IMAGINATION, COGNITION AND PERSONALITY, Vol.28(1)37-48,2008-2009 DREAM CONTENT IN A REPRESENTATIVE GERMAN SAMPLE: GENDER DIFFERENCES AND THE EFFECTS OF OTHER SOCIO-DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABL E S MICHA E LS CHRE D L KATHARINAKELLER Central lnstituteof Mental Health,Mannheim,Germany ABSTRACT The presentstudy investigatedthe efl.ectsof socio-demographicvariableson dream contentin a representativesample.The analysesof 380 most recent variableslike age, dreamsshowedthat almostnoneof the socio-demographic gender,marital status,education,income,nor town (or city) of residencesize was significantlyrelatedto generaldreamcharacteristics like dream length, bizarreness,and intensityof dream emotions,thus indicatingthat dreaming is a universal phenomenonshared by all humans and is experiencedin similar ways. On the other hand, dream content-in contrastto the general dream characteristics is determinedby waking-life experiences.This is clearly shown by the gender differences fbund in the present study: more work-relatedthemesand physical aggressionin men's dreams.ln order to generalizethesefindings,it will be necessaryto apply otherdreamcollection methodslike dream diary or laboratoryawakeningsin large. representative samplesin order to obtain and analyzethe dream reports of personswho do not often recall their dreamsand who are not able to reooft their most recent dreamto an interviewer. 37 O 2008,BaywoodPublishingCo., Inc. d o i : 10 . 2 19 0 / 1 C . 2 8. .d1 http://baywood.com 38 / KELLER SCHREDLAND INTRODUCTION The f,rrstcontentanalysisof a large sampleof dream reportswas publishedby Hall and Van de Castle(1966).They included1,000diary dreamreports(length: 50 to 300 words) from 200 studentsaged l8 to 25 years, clicited in the timc period from 1947 to 1950.In chapter14 of their book entitledNorms, Hall and Van de Castle (1966) presentedthe coding resultsfor women and men separately.Many of thc findings,for example,regardingthe genderdifferences(more physical aggression,more sexual activity, more outdoor settings,higher percentageof male drcam charactersin men's dreams) have been replicatedby subseqnentstudies(e.g., Hall, Domhoff, Blick, & Weesner,1982. Schredl, Sahin, & Schäfer, 1998) which were also most often carried out in student samples.A preponderanceof male drearncharactersin tnen's dreamsand an equal proportion of male and femalc charactersin women's dreams were reported in many different samples (Hall, 1984). Findings based on nonstudent samplessometimesyielded difTerentresults; for example, Rubinstein and Krippner ( 1991)found no genderdifferencein the percentageof male dream charactcrsin dreamsof personswho answereda television announccmentfor free dream intcrpretation.Similar, the rnale/femalepercentof dream characters was corxparablefor men (58%) and women (51%) in thc study of Krippner et al. (1998) who recruited participantsof drcarn seminars,mostly working adults. However, the dream samplesof the non-studentsarnplesmight also be biascd, for example,the dream selectedfor dream analysismight not be representative for the dream life of this person. For investigating the effects of sociodemographicvariableslike age, gender,marital status,and cducationallevel. dreamsamplesshouldbe studied. representative study of dreamcontenthas bccn reported Up to now, only one representative in the scientificliterature(Kramer, Winget, & Whitman, l97l; Winget, Kramer, & Whitrnan, 1972).ln this study, 300 citizensof Cincinnati(a stratifiedrandorn sample)were interviewedabout generalenvironmentalhealth issuesat home; 60.7% reporled a most recent dream in the course of the interview. Women reporteddreamsmore often than men (65% vs. 53%). Mean dream length was 21 words. Most of the dreamreportswere describedas mundanewith a realistic focus(Kramerct al., l97l). Using the Hall and Van de Castleratingscales,120% of the dream reports included unpleasanternotionswhcreas only 40Ä includcd positiveemotions.Applying anothercontentanalyticrating system(GottschalkGleser),the dreamreportswere divided into unpleasant(54o ), pleasant(26%). and neutral (20%) dreams.The higher percentageof dreamswith ernotionsis explainedby the differing codingrules sinceHall and Van de Castle( 1966)coded only emotionsthat were explicitly mentionedwhereasthe Gottschalk-Glescr systemsallows inferencesregardingemotionsfrom the drcatnaction.Work and topicswcrc prescntin I 1.3%of the drearnreports(Kramer et al., business-related 1971\.Genderwas an importantfactor in cxplaininginterindividualdifferences, DR E A MC ON TE N TS TU/ D39 Y e.g.,men reportedmore dreamswith aggressionandwomenmore dreamemotions (Winget et al., 1972). The study, however,had severalmethodologicalproblems.First, the authors did not report the percentageof participantswho did not consent to being interviewed.Second.the time intervalsbetweeninterview and the occumenceof the most recent dreams were not given in the published papers. Third, the varistatisticalanalysisdid not investigatethe effectsof all socio-demographic the for example, might be biased, the findings ables simultaneouslyand, thus, explained might be in widower's dreams heightenedoccurrenceof deathanxiety by age (cf. Winget et al., 1972). ln the present study, the most recent dreams of a representativeGerman samplewere analyzed.Regressionanalyseswere computedin order to test the variablessimultaneously. effect of all socio-demographic ME T H OD lnterview The participantswere interviewedat home. ln order to elicit dream recall, a 7-point dreamrecall frequencyscale(codedfrom 0: never, I : lessthan once a month. 2 : aboutonce a month, 3 : two or threetimes a month,4 : aboutonce a week, 5 : Severaltimes a week, and 6 : almostevery morning) was presentedon the monitor of a portablecomputer.The dreamrecall frequencyscalehas a high retestreliability (r : .85; Schredl,2004a).Next, the participantswere askcd to report their most recent drcam as completelyas possible(including characters. actions,and emotions).The intervicwcr askedonce whetherthcre was anything elsethey could remember.The dreamwas typed in by the interviewerwhile the participantwas telling the dreamusing thc words of the dreamer.Lastly,the time intervalbetweenthe dateof the dreamandthedateof the interviewwas recorded. The following socio-dcmographicvariablcswere included in the study: age, gender,education(five levels), income (15 levels ranging from "below 249 Euro per rnonth" to "over 5,000 Euro per month"), marital status(married/living with partner,single/livingwithout parlner),and town (or city) of residencesizc (7 levelsrangingfrom "below 2000 inhabitants"to "over 500,000inhabitants"). Dream Content Analysis The dreamcontentanalyticscalesusedin this studywere adoptedfrom Schredl, (l : realisticto 4 - two or more Sahin,and Schäfer(1998):realism/bizarreness bizane elementswithin the dream),positiveand negativeemotions(two 4-point scales:0 : none, I : mild, 2 : moderate,3 : strong),numberofmale and female dream characters,occurrenceof work-relateddreamsand occuffenceof verbal and/or physical aggression.The interater reliability of these scalesare satisr : .642 (positive emotions),r : .825 factory, r : .765 (realism/bizarreness), 40 / SC HRE DLA ND K E LLER (negativeemotions),occurrenceof aggression(88% exact agreement;all data from Schredl,Burchert,& Grabatin,2004). The exactagreementfor work-related themeswas94.2oÄ(Schredl,1998). Participants Overall, a representativesample of 1,380 personswas drawn from German householdsthat includepersonsover 14 yearsold. The samplesizewas reduced to 1,033 (550 women, 483 men) becauseof dropouts due to the following reasons:"not available for the interview" (1V: 180), refusing to participate (N: 145),and other reasons(N :22). The responserate was 74.90Ä.The mean age of the samplewas 47.9 years(^SD: 18.3). Procedure The study was carriedout by IpsosGmbH, Mölln, Germany.The participants were contactedat home and interviewedconcerninga variety of topics (attitudes toward chocolate,familiarity of specificproducts,amountof mediaconsumption, etc.). The typed dreamreportswere edited in order to remove all information not relatedto the dreamexperience(cf. Schredl,1999).Then the dreamreportswere rated along the scalesdescribedin the dreamcontentanalysissection.Statistical procedureswere carriedout with the SAS 9.1 softwarepackagefor Windows. R E SU L T S Overall, 36.8% of the participantsreporteda most recentdreamto the interviewer. The percentageof personsreportinga dreamdeclinedwith age whereas variableshad no effect (see Table 1). Participants the other socio-demographic recall reported with high dream a drcam more often than pcrsons with low dream recall, the correlation between dream recall frequency and reporting was s ign i fi c a n(r: t .4 8 J ,p < .0 0 0 1 ;y ' /: 933). The meanage a dre a m(Ye s A r lo) (214 women, 166rnen)reporlinga dreamwas46.4 years of the 380 participants (sD: 17.8). For 322 dreamreports,thc participantsstatedwhcn thc dream occurred.The answerswere classifiedinto threecategories57.8oÄof the dreamswere not older than 14 days. 33.9% of the dreamswere drcamt between 14 days and one year and only 8.4oÄof the dreamsoccurredrlorc than one year before the interr'ziew, prior to the interview. None of the socio-demographicvariables(age. gender, maritalstatus,educationallevel, income,residencelocationsize)had an effect on the time intervalbetweentelling the dreamandthe actualoccuffenceof the dream. The mean length of the 380 dream reportsamountedro23.1 words (.SDl4). Therewasno genderdifference(cf. Table2, women:23.5ISD I5.3] vs. men 22.5 variableswere also not associatedwith ISD 14.2]).The other socio-demographic dreamreportlength(seeTable 2). D R E A MC ON TE N TS TUI D4. Y for DreamReportVariable(Yes/No) Table1. LogisticRegression (N : 1033) Variable Standardized estimate Stat.test chi 2 : P: -.1035 7.5 .0062 .0541 2.3 .1327 0,4 .5146 Education .0268 -.0298 0.6 .4333 Mo nt hlyinc om e -.0337 0.7 .4168 .0468 1.7 .1970 Age Ge n der Maritalstatus Placeof residentsize scaleis depictedin Table 3. About The distributionof the realism/bizaffeness one-thirdof the dreamsincluded at leastone bizarre elementwhereasthe other two-thirds of the dream reports were realistic but to some extent implausible regarding the everyday world of the dreamer.Out of the socio-demographic variables.only gender was associatedwith dream bizaneness,that is, women reportedmore bizane dreamsthan men did (seeTable 2). The intensitiesof positive and negative dream emotions are presentedin Table 4. In 105 dreamreports(27.60Ä),the rater did not code any emotions.The predominantlypositivedreams dreamreportswere classifiedinto threecategories: (28.2oÄ),and predominantlynegativedreams(42.1%\. (29.7%),neutral/balanced The difference in percentagebetween positive and negative dreamswas significant(Sign Rank test:S : 5056,p < .0001).None of the socio-demographic variablescorrelatedwith positive or negativeemotionsexcept for the negative associationbetween educationallevel and negative dream emotions. Participantswith higher educationallevelsreportednegativelytoned dreamslessoften (se eT able2) . Men reportedwork-relateddreamsmore often than women(19.3oÄvs. 10.3%). This difference was statisticallysignificant (see Table 2) whereas the other variablesdid not affect the amountof work-relateddreams. socio-demographic In men's dreams29 of the dreamcharacterswere male and24 were female.On the other hand, 56 male dream charactersand 30 femaledream characterswere found in women's dreams.The ratio of male charactersto the sum of male and f'emalecharactersdiffered significantlybetweenthe sexes(men: 51.8% vs. w o men:65. 1o/ o,ef f e c ts i z e d :0 .2 7 7 , z : -2 .6 ,p :. 0088). In a secondstep,the analyseswas repeatedfor singlepersons(1/: 160; 60 men and 100 women) and (N:220; 106men, 114 women).For men in for personswith stablepartnerships 42 / SCHREDLANDKELLER 2P (ötr ** OF TE EE 6.N pp LocDo)o@o) tf)!Otol"-!OO oooo)tof.. oc\toooo rt'l T(6 +o > - a. Y o (/)(6 o.= x0) O @ cf) v0) q ON o .>ö (s(g O) rC oc z6 a (ü '= (/)g tl 3 a (d c c.) P c o O L (d { ooCf)(oNot O(oO(or@ Nf..-NrOCr) q q '. '. q I o) ^.= - F oo) Ec oo) -.N Yo 'ö* sfO)F-(9Ocr) o)(o@cDo)f.OrrOtOCr) rqqqqq ttl ots r(E c) a o o L rl a c .9 t a ' E iÄa öq) a a o) ö E =N o) c) E o .9. o) o NY i nE1 6 _ c\J c) Fa =0) CD O c (Ü a o) - J O(l) -N F NI^FOC.)N toiXocrrrFO) X C\I C\I q=qqqq 'ttl o)CD(orCDN Olc9(osf@c\ O$ONr- '. q ': q q q xp >(s >E c Htn ': (>6 ( 6 (g a O(E q) .N @ -'a 11 - a 0 a) .9 Vo) ^9 - n\ v J ^ n co L *.= L o _o .g (o sFsI rr) o) qE L a J ) Y o=?i7(D oivii=o 8,38;E (u o-(L B *ß D R E A M C O N T E N T S T U D IY 4 3 Table 3. Dream Bizarreness (N = 380 Dream Reports) Women (N:214) Category Men (N: 166) Total (N: 380) Resembleordinaryeveryday experiences 37.4"/" 50.o% 42s% Dreamactionis possiblein waking life but extraordinaryfor the dreamer 3 6 .9 % 27.7% 32.9% element One bizarre(impossible) withinthe dream 25.2"/" 223% 23.9"/o 0.0/" 0.37" Two or more bizarreelements withinthe dream 05% Table4. Positiveand NegativeDream Emotions(N : 380 DreamReports) Category Positive emotions Negative emotions No n e 69.7% 57j % Mitd 1 4 .5 % 13.4% Moderate 12.6% 21.6/" 3.2V" 7.9% Strong a paftncrshipthe rnale/femalepercentamountedto 47.2oÄ;for women living in a partnershipit amountedto 63.4%. This differencewas significant (effect size: -0.327,z: 2.4,p: .0153).For singlepcrsons,no significantdifferenccfor men (60.0%) and women (66.7%) was found for the male/femalepercentof dream characters.Interestingly,a significantdifferencein the ratio of male and female dreamcharacterswas presentwhen women that were older than 40 (1/ - 49) were comparedwith women youngerthan 40 (N : 5 I ). The ratio was balancedfor the youngerwomen (47.1%)but not for the oldcr wornen(78.6%).The difference ( d: -0 .6 6 7 ,z : -3 .3 ,p : .0 0 0 9 ). was s ignif ic ant Forty percent of all aggrcssiveinteractionswere physical in men's dreams whereas only 27.8oÄof the aggressiveinteractionsin women's dreamswere 44 / SCHREDLANDKELLER physical.This differenceis statisticallysignificant(effectsize:d : 0.214 z : ) 5 p: .0124). DISCUSSION The findings of the presentstudy indicatethat the effect of socio-demographic like dream length,dreambizarreness, variableson generaldreamcharacteristics and dream emotions is very small. On the other hand, gender differenceswere found for specificdreamtopics(e.9.,morephysicalaggressionin men'sdreams). In comparisonto the Kramer et al. (197l) study,the reportingrate was much lower (36.8% vs.60.7oÄ).This might be explainedby the topicsof the interviews which were relatedto health in the Kramer et al. study whereasin the present study a wide rangeof topicsnot remotelyrelatedto sleepor dreamswas elicited. Thus, by talking about dream-relatedissues in the Kramer et al. study, the participantsmight have beenprimed, and the recall of most recentdreamsmight have been facilitated.On the other hand, Kramer et al. ( 197l) did not reporl the percentageof personswho refusedto participate.In the presentsample,25.IoÄof the personsin the original random samplewere not intervieweddue to different reasons(not availablefor the interview,refusing to participate).Similar figures werc not reportedby Kramer et al. (1911). As expected,reporting a dream was strongly correlatedwith dream recall frequency.Similarto the Kramer et al. study,dreamreportingdecreascdwith age. In order to include more dreamsof low dream recallers.other dreamcollection methods with higher reporting rates would be necessary.Schredl (2002), for example,reportedthat low dreamrecallersincreasedtheir ability to recall dreams drasticallyby keepinga dreamdiary. Sirnilarly,Goodenoughet al. (1959)found that 460Äof the REM awakeningsin the sleeplaboratoryyieldcd a drcam report in personswith low dream recall at home. This figure is much lower than in high dream recallers (93%) but it was a considerablcincreasecompared to home dreamrecall which was about once a month. Using laboratorydreamsfor sftrdyingrepresentativesamplesis difficult becausedream content is affected by this measurementmethod. The overview of Schredl (2008) showcd that 19.4% of the laboratorydreamsincluded laboratoryelements(staff, electrodes, etc.). This hgure increasedto 38.4oÄif indirect references(c.g., participatingin an experiment)were also analyzed.Thus, laboratorydreamsdo not mirror the ordinarydreamsof peoplein their everydaysetting. Whether the interview settinghad an effect on the report rate or on the reported drcam content (cf. Cartwright & Kaszniak, 1991) could not be determinedby the presentdata. It would be interestingto carry out a represcntativestudy with questionnairespostedto the participantsto investigatewhether the anonymity of this settinghas an effect on reportrate and dreamcontent. Over 90oÄof the dream reports occurredduring the last year, older dreams, for examplc,childhooddreamswere extremelyrare.Kramer et al. (1971) did not DR E A MC ON TE N TS TUI D45 Y report the time intervalsbetweentelling the dreamand its occuffence.The higher ratio of negativeto positive dreamsin the Kramer et al. study might be explained variby including more childhood dreams.The investigatedsocio-demographic ables were not associatedwith the time interval between dream reporting and the occurrenceof dreams; thus, the findings regarding the effect of sociodemographicvariableson dream content were not affectedby this variable. The meanlength of the dreamreportswas comparableto the findings of Kramer et al. ( 197l). The interview dreamsare much shorterthan diary dreamsin young adults (136 + lll.2 words; Schredl, 2004b) or elderly persons(41.2 + 43.4; Schredl,Schroder,& Löw, 1996). Thus, the diary method might be useful to obtain more elaboratedream material.On the other hand. dream bizarrenessis comparableto diary dreams:about 25oÄof the presentdreamsincluded at least one bizarre element(versus3l%; N : 365 diary dreams;Schredl, 1999; l9%; N: 500 laboratorydreams;Strauch& Meier, 1996).The genderdifferencewas small and not in accordancewith Schredl,Sahin, and Schäfer(1998) who did not find a difference between the sexes.Thus, the presentfinding needs further replication. Regarding dream emotions, the representativesample of the most recent of negativedreams.This might be dreamsindicatethat there is a preponderance explainedby recall bias becauseemotionalquality is balancedin diary dreams (Schredl& Doll, l99B) and laboratorydreams(Strauch& Meier, 1996).It might be that more negatively toned dreamsare more easily recalled-even after long periodsof tirne (cf. Schredl,1999). Marked genderdifferenceswere found for work-related drearns,ratio of male and fernaledreamcharacters,and physicalaggression.The percentageof workrelated dreams in the present study (14.2%) were comparableto the figure (11.3%) reportedby Kramer et al. (197l). Confirming the resultsof Schredland Piel (2005), men reportedwork-relateddrearnsmore often than women. This is in line with the continuityhypothesisof dreaming(cf-.Schredl,2003)becausethe employmentrate is higherin men comparedto women (64.9% [men] vs.45.60Ä Bundesamt, fwornen]:personsolder than 20 years;Mikrozensus,Statistisches Bonn,Gennany;cf-.Schredl& Piel, 2005). hr the total sample, women's dream showed a higher percentageof male dream charactersthan rnen's dreams; a finding never reported before (cf. Domhoff, 1996;Hall. 1984;Hall & Dornhoff,1963).However,if the percentages of male and t-emaledream charactersfor the dreams of young singles and the figures were computedseparately, the dreamsof personswith relationships resernblethe findingsof Schredl(2001).This authorreportedthe "ubiquitous" gender differences for singles (preponderanceof male dream charactersin men's dreams and a balanced ratio of the dream characters'gender in women'sdreams)but a differentsituationfor personswith stablepartnerships: 62.3'%rnaledreamcharacters werc fbund in women's dreamsand in 48.7nÄin men'sdrearns. 4 6 / SC HRE DLA NDK E L L E R The major difference betweenthe presentstudy and the results of the student sample(Schredl,Sahin,and Schäfer,1998)is the high percentageof male dream charactersin dreamsof women older than 40 yearsof age.Basedon the findings that the amount of time spent with the different sexesin waking life is colrelated with the male/femalepercentof dreamcharacters(Schredl& Jacob,1998; Schredl,LoBnitzer,& Vetter, 1998),one might speculatethat singlewomen over 40 spenda lot of time with males(friends,colleagues).Another line of thinking might draw on the findings of Strauch and Meier ( 1996) that waking thoughts affect dream contentas well and some older women who were widowed might have strong memories of their former husbands and, thus, dream more often about men. The present study confirmed findings in student samples reported earlier (Domhoff, 1996;Hall & Van de Castle,1966;Schredl,Sahin,& Schäfer,1998) that men dream more often about physical aggression than women. This might reflect a higher pronenessto violence in men (cf. Schredl, Sahin, & Schäfer,1998). Overall, the findings that almost none of the socio-demographicvariables like age, gender,marital status,education,income, and residencelocation size was significantly related to general dream characteristicslike dream length, and intensityof dreamemotionsindicatethat dreaming,asa universal bizarreness, phenomenonshared by all humans, is experiencedin similar ways. On the other hand, dream content-in contrastto the general dream characteristics-is determinedby waking-life experiences,this being clearly shown by the gender differences found in the present study. In order to generalizethese findings, it will be necessaryto apply other dream collection methodslike dream diary or laboratory awakenings in large, representativesamples in order to obtain and analyzedream reports of personswho do not often recall their dreamsand who are not able to report a most recent dream to an interviewer. REFERENCES Cartwright,R. D., & Kaszniak,A. (1991). The social psychologyof dream reporting.ln S. J. Ellman & J. S. Antrobus (Eds.), The mind in sleep-Psyc:hologyand psychophysiology(pp.25l-264).New York: JohnWiley. Domhoff, G. W. (1996).Finding meaningin dreams:A quantitativeapproacft.New York: PlenumPress. Goodenough,D. R., Shapiro,A., Holden, M., & Steinschriber,L. (1959).A comparison of "dreamers" and "nondreamers."Journal o/'Abnormal and Social Psychology',59, 295-302. Hall, C. S. (1984). "A ubiquitous sex differencein dreams" revisited.Journal of PerPsychology,46, 1109-1117. sonalityand Soc:ial Hall, C. S., & Domhofl B. J. (1963). A ubiquitoussex differencein dreams.Journal of' Abnormal and SocialPsychologl',66,278-280. D R E A M C O N T E N T S T U DIY 4 7 Hall, C. S.,Domhoff,G. W., Blick, K. A., & Weesner,K. E. (1982).The dreamsof college men and women in 1959 and 1980: A comparison of dream contents and sex differences. Sleep,-5,188-194. Hall, C. S., & Van de Castle,R. L. (1966). The contentanalysisofdreams. New York: App I eton-C entury-Cro ft s. Kramer, M., Winget, C., & Whitman, R. M. (1971).A city dream:A surveyapproachto normativedreamcontent.AmericanJournal of Psyc:hiatry,127, i350-1356. Krippner, S., Winkler, M., Rochlen, A., & Yashar, B. (1998). Gender, national, and regional differences in a content analysis of 199 dream reports from research participants in Argentina, Brazil, and the United States.Interamerican Journal of' Psychology, 32,71-19. Rubinstein,K., & Krippner, S. (1991). Genderdifferencesand geographicaldifferences in content from dreamselicited by a television announcement.International Journal o/'Psychosomatics,38, 40-44. Schredl, M. (1998). Trciumeund Sc:hlafitörungen:Empirische Studie zur Traumerinnerungshciu/igkeitund zum Trauminhalt vonschlafgestörtenPatientlnnen.Marburg: Tectum. Schredl, M. (1999). Die nächtliche Traumwelt: Ein/ührung in die psychologische 'o Tr aum.f r schung. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. Schredl,M. (2001). Dreams of singles:Effects of waking-life social contactson dream content.Personalitl' and Individual Di//brences,3 I, 269-275. Schredl, M. (2002). Questionnaires and diaries as research instruments in dream research:Methodologicalissues. Dreaming, I 2, 17-26. Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematicalmodel. Sleepand Hypnosrs,5, 38-52. Schredl,M. (2004a).Reliability and stabilityof a dreamrecall frequencyscale.Perceptual and Motor Skills,98, 1422-1426. Schredl,M. (2004b).Length of diary dreams:Single dreamsvs. two or more dreamsper 6, 121-124. morning.North AmericanJournal o/'Ps.ltchology, Schredl, M. (2008). Laboratory referencesin dreams: Methodological problem and/or evidencefor the continuity hypothesisof dreaming'l International Journal of'Dream Research,I, 3-6. Schredl,M., Burchefr, N., & Grabatin, Y. (2004). The effect of training on intemater reliability in dream content analysis.Sleepand Hypnosis, 6, 139-144. and Cognition, Schredl,M., & Doll, E. (1998).Emotionsin diary dreams.Consciousness 7.634-646. Schredl.M., & Jacob,S. (1998).Ratio of male and female charactersin a dream series. Perceptual and Motor Skills,86, 198-200. Schredl.M.. LoBnitzer,T., & Vetter, S. (1998). ls the ratio of male and female dream charactersrelatedto the waking-life patternof social contacts?Perceptual and Motor Skills,87, 513-514. Schredl,M.. & Piel. E. (2005). Gender difTerencesin dreaming:Are they stable over time? Personaliry^and Individual Diflbrences,39, 309-316. Schredl,M., Sahin,V., & Schäfer.G. ( 1998).Genderdifferencesin dreams:Do they reflect genderdifferencesin waking-ltfe?Personalityand Individual Di/fbrences,25, 433-442. Schredl,M.. Schröder,A., & Löw. H. (1996). Traumerlebenvon älteren Menschenund Terl 2: Empirische Studie und Diskussion. Zeitschri/i./ür Gerontops-vchologie -psychiatrie, 9, 43-53. 48 / SCHRE DLA NDK EL L E R dream Resultsof experimentql Strauch,1., & Meier, B. (1996).In searchoJ-dreams: Albany:StateUniversityof New York Press. research. Canadian Winget,C., Kramer,M., & Whitman,R. M. (1912).Dreamsanddemography. Journal,I 7, 203-208. PsychiatricAssociation Direct reprintrequeststo: M. Schredl,Ph.D. Schlaflabor, Zentralinstitutfür SeelischeGesundheit Postfach12 21 20 68072 Mannheim, Germany e-mail: Michael.Schredl(d,zi-mannheim.de
© Copyright 2024