to nine-year-old children (Foxcroft, Shillington & Turk, 1990). It measures the cognitive abilities of simultaneous visual-motor functioning, visual sequential processing, cognitive flexibility, attention, and incidental memory (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). These abilities are comprised within four separate subtests, namely Visual-motor, Reasoning, Incidental Memory and Visual-spatial. According to Luiz and Jansen (2001) the SGSM has been found to predict accurately if a child is at risk for later scholastic difficulties. In addition, test instructions are available in several South African languages, namely English, Afrikaans, Xhosa, Zulu, and Southern Sotho (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). Knoesen (2003) noted that an advantage of the SGSM is its time efficiency, as well as that psychologists, psychometrists and teachers trained on the SGSM can administer the test. Furthermore, Knoesen (2003) highlighted the fact that as the SGSM is a screening measure, it is best to categorize performance in terms of qualitative, descriptive categories as opposed to quantitative norms, such as stanines or scaled scores. Cut-points have also been developed for each cultural group. 4.5.3.3 Aptitude Test for School Beginners (ASB) The ASB was published in 1974 and was the first standardised test for Black, Coloured and White South African children. It is also the only test that has been standardised for black school beginners (Knoesen, 2003; Ras, 1987). The aim of the ASB is to obtain a comprehensive picture of particular aptitudes of the school beginner and evaluates the cognitive aspects of school readiness. The ASB consists of eight subtests: Perception, Spatial, Comprehension, Numerical, Gestalt, Co-ordination, Memory, and Verbal Comprehension. The instructions are available in the seven most commonly spoken African languages (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). The ASB is not without its criticisms, some of which are that it excludes gross motor and personal-social development, the cumbersome administration of the test and lengthiness of completion time, difficult scoring procedures for some of the subtests, the paper-and-pencil requirement when many children have limited experience with such mediums at school-entry level, and lastly that children battle to understand and follow the instructions independently (Knoesen, 2003). However, Hoar (1983) and Mothuloe (1990) 69 have found significant positive correlations with the ASB scores and Grade One school results, indicating that it could be a useful screening measure for Black school beginners (Knoesen, 2003). 4.6 International developmental assessment measures adapted for use in South Africa As mentioned earlier, South African researchers identified the need to adapt, refine and norm appropriate tests that have been constructed and proven valid and reliable in other countries for use in the South Africa context. However, this would need to be undertaken in a cost-efficient way due to the financial challenges that face a third world country such as South Africa. Therefore, through adapting assessment measures such as the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II, the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities, the Wechsler Scales and the GMDS-ER, diagnostic developmental measures have become more accessible to the South African context. These measures will be described briefly below, however it must be emphasized that these measures are not the only measures in use. 4.6.1 The Wecshler Scales The Wechlser Intelligence Scale for Children was developed in 1949 and was replaced by a standardised version known as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WICS-R) in 1974 (Wechsler, 1974). The WISC-R assesses the cognitive and intellectual abilities in children aged 6 to 16 years old. According to Anastasi (1982), whilst the WISC-R is said to be technically superior in terms of its construction processes, reliability and validity studies have been insufficient and inconclusive. The WISC-R was updated to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (WISC-III) in 1991, and extends across the same age range as the WISC-R (Strauss et al., 2006). The WISC-III enabled the comparison of verbal and non-verbal ability, and the revision process intended to improve the “contemporaneous nature of the norms as well as updating the content coverage” (Baker, 2005, p.66). However, Edelman (1996) found that whilst the WISC-III may have acceptable internal consistency reliability co-efficients, there is a significant practice effect on the Performance scale if a retest is completed within a 12-63 day interval. Thus, the WISC-III was not a suitable test for testing developmental changes if a retest is to be done within two months after the initial testing (Van Rooyen, 2005). The WISC-III was updated to the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) in 2003. The WISC-IV embodies a fundamental shift from previous editions, as the test’s theoretical underpinnings now reflect more updated factor 70 analytic theories of intelligence collectively termed Carroll-Horn-Cattel (CHC) theory, which has been termed one of the most important theories in contemporary intelligence research (Strauss et al., 2006). The CHC theory is a model that stresses broad classes of abilities at the higher level (fluid ability, crystallized intelligence, short-term memory, long-term storage and retrieval, processing speed), and a number of primary factors at the lower level (quantitative reasoning, spelling ability, free recall, simple reaction time) (p.98). Their thinking reflects modern day contemporary theories of the structure of cognitive functioning that emphasize multiple, somewhat independent factors of intelligence, and thus believe that it is best evaluated with multifaceted instruments and techniques (Strauss et al., 2006). Whilst the WISC-IV has dominated the international field and is used by so many psychologists, the adoption of this model represents a major historical paradigm shift for the assessment of intelligence in children. However, the WISC-IV has been criticised by Wechsler traditionalists and by the CHC proponents for not providing coverage of all CHC domains and for an a posteriori adherence to the model (Flanagan & Kaufman, 2004). The Wechsler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R) was constructed as an extension of the WISC in 1989, and was designed for children in the three to seven year age group. The twelve subtests are grouped into a verbal and performance scale, and whilst it is has been regarded as easy to administer it has also been heavily criticised for its inability to estimate the IQ of severely retarded children, as well as ethnic minority children from low socio-economic backgrounds (Groth-Marant, 1984). Further criticisms focused on the WPPSI-Rs limited range of measured abilities, low reliability of some subtests, limited floors and ceilings, long administration time, complex scoring criteria for some subtests and for overemphasizing speed which put some children at a disadvantage (Strauss et al., 2006). The Wechsler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence-III (WPPSI-III) was released in 2002, and it improved on the abovementioned limitations, as well as being more consistent with current factor-analytic theories of intelligence and cognitive functioning such as CHC theory. The age range was extended down from 2 years 11 months to 2 years 6 months and there are now two distinct age groups, namely 1) 2 years 6 months to 3 years 11 months, 2) 4 years 0 months to 7 years 3 months. The two age groups differ in their score structure, administration time, item difficulty, and reliance on verbal expression (Strauss et al., 2006). While none of the Wechsler Scales for children have been fully adapted for the South African context, they are widely used. Some preliminary studies have been conducted with 71 respect to translating some of the subtests and to establish preliminary local norms. For example, Horsmann (2007) investigated the effects of the quality of education on the WISCIV Coding B-Incidental Learning subtest for a sample of Xhosa and English-speaking Grade 7 learners aged between 12 and 13 years in the Eastern Cape. As one of the outcomes of this study, normative guidelines were developed that can be used in clinical practice. Furthermore, Van Tonder (2007) provided preliminary normative information in terms of means and standard deviations for all the subtests, the four indices, and Full Scale IQ of the WISC-IV for English and Xhosa-speaking children in Grade 7 aged between 12 and 13 years in the Eastern Cape. 4.6.2 McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (McCarthy Scales) The McCarthy Scales were published in 1972, and are utilized with children between the ages of 3 years 6 months and 8 years 6 months. The tasks are designed to be suitable for both boys and girls, as well as for children from different cultural and socio-economic groups (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). There are eighteen separate tests, grouped into five scales, namely: 1) verbal, 2) perceptual performance, 3) quantitative, 4) motor, and 5) memory (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). An overall measure of the child’s intellectual functioning is obtained when the Verbal, Quantitative, and Perceptual Performance Scales are combined (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). The general cognitive score is expressed as a General Cognitive Index (GCI) and indicates the child’s functioning at the time of testing, with no implications of immutability or etiology. Furthermore, Bhamjee states that the GCI is reported to come the closest to the traditional global measure of intellectual development, IQ (Bhamjee, 1991). Luiz and Jansen (2001) have highlighted the fact that the McCarthy Scales have normative information available for various groups of South African children. 4.6.3 Bayley Scales of Infant Development-Second Edition (BSID-II) The BSID-II is used to identify children between the ages of 1- to 42-months (3 years 5 months) with developmental delays or those suspected of being ‘at risk’ (Luiz & Jansen, 2001). The original Bayley Scales were published in 1969, a second edition was published in 1993, and a new edition is forthcoming, the BSID-III (Strauss et al., 2006). Items from the Bayley Scales have as origins some of the earliest tests of infant development (Bayley, 1936). The BSID-II is designed to assess cognitive, physical, language, and psychosocial development of infants, toddlers and pre-schoolers (Bayley, 1993). Norms published in 1969 on the original Bayley Scales are available for interpreting the performance of black South 72 African children (Richter & Griesel, 1988). However, no other South African norms are available for the revised scales. In accordance, Barnard (2000) has recommended that further studies should be undertaken to investigate the BSID-II’s suitability with special populations. 4.6.4 Griffiths Scales of Mental Development (GMSD) The original Griffiths Scales of Mental Development covered the first two years of life and was first published in 1954. It assessed areas of development, such as Locomotor, Personal-Social, Hearing and Speech, Eye and Hand Co-ordination, and Performance. The Griffiths Extended Scales of Mental Development were initially developed in the 1960s as an extension to the original Scales and tested children aged two to eight years. A sixth subscale, Practical Reasoning, was introduced with the Extended Scales (Griffiths, 1970; 1984). Recently, the Extended Griffiths Scales have been revised to further update the items. As the focus of the present study is on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER), it will be discussed in greater depth in the following section. 4.7 Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) Dr. Ruth Griffiths is regarded as one of the most influential developmental psychologists for her remarkable contributions to the area of developmental assessment. Dr. Griffiths emphasized that in order to work with children one needs to understand them and that one of the keys needed to unlock that process is that of observation. One cannot stress too often the significance of careful and detailed observation of children if we are to understand them, perceive their thoughts or interpret their behaviour. There is in fact scarcely any limit in this field of work, to what can be learned by the observation of children, at play, at home, on the street, in trains or buses, and in their own homes and gardens (Griffiths, 1954, p.7). Using observation of children in their natural settings, the Griffiths Scales were developed as an instrument to assess early childhood development (Griffiths, 1984). Griffiths understood that the best indication of a child’s development is to include as many facets as possible that will predict their development in later years. As illustrated in the previous chapter, Griffiths’ basic avenues of learning formed the foundation of the original Griffiths Scales and it is of no doubt that in the revision and extension process, these avenues guided its further development. It has been said that Griffiths’ thinking around child development was somewhat before her time. This can be safely assumed as her basic avenues of learning, 73 which were published in 1954, are in line with modern systems theory, which was developed over a decade later. 4.7.1 The development of the GMDS-ER: A historical journey The inventor of the Griffiths Scales, Dr. Ruth Griffiths, is bestowed the title of the “architect of the most carefully constructed infant scales for her development of the Griffiths Scales” (Luiz, 1994, p.5) Griffiths recognized that no sector of the community is more deserving of the time and expertise of professionals than infants, children and adolescents (Luiz et al., 2006a). Griffiths had been devoted to her sister who died of polio at the age of five years in 1914 soon after the family’s emigration to Australia. According to Burne (cofounder and trustee of the ARICD), Griffiths was determined to use her talents and skills to celebrate the memory of her sister, and her own life (Luiz et al., 2006a). Therefore, in 1954 Griffiths published the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development for British infants in the birth to two years age group, and introduced a new diagnostic technique for studying the development of babies. She also wrote a book in 1954 entitled The Abilities of Babies in which her philosophy of learning avenues within child development became known. The Griffiths Scales assessed development in five areas, namely Locomotor, Personal-Social, Hearing and Speech, Eye and Hand Co-ordination and Performance (Luiz et al., 2006a). The construction of the Griffiths Scales arose out of an identified need for the early identification and diagnosis of mental conditions in children (Griffiths, 1954; 1984). The Scales were compiled after an extensive study of development and were then standardised on 571 British babies (Stewart, 2005). The implications of having a measure that assisted with the clinical diagnosis of normal and handicapped children received wide acclaim from practitioners out of numerous disciplines (Griffiths, 1970; 1984). Griffiths gained inspiration from the extensive writings of Professor Arnold Gesell on babies and young children, as well as from the work of Professor L.M. Terman and Professor Maud Merrill for their revisions of the Binet-Simon tests (Griffiths, 1970). Dr. Ruth Griffiths, Dr. Rose Unmack and Dr. Brian Henry Burne founded the Association for Research in Infant and Child Development (ARICD) in 1957. It supported and advised Ruth Griffiths until she died in December 1973 (Luiz et al., 2006b). Griffiths received many requests for the extension of the infant scales for use in clinical practice with older children. Therefore, the Extended Griffiths Scales were developed in the 1960s and described in her book entitled The Abilities of Young Children (Griffiths, 1970). The Scales were expanded to cover from birth to eight years (Griffiths, 1970) and a sixth subscale, Practical Reasoning, was added to the Extended Griffiths Scales for children aged 74 two years and older. The Practical Reasoning subscale was aimed at providing a more comprehensive coverage of the young child’s emerging problem-solving and logical reasoning skills (Luiz et al., 2006a). According to Griffiths (1970; 1984; Luiz et al., 2006a), the Scales were developed according to five stringent criteria: 1. The development of the Scales was based on detailed systematic observation of children in their natural environments and their behaviour was recorded. These observations formed the basis of the material for the test items. 2. Previous and existing test methods and tests were taken into account and items from significant tests were included in the Griffiths Scales. 3. The Scales had to fulfil stringent statistical requirements of reliability and validity. 4. The Scales took into account the requirements of both special needs and normally developing children. 5. The Scales were based on a study of: (i) trends that appeared significant for mental growth; and (ii) the interrelations between the basic avenues of learning (such as physiological/locomotor, eye/hand, voice/hearing) which develop with rhythm in time and space and are influenced also by environmental and social factors. Griffiths not only extended the Infant Scales in the 1960s to expand its coverage to eight-year-olds, but also revised and restandardized the original Infant Scales (Van Heerden, 2007). The items on the Extended Scales are diverse and tap into the main aspects of a child’s development. In addition, they are norm-referenced and the items of each subscale are placed in order of gradually increasing difficulty (Luiz et al., 2006a; Van Heerden, 2007). In line with the first criteria mentioned above, the items are based on natural activities such as walking, talking and playing and as Kagan (1981) has stated, play is a universal activity that emerges at the same age in all children and is found within all cultures. Furthermore, each of the six subscales of the Scales was devised to be a separate and complete scale in itself (Griffiths, 1970). This means that any one process of development can be measured independently and as completely as possible. Although the majority of initial research took place in Great Britain, research was further conducted in Canada, Columbia, France, Germany, and China and more recently in Australia, Greece, Lebanon, the United States of America and South Africa. Such research on the Scales has shown that they have practical 75 and diverse applications in the evaluation and treatment of infants and young children from a variety of cultural backgrounds (Brandt, 1983; 1984; Cobos et al., 1971; Collins et al., 1987; Laroche et al., 1974; 1976; Luiz et al., 1999; 2001; 2003; Ramsay & Fitzharding, 1977; Sletten, 1970; 1977). Furthermore, Luiz (1994) confirms that the Griffiths Scales could be considered to be ‘culture-fair’. In other words, the Scales utilize experiences that are common to different cultures, an attribute which is obviously very significant within the South African context. The Scales are widely used by professionals as a developmental assessment tool in countries including the United Kingdom, Eire, Portugal, Australia, Hong Kong, South Africa and the United States of America and have been used in a wide array of research. In an international survey conducted with over 100 tutors and registered users of the Scales, it was found that the Extended Griffiths Scales are most frequently used with problems such as general developmental delay, environmental deprivation, and specific child developmental problems, such as delayed speech, clumsiness, poor eye-hand co-ordination and Down’s syndrome (Luiz et al., 1995; 2006a). According to Luiz et al. (2006a) such findings indicate that the test is sensitive enough to provide practitioners with valuable diagnostic information on a range of developmental disorders. Dr. Ruth Hanson first introduced South Africa to the Griffiths Scales in 1977 and Dr. Brian Burne fostered further interest in this developmental measure in 1989 when he visited South Africa (Davidson, 2008). Therefore, to make it user friendly within the South African context, translations and adaptations had to be made to suit our diverse population. The test was originally developed in English and translated into Afrikaans by Allan in 1988. The translation of test instructions and verbal items into Xhosa has subsequently been undertaken by means of the back-translation technique (Allan et al., 1992; Luiz, 1994; Tukulu, 1996). While, there is an extensive amount of support for the Griffiths Scales, several comprehensive reviews in the 1980s and 1990s identified areas in which they could be improved (Allan, 1988; 1992; Bhamjee, 1991; Hanson, 1982; 1983; Hanson & Aldridge Smith, 1982; 1987; Hanson et al., 1985; Luiz et al., 1995). These studies found that various items of the Scales were outdated and several items were culturally biased and ambiguous (Kotras, 2003). Therefore, the original Griffiths Scales were no longer providing the examiner with reliable and valid results on current evaluations of the child. This provided the rationale for the revision of the Scales. The revision began with the Scales from Birth to Two Years and, in March 1994, the ARICD introduced a draft version of the Revised Baby Scales from Birth to Two Years, which was published in 1996 (Huntley, 1996). Also, at the ARICD conference 76 (1994), the need to revise and standardise the Extended Scales from Two to Eight Years was emphasized and a working team was recruited. The late Professor D.M Luiz of the University of Port Elizabeth was appointed by the ARICD to lead this team aiming to revise and re-norm the extended scales in the British Isles and Eire (Davidson, 2008; Van Heerden, 2007). According to Luiz et al. (2006b) the aims of the revision process was seven-fold: 1. To update the norms; 2. To improve scoring standards, making them explicit and less open to subjective interpretation; 3. To include additional new items to assess previously untapped areas of development, to improve the content coverage, and to also strengthen the reliability of the subscales and scale as a whole; 4. To remove items that are not adding any value to the subscales and the scale as a whole; 5. To improve administration instructions (including the provision of practice examples for selected items); 6. To improve the visual and aesthetic properties of the test material; 7. To replace outdated training aids. The revision and standardisation process of the Extended Griffiths Scales proceeded over a ten-year period and the GMDS-ER was launched and published in 2004 and again in 2006 (Luiz et al., 2004a; 2004b; 2006a). It is vitally important that the norms of the GMDSER are revised and regularly updated. If this is not done, as with the real phenomenon of IQscore inflation over time, the average GQ will gradually drift upward and give a progressively deceptive picture of a child’s performance (Luiz et al., 2006b). 4.7.2 Description of the GMDS-ER Subscales The GMDS-ER consists of the five subscales used in the Birth to Two Years Scales, namely Locomotor, Personal-Social, Hearing and Language, Eye and Hand Co-ordination and Performance. When the Scales were expanded to cover the ages from birth to eight years six months, a sixth subscale was added to the Extended Griffiths Scales for children aged two years and older. While in the Birth to Two years Scale, Subscale C is known as the Hearing and Language Subscale, it is known as the Language Subscale in the GMDS-ER as no hearing 77 items are included (Luiz et al., 2006a). Each of the individual subscales which together form the GMDS-ER is briefly described below. 4.7.2.1 Subscale A: Locomotor This subscale allows the examiner to assess the child’s gross motor skills including his or her ability to balance and to co-ordinate and control movements (Luiz et al., 2006a). This subscale enables the examiner to observe physical weakness or disabilities or more definite gross motor defects in young children. The items administered include age-appropriate activities such as walking up and down stairs, kicking a ball, jumping, and skipping, kneeling on the floor, and so on (Stewart, 2005). According to Sweeney (1994), this subscale was placed first to provide a basis for objective assessment, to set the child at ease and to gain an initial impression of the child’s overall maturity. Furthermore, “performance is also influenced by the child’s ability to focus and concentrate on the task at hand, and the emotional determination to succeed” (Stewart, 2005, p.9). 4.7.2.2 Subscale B: Personal-Social This subscale assesses the child’s proficiency in the activities of daily living, his level of independence and his ability to interact with other children (Luiz et al., 2006a). A degree of self-help, appropriate to the child’s age, is required in terms of personal cleanliness, efficiency at the table, and so forth. There is an expectation for some degree of social interaction from the child, as well as in their co-operation in play with their peers (Stewart, 2005). For instance, the examiner obtains information about the child’s name, age, home address, family name and so on, by interacting with the child and in as natural a manner as possible. This means that the items on this particular subscale can be scored without the child even detecting that it forms part of the assessment (Stewart, 2005). According to Kotras (2001), whilst emotional factors will affect performance across all the subscales, it exerts a particular influence on the Personal-Social subscale. It is interesting to note that Griffiths (1984) has found that children who are neglected or overly protected usually perform poorly on this subscale in comparison to the other subscales. The reason for this is that apparently over protected children are usually slower in learning self-help and personal care (Griffiths, 1984). 4.7.2.3 Subscale C: Language This subscale allows the examiner to assess the child’s receptive and expressive language. The items administered include age-appropriate items such as naming colours and 78 objects, describing a picture, repeating a sentence, analysis of spontaneous sentence structures, and answering comprehension questions such as ‘What should you do if are tired?’ and questions about the differences and similarities between objects (Luiz et al., 2006a). According to Griffiths (1984, p.45), “this is the most intellectual of the scales”. However, it should be noted that poor performance on this particular subscale does not reflect impaired intellectual functioning, Kotras (1998) stated that poor performance could be the result of complete or partial hearing loss, a result of mixed language families or even a lack of verbal stimulation. In agreement with Kotras, Stewart (2005) noted that poor performance could be a “manifestation of some degree of deafness or hearing impairment” (p.10). 4.7.2.4 Subscale D: Eye and Hand Co-ordination This subscale assesses the child’s fine-motor skills, manual dexterity and visual monitoring skills (Luiz et al., 2006a). The items administered include age-appropriate items such as cutting with scissors, copying shapes and writing letters and numbers. Co-ordination, careful work and persistence at a task are also measured (Davidson, 2008). The child’s drawings of geometric shapes provide information on his or her conception of space and form, in addition information about the child’s personality can be gauged from the drawings as reflected by the use of projective techniques for such purposes like the Draw-A-Person (Harris, 1963; Kotras, 1998; Stewart, 2005). 4.7.2.5 Subscale E: Performance Subscale This subscale allows the examiner to assess the child’s visuo-spatial skills including speed of working and precision (Luiz et al., 2006a). The items administered include ageappropriate activities such as building bridges, gates and stairs, completion of formboards and pattern making. As its name implies, this particular subscale is largely one of performance and requires that practical tasks are performed as well as that the child manipulate material (Stewart, 2005). This subscale does complement Subscale D to a certain extent in that the components of Subscale D, namely manual dexterity and eye-hand co-ordination are assumed in the Performance Subscale. Thus, the child is expected to apply these skills in original situations (Stewart, 2005). 4.7.2.6 Subscale F: Practical Reasoning This subscale assesses the child’s ability to solve practical problems, his or her understanding of basic mathematical concepts and questions about moral and sequential 79 issues (Luiz et al., 2006a). The items administered include age-appropriate activities such as counting and comparison of size, length and height and repetition of digits, recognition of coins, picture arrangement cards and so on. It also examines the child’s visual sequential skills, understanding of right or wrong as well as the child’s knowledge of the days of the week. This subscale commences when the testee is three years of age, and attention and concentration span have a major impact on this subscale in terms of the child’s performance (Stewart, 2005). As Davidson (2008) noted that although the Scales were not originally constructed to predict scholastic success, the results of this subscale do provide an indication of the child’s ability to benefit from formal schooling. Therefore, it has often been included in school readiness assessments to aid in a more comprehensive assessment of a child’s abilities. According to Foxcroft and Roodt (2001; 2006), an age equivalent is obtained for each subscale and by combining the subscales a comprehensive score called the General Quotient (GQ) is obtained. In addition to producing a GQ, a mental age (in months) is generated per subscale as well as an overall mental age (MA) that can be correlated with the child’s chronological age to determine whether the child’s performance was above or below his actual age. Sattler (1974) defined MA as the “degree of general mental ability possessed by the average child of a chronological age corresponding to that expressed by the MA score” (p.19). Jensen (1979) described MA as indicating a level of cognitive functioning, achievement, cerebral development and readiness to learn. In addition, the Scales do not only assess the general or overall development of the child but also specific areas of development integrated by the basic avenues of learning (Kotras, 2001), which are known as developmental quotients. An added value of the GMDS-ER is that using histograms based on the developmental quotient of each scale enables a comparison to be made of the child’s performance on the different scales. This developmental profile will then provide an indication of the child’s range of abilities and relative disabilities and enable comparison of these at different times (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). 4.7.3 Scoring and administration of the GMDS-ER The GMDS-ER consists of a total of 228 items. The examiner administers the test items in each subscale until the child obtains a basal and a ceiling score within the subscale. A basal is calculated as the first of the six consecutive passed items, and a ceiling is calculated as the first of six consecutive failed items. The child incurs no penalty for items failed below the basal, and obtains no credit for items passed beyond the ceiling. For the ages 0 to 24 months, there are two items per month in each of the five relevant subscales, and each item 80 completed correctly is credited with half a month. For years three to eight, there are six items for each year in every subscale, plus two extra items for the eighth year. The subscale items are graded in order of difficulty per age group (Luiz et al., 2006b; Stewart, 2005). The examiner administers the test items, starting approximately four months below the child’s chronological age (Bhamjee, 1991). For each subscale separately, the items passed, up to the ceiling, are added and multiplied by two. The figures in each column are then summed to produce the total raw score for each subscale. As there is no Practical Reasoning Subscale in the first and second years, the examiner calculates the average mental age across the other five subscales for each of the two years. Children who did not need to be administered items from the Birth to Two Years Scales due to their basal being higher are credited with a score of 12 months for the first and second years respectively. The total raw score represents the sum of the scores in all the sections under that subscale and reflects the child’s mental age (MA) for that subscale. This study will report on each of the subscale total scores and on the General Quotient (GQ). The GQ is obtained by taking the average of the scores for the six subscales. The scores can also be converted to percentiles, standard scores and age equivalents by using the appropriate table in Appendix B in the Analysis Manual (Luiz et al., 2006b). Any psychological measure must fulfil two standard requirements, namely reliability and validity (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Therefore, the GMDS-ER will be examined with regards to its reliability and validity in the following two sections. 4.7.4 Reliability Reliability can be defined as the accuracy, consistency and stability of test scores across situations (Anastasi, 1961). It refers to the consistency of test scores over time, or the extent to which one is confident that a measure will produce the same result if measured again (Murphy & Davidshoffer, 1991; Nuttall et al., 1992; Smit, 1996). There are two kinds of reliability, namely internal and external. Internal reliability measures whether the test is consistent within itself, and it is generally agreed by test constructors that internal consistency should be high for a test to be of any practical use (Coolican, 2004). External reliability refers to the stability of a measure across time. According to Kline (1993) internal consistency coefficients are the most suitable test of reliability in instances where an individual’s score on some attribute may change significantly in a short period of time. An appropriate example of such an attribute is that of child development. In 1984, Griffiths used the test-retest method in determining the reliability of the Griffiths Scales. Data generated from a sample of N = 270 81 children from various regions within the United Kingdom was collected by Griffiths. The age range of the children was between one to seven years old, and the interval between the assessments ranged between 3 and 63 months. Griffiths found a test-retest reliability of 0.77 (Baker, 2005; Stewart, 2005). Honzik, McFarlane and Allan (1966) found reliability coefficients between 0.71 and 0.76 for test-retest periods of 6 to 12 months for a sample of 3to 5-year old children. According to Barnard (2000), these studies indicated that the Griffiths Scales are a stable measure of development. Aldrige Smith, Bidder, Gardner and Gray (1980) measured the inter-rater reliability of the Griffiths Scales. A video recording of eight normal children between the ages of six months to seven years three months was rated and scored by a panel. An acceptable overall reliability level of between 0.60 and 1.00 in 78% of the cases was found. Greater agreement was obtained between all raters on the Eye-Hand Co-ordination Subscale (84%), the Performance Subscale (91%), and Practical Reasoning Subscale (95%), than on the Locomotor, Personal-Social and Hearing and Speech Subscales. It was hypothesized that the latter three subscales may be more sensitive to interpretation and that the lower inter-rater reliability obtained for these three subscales could be a result of the small sample size, few scorers and that scoring was based on the mother’s report. Suggestions that arose in order to improve their inter-rater reliability proposed more detailed instructions in the test manual for Subscales A, B, and C (Stewart, 2005). However, Hanson (1982) was unable to replicate the findings of Aldridge Smith et al. (1980), in her attempt to study the item reliability in terms of inter-observer agreement and understandably she questioned the conclusions of their study (Baker, 2005). In addition, scorers were asked to identify those items with ambiguous manual instructions. This led to the finding of Hanson, Aldridge Smith and Humes (1984) that “when the manual’s administration instructions and scoring criteria were ambiguous in respect of a certain item, such an item was found to be unreliable” (Stewart, 2005, p.13). In review of test construction issues, Clark and Watson (1995) recommended that the Scales strive for a coefficient alpha of approximately 0.80, and maintained that this benchmark needs to be established. They cautioned that an over concern with internal consistency can be counterproductive as it almost inevitably produces a scale which is narrow in construct (Stewart, 2005). On the Extended Revised version of the Griffiths Scales, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were calculated for each subscale independently as well as for the GQ as an indication of the reliability of the subscales as a measure of mental development (Van Heerden, 2007). Luiz et al. (2006b) found that the overall reliability of the GMDS-ER is 82 0.993, which is highly satisfactory. On the whole, the reliability of the individual subscales ranges between 0.90 and 0.99 and thus is indicative of a high level of internal consistency (Luiz et al., 2006b). As previously mentioned, Griffiths (1970) stated that “each subscale was devised to be a separate and complete scale in itself, each measuring only one avenue of development or process of development, but measuring this one aspect as completely as possible” (p.34). In light of this, it is reasonable to expect that low intercorrelations between some, seemingly unrelated, subscales will exist as the GMDS-ER is comprised of six separate subscales which are all “vastly different in content” (Griffiths, 1970, p.34) rather than one global scale. Several recent studies have been conducted on exploring the reliability of the GMDS-ER and this will be described later in the technical studies section. The validity of the GMDS-ER will now be examined in the following section. 4.7.5 Validity Validity can be defined as the “the degree to which evidence and theory support the interpretations of test scores entailed by proposed users of tests” (AERA, 1999, p.9). There are three types of validity or validation procedures, namely content-description, criterionprediction and construct-identification procedures (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Construct validity refers to the extent to which it measures the theoretical construct or trait it is supposed to measure (Aiken, 1997; Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Foxcroft and Roodt (2001; 2006) document three ways in which construct validity can be ascertained, namely: 1) a high correlation between a new measure and similar earlier measures of the same name; 2) by using factor analysis for analysing the interrelationships of variables; and 3) by examining whether it correlates highly with other variables with which it should theoretically correlate with (convergent validity) and examines whether it correlates minimally with variables from which it should differ (discriminant validity). Part of construct validity is concurrent validity, which is defined by Coolican (2004) as the validation of a new test by comparing it with a currently existing measure of the construct. In other words, it can be described as the correlation of a previously unvalidated test with an already validated one (Jensen, 1980). To establish the concurrent validity of the Extended Griffiths Scales, they were compared to the Terman-Merrill Scale, which is a version of the Stanford-Binet. The GQ on the Griffiths Scales ranged from 99.45 to 101.92 for the different age groups, while the Terman-Merrill Intelligence Quotient (IQ) ranged from 102.77 to 106.87. The correlations between the GQ and IQ obtained were satisfactory as they varied from r = 0.79 to r = 0.81 for the different year groups (Stewart, 2005). Luiz and Heimes (1994) studied the construct validity of the 83 Griffiths Extended Scales on a South African sample. They compared the Griffiths Scales with the Junior South African Intelligence Scale (JSAIS) and found that the GQ and General Intelligence Quotient (GIQ) of the JSAIS showed highly positive correlations, suggesting that the Griffiths Scales and the JSAIS measure a similar construct. Mothuloe (1990) administered the Griffiths Scales and the Aptitude Test for School Beginners (ASB) to a sample of 45 Black Setswana-speaking Grade One children between the ages of 5 years 9 months and 7 years 3 months. Significant correlations were found between these assessment measures, ranging from r = 0.32 to r = 0.62. Tukulu (1996) completed a correlational study using the Denver II Scales and the Griffiths Scales with Xhosa-speaking pre-school children. Tukulu (1996) found that the Griffiths Scales is a “relevant diagnostic measure for use with South African Xhosa-speaking children” (Baker, 2005, p.90). Luiz, Foxcroft and Stewart (1999) investigated the construct validity of the Griffiths Scales, in which a common factor analysis was used to examine the underlying dimensions of the Scales. The sample of N = 430 South African children ranged between the ages of 54 and 83 months. The results of the study revealed that the Griffiths Scales tend to measure one factor, and that the factor appears to be similar in the different race/cultural groups. The pattern of correlation for South African and British subjects was also found to be similar, indicating that the Scales are measuring a construct that is consistent across cultures and through time (Stewart, 2005). Knoesen (2005) explored the reliability and construct-related validity of the Locomotor Subscale of the GMDS-ER. Sixteen experts within the field of gross motor development and assessment participated in a facet analysis with the aim of identifying the constructs underlying the Locomotor Subscale to enable the researcher to develop a construct model. In addition, a sample was collected of N = 1026 children between the ages of 30.9 months and 89.7 months from across the United Kingdom and Eire. Knoesen (2005) found that satisfactory reliability coefficients and minimal measurement errors calculated for the sample as whole as well as per gender and socio-economic status group, and provided evidence for the reliability and internal consistency of the Locomotor Subscale as a whole as well as per year group. The predominant construct underlying each unique item on the Subscale was qualitatively identified and empirically verified for the sample as a whole, as well as per gender and socio-economic status. Due to the fact that the Griffiths Scales-Extended Revised is a diagnostic measure, Luiz et al. (2006b) explored the Extended Revised Scales content-based evidence, including an empirical analysis of the adequacy with which the test content represents the content domain by means of a literature review and the consultation of experts. Content validity is 84 when researchers evaluate the content of a measure to ensure that it is representative of the area it is intended to cover, by using their expertise in the topic area to judge whether the collection of items has failed to test certain skills or is unduly weighted towards some aspects of the domain compared with others (Coolican, 2004). A facet analysis was conducted separately on each subscale in order to provide proof of the validity of its contents. The results indicated that the items in the six subscales are representative of their respective content domain and that each item has a satisfactory degree of relevance to the construct being measured. 4.7.6 The need for South African research on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) South Africa is a nation characterised by immense diversity in terms of the cultures, languages, and socio-economic status of our people. Poverty and poor living conditions have a major impact on the health status of children in South Africa and result in the classification of ‘children at risk’ (Luiz, 1994). Despite a concerted effort by researchers to address the need for more reliable and valid assessment measures of pre-school South African children, the following shortcomings are still apparent: 1) specific tests are standardised for specific ethnic groups to the exclusion of others, and there are limited standardised tests that assess the development of black pre-school children; 2) specific tests are standardised for specific age groups to the exclusion of others; and 3) existing developmental assessment measures are not comprehensive, with most tests (except the Griffiths Scales) focusing on specific aspects of development (Luiz, 1994). The Griffiths Scales have been widely used in the South African context, as it is applicable to the wider population and has also undoubtedly demonstrated its invaluable role in the assessment of South African children of all ethnic and socio-economic groups. Luiz (1994) recognised that social and cultural factors impact performance on the Scales, and suggested that it would be more valid to replace culturally loaded items that have more influence on the verbal items with items that are equal in difficulty but more culturally relevant. Luiz (1994) believed that: Future research will focus on adapting the Scales taking into account those biographic and social factors, which previous studies have found to have a significant influence on their performance. The goal is to construct a developmental profile for all South African children (p.86). 85 In accordance with the abovementioned goal, more and more studies have been undertaken on the GMDS-ER in the South African context to ensure its applicability with different cultural groups as well as contribute to the growing knowledge base of child development in South Africa. Furthermore, the growing need for the GMDS-ER to be ‘tried and tested’ within the South African society has led to the establishment of an ongoing research project involving the testing of clinical as well as normal samples on the GMDS-ER. This South African project is linked to ongoing and extensive research on the GMDS-ER in the United Kingdom (Davidson, 2008). It is hoped that the findings from these studies will contribute towards the standardisation and norming of the GMDS-ER for South African children in particular (L. Stroud, personal communication, May 25, 2007). 4.7.7 Research studies conducted on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales and Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) The clinical merit of the Griffiths Scales is increasing. Research on the Griffiths Scales have been conducted internationally from as far a field as Canada (Ramsey & Fitzharding, 1977), Columbia (Cobos, Rodriques & De Venegas, 1971), China (Collins, Jupp, Maberley, Morris & Eastman, 1987), Norway (Sletten, 1977), Australia, Greece, France, Lebanon, Germany and the United States of America. Research on the Griffiths Scales has also taken on local prominence in child development due to its application on a wide range of populations (Allan, 1988; Bhamjee, 1991; Mothuloe, 1990; Schröder, 2004; Tukulu, 1996). Initially, research studies consisted of case studies (Luiz, 1988a; 1988b; 1988c) and correlational studies which examined the relationship between the Griffiths Scales and other measures (Heimes, 1983; Lombard, 1989, Mothuloe, 1990; Worsfold, 1993). Such studies preceded normative studies using larger samples of Black, White, Asian and Coloured Children (Allan, 1988; 1992; Bhamjee, 1991; Mothuloe, 1990; Tukulu, 1996). These studies were followed by validity studies (Stewart, 1997; Luiz, Foxcroft & Stewart, 1999; Povey, 2002). The research focus then shifted to the overall revision process of the Griffiths Extended Scales (Barnard, 2000; Kotras, 2003) and on clinical populations (Kotras, 2001; Schröder, 2004). Research on the Griffiths Scales has primarily been conducted in two areas, namely clinical and technical studies. Since the revision of the Scales, several studies have focused on the performance of clinical and normal populations on the original Griffiths Scales and on the GMSD-ER. In addition, technical studies have been conducted on the Subscales of the GMDS-ER, its psychometric properties and its validity and reliability as a developmental assessment. Some of these studies will be reviewed in the following subsection. 86 4.7.7.1 Clinical studies Research relating to the clinical utility of the Scales has provided evidence that the Scales are useful in the clinical assessment and diagnosis of children from normal as well as diverse population groups. Stewart (2005) has agreed that the value of the Griffiths Scales is illustrated in that they can be administered across all clinical populations and “have proved to be a most effective and efficient tool in the assessment of young children, in a diversity of cultural and social contexts” (p.25). The original Scales have been administered to a wide range of children, including a hearing-impaired child (Luiz, 1988a), a battered child (Luiz, 1988b), borderline mentally handicapped pre-schoolers (Houston-McMillan, 1988), and a physically disabled child (Krige, 1988). Research on the original Scales also focused on the need for South African norms and more specifically looking at whether British norms were appropriate for South African children. Allan (1988) studied the extent to which the subject variables of gender, language and socio-economic status influenced performance on the Scales. The possible confounding variables of age, central nervous system development and urban-rural residence were held constant, while gender, language and socio-economic status were built into the design. The South African sample consisted of 60 children aged 5 years with equal numbers of each gender and language group (English and Afrikaans-speaking children). The major findings of Allan’s (1988) study were that: 1) South African 5-year-olds and their 5-year-old British counterparts differed significantly on the General Quotient and in their performance on four of the six Subscales; 2) the influence of the subject variables of gender and language group on test performance was minimal; and 3) children in the different socio-economic groups differed significantly on the General Quotient and in their performance on four of the six Subscales. Therefore, this study indicated that the present British norms of the Griffiths Scales were not that applicable for use with South African children. It was suggested that the findings of this study should be taken into consideration in the interpretation of individual Griffith’s assessments until the re-standardisation of the Scales is completed (Allan, 1988). Allan’s (1992) doctoral thesis focused on the performance of South African normal pre-school children on the original Griffiths Scales, and specifically her study explored the claims of culture bias within the measure. The aims of her research were: a) to compare the performance of South African black, coloured and white children on the Scales; b) to establish the applicability of British norms for a South African sample of children from different backgrounds; and c) to establish the extent to which variables such as gender, language and socio-economic status influence test performance. In analysis of the first aim, 87 the results of the study showed that overall there were performance differences within and between the ethnic groups but no consistent patterns were noted. In analysis of the second aim, which focused on the performances of the separate ethnic groups, 1) the Coloured and Black group scores differed significantly from the White and Indian groups on all Subscales except for the Personal-Social and Practical Reasoning Subscales; 2) significant differences were also noted between the Indian and White groups on these Subscales; 3) Indian children performed significantly lower than all others on the Locomotor Subscale; 4) Indian children performed significantly higher than Black children on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale; 5) White children performed significantly higher than Black or Coloured children on the Language Subscale; and 6) Indian and White children performed significantly higher than Black children on the Performance Subscale. Results of the second aim reported that, compared to the British sample, all South African 5-year-olds performed significantly better on the Locomotor Subscale. When the results obtained were taken into account, it was concluded that: 1) item bias may hinder a national multi-cultural standardisation of the GMDS for South African children; 2) British norms cannot be applied to White or Indian South African children but appeared to be more applicable to Black and Coloured South African children; and lastly, 3) when considering the final aim, gender was found to have minimal influence on the performance of 5-year-old South African children, whereas language and socio-economic status do. Whilst the abovementioned research studies investigated performance on the original Griffiths Scales, the attention will now turn to studies that have been conducted utilizing the GMDS-ER. Since the introduction of the GMDS-ER, this updated measure has significantly contributed to our growing knowledge base of child development within such clinical samples. Clinical studies utilizing the GMDS-ER have focused on: autistic children (Gowar, 2003), HIV positive/AIDS infants (Kotras, 2001; Sandison, 2005), hearing impaired children (Schröder, 2004), and children with cochlear implants (Makowem, 2005). Baker (2005) investigated the performance of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder on the GMDS-ER using a sample of N = 38 children, drawn from an existing database created during the revision of the Scales. Baker (2005) found that the general performance of the ADHD sample on the GMDS-ER was above average, and that their performance across the six Subscales ranged from average to superior, with the poorest performance being on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale, and the best performance being on the Performance Subscale. Baker also compared the performance of her sample with a normal South African sample. Significant differences were obtained between the ADHD and normal sample on the 88 General Quotient (GQ) as well as on three of the six Subscales, namely the Hearing and Speech, Eye and Hand Co-ordination and Performance Subscales. Davidson (2008) compared the performance of first and second born twins on the GMDS-ER using a sample that consisted of 22 pairs of twins. Davidson (2008) found no significant differences between the first and second born twins’ general development. However, Davidson (2008) concluded that the information generated from her study contributed to: 1) child development research; 2) twin developmental research within a South African context; and 3) a greater group of studies on the GMDS-ER currently underway in the United Kingdom and South Africa, which is aiming to contribute to the international credibility of this measure. Such studies emphasize the importance of regular developmental assessments and the importance of the Griffiths Scales as a diagnostic tool (Luiz, 1997). 4.7.7.2 Technical studies Research relating to technical studies refers to studies on the reliability and validity of the Scales, in which research is generated to show that the GMDS-ER is a reliable and valid assessment measure (Beail, 1985; Griffiths, 1984; Luiz, 1988c; Mothuloe, 1990; Stewart, 1997; Worsfold, 1993). Furthermore, technical research has also provided information on the normal performance of South African children of different ages and population groups on the GMDS and GMDS-ER (Baker, 2005). Kotras (1998) investigated the Language Subscale, with a revision of the 20 small pictures and the large picture, with the goal of making them more culturally relevant in a contemporary South African context. The results of Kotras’s (1998) study recommended that the order of the 20 small pictures should be revised so that separate norms for South African children could be constructed. Kotras (2003) extended her research within the Language Subscale to her doctoral thesis in which she explored its construct validity on the Revised Extended Griffiths Scales. The results of this study provided evidence regarding the construct validity of the Language Subscale as well as a verified construct-model. One of the more recent technical studies was conducted by Stewart (2005) on the Extended Griffiths Scales, which aimed to propose new items and adapt existing items for Subscales A, B, C, D, and E as part of a larger project which aimed to revise and restandardise the Extended Griffiths Scales. The research was guided by seven aims and divided into seven steps, which consisted of reviewing the existing items, adapting problematic items, and writing and testing out new items. As the study was exploratory no specific hypotheses were generated and the seven aims and steps mentioned previously guided the methodological process. Stewart’s (2005) findings included the consolidation of 89 the theoretical tenets on which the Griffiths Scales are based, and a content analysis of the original items. Furthermore, these findings in the theoretical and the content arenas in turn influenced the creation of the pool of new and adapted items, as well as the final item recommendations. 31 Item changes were recommended for the Experimental version of the Extended Griffiths Scales, as well as the adjustment of 31 cut-off time limits (Stewart, 2005). The recent revision and restandardisation of the GMDS-ER has necessitated investigations into its psychometric properties. This need for technical studies has resulted in numerous research findings based on studies conducted on the Subscales of the GMDS-ER. Barnard (2000) revised the Practical Reasoning Subscale, and Knoesen (2003) completed a predictive validity study involving the assessment of urban pre-school children to determine whether the GMDS-ER could be used to predict the scholastic performance of Grade One learners. Furthermore, doctoral theses have also been conducted on the Subscales of the GMDS-ER, such as Kotras’s (2003) exploration of the construct validity of the Revised Extended Griffiths Language Subscale; Barnard’s (2004) exploration of the validity of the Revised Griffiths Practical Reasoning Subscales; Knoesen’s (2005) exploration of the reliability and construct-validity of the revised Locomotor Subscale; Moosajee’s (2007) exploration of the validity of the revised Personal-Social Subscale, and Povey’s (2008) exploration of the validity of the revised Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale. The research findings of these studies found that these five subscales, namely Language, Practical Reasoning, Locomotor, Personal-Social and Eye and Hand Co-ordination, yielded more than one construct. Other studies have also focused on studying normal South African children. Van Rooyen (2005) conducted comparative research on normal South African (N = 129) and British (N = 161) children on the GMDS-ER. Van Rooyen’s (2005) findings include: 1) South African and British children’s overall performances on the GMDS-ER GQ are similar; 2) a great deal of variability exists between the GMDS-ER profiles of normal South African and British children (when individual subscales and year groups are considered); and 3) South African children performed better on the Locomotor and Personal-Social Subscales, while British children performed better on the Language and Practical Reasoning Subscales. Performance on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale was similar for the two samples, while the findings obtained on the Performance Subscale indicated that the performance was too variable to come to any general conclusions. Van Heerden (2007) replicated Van Rooyen’s (2005) study, and aimed to generate information on the applicability of the British norms for the contemporary South African population. Van Heerden (2007) found that in 90 contrast to Van Rooyen’s (2005) results, a significant difference does in fact exist between the South African and British children’s overall developmental profiles as measured on the GMDS-ER. In addition, Van Heerden also found that British children performed significantly better on the Language, Eye and Hand Co-ordination and Practical Reasoning Subscales (2007). Furthermore, she found that there were no significant differences between the two samples on the Performance Subscale, which indicates that the performance on this particular Scale are similar (Van Heerden, 2007). Recommendations from these studies suggested that further investigations into the applicability of the GMDS-ER for the diverse South African context are needed and that the establishment of South African norms for the clinical utilization of the GMDS-ER is essential. Both Van Rooyen (2005) and Van Heerden (2007) recommended that caution should be exercised with regard to the utilization of the Britishbased norms in the South African context. The current study falls within the technical area as it is assessing a normal population of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls on the GMDS-ER. In conclusion, Davidson (2008) highlighted the value of technical studies and stated, “without technical studies continuously enhancing the measure’s psychometric properties, the clinical studies performed using the same measure would be invalid and unreliable” (p.63). 4.8 Chapter overview Firstly, an overview was provided of the importance of child developmental assessment, and specific mention was made of the goal of early childhood assessment within the South African context. Developmental assessment was defined as “a process designed to deepen understanding of a child’s competencies and resources and of the caregiving and learning environments most likely to help a child make fullest use of his or her developmental potential” (Greenspan, & Meissels, 1996, p.11). Two forms of developmental measures were described, namely screening and diagnostic measures. It was mentioned that the GMDS-ER falls within the diagnostic category. Secondly, it was emphasized that cultural differences should be taken into account when test performance is interpreted (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). A review was provided of developmental assessment measures within South Africa, and in this regard it was highlighted by Foxcroft and Roodt (2001; 2006) that during the last decade, a concerted effort has been made by researchers to attend to the need for more reliable and valid developmental measures for use with South African children. It was also explained that researchers have focused their efforts on the construction of culture-reduced tests and 91 have adapted, revised and normed assessment measures that have been extensively used in other countries (Foxcroft & Roodt, 2001; 2006). Selected international developmental measures that have been adapted for use in South Africa were then presented. Thirdly, a historical overview of the Griffiths Scales was provided including the revision process, which led to the development of the GMDS-ER. Furthermore, the nature and description of the Subscales of the GMDS-ER was provided, followed by an overview of research studies that have been conducted on the Scales to date. The following chapter will present the problem statement and research methodology that guided the completion of this study. 92 CHAPTER FIVE RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 5.1 Introduction “Psychologists conduct research because they know they cannot rely on ‘feeling’, ‘intuition’ or just plain old ‘common sense’ to answer questions about people and their behaviour” (Coolican, 2004, p.1). Many so called ‘common sense’ claims have been made about gender differences that are mostly based on stereotypical notions or casual observations of children in a playground. It is probable that such claims have not been based on supporting empirical evidence. One may speculate that the reason why such claims are made is because people have a desire to arrive at their own conclusions regarding the existence of gender differences. This chapter outlines the problem investigated in the present study and the primary and specific aims that guided the research process. The methodology employed in conducting the study will be delineated, including the research design, the participants, the sampling procedure and relevant inclusion criteria, as well as the assessment measures utilized and the procedure followed in the collection of the data. Thereafter, a description of the statistical analysis will be provided, followed by the ethical considerations relevant to the present study. 5.2 Problem formulation and aims of the study Despite the original and revised Griffiths Scales reflecting varied or insignificant statistical differences in the performance of boys and girls on the Scales as documented in chapter two, it is nonetheless imperative that variables such as gender be explored on the Extended Revised version. As mentioned in the previous chapter, to date little research has focused on comparing the differences in the development of South African pre-school boys and girls utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). Therefore, it is to this much-needed area of research that the present study devoted itself. In this regard, the first step in the research process is to formulate a research question that will guide this investigative process. The research question that was formulated in the present study focused on whether the boys and girls performed differently on the GMDS-ER. The prevailing gender stereotypes, such as that boys excel in mathematics and girls excel in language, prompted researchers to investigate gender differences in intellectual ability. As discussed in chapter two, gender-related research during the 1960s and 1970s focused narrowly on verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative abilities. The literature review reflected 93 that no gender differences were found within overall intellectual functioning. However, contradictory and inconclusive findings were obtained on whether a pattern of intellectual differences exists between the gender groups. Furthermore, limited empirical investigations have been conducted on developmental differences between the gender groups. As a result of the myths surrounding gender differences coupled with the limited knowledge regarding the developmental differences between the gender groups, the researcher chose to explore the developmental similarities and differences between boys and girls on the GMDS-ER. As the GMDS-ER was standardised for use in the United Kingdom, this study will not only add to the growing body of knowledge regarding the possible differences in the development of 5- and 6-year old boys and girls, but will also contribute towards expanding our understanding of how South African children perform on the GMDS-ER. As more and more South African studies are conducted, it will be possible to standardise and norm the GMDS-ER for South Africa. Against this backdrop, this study aimed to explore and compare the developmental profiles of a sample of normal South African pre-school boys and girls between the ages of 5 and 6 years utilizing the GMDS-ER. In order to achieve this, the following more specific aims were explored: 1. To explore and describe the developmental profiles of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group), with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as their performance on the six Subscales. 2. To compare the General Quotient (GQ) and the developmental profiles across the six Subscales of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group) on the GMDS-ER. 5.3 Research method According to Knoesen (2003) the choice of a research method is an integral part of any study and should be approached with care. The appropriate method that meets the aims and requirements of the study is crucial to the success of the study. Mouton (2001) defined a research method as a plan or blueprint of how one intends conducting the research. In accordance, De Vos, Fouché, Strydom and Delport (2002) explain that a research method focuses on the end product, formulates a research problem as a point of departure and focuses on the logic of the research. The present study employed a quantitative method in order to establish the similarities and differences that exist between normal South African pre-school boys and girls between the ages of 5- and 6-years old. Quantitative research seeks to quantify 94 or numerically portray observations about human behaviour. The quantitative researcher attempts to describe relationships among variables mathematically, and to apply some form of numerical analysis to the social relations being examined (Jackson, 1995). Furthermore, according to Louw and Edwards (1997) “quantification makes comparisons possible” (p.31). This is evident in this study as the analysis of the results is numerically presented. In order to achieve the aims of the study, an exploratory-descriptive research method was utilized. Exploratory research is conducted to gain insight into a situation or phenomenon where there is limited knowledge (Bless & Higson-Smith, 1995; De Vos et al., 2002). The current study adopted the exploratory approach in order to gain insight into the area of gender differences in South African pre-school children utilizing the GMDS-ER, as there is limited research and information in this particular area. Exploratory-descriptive research is a primary and necessary goal for the development of scientific knowledge (Cozby, 1993). It is hoped that the results of the current study will contribute to the knowledge base on the performance of South African children and in this regard prompt further studies to be undertaken utilizing the GMDS-ER. Exploratory-descriptive research attempts to observe, record and describe the behaviour of interest (Cozby, 1993). The developmental profiles of the sample will be described with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as within the six Subscales. Exploratory-descriptive research involves the systematic examination and organisation of carefully observed information about the construct under study (Cozby, 1989, 1993; Dane, 1990). Within this framework, a between-subjects design in which a form of matching was used to control extraneous variables was employed. Foxcroft (personal communication, May 25, 2007) described the need for matching participants when conducting gender-related research: When a non-manipulated independent variable is used, the participants naturally fall into pre-determined groups on the basis of particular subject characteristics. One of the difficulties in having pre-determined groups is that they could differ on a number of other variables. For example, although the children in this study naturally fall into a male or female group by virtue of their gender, there may be a number of other variables that could influence their performance on the GMDS-ER and ultimately provide alternative explanations for the differences between the gender groups. In view of this and in order to obtain a meaningful interpretation of the test results, it is necessary to control for these potential extraneous variables by matching the participants on age, socio-economic status and cultural group. It has repeatedly been found in South African research that the variables of culture and socio-economic status are related to performance on developmental and cognitive measures (Allan, 1988; 1992; Bhamjee, 1991; Knoesen, 2003; Schröder, 2004; Stewart, 2005; Van 95 Heerden, 2007; Worsfold, 1993). As these extraneous variables could have affected the interpretation of the test results, they had to be controlled for in this study. Pairs were created and thus matched on the combination of these variables (age, socio-economic status and culture). In practise, the researcher matched a coloured boy aged six years from a middle class background with a coloured girl aged six years from a middle class background, and then the boy in the pair was assigned to the six-year-old male group and the girl to the six-year-old female group. By doing so, a meaningful comparison could be made of the developmental profiles between the 5- and 6-year-old gender groups. The extraneous variables of cultural group and socio-economic status which could have affected the outcome of the study, and the manner in which they were controlled for in the present study, is discussed below. a) Culture “Our culture has an all pervasive influence on the way we learn, think about things, and behave…culture cannot be considered as a separate factor, exerting an influence apart from other factors in the person’s environment as it is an integral part of that environment” (Grieve, 2001, p.328). Research into development across different cultures has led to the conclusion that cognitive development is inseparable from the context in which it occurs, and those contexts are determined by the economic, family, and religious values provided by one’s culture (Bjorklund, 1995). Cultural norms define appropriate behaviour for the group members. Different developmental experiences across the cultural groups may thus lead to differences in the behaviour of children from different cultural backgrounds (Allan, 1988). In addition, Allan (1988) summed up the influence of culture on testing behaviour and explained that the cultural milieu in which the child is reared will influence test behaviour and that such cultural influences will be reflected in the child’s performance. In the South African context, cultural factors need to be considered during the test adaptation and test construction processes. The development of culture-fair assessments is crucial for our multi-cultural context, in that it is a more realistic approach to test construction by focusing on experiences that are common across different cultures. The GMDS-ER has been termed ‘culture-fair’ in that it has been successfully used in several countries (e.g., United Kingdom, Eire, Portugal, Australia, Hong Kong, South Africa and United States of America) and extensively investigated to reduce cultural bias as far as possible (Luiz et al., 2006a; 2006b). Items that were identified as being biased were removed and improvements subsequently made to ensure that the impact of culture was minimised (Kotras, 1998; Luiz et al., 2006a; Stewart, 2005). 96 It is interesting to note that South African studies have found that cultural influences have less of an impact on a child’s performance on the Griffiths Scales, when compared to the impact of other variables. For example, studies have found that socio-economic status has a greater impact on children’s performance on the Scales than culture (Allan, 1988; 1992; Baker, 2005; Barnard, 2000; Bhamjee, 1991; Knoesen, 2003; Schröder, 2004). Due to the fact that the GMDS-ER has not been translated into other languages as yet, only participants whose first language was English could be included in the present study. As a result, an equal distribution of the four cultural groups could not be obtained, as most of the participants who fulfilled this language criterion were within the White and Coloured cultural groups. b) Socio-economic status (SES) Socio-economic status (SES) refers to the “broader indices of a person or family’s social standing in society, where the major indicators of SES are education, occupation and income” (Grieve, 2001, p.332). The socio-economic aspects of the environment have a significant impact on the experiences that moderate abilities, attitudes and behaviour (Grieve, 2001). One of the areas in which SES has been documented to have a highly negative effect is on cognitive development. Socio-economic status (SES) directly affects the available educational resources along with the quality of the child’s psychosocial rearing environment. Parents within a low SES group may lack the time to spend with their children as well as the level of stimulation they can provide (González, 2001). Their ability to create a high quality home environment is critical as the quality of a child’s early life environment plays a determining role in the child’s level of brain stimulation and thus brain development (Najman et al., 2004). Research has found that SES differences influence performance on a variety of measures for children from all cultural groups (Allan, 1988; 1992; Baker, 2005; Barnard, 2000; Bhamjee, 1991; Knoesen, 2003, Schröder, 2004; Van Heerden, 2007). Schröder (2004) has attributed this differential performance to the factors described above, namely that children from the different SES groupings have different opportunities and access to the necessary social and educational facilities. The SES of the participants in the present study was controlled for and was used as one of the variables on which the boy and girl pairs for the 5- and 6-year-old gender groups were matched. Riordan’s (1978) socio-economic classification system was used to determine the socio-economic status of the participants. Riordan (1978) set boundaries for upper, middle and lower classes for the Black, Coloured, Asian and White population groups in Port 97 Elizabeth, based on the family breadwinners’ educational achievements and occupational status. Foxcroft (1985) suggested that educational level provides a more reliable indicator of socio-economic status than does the level of income, since the former is less likely to invoke the emotional responses that questions regarding income might. Riordan’s (1978) classification system has frequently been used for Griffiths’ studies in Port Elizabeth on both the original and Extended Revised versions (Allan, 1988; 1992; Baker, 2005; Barnard, 2000; Bhamjee, 1991; Foxcroft, 1985; Knoesen, 2003; Stewart. 2005; Van Heerden, 2007; Van Rooyen, 2005). Furthermore, Allan (1988) investigated the validity of this classification system by comparing the results to that of alternative systems and concluded that the system proved valid for use in Port Elizabeth. It should be noted that although Riordan’s (1978) classification system is fairly dated, it is the only classification system of its kind available in South Africa, and was therefore the most useful way of obtaining the necessary information (Baker, 2005). The breadwinners or primary caregivers of the children were required to record their highest level of education on the Biographical Questionnaire. This was converted to a numerical value according to Riordan’s (1978) classification system. The educational classification of the breadwinner is provided in Table 2. Table 2: Classification of breadwinner’s education Breadwinner’s education Score University attendance 7 Post-matric training (not university) 6 Matric 5 Apprenticeship 4 Junior Certificate 3 Primary school 2 None at all 1 No response 0 The breadwinners or primary caregivers were requested to record their occupation on the Biographical Questionnaire. The occupational classification of the breadwinner is provided in Table 3. 98 Table 3: Classification of breadwinners occupation Occupational Classification Score Top professional, executive, administrative and technical occupations 9 Professional, administrative and managerial workers 8 Independent commercial 7 Lower grade administrative, technical, clerical, with limited supervisory and 6 administrative responsibility Artisans and skilled workers with trade qualifications 5 Routine clerical and administrative workers, service and sales workers 4 Semi-skilled production and manual workers 3 Unskilled production and manual workers 2 Not economically active or productive 1 No response 0 The aspect of this classification system that is the most dated is the classification categories for the different cultural groups, and it was initially not going to be used in the present study. Rather, the participants were initially going to be matched according to the composite score from the occupation and education of the breadwinner. However, it became clear that matching using this composite score severely restricted the number of pairs that could be generated as only 13 pairs were obtained. It was then decided to use the boundaries Riordan (1978) had set to classify the upper, middle and lower socio-economic groups to allow for more scope with the matching procedure. Riordan’s (1978) classification of socioeconomic status according to these boundaries is provided in Table 4. Table 4: Classification of socio-economic status Lower Middle Upper Black 2-5 6-10 11-16 Coloured 2-6 7-10 11-16 Asian 2-6 7-10 11-16 White 2-10 11-13 14-16 The matching procedure will be described in more detail in section 5.5.1 after the participants and sampling procedure have been described. 99 5.4 Sampling procedure A sample is a “small portion of the total set of objects, events or persons that together comprise the subject of our study” (Seaberg, 1988, p.240). According to Powers et al. (1985), a sample is studied in an effort to understand the population in which we are interested and as such researchers are interested in describing the sample not primarily as an end in itself, but rather as a means of helping us to explain some facet of the population. For the purposes of the present study, the participants were selected through the use of non-probability, purposive and convenience sampling. The non-probability sampling method is based on the fact that the principle of randomisation is not implemented when selecting the participants (De Vos et al., 2002). The probability of any particular member of the population being selected is not known (Cozby, 1989). The disadvantage of non-probability sampling is that the researcher cannot claim that the sample is representative of the larger population due to the unknown probability that a participant will be selected. In addition, since statistical theories cannot be applied to non-random samples, this results in the limited ability to generalise the properties of the sample to the wider population. Replications of a non-probability study would need to be undertaken in order to determine the generalisability of the results to the population (Davidson, 2008; Rossouw, 1996; Worsfold, 1993). Furthermore, the researcher cannot estimate the sampling error (Baker, 2005). However, non-probability sampling is advantageous as the monetary costs and the time consuming process involved in probability sampling is too high (Struwig & Stead, 2001); it is far less complicated, more economical, and can be conducted “so as to take advantage of available subjects without the statistical complexity of a probability sample” (Baker, 2005, p.114). Within the non-probability framework, the methods of purposive and convenience sampling were employed. In purposive sampling, the judgement of the researcher is used to select the subjects who meet the objectives of the study (Siraj-Blatchford & Siraj-Blatchford, 2002). According to De Vos et al. (2002), in purposive sampling the sample is composed of elements that contain the most “characteristic, representative or typical attributes of the population” (p.207). In the present study, the inclusion criteria guided this selection process and will be discussed shortly. Convenience sampling was also employed, as the sample was chosen “purely on the basis of availability” (Struwig & Stead, 2001, p.111). Six pre-primary and primary schools were contacted within Port Elizabeth to determine if they were willing to have their learners who were in the required age range of 5- and 6-years-old assessed for the present study and four schools consented. Participants meeting the inclusion criteria were also 100 selected from the existing database on the GMDS-ER. Thus, the participants were chosen on the basis of availability. The participants were only included in the study if they met the following inclusion criteria: 1. Age The participants had to fall between the age range of 5 years and 6 years 11 months. 2. Gender As the present study aimed to explore and compare the development of South African pre-school boys and girls utilizing the GMDS-ER, the sample needed to consist of at least 30 boys and 30 girls to enable comparisons of the data to be made. 3. Language The participants had to have English as their first or home language, as the GMDS-ER has not been translated into other languages as yet. 4. Normalcy The Biographical Questionnaire served as a screening measure to identify if any of the participants had been diagnosed with a mental or physical disorder that would have had an influence on their development and subsequent performance on the GMDS-ER. Examples of such disorders include attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism, dyslexia, learning problems, delayed development as evidenced by developmental lags etc. The Biographical Questionnaire requested relevant biographical and developmental information about the participants and assisted in the application of these inclusion criteria. If any physical or mental disorders were identified, the participant was not included in the sample due to the possibility of skewed results. According to Luiz (2000), this method has been successfully used in previous studies to determine normalcy. 5.5 Participants Originally the total sample consisted of 79 children, of which 64 were assessed for the purposes of the study and 15 were selected from the existing GMDS-ER database of the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University’s (NMMU) Psychology Clinic. To identify the participants to be assessed, the researcher selected four pre-primary and primary schools that spanned across Port Elizabeth. Two schools were selected from the middle-to-upper-class 101 geographical region and two schools were selected from the lower-class geographical region as it was hoped to have a sample with equal numbers from the upper, middle and lower socioeconomic status groups. Riordan’s (1978) socio-economic classification system was used to classify the participants into the three groups. The principals and class teachers of the schools selected those children who they believed met the age and normalcy criteria. The Biographical Questionnaire screened the children to determine whether the potential participants met the inclusion criteria. One parent requested that their child be excluded from the study. Five children were excluded before the matched-pairs procedure began, as their first languages were Xhosa and Afrikaans and thus they did not comply with the language criterion for this study. The sample size reduced during the matched-pairs procedure, which is described in the following section. 5.5.1 Matched-pairs procedure When gender comparisons are made, it is vitally important that potential extraneous variables are identified that may provide alternative explanations for the differences between gender groups, and controlled for as strictly as possible. If this does not occur, the researcher cannot conclude with certainty that if differences are found, that it is due to the nonmanipulated variable of gender. It was previously documented that variables such as socioeconomic status and culture have been found to impact child development and can influence performance on assessment measures. Therefore, in order to arrive at any conclusions regarding gender similarities or differences based on the findings of this study, matching the participants on age, socio-economic status and culture controlled for these extraneous variables. More specifically, the researcher created pairs based on a combination of the abovementioned variables. In addition, to further control the impact of age, the data were analysed separately for 5- and 6-year-olds. That is, a male and female group of 5-year-olds and a male and female group of 6-year-olds were created. The boys in the pairs were assigned to their corresponding male group and the girls in the pairs were assigned to their corresponding female group. Initially, the participants were matched on a combination of the following variables, namely the boys were matched with girls of the same cultural group within a three-month age interval on the same composite SES score. For example, a coloured boy aged 5 years 1 month with a composite SES score of 11 was matched with a coloured girl aged 5 years 3 months with a composite SES score of 11. Matching in this manner did not prove to be successful as only 7 pairs could be generated. The age interval was then adjusted to five months to allow 102 for more scope with matching the pairs. Literature on child development indicates that development in 5- and 6-year-old children is not characterised by rapid growth as found in the younger age groups. Therefore, 5- and 6-year old children can be matched according to 5month intervals, as their development is stable. When the participants were matched on the combination of culture, 5-month age interval and same composite SES score, 16 pairs were generated, which was still not sufficient for effective comparisons to be made. It was then decided to classify the composite SES scores according to upper, middle and lower socioeconomic status using Riordan’s (1978) classification system in which boundaries were set for the different cultural groups. This allowed for greater scope in matching the participants, and the age range was further extended to within six-months and no greater. Therefore, the participants were matched on the following variables, namely the boys and girls from the same culture were matched using an age interval of 6-months within the same SES classification group. Using the example above, a coloured boy aged 5 years 6 months from an upper SES class was matched with a coloured girl of 5 years 1 month from an upper SES class. Then the boy in the pair was assigned to the 5-year-old male group and the girl in the pair to the 5-year-old female group. As a result, 32 pairs were obtained consisting of 64 children in total. The sample initially started off with 74 children; therefore 10 children were excluded as a result of the matched-pairs procedure. 5.5.2 Description of the sample The total sample included in the current study consisted of 32 matched pairs of boys and girls, with 17 pairs in the age range of 5 years 0 months to 5 years 11 months; and 15 pairs in the age range of 6 years 0 months to 6 years 11 months. Thus, the total sample included 64 children, with 34 in the 5-year group and 30 in the 6-year group. Table 5 presents the breakdown of the sample by age category, culture, socio-economic status and gender. 103 Table 5: Sample breakdown in terms of age, gender, cultural group and socio-economic status Year V Year VI Total 5 years 0 months 6 years 0 months 5 years 0 months to to to 5 years 11 months 6 years 11 months 6 years 11 months n = 34 n = 30 N = 64 Male 17 (50%) 15 (50%) 32 (50%) Female 17 (50%) 15 (50%) 32 (50%) Black 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) White 16 (47%) 28 (93%) 44 (69%) Coloured 16 (47%) 2 (7%) 18 (28%) Asian 2 (6%) 0 (0%) 2 (3%) Upper 22 (65%) 22 (73%) 44 (69%) Middle 6 (18%) 6 (20%) 12 (19%) Lower 6 (18%) 2 (7%) 8 (13%) Sample Size Gender: Cultural Group: Socio-economic status: The demographic breakdown of the sample in terms of age, cultural group and socioeconomic status will be discussed separately. a) Age The mean age of the total sample was 71.31 months, with a standard deviation of 5.00, the minimum age was 61.10 months and the maximum age was 82.30 months. The breakdown of the total sample according to the two age categories for this study, namely the 5-and 6-year-old age groups is depicted in Figure 5. 104 Figure 5: Sample breakdown in terms of age categories The total sample consisted of approximately equal numbers of participants in the two age categories required for this study. There were 53% of participants in the 5-year-old age group and 47% of participants in the 6-year-old age group. It is interesting to note that the majority of the participants in the sample were from the upper socio-economic status group, despite attempts being made by the researcher to select approximately equal numbers representing each grouping. Pre-primary schools were selected from a range of geographical locations in Port Elizabeth, which should have resulted in more children from a low socioeconomic status background. According to Potgieter (personal communication, September 20, 2008), classifying individuals according to the suburb in which they live reflects somewhat of a Westernised mindset. It was found that many of the breadwinners or primary caregivers in the perceived lower SES areas were educated and many of them worked within the managerial and professional occupations. b) Cultural group As previously mentioned, it was not possible to obtain an equal distribution of the four cultural groups due to the English inclusion criterion. Due to the sampling method employed in the present study, participants who met the inclusion criteria regardless of cultural group were included. The variable of culture became important purely for matching the participants in pairs, and not for comparative purposes. Therefore, most of the participants who met this language criterion fell within the White and Coloured cultural groups. Figure 6 illustrates the sample breakdown in terms of the cultural groups. 105 Percentage of sample Figure 6: Sample breakdown in terms of cultural group 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 5-years-old 6-years-old White Asian Coloured Cultural groups In the 5-year-old sample, 47% of the participants were in the White cultural group, 47% in the Coloured cultural group, and only 6% in the Asian cultural group. In the 6-yearold sample, 93% of the participants were in the White cultural group, 7% in the Coloured cultural group and no participants from the Asian group. c) Socio-economic status (SES) Figure 7 illustrates the breakdown of the sample into the various SES groups according to the 5- and 6-year-old groups. The SES of the 5-year-old group comprised 65% of the participants from the upper socio-economic group, 18% from the middle socio-economic group, and 18% from the lower socio-economic status group. The SES of the 6-year-old group comprised 73% from the upper socio-economic group, 20% from the middle socioeconomic group and 7% from the lower socio-economic group. Despite the fact that the participants’ performance within the different SES groups will not be compared, the unequal distribution of the SES groups will have to be considered carefully when interpreting the results of this study as the majority of the sample in both the 5- and 6-year-old age groups consisted of participants from the upper socio-economic group. 106 Figure 7: Sample breakdown in terms of socio-economic status Percentage of sample 80 70 60 50 5-years-old 6-years-old 40 30 20 10 0 Lower Middle Upper Socio-economic status groups 5.6 Assessment measures The measures that were used to collect the data for the present study were the GMDS- ER and the Biographical Questionnaire. 5.6.1 The Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER) The GMDS-ER was the assessment measure selected for the present study, as it provides valuable insight into the child’s ability in comparison to other children within the same age range (Davidson, 2008). A comprehensive literature review and conclusive NEXUS search revealed that no studies on gender have been embarked upon in South Africa utilizing the GMDS-ER. Developmental assessment measures like the GMDS-ER provide a platform for assessing children’s performance relative to their peers, and emerges as a logical choice for a study such as this to determine whether gender differences do in fact exist between preschool South African boys and girls. The GMDS-ER provided information on the developmental profiles of the boys and girls in this sample, and enabled comparison of the overall developmental profiles as well as on the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER to ascertain where similarities and differences were obtained. As the GMDS-ER was discussed in detail in chapter four, it will not be dealt with at length in this section. Luiz et al. (2006b) investigated the reliability and validity of the GMDS-ER and reported that the overall reliability of the GMDS-ER was highly satisfactory (0.993) and that it was a valid diagnostic developmental measure in that content-based and construct-related evidence was established. The numerous studies that investigated the Griffiths Scales’ validity and reliability were documented in chapter four. It is worth mentioning that the new 107 normative information and technical properties of the GMDS-ER enable the examiner to: 1) convert the raw scores into percentiles and standard scores, 2) comment on significant differences between subscales, and 3) make a qualitative interpretation of the standard scores according to commonly accepted diagnostic criteria. However, it should be pointed out that as the GMDS-ER has not been standardised for use in South Africa as yet, the norms developed in the United Kingdom could not be used in this study. Instead, the raw scores obtained were used in a criterion-referenced way by converting them to quotients based on the child’s chronological age. This system was used for the original Griffiths Scales and still appears to be the most appropriate method to use in South Africa (L. Stroud, personal communication, May 25, 2007). Participants were assessed on the GMDS-ER at their respective pre-primary schools, and the test duration was approximately 90 minutes long. The participants were assessed by psychologists, intern psychologists and masters’ psychology students-in-training that had all received training in the administration and scoring of the GMDS-ER as part of a registered Griffiths Course conducted in the Department of Psychology at the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University (NMMU). 5.6.2 Biographical Questionnaire According to Foxcroft and Roodt (2001), “assessment practitioners need a thorough knowledge of the individuals they assess prior to the assessment” (p.135). Van Heerden (2007) highlighted the fact that the personal circumstances of the test-taker and the timing of the assessment influence the test scores obtained. Therefore, the Biographical Questionnaire was used to extract relevant biographical and developmental information about the participants, and as such served as the screening measure for this study. The questionnaire was in the form of a mailed questionnaire and provided the parent or guardian with dichotomous questioning that required a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answer (Davidson, 2008). Information regarding the participants’ gender, age, cultural group, parent or guardian’s occupation and educational level, as well as whether the participant had any physical or mental disorders was requested. In addition, information regarding the participants’ developmental history and competence in numerous personal-social areas was required in order to enable the scoring of the Personal-Social Subscale of the GMDS-ER. The information on the participants’ developmental history furnished the researcher with sufficient information to determine whether any delays in development may have occurred that would impact the child’s test performance. Furthermore, the information obtained from the Biographical Questionnaire 108 enabled the researcher to calculate the socio-economic status of the participants according to Riordan’s (1978) socio-economic classification system as discussed previously. A copy of the Biographical Questionnaire is included in Appendix D. 5.7 Research procedure The following procedure was followed in order to achieve the aims of this study. The research proposal was presented at a Department of Psychology research proposal meeting, where the research proposal was accepted and submitted to the Faculty Research, Technology and Innovation Committee (FRTI) and the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University (NMMU) Ethics Committee (Human). Once approved by the respective committees, various principals of pre-primary schools in Port Elizabeth were contacted and information letters were sent to them requesting their permission to allow children from their school to participate in the study. Once their consent was obtained to do so, the principals and class teachers were made aware of the inclusion criteria of age and normalcy and they identified children in their preprimary school that could participate. Information letters, informed consent forms and the Biographical Questionnaires were delivered personally to the pre-primary schools to be given to the parents or guardians of the potential participants. The signed and return Informed Consent Forms were then screened to ensure that the children met the inclusion criteria. Testing sessions were arranged to take place at the pre-primary schools, where an empty classroom was allocated for the testing, as it proved more convenient for the parents to have the assessments conducted in this manner. The children were assessed on the GMDS-ER in the language in which they are schooled. Whilst, the class teachers were instructed to only select English-speaking children, the teachers included two Afrikaans speaking children. Test administrators who were fluent in Afrikaans were requested to conduct those assessments, and the children’s profiles were subsequently excluded from the sample. Test administrators scored their own protocols and compiled an individualised report for each participant assessed. Furthermore, the parents of the participants whose performance fell within the below average range were contacted telephonically to provide feedback in addition to the individual report. No child needed to be referred to the University Psychology Clinic for further scholastic assessment. In addition to the individual reports, Pearson Publishers kindly donated School Readiness parent guides and workbooks for the children in the study who fell within the middle-to-low socio-economic status group. These were distributed along with the reports to assist with the recommendations provided. Permission was obtained from Dr. Louise Stroud, 109 a Registered Griffiths Tutor at the NMMU, to access the Griffiths database to select additional participants for this study. Each client whose information was captured in the database had signed a contract, which gave permission for their raw data to be used for research purposes. Therefore, the database was accessed and the necessary raw data was extracted for the purposes of this study. Data capturing then took place and a statistical analysis was conducted. The treatise was then compiled and completed and feedback regarding the overall findings will be provided to the parents in the form of a comprehensive letter. 5.8 Ethical considerations “Ethical principles are vital in assisting the researcher in preventing abuses and they delineate the responsibilities of the investigator” (Barnard & Seymour, 2003, p.6). The primary purpose of ethical principles and values is to protect the welfare and rights of research participants and to reflect the basic ethical values of respect for individuals, beneficence and justice (Ethics in Health Research in South Africa, 2000). Permission for the proposed study was obtained from the Ethics Committee (Human) of the NMMU. The following ethical considerations were adhered to and upheld throughout this study. 5.8.1 Informed consent Firstly, consent was obtained from the principals of the pre-primary schools to select participants from their schools for the present study. Then, informed consent was obtained from the parents of the participants before the data collection was undertaken. Accompanying the informed consent form was a cover letter that provided the parent with the necessary information regarding the study, and its potential risks and benefits. If the parents did not understand the information or had queries, the researcher’s contact details were provided on the informed consent form. The informed consent form also informed the parents that consent was voluntary, and that they were allowed to withdraw their children from the research project at any point. When dealing with child participants in research, their wishes need to be respected irrespective of whether their parents have provided consent. This form of consent is referred to as substitute or third party consent. Third party consent is necessary when the participants do not have the capacity to make decisions on their own, and in such an instance consent needs to be obtained from the parent and child (Davidson, 2008; Drew, Hardman & Hart, 1996). Furthermore, when working with a child, it is imperative that they are assured that they do not have to participate if they do not want to and that they can terminate the 110 testing whenever they want to stop (Drew et al., 1996). Such an instance occurred in the present study when a parent had indicated that they wanted their child to be assessed and included in the study yet the child was very tearful and did not want to participate. Her wishes were respected and her class teacher was informed of the decision. Therefore, in line with the principle of informed consent the administration of the GMDS-ER only commenced once the informed consent forms had been signed and received by the researcher. 5.8.2 Confidentiality and privacy Research participants have a right to privacy and confidentiality. Participant confidentiality was maintained at all times throughout the study as the researcher captured the data. In addition, codes were assigned in place of participants’ names and surnames when the data was sent to the statistician to be analysed. The test administrator who assessed the child compiled confidential individualised reports and the supervisor of the present study checked the reports to ensure they were accurate and of high quality. No names of participants have been mentioned when writing up this research treatise, nor will any names of participants be mentioned when the research findings are disseminated to the principals of the schools. 5.8.3 Fairness As the participants were between the age range of 5- to 6-years-old and the items administered were of varying difficulty, the standard GMDS-ER administration procedure was followed. It was ensured that the participants were not faced with too many excessively easy or difficult items by administering the Scales at approximately 3 months below their chronological age to obtain a basal score, and to cease administration after the ceiling was obtained, namely after 6 consecutive negative scores. Fairness entails treating the participant with respect and dignity and one of the ways this can be ensured is by preventing them from undergoing any physical or psychological harm during the testing process. During the present study, it was apparent that the children enjoyed the process as it was in a familiar environment as well as the fact that most of their friends were also being assessed. Therefore, the test administrators were dealing with bundles of excitement during the testing process. 5.8.4 Inclusion criteria The selection, exclusion and inclusion of participants was just and fair, based on the inclusion criteria. No participant was inappropriately excluded on the basis of race, disability or religious beliefs (Barnard & Seymour, 2003). This ethical consideration was upheld 111 throughout the study, as the participants that were excluded were done so on the basis of either not meeting the inclusion criteria or not finding a match during the matched-pairs procedure. 5.8.5 Investigator competence “Researchers are ethically obliged to ensure that they are competent and adequately skilled to undertake a proposed investigation” (De Vos et al., 2002, p.69). The test administrators who assessed the children for the present study consisted of Masters psychology students-in-training, as well as registered intern psychologists and registered psychologists. These test administrators were reliable professionals who had been trained in the administration of the GMDS-ER and who were ethically allowed to administer the GMDS-ER. 5.8.6 Feedback Feedback in the form of a confidential report for each participant was either given personally or sent directly to the parents concerned. The supervisor of the present study checked the individual reports to ensure that the test administrators exercised caution when providing recommendations to the parents. Health professionals should not underestimate the weight parents ascribe to the evaluative statements provided in reports, and this thought guided the report process in this study. Verbal feedback was provided to those parents whose children performed within the below average range on the GQ of the GMDS-ER. When this was the case, the test administrators who assessed the children were required to provide verbal feedback to the parents telephonically after having consulted with the researcher. In addition, feedback regarding the overall findings of the research project will be mailed to parents and principals of the pre-primary schools in the form of a comprehensive letter. 5.9 Data analysis Data analysis is the statistical procedure that provides the answers to the specific aims of a study. The objective of the present study was to explore and compare the developmental profiles of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls within the ages of 5- and 6years-old utilizing the GMDS-ER. Therefore, the data was analysed according to the specific aims of the study, which were developed to realise the objective. 112 5.9.1 The first aim of the study The first aim of the study was to explore and describe the developmental profiles of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls (between the ages of 5- and 6-years-old), with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as their performance on the six Subscales. Descriptive statistics were employed to analyse the data according to the first aim of the study. Descriptive statistics can be divided into two categories, namely measures of central tendency and measures of dispersion (Davidson, 2008). Measures of central tendency, such as means as well as measures of dispersion or variability, such as ranges and standard deviations, were used to describe the data for this sample of boys and girls per age group separately. The goal in measuring central tendency is to describe a distribution of scores by determining a single value that identifies the centre of a distribution. “The central value will be the score that is the best representative value for all of the individuals in the distribution” (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002, p.51). The mean is the arithmetic average of a distribution and is the sum of the scores divided by the number of scores (Drew et al., 1996; Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002). According to Coolican (2004), the mean is a powerful statistic that is used in estimating population parameters and he believes that this estimation is the basis for the more powerful parametric tests that can be used to investigate significant differences or correlations. Therefore, the mean was used to describe the average of the sample’s performance per age group on the GMDS-ER. Measures of dispersion or variability were also used to analyse the data of the present study, by looking at the range and standard deviation of the scores. “Variability provides a quantitative measure of the degree to which scores in a distribution are spread out or clustered together” (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002, p.76). In this regard, the range is calculated by working out the difference between the highest and the lowest raw scores within a distribution (Drew et al., 1996). Thus, the range (indicating the highest and lowest scores) provides a comprehensive description of the developmental profiles of the sample per gender group and age group separately. The standard deviation approximates the average distance from the mean and measures the extent to which scores are spread around the mean (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002; Louw & Edwards, 1997). Descriptive statistics were employed to explore and describe the developmental profiles of the sample on the GMDS-ER for the boys and girls per age group separately. 113 5.9.2 The second aim of the study The second aim of the study was to compare the General Quotient (GQ) and the developmental profiles across the six Subscales of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6year-old age group) on the GMDS-ER. The second aim was achieved by conducting independent sample t-tests to compare the GQs of the boys and girls per age group. This enabled the researcher to determine whether the samples of boys in the 5-year-old age group differed significantly from the girls in the 5-year-old age group, as well as with the boys in the 6-year-old age group compared with the girls in the 6-year-old age group. The independent sample t-test is used to compare the means of two different groups of scores. The larger the absolute value of t, the more likely it is to reflect a significant difference between the two groups under comparison (Barnard, 2004). A Hotellings T2 test would be used to compare the subscale profiles of boys and girls, followed by a Scheffe’s post-hoc t-test if an overall difference in the profiles was found. With a Hotellings T2 test, the six subscales are compared in one analysis, thereby enabling one to draw conclusions as to whether or not a difference existed between the sample of boys and girls. Scheffe’s post-hoc t-test has the distinction of being one of the safest of all possible post-hoc tests, because it uses an extremely cautious method for reducing the risk of a Type I error. The Scheffe’s test uses an F-ratio to test for a significant difference between any two-treatment conditions (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002). The reason why Scheffe’s test provides the greatest protection from Type I errors is because it requires a larger sample mean difference before it is willing to conclude that the difference is significant (Gravetter & Wallnau, 2002). 5.10 Chapter overview This chapter delineated the methodological approach used in this study. The problem statement, aims and research question were described. The research and sampling method was explained, followed by a discussion of the sample composition and research procedure. A description of the data analysis and ethical considerations relevant to the study were outlined. The results obtained from the data analysis are presented and discussed in the following chapter. 114 CHAPTER SIX RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 6.1 Introduction Before the results are discussed in this chapter, it is important to review the aims of this study. As mentioned in chapter five, the primary aim was to explore and compare the developmental profiles of a sample of normal South African pre-school boys and girls between the ages of 5 and 6 years utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development ScalesExtended Revised (GMDS-ER). In order to achieve this, the specific aims were: to explore and describe the developmental profiles of pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group) with respect to their overall performance on the GMDS-ER as well as their performance on the six Subscales; and to compare the General Quotients (GQs) and the developmental profiles across the six Subscales of the pre-school boys and girls (in the 5- and 6-year-old age group) on the GMDS-ER. Before the results of the descriptive statistics are provided and discussed in accordance with the first aim of the study, the results from independent sample t-tests conducted on the mean chronological age of the boys and girls will be presented, as this determined whether the matching process was accurate. Thereafter, the results of the statistical analysis conducted to achieve the two aims of the study will be presented. To achieve the first aim, empirical findings will be presented in terms of a description of the mean developmental profile of the whole sample on the GMDS-ER, including a description of the sample’s performance across the six Subscales. Then, the mean developmental profiles of the pre-school boys and girls per age group will be described separately. These empirical findings were obtained using descriptive statistics. To achieve the second aim, a comparison of the overall performance of the pre-school boys and girls on the GQ is provided for the whole sample as well as per age group, followed by a comparison of the sample’s Subscale profiles. These findings were obtained using an independent sample ttest to compare the GQs of the sample, and comparisons of the Subscale profiles were obtained using a Hotellings T2. For statistical purposes, it is important to note that mental age (MA), subquotient and GQ scores were utilized in the data analysis of this study. As with other recent South African studies (Davidson, 2008; Van Rooyen, 2005; Van Heerden, 2007), the scores were not converted to percentiles and standard scores using the Analysis Manual (Luiz et al., 2006b), as the GMDS-ER has not been standardised for South Africa as yet. 115 6.2 Comparison of gender groups in terms of age Independent sample t-tests are used to compare the means of two different groups of scores. The larger the absolute value of t, the more likely it is to reflect a significant difference between the two groups under comparison (Barnard, 2004). Independent sample t-tests were conducted on the overall mean chronological age between the genders as well as per age group to determine whether there were any significant differences between them. If significant differences were found, then this would indicate that the matched-pairs procedure discussed in chapter five was not done correctly. If the differences were not significant, then this would indicate that the matched-pairs procedure was accurate. The results of the independent sample t-test conducted on the whole sample is presented in Table 6, followed by the t-tests conducted per age group in Tables 7 and 8. Table 6 Independent sample t-test comparing the mean chronological age of the boys and girls in the whole sample Mean SD Boys 70.94 5.13 Girls 71.69 4.92 * Non-significant p-value (p>0.05) n 32 32 t 0.59 df 62 p* 0.56 Table 6 indicates that there was no significant difference between the mean chronological age of the male versus the female group in the whole sample. This is important for the matching procedure as it indicated that the matching was implemented correctly, and that comparisons can be made as the mean chronological age between the gender groups is similar. If a statistically significant difference were found, no comparisons would have been possible. Statistical results for the two age groups separately will be presented in Tables 7 and 8. Table 7 Independent sample t-test comparing the 5-year-old boys and girls on mean chronological age Mean Boys 67.13 Girls 68.03 * Non-significant p-value (p>0.05) SD 3.04 2.82 n 17 17 t 0.88 df 31 p* 0.39 116 Table 7 indicates that there was no significant difference between the mean chronological age of the male and female 5-year-old groups. Therefore, accurate comparisons could be made between the gender groups in this age group. Table 8 Independent sample t-test comparing the 6-year-old boys and girls on mean chronological age Mean SD Boys 75.41 2.83 Girls 75.83 3.14 * Non-significant p -value (p >0.05) n 15 15 t 0.39 df 28 p* 0.70 Table 8 indicates that there was no significant difference between the mean chronological age of the male and female 6-year-old groups. Therefore, accurate comparisons could be made between the gender groups in this age group. 6.3 Descriptive analysis of the sample’s performance on the GMDS-ER (Aim 1) This section presents and discusses the findings of the study in relation to aim one, which is to explore and describe the performance of the sample on the GMDS-ER. The mean developmental profiles are provided for the sample as a whole, followed by the boys’ and girls’ developmental profiles per age group. 6.3.1 Mean developmental profile of the whole sample Table 9 provides a summary of the mean developmental quotient and sub-quotients for the whole sample. Table 9: Overall mean and Subscale performance of the whole sample (N = 64) GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Mean 116 120 116 120 109 111 118 Minimum Maximum 91 135 78 141 92 135 90 143 65 134 64 145 68 142 Range 44 63 43 53 69 81 74 SD 10 12 11 12 14 18 14 117 Figure 8 depicts the mean performance of the whole sample on the GQ and across the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER. Performance Figure 8: Developmental mean performance of the whole sample on the GMDS-ER 122 120 118 116 114 112 110 108 106 104 102 GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Subscales The above Table and Figure reveal that the mean General Quotient (GQ) for the whole sample was above average (Xˉ GQ = 116). Results on the Personal-Social (BQ) (Xˉ BQ = 116), Performance (EQ) (Xˉ EQ = 111), and Practical Reasoning (FQ) (Xˉ FQ = 118) Subscales all revealed above average performance. Superior performance was noted on the Locomotor (AQ) (Xˉ AQ = 120) and Language (CQ) (Xˉ CQ = 120) Subscales. The Language Subscale has been considered as the most intellectual of the Scales, and the Practical Reasoning Subscale provides a good estimate of the higher order processes involved in logical reasoning. As can be seen in Table 9, the sample’s performance on these Subscales was among their strongest on the GMDS-ER. A possible explanation for this sample’s superior to above average performance on five of the six Subscales could be due to the fact that 88% of the sample consists of participants from the upper and middle socio-economic groups, with 69% from the upper socio-economic group. Therefore, it was assumed that their basic needs for food and shelter had been met, and adequate resources had been provided in their early childhood to ensure healthy development. This is evident as the participants have met the normalcy criteria and thus been included in this study. The Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale (DQ) (Xˉ DQ =109) was the only subscale that recorded average performance. The largest range was found within the Performance 118 Subscale (range 81), with the highest developmental sub-quotient of 145 and the lowest developmental sub-quotient of 64. The lowest range was found within the Personal-Social Subscale (range 43), with the highest developmental sub-quotient of 135 and the lowest developmental sub-quotient of 92. The greatest variability in scores occurred within the Eye and Hand Co-ordination, Performance and Practical Reasoning Subscales, as reflected by their large ranges. It is useful to review the findings of South African studies conducted on the GMDSER which have investigated the overall performance of normal 5- and 6-year-old children, as they provide a benchmark against which to measure this sample’s performance (Knoesen, 2003; Van Heerden, 2007). In Van Heerden’s (2007) study, the Locomotor Subscale was elevated above the other five Subscales, which is similar to the present study. Similarly, Knoesen (2003) also found her sample’s performance on the Locomotor Subscale to be better than on the other Subscales. South African children spend a considerable amount of time outside playing with friends and family, thereby perfecting their locomotor skills. This coupled with the fact that children are only required to attend formal schooling when they are aged six turning seven years, could be possible contributing factors towards South African children’s superior performance on this Subscale (Van Rooyen, 2005). It is interesting to note that this sample’s performance on the Language Subscale is considerably higher than in Van Heerden’s (2007) study, with a nine-point difference between the two samples. The sub-quotients of the Locomotor and Personal-Social Subscales in Van Heerden’s study are identical with the present study’s corresponding Subscales, whereas only the Personal-Social Subscale and Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscales of Knoesen’s (2003) study were similar to the present study’s findings. Both Knoesen (2003) and Van Heerden (2007) found their sample’s performance on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale to be one of the weakest. In the present study a similar trend is found. Reasons that are often provided to explain the poor performance of South African children on this Subscale revolve around inadequate day-care facilities and pre-schools in the disadvantaged areas, which lay a weak foundation when children enter formal schooling (Davidson, 2008). However, the majority of this sample is from the upper socio-economic group, and they attend pre-schools reputed to be well-equipped with the latest resources and experienced teachers. It may be possible that the results obtained by the children in the low socio-economic group skewed the mean sub-quotient for this Subscale. This is plausible as the minimum score within the low SES group obtained on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale fell within the mild mental 119 retardation descriptive category. Therefore, it is important to analyse the findings of a study in the context of its sample and individual participants. 6.3.2 Mean developmental profiles of the 5-year-old boys and girls The mean developmental quotients in this age group are presented in Tables 10 and 11 below. Table 10 Mean developmental profile for the 5-year-old boys (n = 17) GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Mean 116 121 118 122 107 108 118 Minimum Maximum 91 135 93 141 93 134 90 143 74 134 64 137 68 140 Range 44 48 41 53 60 73 72 SD 14 16 11 18 19 22 18 Range 38 41 35 44 66 72 58 SD 11 12 10 12 18 23 14 Table 11 Mean developmental profile for the 5-year-old girls (n = 17) GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Mean 116 118 122 121 109 108 118 Minimum Maximum 92 130 94 135 100 135 99 143 65 131 73 145 84 142 In reviewing the above results, it appears that the 5-year-old boys and girls obtained similar results on the GMDS-ER. Interestingly, both the boys and girls obtained the same above average GQ of 116, as illustrated by the purple shading in Tables 10 and 11. In addition, the 5-year-old boys and girls obtained the same sub-quotients on the Performance and Practical Reasoning Subscales, as indicated by the yellow shading. Their results on the Performance Subscale (Subscale E) reveals average performance (Xˉ EQ =108), and their results on the Practical Reasoning Subscales (Subscale F) reveals above average performance 120 (Xˉ FQ =118). The boys performed better on the Locomotor Subscale and only slightly higher on the Language Subscale. The boys’ performance was superior on both the Locomotor Subscale (Xˉ AQ =121) and the Language Subscale (Xˉ CQ =122). The girls performed better on the Personal-Social Subscale and performed only slightly better on the Eye and Hand Coordination Subscale. The girls’ results on the Personal-Social Subscale indicate superior performance (Xˉ BQ =122) and average performance on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale (Xˉ DQ =109). Overall, the boys obtained their strongest performance on the Language Subscale and their weakest performance was noted on the Performance Subscale. Overall, the girls’ obtained their strongest performance on the Personal-Social Subscale and similarly, their weakest performance was also on the Performance Subscale. When analysing the ranges of the 5-year-old boys and girls, it appears that the GQ range was greater in the boys’ performance (range 44) than the girls, with the highest developmental quotient recorded at 135. Both 5-year-old boys and girls showed their largest range on the Performance Subscale. The mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 5-year-old boys and girls is presented graphically in Figure 9. Figure 9: Mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 5-year-old sample 81 Age (in months) 78 75 72 Boys (5-years-old) 69 Girls (5-years-old) 66 63 60 Chronological Age Mental Age The 5-year-old girls’ mean chronological age was 68 months and the 5-year-old boys’ mean chronological age was 67 months. As reflected in Figure 9, the 5-year-old girls’ mean mental age was higher than the 5-year-old boys’. The girls obtained a mean mental age of 80 months, whereas the boys obtained a mean mental age of 78 months. Comprehensive descriptive and inferential statistics will be dealt with under the umbrella of the second aim, 121 which focuses on the comparative performances between the genders per year group on the GQ as well as across the six Subscales. 6.3.3 Mean developmental profiles of the 6-year-old boys and girls The mean developmental quotients in this age group are presented in Tables 12 and 13. Table 12 Mean developmental profile for the 6-year-old boys (n = 15) GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Mean 117 121 113 120 111 116 119 Minimum Maximum 106 128 108 133 92 127 109 136 97 125 93 133 103 138 Range 22 25 35 27 28 40 35 SD 7 8 11 8 8 13 12 Range 24 52 30 39 31 38 58 SD 5 12 10 10 8 12 13 Table 13 Mean developmental profile for the 6-year-old girls (n = 15) GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Mean 114 117 110 119 111 110 117 Minimum Maximum 100 124 78 130 93 123 95 134 93 124 88 126 77 135 In reviewing the above findings in Tables 12 and 13, it is apparent that the genders’ performance within this age group differed more in the quotients and sub-quotients obtained across the six Subscales, than was the case in the 5-year-old age group. In careful analysis of the quotients, it is evident that the 6-year-old boys obtained higher quotients than the girls on each Subscale; with the exception of the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale, in which the same sub-quotient was obtained as illustrated by the blue shading in Tables 12 and 13. Both genders obtained an above average GQ, however the 6-year-old boys’ mean GQ was slightly 122 higher (Xˉ GQ =117, SD 7, range 22) than the girls’ mean GQ (Xˉ GQ =114, SD 5, range 24). The 6-year-old boys in the present study obtained superior performance on the Locomotor (Xˉ AQ =121) and the Language Subscales (Xˉ CQ =120). On the rest of the Subscales, the boys’ obtained above average performances. The 6-year-old girls in the present study obtained above average performance across all six Subscales. The greatest discrepancy between the genders’ existed on the Performance Subscale, with a six-point difference in the sub-quotients obtained. Across the other five Subscales, the difference favouring the 6-year-old boys’ in the sample was small. The 6-year-old girls in the present study did not perform better than the boys on any of the Subscales. The girls’ strongest performance was on the Language Subscale, and their weakest performance relative to their other Subscales was on the Performance Subscale. The boys’ strongest performance was on the Locomotor Subscale, and their weakest performance relative to their other Subscales was on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale. In the 6-year-old girls’ sample, the largest range occurred on the Practical Reasoning (range 58) and Locomotor (range 52) Subscales, with both recording a maximum score within the very superior descriptive category, and recording a minimum score within the borderline descriptive category. In the 6year-old boys’ sample, the ranges were considerably smaller, with the largest range occurring on the Performance Subscale (range 40), followed by the Personal-Social and Practical Reasoning Subscales, in which the same range was obtained (range 35). The mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 6-year-old boys and girls is presented graphically in Figure 10. Figure 10: Mean chronological ages and mental ages of the 6-year-old sample 90 Age (in months) 87 84 81 Boys (6-years-old) 78 Girls (6-years-old) 75 72 69 Chronological Age Mental Age 123 The 6-year-old girls’ mean chronological age was 76 months and the 6-year-old boys’ mean chronological age was 75 months. Despite the observation that the 6-year-old boys obtained higher developmental sub-quotients, both 6-year-old genders obtained the same mental age on the GMDS-ER, which was 88 months old. The youngest mental age recorded for the 6-year-old girls’ sample was 73 months and the oldest mental age recorded was 117 months. The youngest mental age recorded for the 6-year-old boys’ sample was 80 months and the oldest mental age recorded was 95 months. 6.4 Comparisons of the sample’s performance on the GMDS-ER (Aim 2) To achieve the second aim, a comparison of the overall performance of the pre-school boys and girls on the GQ is presented and discussed in this section, followed by a comparison of the performance per age group on the GQ. These findings were obtained using an independent samples t-test to compare the mean GQs of the boys and girls at the α-level of 0.05. 6.4.1 Comparison of overall performance of the gender groups The results of the independent sample t-test that tested for a significant difference between the GQ of the boys and girls in the whole sample are presented in Table 14. Table 14 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the whole sample Mean SD 116.44 10.80 Boys 115.19 8.83 Girls * Non-significant p-value (p>0.05) n 32 32 t -0.51 df 62 p* 0.6131 When comparing the performance of the 32 pairs of pre-school boys and girls in the whole sample, no significant difference was found between the mean GQs of the boys and the girls. The results of the independent sample t-test conducted on the GQs of the pre-school boys and girls within the 5-year-old age group are presented in Table 15. 124 Table 15 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the 5-year-old age group Mean SD 115.91 13.99 Boys 116.14 11.11 Girls * Non-significant p-value (p>0.05) n 17 17 t 0.05 df 31 p* 0.9587 As reflected in Table 15, no significant differences were found between the 5-year-old boys and girls on the mean GQs. The results of the independent sample t-test conducted on the GQs of the pre-school boys and girls within the 6-year-old age group are presented in Table 16. Table 16 Independent sample t-test comparing the GQ of the pre-school boys and girls in the 6-year-old age group Mean SD 116.71 6.83 Boys 114.12 5.40 Girls * Non-significant p-value (p>0.05) n 15 15 t -1.15 df 28 p* 0.2583 Table 16 indicates that no significant differences were found in the overall performance of the 6-year-old boys and girls. According to Foxcroft (personal communication, December 3, 2008), the fact that these results indicate non-significant differences between boys and girls in this study reflects positively on the revision of the Griffiths Scales, as it suggests that the revision process was sensitive to not building in gender bias. Previous South African studies conducted on the original Griffiths Scales done by Allan (1988), Tukulu (1996) and Ward (1997) investigated the influence of gender on the performance of their samples, and also found that the mean GQs of the two gender groups did not differ significantly. Allan (1988) focused on the performance of White 5-year-old boys and girls, and the results of a multiple t-test indicated that there was no significant difference between the GQs of the boys (M = 109.13, SD = 10.5) and girls (M = 110.27, SD = 7.9). Tukulu (1996) focused on the performance of Black, Xhosa-speaking children between the ages of 3 and 6 years, and the results of an independent sample t-test revealed that there was no significant difference between the mean GQs of the boys (M = 95.57, SD = 9.48) and girls 125 (M = 99.47, SD = 11.12). In a similar study, Ward (1997) focused on the performance of Coloured children between the ages of 3 and 6 years, and the results of the t-test revealed no significant differences between the GQs of the boys. Asian, Coloured and White children were included in the current sample, however no comparisons were made of the performance of the different cultural groups, as it was not within the focus of this study. Therefore, the results of the independent sample t-tests conducted in the present study coincide with the findings obtained in previous studies on the topic. Only one study on the GMDS-ER utilized an independent sample t-test to briefly investigate whether gender influenced the performance of his sample (Van Rooyen, 2005). In contrast with the findings of the present study, Van Rooyen (2005) compared a South African and British sample, and found a significant difference between the GQ performance of the South African boys (M = 110.47) and girls (M = 116.48). Despite the fact that these studies have also looked at the influence of gender on the Griffiths Scales and will be referred to extensively in the following section, it needs to be pointed out that none of these studies have focused solely on comparative performances of their samples, and as such have not controlled for extraneous variables. Therefore, it cannot be claimed with certainty that the results reported on from their studies are due to the sole influence of gender. This study is the first of its kind on the GMDS-ER in which gender differences were explored. Furthermore, the strict process of matching the boys and girls into pairs and assigning them to their relevant age groups to control for the extraneous variables of age, socio-economic status and culture has not been done before in studies on the GMDS-ER. 6.5 Comparison of the Subscale profiles on the GMDS-ER (Aim 2) A Hotellings T2 test was used in this study to compare the subscale profiles of the whole sample, as well as the subscale profiles of the boys and girls per age group. An overall Hotellings T2 test revealed that there were no significant differences across the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER in the whole sample (p = 0.720). With regards to the 5-year-old boys and girls in the sample, an overall Hotellings T2 test revealed that there were no significant differences between the genders in this age group across the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER (p = 0.7135). With regards to the 6-year-old boys and girls in the sample, an overall Hotellings T2 test revealed that there were no significant differences between the genders in this age group across the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER (p = 0.8351). Although the results of the independent sample t-tests and Hotellings T2 tests have shown that there were no significant differences in the performance of this sample on the GMDS-ER, the descriptive analysis indicated that the boys and girls in the sample performed 126 differently on some of the Subscales. Figure 11 graphically depicts the developmental profiles of the genders within the two age groups under comparison. In addition, Table 17 summarises the comparison between the boys and girls in the two age groups in terms of their mean GQs and sub-quotients. It is important to alert the reader to the fact that in the following section, the researcher is not attempting to make any causal inferences based on the results of the descriptive analysis, but aims to merely emphasize the descriptive observations noted and to point out areas that would benefit from further investigation. Figure 11: The comparative Subscale performances of 5- and 6-year-old boys and girls 125 Developmental Sub-quotients 120 Girls (5-years-old) Boys (5-years-old) Girls (6 years old) Boys (6 years-old) 115 110 105 100 95 GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ Subscales Table 17 Summary of the sample’s mean performance in terms of GQ and sub-quotients across the 5and 6-year-old age groups GQ AQ BQ CQ DQ EQ FQ 5-years-old Boys Girls 116 116 121 118 118 122 122 121 107 109 108 108 118 118 6-years-old Boys Girls 117 114 121 117 113 110 120 119 111 111 116 110 119 117 127 Although no significant differences were found, the 6-year-old boys appeared to obtain the highest mean GQ in the overall sample. In both age groups, the boys appeared to perform better than the girls on the Locomotor Subscale (AQ). The 5-year-old boys performed slightly better on the Language Subscale (CQ), with a one-point difference separating their mean score from the 5-year-old girls’ score. Both the 6-year-old boys and girls obtained the highest mean performance across the sample on the Eye and Hand Coordination Subscale (DQ). The 6-year-old boys appeared to obtain the highest mean performance on the Performance Subscale (EQ) and only performed slightly better than the rest of the sample on the Practical Reasoning Subscale (FQ). The 5-year-old girls appeared to perform better than the rest of the sample on the Personal-Social Subscale (BQ). Figure 11 illustrates that the sample as whole performed strongest on the Personal-Social (BQ), Language (CQ) and Locomotor (AQ) Subscales, and their weakest performance relative to the other Subscales was on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination (DQ) and Performance (EQ) Subscales. The genders’ performance across the six Subscales will now be discussed. 6.5.1 Locomotor Subscale Whilst not being statistically significant, it was noted in Figure 11 that the boys in both age groups performed better than the girls on the Locomotor Subscale. Both 5- and 6year-old boys displayed superior performance on this Subscale (Xˉ AQ = 121). The 6-year-old girls obtained the lowest sub-quotient on this Subscale within the sample. It is generally accepted that developmental gender differences exist in gross-motor skills. On tasks related to muscular strength, pre-school boys perform significantly better than girls, such as ball throwing speed and throwing distance (Blakemore et al., 2009). In accordance, Van Rooyen (2005) found that the boys in his sample obtained a higher sub-quotient than the girls on the Locomotor Subscale of the GMDS-ER. Although it was not significantly different, he found that the boys performed within the superior range. Furthermore, Knoesen (2003) also found that the boys in her sample performed better than the girls on the Locomotor Subscale of the Revised Griffiths Scales. Knoesen (2003) did not test whether the differences were significant. The results presented above support the findings of the present study that boys perform better on gross-motor skills and activities. Studies investigating the influence of gender on the original Griffiths Scales add additional support for the observed difference noted on this Subscale favouring boys. Allan (1988) also found that the 5-year-old boys in her sample obtained superior performance on the Locomotor Subscale. Similarly, Ward (1997) found a difference in favour of boys on the 128 Locomotor Subscale, in which superior performance was obtained. Both findings were merely descriptive, and thus non-significant. Following the same trend, superior performance was noted in the findings of the present study when analysing the boys’ performance within the sample. In contrast with the above findings, Tukulu (1996) found that the girls in her sample obtained a higher sub-quotient on this Subscale, which was in the average range. Despite the fact that the above findings support the descriptive gender difference noted on this Subscale in the present study, it should be emphasized that these differences did not reach the required level of significance. Consequently, it can be concluded that the performance of the boys and girls on the Locomotor Subscale was essentially similar in this study. 6.5.2 Personal-Social Subscale On this particular Subscale, Figure 11 illustrates that the 5-year-old girls obtained the highest sub-quotient within the overall sample (Xˉ BQ = 122), which was within the superior range, yet not significantly different in comparison to the 5-year-old boys. The 6-year-old girls obtained the lowest sub-quotient in the sample, which was above average (Xˉ BQ = 110). Van Rooyen (2005) found that the girls within his sample obtained a superior sub-quotient to the boys on the GMDS-ER. Similarly, the girls within Knoesen’s (2003) study also performed better than the boys on the Revised Griffiths Scales. In addition, Tukulu’s (1996) study on the original Griffiths Scales revealed that the girls within her sample performed better than the boys. All these studies support the findings obtained in the present study on the PersonalSocial Subscale. However, three previous studies conducted on the original Griffiths Scales paint a different picture. In Allan’s (1988) study, the boys obtained a higher sub-quotient on Personal-Social Subscale when compared to the girls. In addition, Ward (1997) too found a difference in favour of the boys on the Personal-Social Subscale. However, none of these differences reached the required level of significance and were merely descriptive observations noted by the respective researchers. In contrast, a significant gender difference was found in Bhamjee’s (1991) study, with the girls obtaining a significantly higher subquotient than the boys on the Personal-Social Subscale. Interestingly, whilst the 5-year-old girls in this sample performed the strongest on this Subscale, the 6-year-old girls obtained the lowest sub-quotient, which was nonetheless above average. Irrespective, an overview of the abovementioned South African studies indicates that there is a clear trend in favour of girls when looking at performance between the gender 129 groups on the Personal-Social Subscale, which adds support to the findings obtained in the present study. It is interesting that across the studies reviewed on the Griffiths Scales, it appears that girls consistently perform better than boys on this Subscale. As a strong emphasis is placed in the Personal-Social Subscale on the child’s ability to interact with peers, a possible explanation for this gender difference could exist in the concept of gender roles and “gender-typed styles of social interaction” (Ding & Littleton, 2005, p.170). Blatchford, Baines and Pellegrini (2003) found that boys were more likely than girls to engage in fantasy play and ball games, whereas girls were more likely to engage in conversation, drawing, reading, and skipping. Thorne (1993) found that boys’ conversations revolve around competitive discussions about strengths and physical ability in which any activity is turned into a contest. As a result, they may experience more conflict in their social interactions with their peers. Gender differences in aggression were documented in chapter two, which further illustrates Thorne’s point about how quickly such competitive discussions can aggressively escalate. “Studies of children’s friendships have revealed gender differences that seem to mirror gender roles in society at large” (Ding & Littleton, 2005, p.171). In addition, gender roles are influenced by assumptions that exist regarding the division of labour between men and women, boys and girls, within the average household. For instance, it is assumed that boys may be less interested in activities on the Personal-Social Subscale such as tying a bow-knot, setting a table, and shampooing hair for instance, as it is not masculine or competitive enough. On the other hand, it is assumed that girls may be more inclined to master these household tasks in order to achieve a sense of competency and identity owing to the fact that it is perceived as gender appropriate. For instance, setting a table and washing one’s hair may interest a little girl far more because she sees that her mommy does it, whereas a little boy may not see his daddy do it as often so he does not view this task through the same lens. However, it is important for the reader to note the abovementioned examples provided, as being assumptions. In addition, it must also be pointed out that these explanations provided are purely speculative and in need of investigation. Despite the fact that the above findings support the descriptive gender difference noted on this Subscale in the present study, it should be emphasized that these differences did not reach the required level of significance. Consequently, it can be concluded that the performance of the boys and girls on the Personal-Social Subscale was essentially similar in this study. 130 6.5.3 Language Subscale Figure 11 illustrates that the 5-year-old boys within this sample obtained the highest sub-quotient (Xˉ CQ = 122) on the Language Subscale, which was within the superior range, followed closely by the 5-year-old girls (Xˉ CQ = 121). The performance of the 6-year-old group of boys and girls was essentially similar, with the boys (Xˉ CQ = 120) performing slightly better than the girls (Xˉ CQ = 119). Whilst, none of these slight differences reached statistical significance, it is interesting to note that the boys in both age groups were slightly ahead of their female counterparts. Overall, it is evident that the sample performed very well on this Subscale. Research findings obtained on both the original Griffiths Scales and the GMDS-ER conflict with the findings of the present study. On the GMDS-ER, Van Rooyen (2005) found that the girls performed significantly better on the Language Subscale than the boys. On the Revised Griffiths Scales, the girls in Knoesen’s (2003) study also obtained a higher subquotient than the boys on this Subscale, which was known as the Hearing and Speech Subscale on this version of the Griffiths Scales. As mentioned, the differences found in Knoesen’s (2003) study were not tested for significance. On the original Griffiths Scales, the girls performed only slightly better than the boys (Allan, 1988; Tukulu, 1996). In Ward’s (1997) study on the original Griffiths Scales, the girls performed significantly better than the boys at the p ≥ 0.01 significance level. However, it is interesting to note that had the significance level not been adjusted to p ≥ 0.01, the boys in Ward’s sample would have performed significantly better than the girls on this Subscale at the p ≥ 0.05 level. Nonetheless, this clear trend reflecting a female advantage in verbal ability was not observed in the results of the present study. Moore (1967) compared the GQ and CQ of boys and girls and found that girls performed significantly better on the CQ as age increased. Yet, in the present study the 5year-old group obtained higher CQs than the 6-year-old group. In addition, a larger percentage of children in the 5-year-old age group are from the lower socio-economic group than in the 6-year-old age group, and a larger percentage of children in the 6-year-old age group are from the middle-to-upper socio-economic group. In this instance, the results in the present study contradict Moore’s (1967) findings. Furthermore, as chapter two indicated gender differences in verbal ability have been one of the ‘well-established’ facts of psychological research. To recap, Bornstein et al. (2004) found that girls between the ages of two and six were consistently ahead of boys on almost all of language development measures they were tested on, but not before or after. Hurlock 131 (1981) asserted that boys’ sentences are shorter and less grammatically correct than girls at every age, with smaller vocabularies and less accurate pronunciations. Also, McCarthy and Kirk (1963) found that boys across all age levels were better than girls at visual coding. In this study, there was only a one-point difference in scores between the boys and girls in both age groups. Therefore, despite this slight (non-significant) difference observed, the results on this Subscale are essentially comparable between the boys and girls. This observation adds support to the gender similarities view in verbal ability discussed in chapter two. In line with this view, several research studies yielded no evidence of a substantial gender difference in verbal ability (Bjorklund, 1995; Feingold, 1988; Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Hyde, 1981; Hyde & Linn, 1988, Marsh, 1989; Plomin & Foch, 1981). 6.5.4 Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale On the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale, Figure 11 illustrates that both boys and girls in the 6-year-old age group showed above average performance, and thus obtained the joint highest sub-quotient. In the 5-year-old age group, the boys displayed average performance and obtained the lowest sub-quotient on this Subscale. No significant differences were found on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale. Research findings obtained on both the original Griffiths Scales and the GMDS-ER have obtained different findings. On the GMDS-ER, Van Rooyen (2005) found a significant difference in his sample’s performance on this Subscale, with the girls performing significantly better than the boys. On the Revised Griffiths Scales, Knoesen (2003) also found that girls performed better than the boys, but could not comment on whether this difference was significant. This trend dates back to the studies conducted on the original Griffiths Scales, in which girls were found to achieve higher sub-quotients than the boys, yet these differences were not significant (Allan, 1988; Ward, 1997). Tukulu (1996) obtained similar findings and remarked that “had the significance level not been adjusted, the girls would have performed significantly better than the boys on Scale D (Eye and Hand Co-ordination)” (p.75). In none of these studies reviewed, did the boys achieve the same sub-quotients as the girls. In this regard, the findings of the present study challenge the studies documented above. According to Blakemore et al. (2009), in assessments where children’s fine-motor skills are assessed, girls are found to perform slightly better. Whilst, this was not observed in the two 6-year-old gender groups, it was observed with the 5-year-old boys and girls where the girls obtained a slightly higher sub-quotient than their male counterparts. The Eye and 132 Hand Co-ordination Subscale also assesses a child’s visual-perceptual and perceptual-motor skills as well as sensorimotor ability. As discussed in chapter two, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) found that gender differences were well-established in the area of visual-spatial ability. A large body of research conducted over the last 25 years has revealed substantial gender differences for some, but not all, of the measures that reflect visual-spatial information processing (Halpern et al., 2007). Levine et al. (1999) concluded that gender differences in favour of boys are present on spatial tasks by age 4 years 5 months. Conversely, these findings are not corroborated by the results of the present study. In this study, the performance of the 6-year-old boys and girls was parallel. Despite the fact that the performance between the gender groups differed slightly in the 5-year-old age group, it was non-significant and essentially similar. Hyde (2005) reviewed the major meta-analyses that have been conducted on psychological gender differences and included studies conducted on the visual-spatial abilities conducted with children on areas of spatial relations, spatial perception, mental rotation, spatial visualizations, and the progressive matrices. The effect sizes from these studies ranged between close-to-zero to small. This adds additional support for the findings obtained by the sample on this Subscale. It should be emphasized that the differences observed between the 5-year-old boys and girls on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale did not reach the required level of significance. Consequently, it can be concluded that the performance of the boys and girls on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale was essentially similar in this study when compared to previous studies. 6.5.5 Performance Subscale On this particular Subscale, Figure 11 illustrates that the 6-year-old boys obtained the strongest performance, which was above average (Xˉ EQ = 116). This observed difference did not reach the required level of significance, and consequently performance between the gender groups on this Subscale was essentially similar in this study. The lowest sub-quotient was shared between the 5-year-old boys and girls, with both showing average performances (Xˉ EQ = 108). In contrast, Van Rooyen (2005) found a significant difference on the Performance Subscale of the GMDS-ER, where the girls performed better than the boys. On the Revised Griffiths Scales, Knoesen (2003) found a difference on this Subscale showing a female advantage, with significance not tested for as mentioned previously. 133 This trend showing a female advantage on the Performance Subscale was also apparent when reviewing findings of studies on the original Griffiths Scales, where girls appeared to perform better than boys (Allan, 1988; Tukulu, 1996; Ward, 1997). Ward (1997) stated that had the significance level not been adjusted in her study, the boys would have performed significantly better on the Performance Subscale. It is interesting to note that in some international studies, in which female superiority in visual-spatial skills during the preschool years has been documented, have shown that in subsequent years the gender difference disappears and that the genders are equal until adolescence (Berry, 1966; Coates, 1974). The above literature suggests that the female advantage Van Rooyen (2005) found in his study is likely to disappear as the children advance in primary school. Whilst not significant, it appears that the findings obtained in the present study go against the grain when compared to other studies looking at the influence of gender on the Griffiths Scales. As discussed in chapter two, Townes et al. (1980) found that boys are more advanced in spatial and motor skills than girls. Boys are found to be more accurate than girls at spatial tasks that measure accuracy of spatial transformations, and have obtained higher scores on the Mazes subtest of the Wecshler Pre-school and Primary Scale of Intelligence (Levine et al., 1999). The results presented above support these findings in the sense that although non-significant, the 6-year-old boys performed better on this Subscale than the girls. However, as mentioned, the fact that this difference was not significant means that statistically the performance of boys and girls on the Performance Subscale was similar. In line with the gender similarities view, Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) located several studies in which visual-spatial ability has been studied in children, such as the spatial subtests of the Differential Aptitudes Test and the Primary Mental Abilities Test, including mazes, formboards and block counting. On the whole, they found no gender differences in these measures in the early childhood and pre-school years. Brainerd and Vanden Huevel (1974) also found no significant differences when they studied a sample of 120 children between the ages of 5- and 6-years-old in which they had to choose two-dimensional drawings to represent three-dimensional objects. 6.5.6 Practical Reasoning Subscale On the Practical Reasoning Subscale, Figure 11 illustrates that the 6-year-old boys obtained the highest sub-quotient and performed within the above average range (Xˉ FQ = 119), followed closely by the boys and girls within the 5-year-old age group who obtained the same sub-quotient (Xˉ FQ = 118). The 6-year-old girls also displayed above average performance, 134 but obtained the lowest sub-quotient on this Subscale. It is important to emphasize that this observed difference did not reach the required level of significance, and consequently performance between the gender groups on this Subscale was essentially similar in this study. In contrast, Van Rooyen (2005) found that girls performed significantly better than boys on this Subscale. Similarly, Knoesen’s (2003) findings revealed a female advantage. On the original Griffiths Scales, Allan (1988) found no significant gender difference on the Practical Reasoning Subscale as the boys and girls in her sample showed the same results. However, Tukulu (1996) found that girls performed slightly better than boys on this Subscale, yet Ward (1997) found the opposite, in which boys were found to perform slightly better. Although non-significant, the results of the present study support the findings obtained by Ward (1997) that boys perform better on quantitative tasks as measured by the Practical Reasoning Subscale. As demonstrated by the various abovementioned findings on the Griffiths Scales, the literature on gender differences also provides contradictory evidence. For instance, while many researchers have found a male advantage when the mathematical concepts require more reasoning and are more spatial in nature (Geary, 1996; Halpern et al., 2007; Hyde et al., 1990), others have found that females have a clear advantage on quantitative tasks, but that this reverses sometime before puberty where males then maintain their superior performance into old age (Hyde et al., 1990; Neisser et al., 1996). However, Hyde et al. (1990) conducted a meta-analysis of gender differences with regard to performance in mathematics. This was based on 100 studies that provided effect sizes for 254 separate samples and represented the testing of over three million people. Hyde et al. (1990) concluded that there were no gender differences in understanding of mathematical concepts and in problem solving in the early childhood years. Therefore, as the results of the Hotellings T2 revealed no significant differences within this Subscale, the findings obtained on this Subscale add support for the gender similarities viewpoint. 6.6 Chapter overview This chapter provided the reader with the results of the present study. The aims of the study were revisited at the beginning of the chapter to contextualise the findings. Before the results of the descriptive statistics were provided in accordance with the first aim, results from independent sample t-tests were presented and they revealed that the matched-pairs procedure was implemented correctly. 135 To achieve the first aim, results of the descriptive analyses were presented on the mean developmental profile of the whole sample on the GMDS-ER, as well as a description of the sample’s performance across the six Subscales. The mean developmental profiles of the pre-school boys and girls per age group were then described separately. To achieve the second aim, the results of the inferential analyses were presented in which the sample’s performance was compared on the GMDS-ER. More specifically, the GQ of the whole sample was compared as well as the GQ of the sample per age group to determine whether any significant differences existed. The sample’s Subscale profiles were compared using a Hotellings T2. No significant difference was found after administering the independent sample t-test and the Hotellings T2. Whilst no statistically significant differences were found between the boys and girls within the sample, the researcher observed some interesting trends in the sample’s performance in comparison with other studies looking at the influence of gender on performance on the Griffiths Scales. It was found that 6-year-old boys obtained the highest mean GQ in the overall sample. In both age groups, the boys appeared to perform better than the girls on the Locomotor Subscale. The 5-year-old boys performed slightly better on the Language Subscale. Both the 6-year-old boys and girls obtained the highest mean performance across the sample on the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale. The 6-year-old boys obtained the highest mean performance on the Performance Subscale and only performed slightly better than the rest of the sample on the Practical Reasoning Subscale. The 5-year-old girls performed better than the rest of the sample on the Personal-Social Subscale. As discussed in chapter three, Griffiths’ (1954; 1970; 1984) theoretical view of child development is consistent with contemporary child development theory such as Demetriou et al.’s (2002) developmental model of information processing. Both theorists acknowledge the influence physiological and socio-cultural factors have on development, and that these factors are the cause of individual differences found in children’s intellectual abilities (Demetriou et al., 2002; Griffiths, 1954). In this study age, socio-economic status and culture were identified as factors that may have influenced the development of the boys and girls in this sample, and were thus controlled for during the matched-pairs procedure discussed in chapter five. In addition, Griffiths and Demetriou identified fairly similar domains of development and Demetriou found that development often progresses unevenly across the domains (Griffiths, 1954; 1970; 1984; Demetriou et al., 2002; Demetriou & Raftapoulos, 2004). Accordingly, the researcher noted variations in the test scores across gender and age group on the six Subscales of the GMDS-ER, with the exception of the Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale in the 6- 136 year-old age group. In conclusion, the utilization of the GMDS-ER enabled a comparison to be made of the gender groups in the sample from a developmental perspective, thus filling a void in gender-related research and setting it apart from previous studies that focused narrowly on gender differences within verbal, visual-spatial and quantitative abilities. The following chapter will integrate the findings of the present study by providing a summary of the trends observed in the sample’s performance on the GMDS-ER as well as allow the researcher to make concluding comments about the study. In addition, limitations and recommendations for future studies will also be provided. 137 CHAPTER SEVEN CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH 7.1 Introduction This chapter presents the conclusions, limitations and recommendations of this study. As the GMDS-ER was standardised for use in the United Kingdom, the current study not only added to the growing body of knowledge regarding the possible developmental differences between a sample of pre-school boys and girls aged between 5-years to 6-years-old, but also contributed towards an understanding of how normal South African pre-school children in this age group perform on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). As more and more South African studies are conducted, it will be possible to standardise and norm the GMDS-ER for this country. In light of this, the present study provided a valuable contribution, as its findings along with many others will be added to the GMDS-ER database, which will be used to generate norms for the South African context. 7.2 Conclusions The overall purpose of this study was to generate comparative information regarding the general development of 5- and 6-year-old South African pre-school boys and girls. In order to achieve this, the study narrowed its focus to the city of Port Elizabeth and aimed to explore and compare the developmental profiles of a sample of normal pre-school boys and girls between the abovementioned age groups utilizing the GMDS-ER. Overall, the total sample included 64 children, consisting of 32 pairs of boys and girls. Results of the data analysis revealed that there were no significant differences between the performance of the boys and girls within the two age groups. However, certain trends did emerge from a qualitative review of the mean scores. It was noted that the 6-year-old boys performed slightly better across the Subscales, and specifically on four of the six Subscales, namely the Locomotor, Eye and Hand Co-ordination, Performance and Practical Reasoning Subscales. The 5-year-old boys obtained the highest sub-quotient on the Language Subscale, and the 5-year-old girls obtained the highest sub-quotient on the Personal-Social Subscale. As no significant differences were found, it can be concluded that the developmental profiles of this particular sample of pre-school boys and girls was essentially similar. Furthermore, the fact that no gender differences were found indicates that the revision process of the GMDS-ER was sensitive to not building in items that are gender biased (Foxcroft, 138 personal communication, December 3, 2008). Many intelligence measures have ensured that test items that have shown gender bias have been removed in the test construction process. The GMDS-ER has also been careful in this regard, thereby ranking it favourably amongst psychometric measures. In chapter two, it was documented that there are two opposing perspectives with regards to gender differences in development. As reflected, literature on the topic is fraught with contradictory empirical evidence, which is understandable in the sense that the many studies conducted differed with regards to methodologies employed and sample sizes used. Ideally, had the studies been replicated as closely as possible to the earliest studies from the 1960s, the differing findings may have proved easier to follow in a literature review on this topic. In this regard, meta-analyses have enabled valuable deductions to be made in this area due to their ability to condense the findings from many studies (Blakemore et al., 2009). However, it is disappointing that very few studies have assessed whether gender differences exist from a holistic developmental perspective. Historically, the majority of the studies shown in the literature review focused on the psychometric conception of intelligence at the time, which was shaped by verbal, quantitative and visual-spatial ability (Deaux, 1984; Fennema & Sherman, 1977; Geary, 1989; 1994; 1996; Halpern, 1986; 1989; 1997; Halpern et al., 2007; Linn & Peterson, 1985; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Naglieri & Rojahn, 2001; Voyer, Voyer & Bryden, 1995). Information generated from this study has made a valuable contribution to the field of child and gender development research in South Africa. It is one of the first studies conducted in this country that has comprehensively investigated whether boys and girls differ in their performance on the recently released GMDS-ER. When it comes to developmental measures, the Griffiths Scales is well-established psychometrically in its holistic view of child development and is useful in detecting developmental difficulties in early childhood. The recently released GMDS-ER is the latest offering of the Scales and it continues the rich tradition of being a valuable diagnostic aid to practitioners with its new normative and technical properties. Thus, the GMDS-ER was considered to be the most appropriate developmental measure to guide the researcher with the comparison of the performance of the boys and girls within the present study. The limitations that were encountered in the present study are acknowledged in the following section. It is hoped that future researchers will peruse the following section to enable them to make the necessary improvements, and thereby further add to the growing body of knowledge on the topic. 139 7.3 Limitations Limitations are inherent in all research studies. The limitations found in this study will be discussed in this section, as it serves to benefit future research conducted on child development using the GMDS-ER. 7.3.1 Limitations of the research method The research method employed in the present study was exploratory-descriptive. When using exploratory research, it is not possible to predict or explain behaviour via the manipulation and measurement of different variables (Knoesen, 2003). As a result, conclusions regarding causal relationships cannot be made. It needs to be pointed out that it was never the intention of the study to make cause-and-effect conclusions regarding the developmental progression of boys and girls, but merely to examine and describe the developmental profiles of the gender groups, to determine whether any differences exist. Therefore, in view of this purpose of the study, this limitation was contained. 7.3.2 Limitations of the sampling procedure The researcher through the use of non-probability, purposive and convenience sampling chose the participants for this study. In non-probability sampling, the probability of any particular member of the population being chosen is unknown, and the selection of the sample is arbitrary as the researcher plays an active role in the selection of the sample to best suit the aims of the study (Struwig & Stead, 2001). As a result, the researcher’s ability to generalise the findings of the study beyond the sample to the larger population is severely limited. However, due to the fact that the research method was exploratory-descriptive, it was not the intention of the present study to generalise the findings to the general population of South African boys and girls within the 5- and 6-year-old age group. In the present study, it was essential that the extraneous variables of socio-economic status and cultural group were controlled for in order to obtain a meaningful interpretation of the test results. As a result, it is reasonable to expect that the findings of this study were mostly due to the non-manipulated independent variable of gender and not due to the influence of uncontrolled extraneous variables. Therefore, the internal validity of this study has been maintained. Another limitation within this study was that the majority of the sample consisted of White children, with a smaller number of children from the Coloured and Asian cultural groups due to the English language inclusion criterion. As the GMDS-ER has not been 140 standardised into other languages as yet, this could not have been prevented. However, it is interesting to note that in a previous research study conducted by Allan (1992) on the original Griffiths Scales, no significant differences were found between the cultural groups with regards to their overall performance on the General Quotient, Personal-Social and Practical Reasoning Subscale. On the remaining four subscales, no significant differences were found between the performances of the Black and Coloured children, as with White and Asian children. White children were found to perform significantly better on the Hearing and Speech Subscale (currently termed ‘Language Subscale’ on the GMDS-ER). When the GMDS-ER becomes normed and standardised for the South African population, it is hoped that it will also be translated into other languages, making it accessible to all South African children. Further studies conducted on the performance of children from these different cultural groups will also contribute towards the growing knowledge base of child development in South Africa. 7.3.3 Limitations regarding various test administrators utilized in the study Ideally, one test administrator should assess all participants and score all test protocols to obtain a standardised procedure. However, due to the time consuming process it entails, this was not feasible for the present study. Therefore, various test administrators were used to assess the participants. According to Stewart (2005), “this increases the chances of inter-tester variance in the administration and scoring of items” (p.416). This variance was contained to a certain extent by having the same moderator evaluate all test protocols. In addition, the supervisors of the study scrutinised the scoring and interpretations of the test protocols so that accurate information could be reported on. Furthermore, the test administrators were all trained by the same registered Griffiths tutor. 7.3.4 Limitations regarding the lack of norms for the South African population on the GMDS-ER As the GMDS-ER has not been standardised for use in South Africa, the norms developed in the United Kingdom were not used in this study. Instead, the scores obtained were used in a criterion-referenced way by converting them to quotients based on the child’s chronological age. As mentioned in chapter five, this scoring system was used for the original Griffiths Scales, and appeared to be the most appropriate method to use in the GMDS-ER assessment of South African children. The lack of South African norms serves as a limitation within this study because it was not possible to ascertain the samples’ performance relative to 141 their South African peers. This would have been an added advantage when analysing the findings of this study. However, this limitation is being addressed as there is an ongoing research project currently underway involving the testing of clinical as well as normal samples of children on the GMDS-ER. It is hoped that the findings from studies such as these will contribute towards the norming and standardisation of the GMDS-ER in South Africa. 7.4 Recommendations for future research With the abovementioned limitations in mind, the following recommendations for future research were made: 1. The present study should be replicated with a larger, more representative sample within the Nelson Mandela Metropole. A large-scale comparison would generate valuable information on child development, such as the development of normative typologies per age group contributing towards the creation of South African norms on the GMDS-ER. An additional advantage to having a larger sample is that it would thoroughly investigate whether gender bias has been eliminated from the construction of the GMDS-ER. 2. Due to the limited representation of children from the Black and Asian cultural groups in the present study, further investigation comparing the genders within these cultural groups should be undertaken. In this regard, future studies should focus on translating the items of the GMDS-ER into other languages to enable the abovementioned recommended studies to be conducted in a fair and ethical manner. Also, test administrators should be fluent in the mother tongue of the child. 3. There is a need for the adjustment of Riordan’s (1978) socio-economic classification system, as it is fairly dated. The aspect of this system that is the most dated is the classification categories for the different cultural groups. A socio-economic classification system for a contemporary South African population ought to incorporate factors such as household type, household composition, education level, work status and access to water, to enable a holistic conceptualisation of socioeconomic status (Potgieter et al., 1999). Thus, the concept of socio-economic status in the South African context needs to be reviewed as several classification systems are inclined to adopt the Westernised perspective, which focuses mostly on level of education and income. In a third world country, environmental and contextual factors 142 influence a family’s socio-economic standing and thus needs to be considered when conceptualising socio-economic status. 4. Finally, it is highly recommended that continued research using the GMDS-ER in South Africa should be conducted. This has become a crucial necessity in the South African context as it plays a fundamental role in the early identification of problems, and therefore a set of norms applicable to our context is required. Continued research will contribute to the generation of South African norms for the GMDS-ER so that this assessment measure can be classified by the Psychometrics Committee of the Professional Board for Psychology of the Health Professions Council of South Africa (HPCSA) as a diagnostic developmental measure for use in this country. 7.5 Chapter overview This chapter provided the reader with the conclusions of the study; as well as an understanding of the limitations encountered in order to facilitate improvements for future research, and also provided recommendations for those wishing to further their interest in this topic. To recap, it was concluded that no significant differences were found between the overall performance of the boys and girls in this study. Therefore, the conclusions support the gender similarities viewpoint. This study not only contributed immensely to the growing body of knowledge regarding the performance of the gender groups on the GMDS-ER, but also towards an understanding of how normal South African pre-school children in this age group perform on the GMDS-ER. Childhood is a fascinating world of discovery, which children actively explore by formulating ideas and testing hypotheses. In a sense perhaps their natural curiosity should be respected as something akin to mini research. In this exploration children think and behave in ways that are both amusing as well as thought provoking (Craig, 1996). The following quotation by Hymes Jr. (2006) perfectly elucidates the value of recognising the importance of play in child development. Play for young children is not recreation activity...It is not leisure-time activity nor escape activity.... Play is thinking time for young children. It is language time. Problem-solving time. It is memory time, planning time, investigating time. It is organization-of-ideas time, when the young child uses his mind and body and his social skills and all his powers in response to the stimuli he has met (Retrieved online December 10, 2008, http://www.enotes.com/famous-quotes/play-for-young-childrenis-not-recreation-activity). 143 The GMDS-ER holds the key that unlocks the door to the young child’s world, and endears itself to children due to its playful character. One of the values of the GMDS-ER is that it is based on the concept of play, which is a universal behaviour across all cultures. It is this researcher’s opinion that the GMDS-ER can be considered to be an essential developmental assessment measure for the multi-cultural South African context. 144 REFERENCE LIST Adler, A. (1930). Individual Psychology. In C. Murchinson (Ed.), Psychologies of 1930 (pp. 395-408). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Aiken, L.R. (1997). Psychological testing and assessment (9th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Aldridge-Smith, J., Bidder, R.T., Gardner, S.M., & Gray, O.P. (1980). Griffiths Scales of Mental Development and Different users. Child: Care, Health and Development, 6, 11-16. Allan, M.M. (1988). A comparison of the performance of normal preschool South African and British children on the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Allan, M. M (1992). The performance of South African normal preschool children on the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A comparative study. Unpublished doctoral theses, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Allan, M.M., Luiz, D.M., & Foxcroft, C.D. (1992). A comparison of the performance of normal preschool South African and British children on the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Paper presented at the National Congress of the Psychological Association of South Africa, Bloemfontein. American Educational Research Association (AERA) (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, D.C. Anastasi, A. (1961). Psychological Tests: Uses and abuses. Teachers College Record, 62, 389-393. Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. Anastasi, A. (1987). Psychological testing (6th ed.). New York: Macmillan. Arceneaux, J.M., Cheramie, G.M., & Smith, C.W. (1996). Gender differences in WAIS-R agecorrected scaled scores. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 1211-1215. Assouline, & D.L. Ambroson (Eds.), Talent development, Vol. 1: Proceedings from the 1991 Henry B. and Jocelyn Wallace National Research Symposium on Talent Development. Unionville, NY: Trillium Press. Aylward, G. P. (1994). Practitioner's Guide to developmental and psychological testing. New York: Plenum Medical. Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Working memory or working attention? In A.D. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention, selection, awareness and control. A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 152-170). Oxford: Clarendon Press. 145 Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. VIII: Recent advances in research and theory (pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press. Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (2000). Development of working memory: Should the Pascual-Leone and the Baddeley and Hitch models be merged? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 128-137. Baker, S. (2005). The performance of children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Barnard, A. (2000). The Griffiths’ Practical Reasoning Scale: A revision. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Barnard, A. (2004). Data analysis techniques. Unpublished class notes. University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Barnard, A., & Seymour, B. (2003). SP316 and SP415 Workbook. Unpublished class notes. University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Bayley, N. (1936). The California Infant Scale of Motor Development. Berkeley: University of California Press. Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development Manual (2nd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation. Beail, N. (1985). A comparative study of profoundly multiply handicapped children’s scores on the Bayley and Griffiths Developmental Scales. Child: Care, Health and Development, 11, 31-36. Beck, L.E. (1994). Child Development (3rd ed.). Massachusetts: Allyn & Bacon. Bender, C.J.G. (1990). ‘n Longitudinale ondersoek na skoolvordering by Blanke, Kleurling- and Asierkinders in die primere skoolfase. Paper presented at the eight national Congress of the Psychological Association of South Africa, Port Elizabeth. Berglund, E., Eriksson, M., & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, child care and socio-economic status in 18-month-old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 485-491. Berry, J.W. (1966). Temne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 207229. Betancourt, H., & Lop´ez, S. R. (1993). The study of culture, ethnicity, and race in American psychology. American Psychologist, 48, 629-637. 146 Bhamjee, R.A. (1991). A comparison of the performances of normal British and South African Indian children on the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished doctoral thesis. University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Bidell, T.R., & Fischer, K.W. (1992). Beyond the stage debate: action, structure and variability in Piagetian theory and research. In R.J. Sternberg & C.A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual Development (pp. 100-140). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Bieri, J., Bradburn, W., & Galinsky, M. (1958). Sex differences in perceptual behaviour. Journal of Personality, 26, 1-12. Bijou, S.W. (1978). In E.B. Hurlock, Child Development (6th ed., p. 26). Tokyo: McGraw-Hill. Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). The development of the intelligence in children, L'Annee Psychologique, 11, 163-244 Bjorklund, D.F. (1995). Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differences (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Blakemore, J.E.O., Berenbaum, S.A., & Liben, L.S. (2009). Gender development. New York: Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. Blatchford, P., Baines, E., & Pellegrini, A.D. (2003). The social context of school playground games: Sex and ethnic differences, and changes over time after entry to junior school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 481-505. Bless, C., & Higson-Smith, C. (1995). Fundamentals of social research methods: An African perspective (2nd ed.). Cape Town: Juta. Blurton Jones, N. (1972). Categories of child-child interaction. In N.B. Jones (Ed.), Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour (pp. 97-127). London: Cambridge University Press. Born, M.P., Bleichrodt, N., & Van der Flier, H. (1987). Cross-cultural comparison of sex-related differences on intelligence tests: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18, 283-314. Bornstein, M.H., Hahn, C.S., & Haynes, O.M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24, 267-304. Braine, M.D.S. (1990). The ‘natural logic’ approach to reasoning. In W.F. Overton (Ed.), Reasoning, necessity, and logic: Developmental perspectives (pp. 133-157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brainerd, C.J., & Vanden Heuvel, K. (1974). Development of geometric imagery in five-to eight-yearolds. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 98, 89-143. Brandt, I. (1983). Griffiths entwicklungsskalen (GES) zur beurteilung der entwicklung in den ersten beiden ledensjahren. Wienheim und Basel: Beltz Verlag. 147 Brandt, I. (1984). Fruherkenning geistiger behinderung im sauglingsalterergebinisse einer lansschittestudoes. In J.W. Dudenhausen & E. Saling (Eds.), Perinatale Medizin, 11. Deutscher kongrefs fr Perinatale Medizin. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag. Bronson, W.C. (1966). Central orientations: A study of behaviour organisation from childhood to adolescence. Child Development, 37, 125-155. Brooks, J.B., & Elliot, D.M. (1971). Prediction of psychological development at age thirty from leisure time activities and satisfactions in childhood. Human Development, 14, 51-61. Brooks, J.B., & Weinraub, M. (1976). A history of infant intelligence testing. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Origins of Intelligence: Infancy and early childhood (pp. 19-58). New York: Plenum Press. Brooks-Gunn, J. (1990). Identifying the vulnerable young children. In D.E. Rogers & E. Ginsberg (Eds.), Improving the life chances of children at risk (pp. 104-124). Boulder, CO: Westview Press Buffery, A.H.W., & Gray, J.A. (1972). Sex differences in the development of spatial and linguistic skills. In C. Ounsted & D.C. Taylor (Eds.), Gender differences: Their ontogeny and significance. London: Churchill Livingstone. Butler, S.R., Marsh, H.W., Sheppard, M.J., & Sheppard, J.L. (1985). Seven-year longitudinal study of the early prediction of reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 349-361. Camarata, S., & Woodcock, R.W. (2006). Sex differences in processing speed: Developmental effects in males and females. Intelligence, 34, 231-252. Caplan, P.J., Crawford, M., Hyde, J.S. & Richardson, J.T.E. (Eds.). (1997). Gender differences in human cognition. New York: Oxford University Press. Caplan, D., & Waters, G.S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Brain and Behavioural Science, 22, 77-126. Cattell, R.B. (1966). The screen test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 1, 245-276. Case, R. (1985). Intellectual Development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press. Casey, M.B. Nutall, R.L., & Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores: A comparison of spatial skills with internalised beliefs and anxieties. Developmental Psychology, 33, 669-680. Casey, M.B. Nutall, R.L., & Pezaris, E., & Benbow, C.P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31, 697-705. Clark, L.A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309-319. 148 Coates, S. (1974). Sex differences in field independence among preschool children. In R.C. Friedman, R.M. Richart, & R.L. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behaviour (pp. 259-274). New York: Wiley. Cobos, F., Rodrigues, C., & De Venegas, A.V. (1971). Practical use of the Griffiths Scales in older children. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 5 (3-4), 163-166. Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power for the behavioural sciences. New York: Academic Press. Cohen, D. (1977). Sex differences in spatial performance in the elderly: A review of the literature and suggestions for research. Educational Gerontology: An International Quarterly, 2, 59-69. Collins, J.K., Jupp, J.J., Maberly, G.F., Morris, J.G., & Eastman, C.J. (1987). An exploratory study of the intellectual functioning of neurological and myxoedematous cretins in China. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Development Disabilities, 19, 13-20. Coie, J.D., & Dodge, K.A. (1998). Aggression and anti-social behaviour. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, emotional and personality development, 3(5th ed., pp. 779-862). New York: Wiley. Coolican, H. (2004). Research methods and statistics in psychology (4th ed.). London: Hodder Arnold. Cook, A.S., Fritz, J.J., McCornack, B.L., & Visperas, C. (1985). Early gender differences in the functional usage of language. Sex Roles, 12, (9/10), 909-915. Cozby, P.C. (1989). Methods in behavioural research (4th ed.). California: Mayfield. Cozby, P.C. (1993). Methods in behavioural research (5th ed.). California: Mayfield. Craig, G.J. (1996). Human Development (7th ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Crandall, V.J., & Sinkeldam, C. (1964). Children’s dependent and achievement behaviours in social situations and their perceptual field dependence. Journal of Personality, 32, 1-22. Crick, N.R., & Rose, A.J. (2000). Toward a gender-balanced approach to the study of social-emotional development: A look at relational aggression. In P.H. Miller & E. Kofsky Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 153-168). Florence, KY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge. Dane, F.C. (1990). Research methods. California: Brooks/Cole. Davidson, G. (2008). First and second born twins: A comparative study utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. De Vos, A.S., Fouche, C.B., Strydom, H., & Delport, C.S.L. (Eds.). (2002). Research at grass roots: For the social sciences and human services professions (2nd ed.). Pretoria: Van Schaik. Deaux, K. (1984). From individual differences to social categories: Analysis of a decade’s research on gender. American Psychologist, 39, 105-116. 149 Demetriou, A. (1993). On the quest of the functional architecture of developing mind. Educational Psychology Review, 5, 1-18. Demetriou, A. (1998). Cognitive development. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, & K.F.M. van Leishout (Eds.), Lifespan developmental psychology (pp. 179-269). London: Wiley. Demetriou, A. (2000). Organisation and development of self-understanding and self-regulation. In M. Boekaerts, P.R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self regulation (pp. 209-251). New York: Academic Press. Demetriou, A. (2004). Mind, intelligence and development: A cognitive, differential and developmental theory of intelligence. In A. Demetriou, & A. Raftapoulos (Eds.), Cognitive developmental change: Theories, models and measurement (pp. 21-73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., & Platsidou, M. (2002). The development of mental processing: Efficiency, working memory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(1, Serial No. 268). Demetriou, A., & Efklides, A. (1985). Structure and sequence of formal and postformal thought: General patterns and individual differences. Child Development, 56, 1062-1091. Demetriou, A., & Efklides, A. (1987). Towards a determination of the dimensions and domains of individual differences in cognitive development. In E. De Corte, H. Lodewijks, R. Parmentier, & P. Span (Eds.), Learning and instruction: European research in an international context, Vol.1 (pp. 41-52). Oxford: Leuven University Press and Pergamon Press. Demetriou, A., Efklides, A., & Platsidou, M. (1993). The architecture and dynamics of developing mind: Experiental structuralism as a frame for unifying cognitive developmental theories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(5-6, Serial No. 234). Demetriou, A., & Kazi, S. (2001). Unity and modularity in the mind and the self: Studies on the relationships between self-awareness, personality, and intellectual development from childhood to adolescence. London: Routledge. Demetriou, A., Kazi, S., & Georgiou, S. (1999). The emerging self: The convergence of mind, self, and thinking styles. Developmental Science, 2(4), 387-409. Demetriou, A., & Raftapoulos, A. (1999). Modelling the developing mind: From structure to change. Developmental Review, 19, 319-368. Demetriou, A., & Raftapoulos, A. (Eds.). (2004). Cognitive developmental change: Theories, models and measurement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Demetriou, A., Raftapoulos, A., & Kargopoulos, P. (1999). Interactions, computations and experience: Interleaved springboards of cognitive emergence. Developmental Review, 19, 389-414. 150 Demetriou, A., & Valanides, N. (1998). A three level theory of the developing mind: Basic principles and implications for instruction and assessment. In R.J. Sternberg & W.M. Williams (Eds.), Intelligence, instruction, and assessment (pp. 149-199). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Diamond, M. & Sigmundson, H.K. (1997). Sex reassignment at birth: A long term review and clinical implications. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 298-304. Dickman, B.J. (1994). An evaluation of the Grover-Counter Test for use in the assessment of black South African township children with mental handicaps. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cape Town. Ding, S., & Littleton, K. (2005). Children’s personal and social development. Milton Keynes: The Open University. Dorfman, P.W. (1977). Timing and anticipation: A developmental perspective. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 9, 67-79. Drew, C.J., Hardman, M.L., & Hart, A.W. (1996). Designing and conducting research: Inquiry in education and social science (2nd ed.). Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon. Eagly, A.H. (1990). On the advantages of reporting sex differences. American Psychologist, 45, 560562. Eagly, A.H. (1994). On comparing women and men [Special Issue]. Feminism and Psychology, 4, 523530. Eagly, A.H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50, 145-158. Edelman, S. (1996). A review of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Third Edition (WISCIII). Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 28(4), 219-225. Edwards, O.L. (1975). Cohort and sex changes in black educational achievement. Sociology and Social Research, 59(2), 110-120. Efklides, A., Demetriou, A., & Metallidou, A. (1994). The structure and development of prepositional reasoning ability. In A. Demetriou & A. Efklides (Eds.), Mind, intelligence, and reasoning: Structure and development (pp. 151-172). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Engle, R.W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19-23. Epstein, C.F. (1988). Deceptive distinctions: Sex, gender, and the social order. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Etaugh, C., & Turton, W.J. (1977). Sex differences in discrimination of forms by elementary school children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 130, 49-55. 151 Ethics in Health Research in South Africa (2000). Statement on ethical principles for health research. Draft Document. Farnham-Diggory, S. (1970). Cognitive synthesis in Negro and White Children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35(2, Serial No. 135). Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43, 95103. Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1977). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement, spatial visualisation, and affective factors. American Educational Research Journal, 4, 51-71. Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S, Bates, E., Thal, D.J., & Pethick, S.J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Serial No. 242). Fischer, K.W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87(6), 477-531. Fischer, K.W., & Bidell, T.R. (1998). Dynamic development of psychological structures in action and thought. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R.M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol.1, Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 467-561). New York: Wiley. Flanagan, D. P., & Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials of WISC-IV assessment. New York: Wiley. Flynn, J.R. (1998). Israeli military IQ tests: Gender differences small: IQ gains large. Journal of Biosocial Science, 30, 541-553. Foxcroft, C.D. (1985). The use of the Reitan-Indiana Neuropsychological Test Battery in South Africa: A cross-ethnic comparison of normal pre-school children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Port Elizabeth. Foxcroft, C.D., & Roodt, G. (Eds.). (2001). An introduction to psychological assessment in the South African context. Cape Town: Oxford University Press. Foxcroft, C.D., & Roodt, G. (Eds.). (2006). An introduction to psychological assessment in the South African context (2nd ed.). Cape Town: Oxford University Press. Foxcroft, C.D., Shillington, S.J., & Turk, M. (1990, October). The development of a “culture-fair” Group Reasoning Test: the play is staged. Paper presented at the 8th National Congress of Psychological Association of South Africa, Port Elizabeth. Fraiberg, S.H. (1959). The magic years. New York: Scribner’s. Freud, S. (1896). The aetiology of hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud (pp.191-221). London: Hogarth Press. Friedman, L. (1995). The space factor in mathematics: Gender differences. Review of Educational Research, 65, 22-50. 152 Gallagher, A.M., & De Lisi, R. (1994). Gender differences in Scholastic Aptitude Test: Mathematics problem solving among high-ability students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 204-211. Gallagher, A.M., De Lisi, R., Holst, P.C., McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A.V., Morely, M., & Cahalan, C. (2000). Gender differences in advanced mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 165-190. Galsworthy, M.J., Dionne, G., Dale, P.S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3, 206-215. Garcia Coll, C., & Magnuson, K. (2000). Cultural differences as sources of developmental vulnerabilities and resources. In J.P. Shonkoff & S.J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention (2nd ed., pp. 94-114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Geary, D.C. (1989). A model for representing gender differences in the pattern of cognitive abilities. American Psychologist, 44, 1155-1156. Geary, D.C. (1994). Children’s mathematical development: Research and practical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Geary, D.C. (1996). Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematical abilities. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 19, 229-284. Geary, D.C., Saults, S.J., Liu, F., & Hoard, M.K. (2000). Sex differences in spatial cognition, computational fluency, and arithmetic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 337-353. Gleitman, H. (1981). Psychology. New York: Norton. Golombok, S., & Fivush, R. (1994). Gender development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gonzalez, V. (2001). The role of socioeconomic and sociocultural factors in language minority children's development: An ecological research view. Bilingual Research Journal, 25, 1-30. Gowar, R.V. (2003). The performance of children with autism on the Revised Extended Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth. Gravetter, F.J., & Wallnau, L.B. (2002). Essentials of statistics for the behavioural sciences (4th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Wadsworth. Grieve, K.W. (2001). Factors affecting assessment results. In C.D. Foxcroft & G. Roodt (Eds.), An introduction to psychological assessment in the South African context (pp. 315-343). Cape Town: Oxford University Press. Greenspan, S. I., & Meisels, S. J. (1996). Toward a new vision for the developmental assessment of infants and young children. In S. J. Meisels & E. Fenichel (Eds.), New visions for the 153 developmental assessment of infants and young children (pp. 11-26). Washington, DC: Zero to Three: National Center for Infants, Toddlers, and Families. Griffiths, R. (1954). The abilities of babies. London: University of London Press. Griffiths, R. (1970). The abilities of young children. London: Child Development Research Centre. Griffiths, R. (1984). The abilities of young children. Amersham: ARICD. Groth-Marant, G. (1984). Handbook of psychological assessment. USA: Van Nostand Reinhold Co. Gustafsson, J.E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence, 8, 179203. Hakstian, A.R., & Cattell, R.B. (1975). An examination of adolescent sex differences in some ability and personality traits. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 7, 295-312. Halpern, D.F. (1986). A different answer to the question: “Do sex-related differences in spatial abilities exist?” American Psychologist, 41, 1014-1015. Halpern, D.F. (1989). The disappearance of cognitive gender differences: What you see depends on where you look. American Psychologist, 44, 1156-1158. Halpern, D.F. (1993). Sex, brains and hands: Gender differences in cognitive abilities. Skeptic, 2(3), 96-103. Halpern, D.F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence: Implications for education. American Psychologist, 52, 1091-1102. Halpern, D.F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Halpern, D.F. (2001). In A. Naglieri & J. Rojahn, Gender differences in Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive (PASS) Cognitive Processes and Achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 430-437. Halpern, D.F., & LaMay, M.L. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 229-246. Halpern, D.F., Benbow, C.P., Geary, D.C., Gur, R.C., Hyde, J.S., & Gernsbacher, M.A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 1-51. Hanson, R. (1982). Item reliability for the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 8, 151-161. Hanson, R. (1983). Proposals for the revision of the Griffiths Scales based on studies carried out at the University of Leeds during the years 1980 to 1982. Unpublished manuscript. Hanson, R., & Aldridge-Smith, J. (1982). Current applications of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales. Association of Educational Psychologists’ Journal, 5, 57-59. 154 Hanson, R., & Aldridge-Smith, J. (1987). Achievements of young children on items of the Griffiths Scales: 1980 compared with 1960. Child: Care, Health and Development, 13, 181-195. Hanson, R., Aldridge-Smith, J., & Humes, W. (1984). Some reasons for disagreement among scorers of infant intelligence test items. Child: Care, Health and Development, 10, 17-30. Hanson, R., Aldridge-Smith, J., & Humes, W. (1985). Achievements of infants on items of the Griffiths Scales: 1980 compared with 1950. Child: Care, Health and Development, 11, 91-104. Hare-Mustin, R.T., & Marecek, J. (1988). The meaning of difference: Gender theory, postmodernism, and psychology. American Psychologist, 43, 455-464. Harris, D.B. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity: A revision and extension of the Goodenough Draw-a-Man test. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World. Hedges, L.V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41-45. Heimes, L. (1983). The comparison of the JSAIS and the Griffiths Developmental Scale scores of 3-5 year old boys and girls. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Hoar, R.M.N. (1983). The assessment of aptitude in black school beginners. Humanitas – RSA, 9, 175294. Hollis, A.L. (1995). Predicting school readiness using cognitive screening, temperament, and school environment. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: The Humanities and Social Sciences, 56. (1-A), 0143. Honzik, M.P., McFarlane, J.W., & Allen, L. (1966). The stability of mental test performance between two and eighteen years. In C.I. Chase & H.G. Ludlow (Eds.), Readings in educational and psychological measurement. Boston: Houghton Miffin. Horacek, H.J., Ramey, C.T., Campbell, F.A., Hoffman, K.P., Fletcher, R.H. (1987). Predicting school failure and assessing early intervention with high-risk children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 758-763. Horsmann, S.M. (2007). The effects of quality of education on WISC-IV Coding B-Incidental Learning performance for a sample of Grade 7, Xhosa and English language children, aged 12-13 years. Unpublished master’s dissertation, Rhodes University, Grahamstown. Houston-McMillan, J.E. (1988). Borderline mentally handicapped preschoolers: Identification and treatment evaluation using the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. In D.M. Luiz (Ed.), Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research paper No. C25, pp. 5258). University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Hoyenga, K.B., & Hoyenga, K.T. (1979). The Question of Sex Differences: Psychological, 155 Cultural and Biological Issues. Canada: Little, Brown and Company. Huntley, M. (1996). The Griffiths Mental Development Scales. From birth to 2 years. Manual (Revision). United Kingdom: ARICD. Hurlock, E.B. (1978). Child Development: International Student Edition (6th ed.). Tokyo: McGrawHill. Hurlock, E.B. (1981). Child Development (6th ed.). Tokyo: McGraw-Hill. Hutt, C. (1972). Neuroendocrinological behavioural and intellectual aspects of sexual differentiation in human development. In C. Ounsted & D.C. Taylor (Eds.), Gender differences. London: Churchill Livingstone. Huttenlocher, J., Haight,W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236-248. Hyde, J.S. (1981). How large are cognitive gender differences? A meta-analysis using ω2 and d. American Psychologist, 36, 892-901. Hyde, J.S. (1985). Half the human experience: The psychology of women (3rd ed.). Lexington, MA: Heath. Hyde, J.S. (1986). Introduction: Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender. In J.S. Hyde & M.C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 1-13). Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press. Hyde, J.S. (1994). Can meta-analysis make feminist transformations in psychology? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 451-462. Hyde, J.S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581-592. Hyde, J.S., & Linn, M.C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53-69. Hyde, J.S., & McKinley, N.M. (1997). Gender differences in cognition: Results from meta-analyses. In P.J. Caplan, M. Crawford, J.S. Hyde & J.T.E. Richardson (Eds.). Gender differences in human cognition (pp. 30-51). New York: Oxford University Press. Hyde, J.S., & Plant, E.A. (1995). Magnitude of psychological gender differences: Another side to the story. American Psychologist, 50, 159-161. Hyde, J.S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S.J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139-155. Hymes Jr., J.L. (2006). “Play for young children is not…” In R. Andrews, M. Biggs, & M. Seidel (Eds.), The Columbia World of Quotations. Retrieved December 10, 2008, from http://www.enotes.com/famous-quotes/play-for-young-children-is-not-recreation-activity Jackson, W. (1995). Methods: Doing Social Research. Canada: Prentice-Hall 156 Jansen, J.M. (1991). Stress and coping in families with physically handicapped children. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth. Jensen, A. R. (1979). g: Outmoded theory or unconquered frontier? Creative Science and Technology, 2, 16-29. Jensen, A.R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York: The Free Press. Johnson, B.T., & Eagly, A.H. (2000). Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research. In H.T. Reis & C.M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 496-528). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kagan, J.S. (1981). The second year: The emergence of self awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kail, R.V., & Cavanaugh, J.C. (2000). Human development: A life-span view (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Karapetsas, A.B., & Vlachos, F.M. (1997). Sex and handedness in development of visuomotor skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 131-140. Kargopoulos, P., & Demetriou, A. (1998). What, why, and whence logic? A response to the commentators. New ideas in psychology, 16, 125-139. Kaufman, A.S., & Lichtenberger, E.O. (1999). Essentials of the WAIS-III. New York: Wiley & Sons. Kemps, E., De Rammelaere, S., & Desmet, T. (2000). The development of working memory: Exploring the complementarily of two models. Journals of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 89-109. Keogh, B.K. (1971). Pattern copying under three conditions of an expanded spatial field. Developmental Psychology, 4, 25-31. Kimball, M.M. (1995). Feminist visions of gender similarities and differences. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press. Kimball, M.M. (2000). Gender similarities and differences as feminist contradictions. In R.K. Unger (Ed.), Handbook of psychology of women and gender (pp. 66-83). New York: Wiley. Kline, P. (1993). A handbook of test construction. New York: Methuen. Knight, G.P., Fabes, R.A., & Higgins, D.A. (1996). Concerns about drawing causal inferences from meta-analyses: An example in the study of gender differences in aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 410-421. Knoesen, N. (2003). An exploration of the relationship between the Revised Griffiths Scales and grade one scholastic development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. 157 Knoesen, N. (2005). Exploring the reliability and construct-related validity of the Locomotor Subscale of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). Unpublished doctoral thesis, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive developmental analysis of children’s sex role concepts and attitudes. In E.E. Macoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82-172). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Kosslyn, S.M. (1983). Ghosts in the mind’s machine. New York: Norton. Kotras, N. (1998). A revision of a section of the Hearing and Speech scales of the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Kotras, H. (2001). Exploring a developmental profile of HIV-infected infants in the Eastern Province of South Africa, utilizing the Revised Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished Master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Kotras, N. (2003). Exploring the construct validity of the Revised Extended Griffiths Language Subscale in the British Isles. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Kraynak, A.R., & Raskin, L.M. (1971). The influence of age and stimulus dimensionality on form perception by preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 4, 389-393. Krige, P. (1988). A physically handicapped child: A longitudinal study. In D.M. Luiz (Ed.), Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research paper No. C25, pp. 38-43). University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth. Kroukamp, T.R. (1991). The use of personality-related and cognitive factors as predictors of scholastic achievement on grade one: A multivariate approach. University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Kruger, M.J.L. (1983). ‘n Ondersoek na die verband tussen kronologiese ouderdom en die graad van skoolgereedheid. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South Africa. Landman, J. (1978). Die voorspelling van waarskynlike skoolprestasie met behulp van die aanlegtoetsbattery (ATB) vir swart leerlinge. Unpublished masters treatise, University of Pretoria, South Africa. Largo, R.H., Caflisch, J.A., Hug, F., Muggli, K., Molnar, A.A., & Molinari, L. (2001a). Neuromotor development from 5 to 18 years. Part 2: Association movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 444-453. 158 Largo, R.H., Caflisch, J.A., Hug, F., Muggli, K., Molnar, A.A., & Molinari, L., et al. (2001b). Neuromotor development from 5 to 18 years. Part 1: Timed performance. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 436-443. Laroche, J.L., Brabant, G., & Brabant, J. (1976). Le temps de fixation comme indice de differenciation entre mere etranger chez le jeune enfant. Journal de Psychologie Appliquee. Montreal. Laroche, J.L., Gutz, H., & Desbiolles, M. (1974). Differenciation entre mere etranger chez jeune enfant. Communication au XVIIe Congres International de Psychologie Appliquee. Montreal. Lefrançois, G.R. (1990). The lifespan (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Levine, S.C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 940-949. Liebert, R.M., Poulos, R.W., & Strauss, G.D. (1978). In E.B. Hurlock, Child Development: International Student Edition (6th ed.). Tokyo: McGraw-Hill. Linn. M.C., & Petersen, A.C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479-1498. Linn, M.C., & Petersen, A.C. (1986). A meta-analysis of gender differences in spatial ability: Implications for mathematics and science achievement. In J.S. Hyde & M.C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 67-101). Baltimore, M.D: Johns Hopkins University Press. Lombard, M. (1989). The SETT and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A correlative study. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Louw, D.A., & Edwards, D.J.A. (1997). Psychology: An introduction for students in Southern Africa (2nd ed.). Sandton, RSA: Heinemann. Luiz, D.M. (1988a). A child with a hearing loss: A longitudinal study. In D.M. Luiz (Ed.), Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research paper No. C25) (pp.44-51). Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M. (1988b). A battered child: A follow-up study. In D.M. Luiz (Ed.), Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research papers No. C25) (pp.52-58). Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M. (1988c). A comparative study of two scales of language development: The Reynell and the Griffiths. In D.M. Luiz (Ed.), Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies (Research paper No. C25) (pp. 16-20). Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M. (1994). Children of South Africa: In search of a developmental profile. Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. 159 Luiz, D.M. (1997). Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: South African studies. Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M. (2000). Identifying the vulnerable infant and young child: the utility of screening and diagnostic measures (Abstract). Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., Kotras, N., McAlinden, P., & O’ Connell, R. (2004a). Griffiths Mental Development Scales – Extended Revised. Two to Eight Years. Administrative Manual. Oxford: Hogrefe- The Test Agency Ltd. Luiz, D.M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., & Kotras, N. (2004b). Griffiths Mental Development Scales Extended Revised (GMDS-ER): Technical Manual. Amersham: A.R.I.C.D. Luiz, D.M., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., Kotras, N., Horrocks, S., McAlinden, P., Challis, D., & O’Connell, R. (2006a). GMDS-ER: Griffiths Mental Development Scales- Extended Revised: Two to eight years: Administration Manual. Oxford: Hogrefe – The Test Agency Ltd. Luiz, D.M., Faragher, B., Barnard, A., Knoesen, N., Kotras, N., Burns, L.E., & Challis, D. (2006b). GMDS-ER: Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised: Two to eight years: Analysis manual. Oxford: Hogrefe-The Test Agency Ltd. Luiz, D.M., Collier, P.T., Stewart, R., Barnard, A., & Kotras, N. (2000). Revised Extended Griffiths Scales of Mental Development Technical Document (3rd ed.). Working document. Luiz, D.M., Foxcroft, C.D., & Knoesen, N. (2003). An exploration of the relationship between the Revised Griffiths Scales and grade one scholastic development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth. Luiz, D.M., Foxcroft, C.D., & Stewart, R. (1999). The construct validity of the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 73-83. Luiz, D.M., Foxcroft, C.D., Worsfold, N., Kotras, N., & Kotras, H. (2001). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: An evaluation of their prediction of scholastic achievement. South African Journal of Education, 21(2), 81-83. Luiz, D.M., & Heimes, L. (1994). The Junior South African Intelligence Scales and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A correlative study. In R. van Eeden, M. Robinson, and A.B. Posthuma (Eds.), Studies on South African Intelligence Scales. Pretoria: Human Sciences Research Council. Luiz, D.M., Oelofsen, N., Stewart, R., & Mitchell, S. (1995). An international survey of users of the Extended Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: Preliminary Results. Unpublished research paper, University of Port Elizabeth. 160 Luiz, D.M., & Jansen, J.M. (2001). Assessment of young children, physically disabled and mentally handicapped individuals. In C.D. Foxcroft & G. Roodt (Eds.), An introduction to psychological assessment in the South African context (pp. 153-174). Cape Town: Oxford University Press. Maccoby, E.E. (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Maccoby, E.E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755-765. Maccoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. MacLeod, C.M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203. Madge, E.M. (1981). Manual for the Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS)-Part I: Development and Standardisation. Pretoria: Human Science Research Council. Makowem, J.K. (2005). The development of preschool children with cochlear implants: A multiple case study. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. Marsh, H.W. (1989). Sex differences in the development of verbal and mathematics constructs: The high school and beyond study. American Educational Research Journal, 26, 191-225. McCarthy, D. (1954). Language development in children. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of Child Psychology (2nd ed., pp. 492-630). New York: Wiley. McCarthy, J.J., & Kirk, S.A. (1963). Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urbana, Illinois: University of Illinois Press. McGhee, P.E. (1976). Children’s appreciation of humour: A test of the cognitive congruency principle. Child Development, 47, 420-426. McGraw, M. (1935). Growth: A study of Johnny and Jimmy. New York: Appleton Century Co. McHough, M.C., Koeske, R.D., & Frieze, I.H. (1986). Issues to consider in conducting non-sexist psychological research: A guide for researchers. American Psychologist, 41, 879-890. Meehan, A.M. (1984). A meta-analysis of sex differences in formal operational thought. Child Development, 55, 1110-1124. Meller, P. J., Ohr, P. S., & Marcus, R. A. (2001). Family oriented, culturally sensitive (FOCUS) assessment of young children. In L. A. Suzuki, J. G. Ponterotto, & P. J. Meller (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural assessment: Clinical, psychological, and educational applications (2nd ed., pp. 461-496). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Inc. Meyer, W.F., Moore, C., & Viljoen, H.G. (2002). Personology: From individual to ecosystem. Johannesburg: Heinemann. Miller, R. (2001). In C.D. Foxcroft, & G. Roodt (Eds.), An introduction to psychological assessment in the South African context (p. 328). Cape Town: Oxford University Press. 161 Mischel, W. (1966). A social learning view of sex differences in behaviour. In E.E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 56-81). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Mondschein, E.R., Adolph, K.E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C.S. (2000). Gender bias in mothers’ expectations about infant crawling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 304-316. Money, J. & Ehrhardt, A.A. (1972). Man and woman, boy and girl: Differentiation and dimorphism of gender identity from conception to maturity. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Moore, T. (1967). Language and intelligence: A longitudinal study of the first eight years (Part I. Patterns of development in boys and girls). Human Development, 10, 88-106. Moosajee, S. (2007). Exploring the construct-related validity of the Personal-Social Subscale of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). Unpublished doctoral thesis. Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Morisset, C.E., Barnard, K.E., & Booth, C.L. (1995). Toddlers’ language development: Sex differences within social risk. Developmental Psychology, 31, 851-865. Morra, S. (2000). A new model of verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 191-227. Moseley, D., Baumfield, V., Elliott, J., Gregson, M., Higgins, S., Miller, J., & Newton, D. (2005). Frameworks for thinking: A handbook for teaching and learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Mosley-Howard, S. (1995). Best practices in considering the role culture. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology III (pp. 337-345). Washington DC: National Association of School Psychologists. Mothuloe, V.B. (1990). The aptitude tests for school beginners and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: An investigation into the assessment of the cognitive abilities of grade 1 children. Pretoria: Medical University of South Africa. Mouton, J. (2001). How to succeed in your master’s and doctoral studies: A South African guide and resource book. Pretoria: J.L. Van Schaik. Mumbauer, C.C., & Miller, J.O. (1970). Socioeconomic background and cognitive functioning in preschool children. Child Development, 41, 471-480. Murphy, K.R., & Davidshofer, C.O. (1991). Psychological Testing: Principles and applications (2nd ed.). Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Mwamwenda, T.S. (1995). Educational psychology: An African perspective (2nd ed.). Durban: Butterworths. Naglieri, J.A. (1999). Essentials of CAS assessment. New York: Wiley. 162 Naglieri, J.A., & Rojahn, J. (2001). Gender differences in Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive (PASS) cognitive processes and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), 430-437. Najman, J. M., Aird, R., Bor, W. et al. (2004). The generational transmission of socioeconomic inequalities in child cognitive development and emotional health. Soc Sei Med, 58, 1147-58. Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T.J., Boykin, A.W., Brody, N., Ceci, S.J., Halpern, D.F., Loehlin, J.C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R.J., & Urbuna, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns. American Psychologist, 51(2), 77-101. Newcombe, N. S. (1996). Child development: Change over time (8th ed.). New York: HarperCollins College Publishers. Newcombe, N.S., Mathason, L., & Terlecki, M. (2002). Maximization of spatial competence: More important than finding the cause of sex differences. In A.V. McGillicuddy-De Lisi & R. De Lisi (Eds.), Biology, society and behaviour: The development of sex differences in cognition (pp. 183206). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing. Nuttall, E.V., Romero, I., & Kalesnik, J. (1992). Assessing and screening preschoolers: Psychological and educational dimensions. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Okagaki, L., & Diamond, K. E. (2000). Responding to cultural and linguistic differences in the beliefs and practices of families with young children. Young Children, 55(3), 74-80. Parsley, K.M., Powell, M., O’Connor, H., & Deutsch, M. (1963). Are there really sex differences in achievement? Journal of Educational Research, 57, 210-212. Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345. Phillips, D.C., & Kelley, M.E. (1975). Hierarchical theories of development in education and psychology. Harvard Educational Review, 45(3), 351-375. Piaget, J. (1930). The child’s conception of physical causality. New York: Harcourt, Brace. Pianta, R.C., & McCoy, S.J. (1997). The first day of school: The predictive validity of early school screening. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 1-22. Plomin, R., & Foch, T.T. (1981). Sex differences and individual differences. Child Development, 52, 383-385. Pollatou, E., Karadimou, K., & Gerodimos, V. (2005). Gender differences in musical aptitude, rhythmic ability and motor performance in preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 175, 361-369. Posner, M.I., & Raichle, M.E. (1994). Images of mind. New York: Freeman. Posner, M.I., & Raichle, M.E. (1997). Images of mind. New York: Scientific American Library. 163 Potgieter, F.E., Thomas, E.P., Seager, J.R., Viljoen, E., Rossouw, A., Tokota, B., McGranahan, G., & Kjellen, M. (1999). Household environment and health in Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Stockholm: Stockholm Environment Institute. Povey, J.L. (2002). The Griffiths Extended Scales of Mental Development: A South African factorial validity study. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth. Povey, J.L. (2008). Exploration of the validity of the revised Eye and Hand Co-ordination Subscale of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. Powers, G.T., Meenaghan, T.M., & Toomey, B.G. (1985). Practice-focused research: integrating human service practice and research. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Ramey, C.T., & Campbell, F. (1991). Poverty, early childhood education, and academic competence: The Abcedarian experiment. In A. Huston (Ed.), Children in poverty (pp.190-221). New York: Cambridge University Press. Ramsay, M., & Fitzharding, P.M. (1977). A comparative study of two developmental scales: The Bayley and the Griffiths. Early Human Development, 1, 151-157. Ras, B. (1987). Design and standardisation of learning readiness assessment procedures for Black preschoolers and school beginners. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Pretoria. Reed, T.E., & Jensen, A.R. (1992). Conduction velocity in a brain nerve pathway of normal adults correlates with intelligence level. Intelligence, 16, 259-272. Reinisch, J.M., & Sanders, S.A. (1992). Prenatal hormonal contributions to sex differences in human cognitive and personality development. In A.A. Gerall, H. Moltz, & I.L. Ward (Eds.), Handbook of behavioural neurology: Vol. 11. Sexual differentiation (pp. 221-243). New York: Plenum Press. Reynolds, A.J. (1991). Early schooling of children at risk: American Education Research Journal, 28, 392-422. Reynolds, A.J. (1994). Effects of a preschool plus follow-on intervention for children at risk Developmental Psychology, 30, 787-804. Ribaupierre, A., de, & Bailleux, C. (2000). The development of working memory: Further note on the comparability of two models of working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 110-127. Richter, L.M., & Griesel, R.D. (1988). Bayley Scales of Infant Development: Norms for interpreting the performance of Black South African infants. Pretoria: Institute of Behavioural Sciences. Riordan, Z.V.A. (1978). Locus of control in South Africa: A cross-ethnic study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. 164 Robinson, M. (1989). Bylae tot die handleiding vir die Junior Suid-Afrikaanse Individuele Skale (JSAIS): Tegniese besonderhede en normtabelle vir 6- tot 8-jarige leerlinge. Pretoria: HSRC. Robinson, N.M., Abbott, R.D., Berninger, V.W., & Busse, J. (1996). Structure of abilities in mathprecocious young children: Gender similarities and differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 341-352. Rogers, L. & Walsh, J. (1982). Shortcomings of the psychomedical research of John Money and coworkers into sex differences in behaviour: Social and political implications. Sex roles, 8, 269281. Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (5th ed.). Chicago, IL, Riverside, 2003 Rome-Flanders, T., & Cronk, C. (1995). A longitudinal study of infant vocalizations during motherinfant games. Journal of Child Language, 22, 259-274. Roopnarine, J. L., & Carter, B. (Eds.). (1992). Parent-child socialization in diverse cultures. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Rossouw, A.M.M. (1996). Research design, methodology and techniques: A student’s guide to empirical research. Port Elizabeth: University of Port Elizabeth. Sandison, A. (2005). Exploring the developmental profile of black HIV positive/ AIDS infants and children. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. Sattler, J.M. (1974). Assessment of children’s intelligence. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company. Schneider, W. (in press). Memory development in childhood. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. London, UK: Blackwell. Schröder, I.A. (2004). The performance of hearing impaired children on the Revised Extended Griffiths Scale. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth. Schubert, J., & Croploy, A. (1972). Verbal regulation of behaviour and IQ in Canadian Indian and white children. Developmental Psychology, 7, 295-301. Seaberg, J. 1988. Utilizing sampling procedures. In R. Grinnell Jr. (Ed.), Social Work Research and Evaluation (3rd ed., pp. 240-257). Itasca, Illinois: Peacock Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 4-27. Shea, D.L., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C.P. (2001). Importance of assessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adolescents: A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 604-614. Sherman, J.A. (1967). Problem of sex differences in space perception and aspects of intellectual functioning. Psychological Review, 74, 290-299. 165 Shipman, V.C. (1971). Disadvantaged children and their first school experiences: Structure and development of cognitive competencies and styles prior to school entry. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Shonkoff, J.P., & Meisels, S.J. (2000). Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Siegel, L.S. (1968). The development of the ability to process information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 368-383. Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Siraj-Blatchford, J. (2002). Surveys and questionnaires: an evaluative case study. In G.M. Naughton, S.A. Rolfe, & I. Siraj-Blatchford (Eds.), Doing early childhood research, 149-161. Simeonsson, R.J. (1986). Psychological and developmental assessment of special children. Newton, Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon Inc. Sletten, T. (1970). Psykologisk vurdering I smabarnalderen. Tisskrift norske Laegeforening, 90, 17941797. Sletten, T. (1977). Intensive treatment of babies and infants based on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales. Unpublished, address to ARICD. Smit, G.R. (1996). Psychometrics, aspects and measurement. Pretoria: Kasigo. Snow, W.G., & Weinstock, J. (1990). Sex differences among non-brain-damaged adults on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales: A review of the literature. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 873-886. Sontag, L.W., & Kagan, J. (1963). The emergence of intellectual achievement motives. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 33, 532-534. Squires, J., Nickel, R.E., & Eisert, D. (1996). Early detection of developmental problems: Strategies for monitoring young children in the practice setting. Developmental and behavioural paediatrics, 17(6), 420-427. Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. New York: Macmillan. Stanley, J.C., Benbow, C.P., Brody, L.E., Dauber, S., & Lupkowski, A. (1992). Gender differences on eighty-six nationally standardized aptitude and achievement tests. In N. Colangelo, S.G. Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: New findings and controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 1-32. Sternberg, R.J., & Detterman, D.K. (Eds.). (1986). What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex. 166 Stevenson, H.W., Parker, T., Wilkinson, A., Hegion, A., & Fish, E. (1976). Longitudinal study of individual differences in cognitive development and scholastic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68(40), 377-400. Stewart, R. (1997). The construct validity of the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Stewart, R. (2005). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: Proposed new items for the revision of Subscales A, B, C, D and E. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press Strayer, J., & Ames, E.W. (1972). Stimulus orientation and the apparent developmental lag between perception and performance. Child Development, 43, 1345-1354. Struwig, F.W., & Stead, G.B. (2001). Planning, designing and reporting research. Cape Town: Masker Miller Longman. Stumpf, H. (1995). Gender differences in performance on tests of cognitive abilities: Experimental design issues and empirical results. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 275-287. Swanson, L.H., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in childhood with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79, 294-321. Swart, D.J. (1987). Standaardisering van ‘n Junior Individuele Skaal vir Indier-Suid-Afrikaners. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Potchefstroom. Sweeney, K. (1994). Cluster analysis of the Griffiths profiles of a white South African clinical population. Unpublished Master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Thomas, J.R., & French, K.E. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance: A metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260-282. Thomas, R.M. (2005). Comparing theories of child development (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Thomson, G.H. (1939). The factorial analysis of human ability. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Townes, B.D., Trupin, E.W., & Fay, G. (1980). Neuropsychological correlates of academic performance. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 2, 189-199. 167 Tukulu, A.N. (1996). The Denver II Scale and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: A correlation study. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Underwood, M.K., Galen, B.R., & Paquette, J.A. (2001). Top ten challenges for understanding gender and aggression in children: Why can’t we all just get along? Social Development, 10, 248-266. Unger, R.K. (1979). Toward a redefinition of sex and gender. American Psychologist, 34, 1085-1094. Van den Berg, A.R. (1987). Using the Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS) (1981) for testees for South African population groups which were not included in the norm population. Pretoria: HSRC. Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems development: Change between complexity and chaos. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Van Heerden, R. (2007). Exploring normal South African and British children: A comparative study utilizing the Griffiths Mental development Scales-Extended Revised. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Van Rooyen, K. (2005). The performance of South African and British children on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised: A comparative study. Unpublished master’s treatise, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Van Tonder, P. (2007). WISC-IV performance of South African Grade 7 English and Xhosa-speaking children with advantaged and disadvantaged education. Unpublished master’s dissertation, Rhodues University, Grahamstown. Vaught, G.M. (1965). The relationship of role identification and ego strength to sex differences in the Rod-and-Frame Test. Journal of Personality, 33, 271-283. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A metaanalysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250-270. Ward, N. (1997). The use of the Denver II Scales and the Griffiths Scales of Mental Development with South African coloured preschool children. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Weinert, F.E., & Weinert, S. (1998). History and systems of developmental psychology. In A. Demetriou, W. Doise, & C. Van Leishout (Eds.), Lifespan developmental psychology (pp. 1-33). Chichester: John Wiley and Son. Weinert, F.E., & Weinert, S. (2005). In Moseley et al. (Eds.), Frameworks for thinking: A handbook for teaching and learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation. 168 Whiting, B., & Pope, C.E. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behaviour. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Winstead, B.A., Derlega, V.J., & Unger, R.K. (1999). Sex and gender. In V.J. Derlega, B.A. Winstead, & W.H. Jones (Eds.), Personality: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 257-281). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Wolf, V.C. (1971). Age and sex performance differences as measured by a new nonverbal visual perceptual test. Psychonomic Science, 25, 85-87. Wood, D. L. (1998). Educating physicians for the 21st century. Academic Medicine, 73, 1280-1281. Worsfold, L. B. (1993). The Griffiths Scales of Mental Development: An evaluation of their prediction of scholastic achievement. Unpublished master’s treatise, University of Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. 169 APPENDIX A NELSON MANDELA METROPOLITAN UNIVERSITY INFORMATION AND INFORMED CONSENT FORM Title of the research project Principal Investigator Address Postal Code Contact telephone number Comparing the Development of a Sample of South African Preschool Boys and Girls Utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised Tamarin Jakins Psychology Department, Faculty of Health Sciences Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University South Campus P.O. Box 77000 Port Elizabeth 6031 (041) 504 2330 A. DECLARATION BY OR ON BEHALF OF PARTICIPANT (Person legally competent to give consent on behalf of the participant) I, the undersigned I.D. number (Full Name) in my capacity as parent / guardian of the participant I.D. number Address (of participant) (Child’s Full Name) Initial 170 A.1. I HEREBY CONFIRM AS FOLLOWS: Initial 1. The participant was invited to participate in the above-mentioned research project that is being undertaken by Ms. Tamarin Jakins of the Department of Psychology, in the Faculty of Health Sciences of the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. 2. The following aspects have been explained to me, the representative Initial of the participant: Aim: The investigator is studying the developmental profiles of a sample of normal South African preschool boys and girls (5 and 6 years old). The information will be used: - to contribute to the completion of a Masters research project; - to contribute to research on the development between the genders within a South African context on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised. Procedures: Initial I understand that my child will be participating in an approximate two hour assessment in which the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised will be administered. Risks: None Initial Possible Benefits: Initial As a result of my child’s participation in this study, I will be informed of any developmental concerns which he or she is experiencing and will have the opportunity to assess them further. Confidentiality: Initial My child’s identity will not be revealed in any discussion, description or scientific publications by the investigator. Access to findings: Any new information/or benefit that develops during the course of the study will be shared as follows: - in the form of an individual report for each child disseminating their assessment results and developmental profile. Initial 171 - general report of the overall results of the research project. Initial Voluntary participation/ refusal/ discontinuation: My child’s participation is voluntary YES NO My decision whether or not to participate will in no way affect my present or future care/ employment/ TRUE lifestyle FALSE 3. The information above was explained to me, the parent / guardian of the Initial participant by : _________________________ (Name of relevant person) in Afrikaans / English / Xhosa / Other ___________________ and I am in command of this language / it was satisfactorily translated to me by ______________________ (Name of translator) I was given the opportunity to ask questions and all these questions were answered satisfactorily. 4. No pressure was exerted on me to consent to participation and I Initial understand that I may withdraw at any stage without penalisation. 5. Participation in this study will not result in any additional cost to myself. A.2 I HEREBY VOLUNTARILY CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE IN THE ABOVE-MENTIONED PROJECT Signed/confirmed at on Signature of witness Signature parent / guardian Full name of witness 2008 172 B. STATEMENT BY OR ON BEHALF OF INVESTIGATOR I, Tamarin Jakins, declare that - I have explained the information given in this document to (name of participant) and/or his/her parent / guardian (name of parent/ guardian) - he/she was encouraged and given ample time to ask me any questions; - this conversation was conducted in Afrikaans / English / Xhosa / Other and no translator was used / this conversation was translated into (language) - by I have detached Section C and handed it to the participant YES Signed/confirmed at on Signature of witness Signature of interviewer Full name of witness NO 2008 173 C. IMPORTANT MESSAGE TO REPRESENTATIVE OF PARTICIPANT Dear representative of the participant Thank you for allowing your child to participate in my study. Should, at any time during the study: - an emergency arise as a result of the research, or you require any further information with regard to the study, or any of the following occur: - The participant needs to terminate participation or - The participant is unable to attend a scheduled appointment (indicate any circumstances which should be reported to the investigator) Kindly contact at telephone number Tamarin Jakins (041) 504 2330 174 APPENDIX B Dear Principal Date Participation in a research study conducted by the Department of Psychology, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University As per our telephonic discussion, my name is Tamarin Jakins and I am currently completing my Masters Degree in Counselling Psychology at the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. A large component of the degree entails conducting a research project. The purpose of this study is to generate comparative information regarding the general development of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls, between the ages of 5 and 6 years old, utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). The six areas of general development that are measured on the subscales of the GMDS-ER include: Locomotor, Personal-Social, Language, Eye-Hand Co-ordination, Performance and Practical Reasoning. The original Griffiths Scales were developed in Britain in the 1960s and are used internationally for the developmental assessment of young children between the ages of 2 and 8 years old. A research team based at the Psychology Department of the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University have recently revised the Scales, making them more culture fair. The Scales provide the researcher with a General Quotient (GQ) and an age equivalent, which provides an indication of the child’s overall development. Participants will be asked to complete tasks that are age appropriate and based on the concept of play. Examples of tasks include: threading beads, naming objects seen in a picture, kicking a ball, tying a knot and drawing. Please remain assured that the children participating in the study will be free to refuse or withdraw at any stage of the assessment and will be informed of this. 175 The administrators of the assessment will consist of Psychologists, Intern Psychologists and Psychologists-in-Training, who have undergone training in the GMDS-ER. The assessment could take approximately two hours for each child and will preferably take place at the University Psychology Clinic (UCLIN). However, alternative arrangements can be made if necessary. A brief feedback session, in addition to an individual report, will be provided to the parents. Upon receiving the results, should further assessment be required, it will be arranged through UCLIN. In addition, feedback regarding the overall findings of the research project will be provided to the parents and principals of the participating preschools in the form of a comprehensive letter. Prior to commencement of the research, I hereby request your permission to allow children at your pre-school, in the 5- and 6-year-old age group, to participate in the study. The information and assessment results obtained will be treated as strictly confidential. Attached please find a copy of the Covering letter, Biographical Questionnaire and the Informed Consent Form to be completed by the parents and/or guardians of the participants in the study. Please note that if parents do not wish their child to participate, they are not under any obligation and can indicate this on the consent form. Should you require any further information regarding the research project, please do not hesitate to contact me at UCLIN on (041) 504 2330, or by e-mail at [email protected]. I would like to stress that the success of this project depends entirely on your voluntary cooperation and I thank you in advance for your interest. Yours Sincerely, _______________ Ms. Tamarin Jakins Intern Counselling Psychologist ______________ _________________ Dr. Louise Stroud Prof. Cheryl Foxcroft Supervisor Co-supervisor 176 APPENDIX C Dear Parent / Guardian Date Participation in a research study conducted by the Department of Psychology, Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University As per our telephonic discussion, my name is Tamarin Jakins and I am currently completing my Masters Degree in Counselling Psychology at the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University. A large component of the degree entails conducting a research project. The purpose of this study is to generate comparative information regarding the general development of a sample of South African pre-school boys and girls, between the ages of 5 and 6 years old, utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER). The six areas of general development that are measured on the subscales of the GMDS-ER include: Locomotor, Personal-Social, Language, Eye-Hand Co-ordination, Performance and Practical Reasoning. The original Griffiths Scales were developed in Britain in the 1960s and are used internationally for the developmental assessment of young children between the ages of 2 and 8 years old. A research team based at the Psychology Department of the Nelson Mandela Metropolitan University have recently revised the Scales, making them more culture fair. The Scales provide the researcher with a General Quotient (GQ) and an age equivalent, which provides an indication of the child’s overall development. Participants will be asked to complete tasks that are age appropriate and based on the concept of play. Examples of tasks include: threading beads, naming objects seen in a picture, kicking a ball, tying a knot and drawing. Please remain assured that your child will be free to refuse or withdraw at any stage of the assessment and will be informed of this. 177 The administrators of the assessment will consist of Psychologists, Intern Psychologists and Psychologists-in-Training, who have undergone training in the GMDS-ER. The assessment could take approximately two hours for each child and will preferably take place at the University Psychology Clinic (UCLIN). However, alternative arrangements can be made if necessary. A brief feedback session, in addition to an individual report, will be provided. Upon receiving the results, should further assessment be required, it will be arranged through UCLIN. Prior to commencement of the research, I hereby request your permission to allow your child to participate. It is kindly requested that you complete the attached Biographical Questionnaire and Information and Informed Consent Form. Upon completion, it would be greatly appreciated if you could enclose the completed documents in the provided pre-paid and addressed envelope and return it to me as soon as possible. The information and assessment results obtained will be treated as strictly confidential. Please note that if you do not wish your child to participate in the project, you may indicate this on the consent form. Should you require any further information regarding the research project, please do not hesitate to contact me at UCLIN on (041) 504 2330, or by e-mail at [email protected]. I would like to stress that the success of this project depends entirely on your voluntary cooperation and I thank you in advance for your interest. Yours Sincerely, ___________________ Ms. Tamarin Jakins Intern Counselling Psychologist __________________ _________________ Dr. Louise Stroud Prof. Cheryl Foxcroft Supervisor Co-supervisor 178 APPENDIX D BIOGRAPHICAL QUESTIONNAIRE SECTION A PERSONAL DETAILS Child’s name and surname:_______________________________________________ Address: _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ Suburb: ___________________________________ Telephone number: __________________________ Date of Birth: _______/_______/_______ Gender: M F Race/Cultural Group: Black Coloured Asian White Current Pre-school/School: ___________________________________ Pre-school/School Telephone No: ______________________________ Home language: ____________________ Has your child been diagnosed with a mental and/or physical disorder? If yes, please specify. _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ _____________________________________________________________________ Breadwinner’s/Guardian’s Occupation: ___________________________________ Breadwinner’s/Guardian’s Educational Level: (Please tick the highest level achieved) None Primary School Junior Certificate Apprenticeship Matric Certificate (standard 10/ grade12) Further Training (not at University) 179 University degree or diploma SECTION B 1. Birth History: Please describe anything unusual about the pregnancy or delivery: ___________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________________________ ____________________ Please tick the appropriate answer (Y = Yes, N = No): 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Was your pregnancy planned? Did you give birth to your child naturally? Was your child anoxic (i.e. did he/she lack oxygen at birth)? Was your child born prematurely or after more than 41 weeks of pregnancy? Length of pregnancy:……………………....months Did you bond easily with your child? Did you breast-feed your child? Did you experience postpartum depression? Motor Development 9 At what age did your child Sit:……………………….. months Crawl: ……………………..months Walk:………………………months 10 Is your child extremely under active? 11 Is your child noticeably physically overactive? 12 Is your child clumsy? Language Development 13 Did your child have difficulty with sucking and chewing? 14 At what age did your child start to babble? ………………(months) 15 Does your child use single words? If yes, at what age? ……….(months) 16 Does your child speak in sentences? If yes, at what age? ………..(years) 17 Does your child ask repetitive questions? 18 Does your child talk to him/herself excessively? 19 Does your child echo words or phrases constantly? Emotional Development 20 Does your child cry or laugh for no reason? 21 Does your child prefer to be alone? 22 Does your child enjoy cuddling and respond to affection? 23 Does your child have temper tantrums regularly? 24 Does your child display extreme distress for no apparent reason? Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N 180 Social Development 25 Does your child have difficulty in mixing with other children? 26 Does your child make little or no eye contact? 27 Does your child form inappropriate attachment to certain objects? Sensory / Hearing Development 28 Does your child appear as if he / she does not hear you? 29 Does your child cover his/her ears? 30 Is your child upset by noises? General 31 Is your child on any kind of medication? If yes, for what? ……………………………………………………………………….. 32 Does your child stutter? 33 Does your child faint frequently? 34 Does your child bite his / her nails excessively? 35 Hs your child ever had any childhood diseases? (If yes, please list all childhood diseases and the ages at which they occurred) ………………………………………………………………..Age…...… ………………………………………………………………..Age…....... ..................................................................................................Age…...… ………………………………………………………………..Age…...… ………………………………………………………………..Age……... Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N SECTION C The following questions are applicable to children of a broad age range; therefore, we do not necessarily expect your child to be capable of the tasks listed below. We would appreciate a completely honest evaluation of your child’s ability. Please do not be concerned if your child is not yet able to complete each of the activities. 1. Does your child help with small household tasks? 2. Does your child help with routine tasks when requested? 3. Does your child help tidy a room? 4. Does your child bath or shower with minimal assistance? 5. Does your child clean his / her own teeth? 6. Does your child wash his / her own hands and face but needs assistance with drying? 7. Does your child wash and dry his / her own hands and face but needs checking? 8. Does your child wash and dry his / her own hands and face without assistance? 9. Does your child need some assistance to bath or shower? 10. Does your child bath or shower without assistance? 11. Does your child bath or shower, and dry him / herself without assistance? 12 Does your child need assistance to put on his / her own shoes and socks, e.g. putting shoes on correct feet? 13. Does your child put on his / her own shoes and socks without assistance? 14. Does your child choose his / her own clothes? Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N 181 15. Does your child deliver a simple message? 16. Does your child go on instruction to get a specific item in a public area, e.g. go and get bread from the counter and bring it to mother? 17. Does your child go alone on errands to nearby shops, etc.? 18. Does your child make a small purchase in a shop with some assistance, e.g. checking the change? 19. Does your child make a small purchase in a shop without assistance? 20. Does your child demonstrate an understanding that it is unsafe to accept rides, foods or money from a stranger? 21. Does your child need to be reminded to follow the rules in a simple game? 22. Does your child follow a rule in a simple game, without being reminded? 23. Does your child neaten (brush or comb) own hair in the morning? 24. Does your child ask to use the toilet? 25. Does your child have bladder control during the day, with a few accidents? 26. Does your child have complete bladder control during the day and night? 27. Does your child get a drink of water from the tap without your assistance? 28. Does your child get a drink of water from the tap with some assistance? 29. Does your child eat without assistance? Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Y/N Thank you for your co-operation in filling in this Questionnaire. All the information that you have supplied us with will be treated as strictly confidential. 182 APPENDIX E CONFIDENTIAL REPORT Mr. and Mrs. Surname Address Port Elizabeth 6001 Date Dear Mr. and Mrs. Surname DEVELOPMENTAL ASSESSMENT OF CHILD Thank you for allowing Child to participate in the study utilizing the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised (GMDS-ER), on exploring and comparing the development of a sample of South African preschool boys and girls. The GMDS-ER assesses the mental development of young children from two to eight years by measuring the development and maturation of a child’s attributes and abilities across six different avenues of learning, namely a) Gross Motor/ Locomotor, b) Personal-Social, c) Language, d) Eye Hand Co-ordination, e) Performance, and f) Practical Reasoning skills and abilities. BIOGRAPHICAL DETAILS Name : Gender : Date of Assessment : Date of Birth : Age at time of assessment : School : Child’s scores on the GMDS-ER provide an estimate of his / her developmental level and are a reflection of his / her performance on the day, at that time. 183 RESULTS OF THE ASSESSMENT Subscale Locomotor Skills: This scale assesses gross motor skills including the ability to balance, co-ordinate and control movements. Examples: the ability to run fast outdoors, bounce and catch a ball, hop on one foot etc. Personal-Social Development: This scale assesses proficiency in the activities of daily living, independence and an ability to interact with other children. Examples: the ability to give a home address, to fasten shoe buckle and eat with knife and fork etc. Language: This scale assesses verbal ability, the understanding of the meaning of words and the ability to use language effectively (receptive and expressive language). Examples: naming colours, relating a story about a given picture and repeating 6-16 syllable sentences etc. Eye and Hand Co-ordination: This scale assesses fine-motor skills and relates to visual abilities with handwork. Examples: drawing, writing, threading beads and cutting with scissors etc. Practical Skills: This scale assesses manipulation skills including speed of working and precision. Examples: working with form boards of 4,6 and 11 shapes; building blocks to resemble given patterns. Practical Reasoning: This scale assesses problem-solving ability, basic mathematical comprehension and moral reasoning. It indicates the child’s ability to benefit from formal schooling. Examples: memory recall and repetition of 1 to 5 digits, picture arrangements of cards to tell a story, visual memory etc. Mental Age Category of (in months) Performance Above/Below age (in months) 184 GENERAL DEVELOPMENT Child’s mental age of ? years and ? month (? months) indicates that his/her general developmental functioning, as measured on the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Extended Revised, is ? months above / below his chronological age. Based on the overall performance, Child’s performance can be described as being below average / average / above average / superior. RECOMMENDATIONS 1. The results presented above are based on an hour and a half assessment. It is possible that your observations of Child may be different to the ones made by myself during this assessment. Please keep this in mind when reading this report and consider that the recommendations are based on the observations made during the assessment session. 2. As suggested to all participants in the study, it is recommended that Child’s sight and hearing is assessed to ensure that these senses are functioning at their full potential. This recommendation should only be followed if Child has not yet had these assessments. 3. Child performed lower on the Practical Reasoning subscale than the other five subscales. Performance on this subscale can be improved by encouraging him or her to complete tasks that will stimulate his or her practical reasoning and numerical abilities, such as: • Learning mathematical concepts such as counting by using buttons, lids of bottles etc. • Learning the names of money (coins) • Learning the difference between ‘morning’ and ‘afternoon’ I trust you will find this information useful. Should you require any further information, please do not hesitate to contact me at the Psychology Clinic (UCLIN) on (041) 504 2330. Yours Sincerely, Ms. Tamarin Jakins Dr. Louise Stroud Test Administrator / Principal Researcher Research Supervisor Intern Counselling Psychologist Clinical Psychologist
© Copyright 2024