Hearing Voices? Do citizens value voice as much as authority? Fred Cutler, Paul Quirk, Ben Nyblade, and Eric Merkley Prepared for MEDW meeting, Paris, May 23 2015 WARNING: THIS PAPER PRESENTS NULL EXPERIMENTAL RESULTS The authors bear no responsibility for reader disappointment of the “this won’t get published so why bother writing it up?” variety. INTRODUCTION Citizens now have high expectations for the democratic system in which they live. Scholarship on the so-called “democratic deficit” has shown that the many features of democratic systems – institutional and cultural – mean that citizens will always see shortcomings in their own system and hope for improvement. Citizens’ judgments of the quality of their democracy are based in these multiple, at least theoretically independent, and sometimes conflicting considerations. Macrooriented scholars have enumerated many of these features (Diamond 2005) and macromeasurement at the country level is now very rich (Bühlmann, Merkel, Müller, and Weẞels 2011). At the individual level, however, the focus has been on the influence of institutional and contextual factors conditioned by one dominant individual level attribute: supporting a party or candidate that wins or one that ends up on the losing side. In this paper we examine the role of what might be considered the opposite of winning and losing, or more broadly the relationship between a citizen and political authority. We use an experimental design to examine the impact of merely hearing one’s political voice, or not, in an election campaign. Our experiments provide very naturalistic manipulations of hearing or not hearing political “voices” about an issue, potentially salient for many voters, in an election campaign. Our previous experiments showed that the voices present in campaign discourse can affect feelings of efficacy and satisfaction even in a lab setting where the election campaign consists of 10 newspaper articles from a provincial election away from subjects’ own province. We carry forward that inquiry by manipulating the issues discussed in the campaign and whether those issues are discussed by a 2 niche party or by civil society groups. In this paper we only describe the design, summarize the results, and explain the direction for continuation of this experimental inquiry. With a small subject pool and weak treatments we found no effects to speak of in this round of experiments. We conclude this report by suggesting changes to the design that may better reveal “voice” effects if they exist. THEORY We focus on differentiating two key mechanisms through which the context of elections and political discourse influence voter efficacy and ultimately, perhaps, their judgments of the quality of their democracy. These are Voice and Authority. The first mechanism – the one that produces the famous “winner-loser gap” – involves citizens’ concern with authority and reflects a conventional notion of representation in government: citizens are satisfied and feel efficacious to the extent that electoral processes and outcomes provide indication that political actors who share their policy preferences will have authority in the resulting government. The simplest way this happens is when one chooses a party or representative who ends up on the government side of the legislature and executive. As Anderson writes, electoral systems shape representation by “producing systematically different kinds of governments.” The now rather extensive literature linking winning and losing to citizens satisfaction, accordingly, mostly seeks to demonstrate that winners are more satisfied than losers and that losers are more satisfied in consensus systems; and it explains these effects mainly as a result of distances between citizens’ and governments’ policy positions. The empirical finding that voting for the winning side promotes short-term satisfaction with democracy no doubt reflects the simple fact that voters want government to serve their interests. Losers are the flip-side but their responses may be more complicated: their degree of short-term dissatisfaction may depend on their perceptions of the distance between their preferences and government policy, their party’s 3 chances of holding office in the future, and the possibility that their views will have influence even though their party is not in government.1 However, if authority and its policy outputs were all that mattered to citizens, many institutional features of democracies in and of themselves would not directly affect satisfaction with democracy. And many normative theorists and social commentators would be out of a job, since they advocate discourse-based solutions to the democratic deficit that seek to change the relative power of discursive actors and the content that makes up the totality of political discourse in a polity. Only those institutions that affected the composition and duration of governments would affect satisfaction (and perhaps indirectly citizens’ relationships with other actors who may be in a position to influence governments). While some of the interpretation of the winner-loser gap, or what might be called the citizen-government authority effect, involves the articulation of voters’ concerns, winning and losing is theoretically distinguishable from the character of the voices that effectively articulate different priorities and positions in a polity, especially during election campaigns. Thus a second mechanism connecting political contexts to satisfaction, largely overlooked in previous empirical literature, involves citizens’ concern with voice and reflects the idea of democracy as a dialogue that promotes intelligence and mutual regard and confers legitimacy. [Do we need a paragraph on normative and deliberative democratic theory here?] We claim that independent of citizens’ relationship with authority, they are more satisfied and feel more efficacious to the extent that electoral processes give voice to their views. In short, they want to be heard. Such voice ensures that their views have received consideration, and promises that their 1 Curini, Luigi, Willy Jou, and Vincenzo Memoli. "Satisfaction with Democracy and the Winner/Loser Debate: The Role of Policy Preferences and Past Experience." British Journal of Political Science 42.02 (2012): 241-261. 4 views will be incorporated in future discussions—even if political actors who hold the same preferences will have no significant or reliable authority in the resulting government. To be sure, some scholars have suggested that voice may matter, but still typically combine it with authority. Most prominently, the seminal piece in the satisfaction literature, Anderson and Guillory, argued that the effects they observed were a result of opportunities for voice and policymaking together: “Given that consensual systems provide the political minority with a voice in the decision-making process, we expect that the more consensual the set of political institutions in a country, the greater is the extent to which negative consequences of losing elections are muted.”2 Note that here voice is not mere voice, independent of authority, but rather a voice in authoritative decisions. However, we believe that regardless of authority, voters are concerned with having their voice heard. That is, mere voice is a distinct component of feelings of represented-ness and efficacy that are central to citizens’ judgements of democracy. Such voice is not, of course, literally the citizen’s direct expression of her own views. Some voters may never state their views aloud, much less in any public forum. Rather, it is the form of voice feasible for most citizens in a mass, representative, party democracy: one hears one’s own political voice articulated by significant political actors, principally through mediated channels. Consistent with this conceptualization of voice, in the most recent, comprehensive work in this area, Anderson and colleagues3 and Ezrow and Xezonakis4 point to the importance of opportunities for voice for losers, both electoral and representational. Anderson (2012) writes that: “a more numerous and differentiated [electoral] 2 Anderson and Guillory. ‘Political institutions and satisfaction with democracy.’ 3 Anderson et al. Losers' Consent: Elections and Democratic Legitimacy. 4 Ezrow and Xezonakis. ‘Citizen Satisfaction with Democracy and Parties’ Policy Offerings.’ 5 supply reduces the negative impact losing has on system attitudes because it provides the next best thing to winning outright: having one’s political voice articulated clearly and visibly”. 5 We suggest, then, that citizens will have more positive views of their democracy if their views are voiced by one or more of the parties than if they are not, and that this effect will occur even if the parties voicing their views end up shut out of political authority. There is indirect evidence for this in Aarts and Thomasson’s finding that citizens’ views on the quality of representation are a more powerful determinant of democractic satisfaction than their views about whether voting “makes a difference”.6 Ezrow and Xezonakis also suggest that voice is important even in the absence of authority.7 For them, satisfaction depends on more accurate “policy representation” and yet they mean by this not representation ultimately through authority, but rather by voice. According to these authors, “diversity of party alternatives” is positively linked to democratic satisfaction because parties “voice citizen demands for policy”.8 They quote Sartori to the effect that: “Parties are channels of expression… They are an instrument, or an agency, for representing the people by expressing their demands.”9 Our experiments speak specifically to these propositions. We share the view that regardless of which party ultimately forms the government, citizens should prefer a democratic system and culture where their issue priorities and positions are articulated by prominent political actors such as parties, especially during an election campaign. It is hard to imagine a citizen feeling that she lives in a democracy in the absence of the effective expression in the public sphere of views that she shares. 5 Anderson. ‘How Electoral Systems Shape What Voters Think About Democracy.’ 6 Aarts, Kees, and Jacques Thomassen. "Satisfaction with democracy: Do institutions matter?." Electoral Studies 27.1 (2008): 5-18. 7 Ezrow and Xezonakis. ‘Citizen Satisfaction with Democracy and Parties’ Policy Offerings.’ 8 Ezrow and Xezonakis. ‘Citizen Satisfaction with Democracy and Parties’ Policy Offerings.’ P. 1153 9 Sartori, Giovanni. Sartori, Giovanni. Parties and party systems: A framework for analysis. 1976: p. 27 6 The authority and voice mechanisms have distinct observable implications and lead to scholars looking at data at different levels. The authority mechanism is simpler to analyze, with its focus on the congruence of an individual with the parties in government. Indeed, a welter of evidence bears out this hypothesis (Anderson and Guillory, Anderson et al, Curini Memoli and Jou, Bernauer and Vatter, etc.). The voice mechanism is more complex because it involves something less tangible. Voters should feel more efficacious when they hear voices articulating those voters’ concerns, but is the discussion of those concerns enough or does it depend, perhaps, on the depth of discussion, the force of opposing views, or the identity of the actors articulating those views? STUDY DESIGN The initial study reported here used computer-based laboratory experiments with undergraduate student subjects. Subjects experienced stimuli that simulated the experience of voters in election campaigns and measured several aspects of their responses. We had intended our subjects to be of two types: undergraduates enrolled in political science classes (which likely makes them a better match to the general voting public than other university students who tend to have much less political socialization and interest) and members of small-scale environmental groups. We targetted the latter so that we could assume that environmental issues were salient to the votersubject and that all else equal those people would prefer that environmental issues received a hearing in an election campaign. But we were unable to secure the timely cooperation of an environmental group and so the current paper presents only results from the student subject pool where we measure environmental concern rather than holding it constant by targetting subjects. Towards the end, we describe the research design that uses environmentalist subjects. The experimental manipulations involve the presentation three versions of an election campaign with important differences in the issue content and the identity of the actors articulating issues. Our design has two distinctive and related features that combine to provide, in our view, an 7 exceptional degree of correspondence with the real-world conditions pertinent to the subjects—a property that should have major benefits with respect to external validity. First, taking advantage of some special features of Canadian party politics, we presented campaigns that featured varying, yet plausible subsets of the same parties that the subjects engage with in real-world political life. While experiments that omit parties or use fictitious party names may tell us something about voters’ responses to policy positions, they are so far removed from the symbolic and cue-giving roles of parties that they leave us guessing about the power of analogous treatments in the real world. Using Canadian subjects then, we presented campaigns that either did or did not include the Green party and did or did not include discussion of environmental issues. Canadian party politics offer distinct conveniences for the experimenter. At both the provincial and federal level, recent elections in English-speaking Canada have included four parties: from left to right on the ideological scale, the environmentalist Greens, the social-democratic New Democrats, the centrist Liberals, and the right-leaning Conservatives. The crucial advantage of the Canadian context for our purposes, however, is that provincial party systems do not closely mirror each other or the national party system. The lists and respective identities of the active, competitive parties vary considerably across provinces, across levels within a province, and over time. In particular, many provincial elections effectively involve only two or three of the four national parties. In addition, most Canadians in a given province do not follow politics in other provinces, especially the smaller provinces, closely. Yet they are exposed to occasional news from other provinces, with enough variation in election line-ups, to make any subset of the four parties plausible as a general matter. These distinctive conditions give us a powerful research opportunity. We presented our subjects with a provincial campaign occurring at an unspecified, “recent” time in Manitoba, a 8 province with which we can assume our subjects are by-and-large unfamiliar.10 We were thus able to manipulate basic features of the party system and election discourse while keeping them, from subjects’ standpoint, completely realistic. Second, we assiduously maintained this realism in the presentation of policy positions and other information from the campaign. Many election experiments present brief, stylized, and highly simplified representations of political actors’ policy or ideological positions—making a vivid impression of their differences essentially inescapable for subjects. Although such designs have advantages, they beg the question of which features of campaign activity actually gain voters’ attention and have an impact in the campaign. In contrast, our design presents a simulated campaign through a series of full-length newspaper stories, carefully created by us—including rich detail on campaign events and rigorously realistic statements of issue positions. Specifically, the simulated campaign consists of subjects reading eleven constructed newspaper articles in a set sequence. The articles were developed by a research assistant, formerly a reporter for a big-city newspaper, by modifying actual articles from Manitoba and other provincial election campaigns to meet the requirements of the three different campaigns specified in our research design. Articles were carefully formatted to look like actual newspaper articles. (See the online Appendix for the full set of articles). Eight articles (hereafter, “policy articles”) covered party leaders’ campaign events or appearances in which party leaders, candidates, and other actors expressed policy positions on one of four issues (two articles for each issue)—work-for-welfare, the minimum wage, crime, and, depending on the condition, either the environment or the arts. Policy positions were quite specific, reflecting the style of rhetoric in the original articles (for example, on crime: building more prisons versus after-school crime prevention 10 We excluded from analysis the 2 subjects (1% per cent) who reported that they had lived in Manitoba. 9 programs). The two final campaign articles summarized the parties’ policy differences, one reporting on a party leaders’ debate, the other giving a pre-election summary of the campaign. Subjects then indicated how they would vote in this election. Finally, they received one of four randomly assigned results stories, indicating that one of the parties had won and would form either a majority or a minority government. We presented the same information—the entire news articles—to all subjects in a given condition. We did nothing to highlight or emphasize the policy content of the news stories, beyond the methods of presentation in the original articles. All policy differences between parties, across multiple issues, were designed to reflect consistent party positions on a single, left-right dimension—a largely accurate portrayal of the Canadian parties. This use of simulated campaign materials that closely resemble real media coverage has significant implications for the study. On one hand, it should promote greater engagement by subjects and lead them to respond to the simulated campaign much as they would to a real-world campaign. Thinking of the campaign as real or potentially real should lead them to care more about what happens. On the other hand, this design dilutes the influence of our treatments by exposing subjects to extensive material that is identical across treatments—namely, the ordinary, non-policy content of campaign reporting. As with typical news coverage, our campaign news stories included locations, logistics, and activities of particular events (ribbon-cuttings, barbecues, rallies); estimates of attendance; names of politicians; their declarations on non-controversial topics; and sundry remarks by ordinary citizens, interest groups, and local notables, among other things. The impact of policy positions depends, as in real-world politics, on the efforts and ability of people to notice them, amid the complexities of concrete issues and the cacophony of a campaign. Given our experimental design, our concerns in fact are the opposite of those frequently considered in political psychology—where the artificial treatments are often so strong as to have dubious 10 external validity. Here, our belief is that the simulated campaign is sufficiently realistic, with so much information distracting from the treatments, that observing effects from a simulated campaign to general political attitudes like efficacy would be strong evidence that these attitudes are indeed affected by the features of party systems. Our efforts in constructing this design were apparently successful. We demonstrate below that our subjects engaged quite enthusiastically and conscientiously with the campaign materials. In addition, we show that the policy positions in our news stories succeeded in spreading the parties fairly neatly across a left-right dimension. In all, we think there are strong grounds to suppose that treatment effects in our experiments will also occur in real-world campaigns. EXPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL Data is from completed experiments with a total of 232 undergraduate student subjects who took one of several political science courses at UBC in Vancouver, Canada and participated in experiments in exchange for course credit in 2015. Working at lab computers running Qualtrics web software, each subject completed a pre-treatment questionnaire, including demographics and general political attitudes including measures of issue concern and salience. Subjects were then given a preamble, explaining that they would read news articles about an election campaign in Manitoba, and later would be asked some questions about it, and would be able to vote for their preferred party. Figure 1 - Protocol Summary 1. Pre-treatment questionnaire 2. Subjects experience simulated Manitoba campaign: 10 articles Eight articles in random order; two at end; click to advance 3. Post-treatment questionnaire: policy, satisfaction, information. 11 Figure 2 Condition Left/Enviro Three party (3p), no Environment Party Policy Locations Right/non-Enviro NDP Liberal Conservative Three party, Environment with Environmental Groups (3pA) Interest Groups NDP Liberal Conservative Four party, Environment with Green Party (4p) Green NDP Liberal Conservative Subjects read a series of news articles corresponding to their randomly assigned campaign. (The articles can be viewed in the online appendix.) A critical feature of the development of these materials was the formulation of policy positions reflecting the appropriate ideological locations of the parties for the given treatment. For example, in the four-party treatment, each article needed an extreme left (Green Party), centre-left (New Democrat), centre-right (Liberal), and extreme right (Conservative) policy position. Depending on the condition to which a subject is randomly assigned, we present one of three kinds of campaigns: 1. a four-party campaign (4p) with the Conservative, Liberal, NDP, and Green parties taking policy positions that correspond to their real-world locations (see below for validation) 12 2. a three-party campaign with only the Conservatives, Liberals, and NDP but with various interest groups, especially environmental ones, expressing the same points of view – usually in the same language – as the Green party in the four-party campaign 3. a three-party campaign (3p) where environmental issues are not discussed and the two explicitly environmental articles are replaced with two articles on the issue of support for culture and the arts (For a schematic representation of the three campaign conditions, see Figure 2, above). Figure 3 - Example Campaign Article A complication in constructing the newspaper stories was that going from three to four parties forced us to choose among three options: making the articles longer, reducing the amount of information on policy for each party, or devoting a larger proportion of the space to policy and less 13 to other aspects of the campaign (logistics of events, and the like, as noted above). In the end we opted mostly for larger proportion of non-policy content in the three-party articles. After reading the articles and voting, subjects completed a post-treatment questionnaire in which we measured our dependent variables and measured attention to the campaign and the success of our manipulations. Subjects were assigned to one of four election results stories in either a threeor a four-party version corresponding to their campaign condition. These four results were a Liberal majority, Liberal minority, NDP majority, and NDP minority. In the minority cases, the articles emphasized that the government would have to cooperate with other parties, while in the majority cases the article stated that the government could ignore other parties. Although we have collected dependent measures for a wide variety of potential effects (e.g. subjects’ own policy positions, satisfaction and efficacy, information, and correct voting), the current paper looks only at satisfaction with the election and a measure of voice that has been used for decades as the central survey measure of “external efficacy”. Subjects also completed a battery of information items about events and positions in the campaign. MANIPULATION CHECKS To generalize from our findings to the effects of party systems in real-world election campaigns, we must have confidence that subjects attended to the series of news articles in a manner broadly comparable to that of actual voters in real elections. We must check that our manipulations created treatments. Our key manipulation is an attempt to treat one set of subjects with a 3-party campaign with no discussion of the environment and instead a discussion of funding for arts and culture. After the campaign we presented subjects with a screen showing 16 issues and asked them to click the issue if it was discussed in the campaign. By this measure, the manipulations were extremely successful. 14 For all issues that were not varied by condition there are no significant differences across a comparison of the 3p vs 3pA and 4p conditions. For Arts and Culture, 88% of those in the 3p condition said it was discussed, as compared with just 1% in the environment conditions. In the environment conditions, by contrast, 91% said the environment was discussed, as compared with 24% in the non-environment condition. For climate change the difference was 61% to 8%. Based on these variables, we defined a variable indicating successful treatment, which identified 88% as being correctly treated. Second, we used two screener questions as recommended by Berinksy and his colleagues (2014). These were long-winded questions asking the respondent to ignore the actual question that appeared at the end. Three-quarters of subjects passed both tests, and only 5% failed both. We conducted analyses both including and excluding the subjects who failed either and both but found no differences in our conclusions. Third, we wanted to know if subjects had noticed the election result and particularly the majority or minority results. We asked at the end how much influence each party would have on policies until the next election, measured on a 1-4 scale. The mean Liberal influence across the Liberal majority, Liberal minority, NDP minority, and NDP majority conditions was 3.4, 3.1, 2.5, and 2.3 and almost exactly the reverse for the NDP, with the Conservatives having more influence in both minority conditions, and the Greens estimated to have much more influence in the NDP minority condition than any other. Clearly, these student subjects were in study mode and noticed, in the last of ten full-length newspaper articles, which party had won. And that brings us to pointing out that subjects engaged at a gut level with the campaign, as a way of showing that they took the treatment of the campaign in general terms. We asked subjects: “How do you think you would feel if you had been through this campaign and gone out and voted for real?”, 14% said “very satified”, 51% “somewhat satisfied”, 31% “somewhat dissatisfied”, and 9% 15 “very dissatisfied”. Among the “winners” – those who voted for or identified with the party that they saw having won the election – only 14% were on the dissatisfied side, while among losers fully 51% were dissatisfied. We are confident that the subjects took the treatment. VALIDATION OF POLICY POSITIONS Figure 4 Mean Party Positions Centred by Article -- Total of Both Questions Crime1 Crime2 Env1 Env2 MW1 MW2 Workfare1 Workfare2 Debate Summary -4 -2 Green 0 NDP Liberal 2 4 Conservative Ratings by New Subjects with Fake Party Names For the sake of internal validity and our expectations of treatment effects, we also must have confidence that our articles are realistic in terms of party platforms and ideological positions. To assess perceived policy positions, we conducted a calibration study on a separate set of subjects and a slightly modified version of the articles. These subjects read our eight main four-party campaign articles with the party names replaced by uninformative, fake names. Subjects were asked to read one of the articles to determine the position of just one of the fake-name parties on the policy issue covered in the article. For the debate and campaign summary articles, subjects were asked to place the fake-name parties on the zero-to-ten left-right ideology scale. Subjects in this study read either 16 four or five articles, identifying the position of a different party each time. By aggregating over subjects, we derived average positions of each fake party—then translated back to the real parties—on each of the ten articles. Results from this study reassure us that the campaign news is highly naturalistic in terms of the party system.11 Figure 3 shows the mean placement of each party, centred for each article. There is some variation in the average placements of parties across the 10 articles, but in total the parties’ positions are arrayed as we expected from the Greens’ policies on the left to the Conservatives’ policies on the Right. For example, the policy positions in the end-ofcampaign summary article were placed at 3.7 (Greens), 4.6 (NDP), 5.2 (Liberals), and 6.4 (Conservatives). Not surprisingly, then, after the campaign when subjects were asked to place the parties on the same left-right scale, the means were 2.97 (Greens), 3.52 (NDP), 4.75 (Liberals), and 6.95 (Conservatives) with distributions depicted in Figure 5. Figure 5 – Subjects’ Placement of Parties on Left-Right Scale Parties Placed on Left-Right Ideology Scale by Subjects 0 2 Green 4 6 Left - Right Ideology NDP Liberal 8 10 Conservative Kernel Density Plots, bandwith 0.8 11 See also the discussion in Lau and Redlawsk , How Voters Decide: Information Processing in Election Campaigns. 17 Unfortunately this global success in creating a realistic campaign obscures the fact that the environment articles in particular do not discriminate the parties very well. We will return to this problem after presenting the results. Summary This experiment exposed subjects, in a crossed randomization, to three different campaigns and four different campaign results. Environmental issues are discussed in the campaign either not at all, by three traditional parties plus interest groups, or by four parties including the Greens. The government formed after the election is either a Liberal or NDP majority that is described as being able to work on its own policy priorities without consultation, or a Liberal or NDP minority that will require collaboration with other parties. HYPOTHESES We express our hypotheses and expectations for treatment effects for these two manipulations as independent, though there is always the possibility that the effects will be interactive. Our dependent variables measure the degree to which subjects think their voice is heard in politics, how well represented they feel, whether politics in Manitoba would be addressing their concerns, and their general satisfaction with democracy in Canada. Our expectations across all these variables are, in a nutshell, that when subjects hear “their issues” discussed, they will be more positive about democracy and representation than when their issues are not discussed. H1: Feelings of Voice and Satisfaction will be increasing among environmentally-conscious subjects across the three conditions: 3p, 3pA, 4p; and decreasing or constant across these conditions for subjects who do not think the environment is an important issue. 18 H1a: The effect in H1 distinguishing 3pA from 4p conditions will be conditional on feelings toward the Green party. H1b: If there are enough subjects who think Arts and Culture are important, their feelings of voice should be marginally higher in the 3p condition than the other two conditions. H2: Feelings of satisfaction but not voice will be higher among “winners” than “losers” and the effect will be stronger or only visible when the winning party forms a majority. RESULTS In short, we found no treatment effects of the three campaigns (3p, 3pA, 4p) whatsoever. Our analysis looked for conditional effects moderated by partisanship, feelings about the Green party, attitudes on greenhouse gas policy, pre-treatment feelings of representedness, minutes in the experiment, correctness on policy and issue recall questions, and general political attention and information. And we looked for an Arts and Culture effect as well (H1b). Nothing. There was certainly no treatment effect on the voice measure, as we had found robustly in previous experiments where subjects experienced two-party or four-party campaigns with different degrees of policy extremity. We do not think that the measurement of voice is particularly problematic, as we have construct validity from observing weaker feelings of voice in politics among those at either end of the self-reported left-right scale, as seen in Figure 6. 19 Figure 6 – Voice by Ideology 1 0 .5 Voice 1.5 2 Voice by Ideology 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Subject Self-Report Left-Right 8 9 10 n=212 But… we did find an interesting effect of subject behaviour in the experiment on the measures of voice and, although weaker, on the measures asking if Manitoba politics would be addressing the subjects’ issues as well as satisfaction with te campaign. It involves a time-in-campaign effect among losers of the election. We admit that this sounds a bit bizarre, but the effect is clear in the data: Among those whose preferred party did not win the election, the longer the respondent spent in the lab reading the campaign materials, the more likely she is to disagree with the statement “people like me have no say in politics”; while among those who ‘won’ the election, there is no such effect. We depict this in the left panel of Figure 6. And this holds even when we control for pretreatment measures of interest, knowledge, and representedness. Now, we admit that this might be the one significant interactive relationship among the many we examined. And it is not a true treatment effect, although we would point out that the minutes in the campaign is prior to the measurement of voice. But the fact that this is theoretically expected in line with our previous experiments’ findings and that the same relationship holds, although more weakly for the 20 Important Issues and Satisfaction-with-the-Campaign variables, gives us confidence that the effect is real. Further work is required to determine whether these results might well be produced by learning about the richness of election campaign discourse. It is suggestive that this effect is twice as strong among those with lower-than-median knowledge and twice as strong among those who could recall the issues discussed in the campaign more accurately. And even more suggestive that there is a robust positive relationship between how much the subject took in during the campaign (measured by party promise questions) and both Voice and Important Issues, even controlling for prior political knowledge and feelings of representedness (see Figure 6). A subsequent experiment could manipulate the length of time reading to establish the causal effect more confidently. Concerning the winner-loser hypothesis (H2), although we observed a winner-loser gap in subjects’ imagined response to the campaign, there is no gap whatsoever in overall democratic satisfaction from winning and losing this election. So although subjects could imagine themselves satisfied or dissatisfied by the experience of the campaign, the experimental campaign did not affect their Figure 7 – Voice by Engagement in Experiment Voice by Minutes in Experiment 10 20 30 40 Minutes in Experiment winner=0 50 60 1 1 1.5 1.5 Voice Voice 2 2 2.5 2.5 Voice by Recall of Party Promises 0 winner=1 20 40 60 Grade on Promise Recall Test 80 n=174, controlling for winner, knowledge, representedness, minutes in exp 21 100 overall satisfaction, just as we had found in previous experiments. NEXT STEPS FOR THE RESEARCH DESIGN Obviously, a failure to find the predicted effect in a small student sample with relatively positive feelings about their voice and their democracy is no reason to immediately abandon the theory. Indeed, we had intended to test the theory in a more targetted way by running the experiment on a captive audience of members of environmental organizations. The logic was that because we could not easily design campaigns with and without each subject’s particular salient issues and then randomize these after a pre-treatment query about issue concern, instead we would create an environment and no-environment campaign and test for effects on voice and satisfaction among people who we were certain cared deeply about the environment and would have good reason to think that environmental concerns were not adequately represented in normal political and especially election discourse. Our next step is to carry out an experiment in this fashion. And indeed we could do so on the other side of the left-right or old-new politics continuums by creating a campaign that included articles on loosening regulation of business or on de-nationalizing industries, or whatever. But we suspect another reason why we found no treatment effects. On reflection, we admit that the treatments are very weak in the sense that they only minimally discussed the big environmental issues and the depth of the discourse was likely rather disappointing to real environmentalists. The discussion included Kyoto carbon emissions targets but also made mention of lake pollution, tax rebates for fuel-efficient vehicles, parkland, and public transit. In the two conditions with the environment discussed, the discourse could be said to be rather scattered and not particularly encouraging for environmentalists to hear. As we saw above in figure 3, our calibration study found that in one of the environment stories there is little perceived policy differentiation among the four parties. In both, the Green party plays a rather acerbic, oppositional role rather than 22 sounding like it owns the issue and would show real leadership if it played a role in a government. Therefore, in subsequent experiments we will create more sophisticated, positive discussions of the environment and use them, as well as the existing ones (randomized), as treatments. We should be able to do this with the power available to us with a pool of 600-1000 environmentalist subjects. CONCLUSION It is possible, nevertheless, that citizens’ attitudes about their voice and their democracy are not affected by the discussion or lack of it, around issues about which citizens’ are concerned. It may be that only a small subset of disaffected non-partisans are susceptible to this kind of effect. Partisans of the major parties, in places like Canada anyway, are used to their voices being heard as their party forms the government at either the federal or provincial levels at least once in a while. And many citizens are satisfied enough that their voices are heard, so an experimental treatment like this is unlikely to have an effect on them, much like consensus democracy is thought to have a positive effect among habitual losers rather than at-least-sometimes-winners (Curini, Memoli, and Jou 2011). We are not ready to give up on an experimental demonstration of such an effect, however. The theoretical rationale for expecting the effect is just too powerful. Feelings of having a political voice are perhaps the most important determinant of a citizen’s judgment of democracy beyond its ability to produce acceptable social and economic outputs (Dahlberg and Holmberg 2014). And surely having a say in government is powerfully affected by hearing one’s issues discussed and one’s positions put forward in the mass media by politicians and governments and parties and experts. Can we continue to provide experimental evidence for this? 23
© Copyright 2024