Neuropsychol Rev DOI 10.1007/s11065-012-9223-0 REVIEW What is Overt and what is Covert in Congenital Prosopagnosia? Davide Rivolta & Romina Palermo & Laura Schmalzl Received: 8 August 2012 / Accepted: 7 November 2012 # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012 Abstract The term covert recognition refers to recognition without awareness. In the context of face recognition, it refers to the fact that some individuals show behavioural, electrophysiological or autonomic indices of recognition in the absence of overt, conscious recognition. Originally described in cases of people that have lost their ability to overtly recognize faces (acquired prosopagnosia, AP), covert face recognition has more recently also been described in cases of congenital prosopagnosia (CP), who never develop typical overt face recognition skills. The presence of covert processing in a developmental disorder such as CP is a particularly intriguing phenomenon, and its investigation is relevant for a variety of reasons. From a theoretical point of view, it is useful to help shed light on the cognitive and neural underpinnings of face D. Rivolta (*) Department of Neurophysiology, Max Planck Institute for Brain Research, Deutschordenstraße 46, 60528 Frankfurt am Main, Germany e-mail: [email protected] D. Rivolta Ernst Strüngmann Institute (ESI) for Neuroscience in Cooperation with Max Planck Society, Frankfurt am Main, Germany D. Rivolta : L. Schmalzl Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia D. Rivolta Slop, Scuola Lombarda di Psicoterapia Cognitiva Neuropsicologica, Retorbido, Pavia, Italy R. Palermo ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, and School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia L. Schmalzl Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm, Sweden recognition deficits. From a clinical point of view, it has the potential to aid the design of rehabilitation protocols aimed at improving face recognition skills in this population. In the current review we selectively summarize the recent literature on covert face recognition in CP, highlight its main findings, and provide a theoretical interpretation for them. Keywords Face perception . Prosopagnosia . Congenital prosopagnosia . Covert face recognition Introduction The ability to recognize faces rapidly and accurately is crucial for distinguishing family members, friends and colleagues from strangers, and consequently for navigating our social environment with a sense of security (Davis et al. 2011; Yardley et al. 2008). Some people lack the ability to recognize identity from faces, a condition known as prosopagnosia, which can either be acquired following a brain injury (acquired prosopagnosia-AP), or developmental in nature, in which case the individuals have never developed an ability to adequately recognize faces (known as congenital prosopagnosia-CP, or developmental prosopagnosia-DP) (e.g., Barton 2008; Behrmann and Avidan 2005; Bodamer 1947; Della Sala and Young 2003; Duchaine and Nakayama 2006b). What are the main features of CP? From the behavioural point of view, in some cases of CP, the recognition deficit is purely selective to faces, with individuals exhibiting no difficulty in visual processing tasks with other kinds of visual stimuli including the within category discrimination of objects (Duchaine and Nakayama 2005). The deficit can even be specific to the processing of facial identity, with a spared ability to recognize other facial cues such as emotional expressions (Duchaine et al. 2003; Humphreys et al. 2007; Lee et al. 2010; Palermo Neuropsychol Rev et al. 2011). In other cases of CP however, the deficit can extend beyond the face domain and include object recognition (Duchaine et al. 2007; Wilson et al. 2010), general configural processing (Behrmann et al. 2005), or even the perception of biological motion (Dobel et al. 2007). Recently, an increasing number of studies has begun to not only investigate “overt” face recognition in CP (i.e., the explicit ability to recognize faces), but also “covert” face recognition (i.e., implicit processing of familiar faces that individuals claim to not recognize). Covert Face Recognition in CP Covert face recognition in CP can be investigated using behavioural (Avidan and Behrmann 2008; Bate et al. 2009, 2008; Bentin et al. 1999; De Haan et al. 1991; Rivolta et al. 2012, 2010), electrophysiological (Eimer et al. 2012) and autonomic measures (Jones and Tranel 2001). We will discuss each type of evidence separately. Behavioural Techniques Two recent group studies have been conducted to determine whether covert recognition is present in CP. Avidan and Behrmann (2008) used a Matching task, in which participants had to judge whether pairs of faces shown sequentially were of the same person or not. Like controls, the group of six CPs was quicker and more accurate when the pairs were familiar rather than unfamiliar people, suggesting that the familiar famous faces were covertly processed by the visual system despite not being overtly recognized. It is important to note that most CPs can overtly recognize at least some faces. Thus, for investigations aimed at the characterization of covert face recognition it is crucial to exclude trials containing faces that were actually overtly recognized from the analysis. Without this precaution one could potentially erroneously conclude that covert recognition is present, when the effect is in fact merely driven by overtly recognized faces. This study attempted to control for overt recognition, but the covert and overt face recognition assessments were carried out in different sessions held on different days (with the overt assessment occurring before the covert one). A limitation of this procedure is that it is possible that a face that had not been recognized during the overt face recognition assessment could actually be recognized a few days later during the covert face recognition assessment, and consequently enhance covert recognition performance. This is particularly relevant since the face images adopted for overt and covert face recognition assessment were different images of the same people, and it is possible that some images were more prototypical (and thus easier to recognize) than others. Rivolta et al. (2012a) tried to overcome this potential pitfall by performing overt and covert recognition assessment on the same day, and by using the exact same images in both assessments. They used three different behavioural tasks with a group of 11 CPs. In a Forced choice familiarity task, participants were asked to select which one of two simultaneously presented faces (one famous and one nonfamous) was familiar. The group of CPs performed above chance, indicating that they were implicitly oriented towards faces of people who they knew. Moreover, self-rated confidence in their choice was unrelated to performance in this task, indicative of covert but not overt recognition. In a Forced choice cued task, participants had to indicate which one of two simultaneously presented faces best matched a name cue shown at the same time. Once again, the group of CPs matched the name to the correct face more often than chance, despite their inability to identify any of the faces overtly. However, confidence was related to performance on this task, suggesting that this may not be a pure measure of covert recognition (see Rivolta et al. 2012a for further discussion). In a Priming task, participants were presented with a face followed by a name, and simply had to judge whether the name was that of an actor or a politician. The name (e.g., “Tom Cruise”) could be preceded either by the face of the same person (Tom Cruise’s face), a face of a person belonging to the other category (e.g., Tony Blair’s face) or a scrambled face. While controls were faster when the name and face matched than in the other two conditions, this pattern was not observed in the group of CPs. Overall, these studies indicate that covert face recognition in CP can be demonstrated with behavioural tasks. However, they may suffer from some limitations that future research should carefully take into consideration. For instance: (i) They were carried out with a relatively small sample size (or even single cases of CP; see Rivolta et al. 2010 and Bate et al. 2008); (ii) Despite adopting many tasks and performing many comparisons between groups, they did not always carefully correct for multiple comparisons, thus potentially increasing the chance of a type I error (Wagenmakers et al. 2011; Wetzels et al. 2011); (iii) Behavioural tasks do not enable one to monitor actual face recognition on-line, that is it could be the case that a face that was recognized during the covert recognition assessment is no longer recognized a few minutes later in the overt recognition assessment, which can again lead to potentially misleading results. This latter issue has been taken into account in the context of electrophysiological investigations that are described below. Electrophysiological Techniques A recent study by Eimer and colleagues (2012) investigated covert recognition in a group of 12 CPs using Event Related Neuropsychol Rev Potentials (ERPs). The main focus of the analysis was on the N250, a negative component which is larger for faces that are familiar than those that are unfamiliar (Gosling and Eimer 2011). Half of the CPs showed an N250 for familiar faces even though they could not overtly recognize them, indicating covert recognition. No covert recognition was seen at the earlier ERP component, the N170, suggesting its primarily involvement in the perceptual rather than recognition related aspects of face processing (Bentin et al. 2000; Eimer et al. 2012; Rivolta et al. 2012; Rossion et al. 2011). In control participants with typical face recognition skills the N250 was followed by a P600f, a component known to be involved in semantic memory (Osternhout and Holcomb 1993) and hence likely to underlie the retrieval of semantic information for the recognition of a specific person (Eimer 2000). Interestingly, this pattern (i.e., the N250 followed by the P600f) was also observed in CPs, but only for trials in which the face was actually overtly recognized. This observation suggests that, when present, overt recognition in CP is reflected by similar physiological mechanisms as in controls. Overall, these results suggest that covert recognition in CP occurs as a result of a disconnection between spared identity-specific visual memory traces and later semantic face processing stages, which in turn indicates that the activation of stored visual representations of familiar faces alone is not sufficient for conscious explicit face recognition. This study has the unquestionable merit of representing the first investigation of electrophysiological correlates of covert face recognition in a relatively large group of CPs. It would be of interest in future studies to not only investigate a limited number of face-sensitive peaks of interest on particular electrodes, but to consider the information from all channels over the entire time course of the recordings, such as in a non-parametric analysis based on a permutation of residual approach (Anderson and ter Braak 2003). This is particularly true if we consider that it is likely that important information is not only represented in the peaks, but in the activity around them (Rousselet and Pernet 2011; Rousselet et al. 2011). In addition, an analysis at the source level could have been very informative for shedding additional light on the neural substrate behind covert face recognition in CP. Future studies would benefit from considering these issues. single case study of a 5 year old boy, who showed higher SCRs for personally familiar than unfamiliar faces (Jones and Tranel 2001). In sum, there is some initial evidence for physiological indices of covert face recognition in CP, but future studies involving more comprehensive assessments with multiple techniques would be of particular relevance to further our knowledge on the relation between different forms of covert recognition. Heterogeneity of Covert Face Recognition in CP Covert face recognition is not a unitary phenomenon displaying the same features in all CPs. Furthermore, not all CPs show indices of covert recognition. Eimer and colleagues (2012) found that only half of the people with CP demonstrated covert recognition. Similarly, some behavioural case studies have also failed to demonstrate covert recognition (Bentin et al. 1999; De Haan et al. 1991). The extent of overt recognition deficits could be an important factor in whether covert recognition is evident. For instance, Barton et al. (2001) reported that individuals with AP or CP with more severe overt recognition deficits are less likely to show covert recognition. This observation can be linked to the original distinction between “apperceptive” (perceptual) and “associative” (disconnection) prosopagnosia (De Renzi et al. 1991). The assumption here is that covert recognition is less likely to occur in the context of an apperceptive deficit affecting early stages of face processing than an associative deficit disrupting face recognition at a later stage related to person identification. In support of this hypothesis, Eimer et al. (2012) found that overt face recognition performance as measured by the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT, Duchaine and Nakayama 2006a) was significantly better in CPs who showed a covert N250 component compared to CPs who did not. Similarly, Rivolta and colleagues 2012a found a correlation between overt recognition on a Famous face recognition task and covert recognition on a Forced choice familiarity task, indicating that the better the individuals’ overt face recognition skills were, the more likely the individuals were to show covert recognition. To conclude, covert face recognition is present in some but not all CPs, and when present it seems to be related to overt face recognition skills. We will now focus on the neural features of covert recognition in CP. Autonomic Responses Indices of covert face recognition can also be detected by monitoring one type of autonomic activity such as the galvanic skin conductance response (SCR). The use of SCR enables to measure the sweat production in response to the perception of a variety of emotional stimuli (Tranel and Damasio 1994). The only study adopting SCRs for the investigation of covert face recognition in CP to date is a Neural Aspects of Covert Face Recognition How can we explain the presence of covert face recognition in individuals with CP who have never acquired typical face recognition skills to begin with? To answer this question, it is important to note that many CPs with a very severe face recognition deficit (e.g., they are unable to reliably recognize Neuropsychol Rev friends and family members and perform very poorly on formal tests) are nevertheless able to recognize at least some faces. For instance, in our work recognition rates are approximately 28 % of famous faces. As proposed by some authors, it can be assumed that this partially spared recognition ability allows CPs to acquire representations for faces they have a lot of exposure to (Avidan and Behrmann 2008; Bate et al. 2009, 2008; Bentin et al. 1999; De Haan et al. 1991; Rivolta et al. 2012a, b, 2010). For most faces however the representations are possibly “degraded”, being good enough to support general access to familiarity and hence covert recognition, but not specific access to identity and hence overt recognition. The reasons behind the origin of degraded face representations are far from clear. It is possible that this is driven by (i) a reduced numbers of saccades within the eyes-mouth-nose region (Schmalzl et al. 2008); (ii) weak holistic processing (Avidan et al. 2011; Palermo et al. 2011) and/or (iii) poor general visual recognition. For instance, it has never been fully investigated whether “pure” prosopagnosia or prosopagnosia with also difficulties in the recognition of, for instance, objects show differences in their overt and covert behaviour. What are the neural substrates of covert face recognition? Face recognition typically activates a “core” network as well as an “extended” network of brain regions (Haxby et al. 2000). The core network includes the Occipital Face Area (OFA, Gauthier et al. 2000) and Fusiform Face Area (FFA, Kanwisher et al. 1997), which have been proposed to be involved in structural encoding and the evaluation face familiarity (Ewbank et al. 2012; Rossion et al. 2003). The extended network includes the anterior temporal cortex, which appears to be primarily involved in the coding of semantic and biographical information (Tsukiura et al. 2008). There is some evidence however, that the right anterior temporal lobe may also be involved in evaluating face familiarity, although at a later point in time with respect to the core areas (Gainotti 2007a, b). The core and extended face areas are anatomically connected via the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (ILF), a white matter bundle which connects the occipital and temporal lobes (Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten 2008; Fox et al. 2008) (See Fig. 1 for a schematic depiction of the structural and functional components of the human face-processing network). While some studies have shown that the core network can be largely spared in CP (Avidan and Behrmann 2009; Avidan et al. 2005), there is evidence for hypofunctionality in the extended network (Avidan and Behrmann 2009; Furl et al. 2011), possibly reflecting a poor connectivity between the two systems (Thomas et al. 2009). This observation fits well with some of the reported patterns of covert face recognition in CP. For example, some CPs perform above chance on behavioural tasks such as Forced choice familiarity and Matching tasks (which require only a familiarity judgment and thus rely upon the core network), but not on a Priming task (which requires access to semantic information and Fig. 1 Structural and functional components of the face-processing system. The figure depicts the anatomical location of the Occipital Face Area (OFA), the Fusiform Face Area (FFA), the Anterior Temporal cortex (AT), as well as the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (ILF, represented in green). The ILF is a white-matter bundle that anatomically connects OFA, FFA and AT. Adapted with permission from Catani and Thiebaut de Schotten (2008) thus relies upon both the core and extended networks) (Rivolta et al. 2012a). Overall, despite being speculative in nature, we can predict that a CP with hypofunctioning core and extended face processing network will fail to show overt as well as covert recognition; whereas a CP with a typically functioning core network, but a hypofunctioning extended face processing network will fail to show overt recognition, but may show behavioural (i.e., above chance performance on Forced choice familiarity and Matching tasks) and ERP (i.e., a familiarity sensitive N250) indices of covert recognition. When it comes to SCRs, the only very limited available information on CPs makes it difficult to make any clear anatomical predictions on which brain structures underlie the responses. In general terms however, SCRs may be mediated by the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (Breen et al. 2000; Tranel and Damasio 1994). Since SCRs for familiar faces are supposedly also driven by the emotional valence these stimuli have for a person, it can be postulated that a lack of SCRs in CP might be correlated with either diminished activity of these areas which code valence, or poor connectivity between these areas and other face processing regions. This hypothesis finds support in the AP literature. For example, Valdes-Sosa and colleagues (2011) described a patient with AP with damaged fusiformtemporal pathways who showed face-selective activation in orbitofrontal areas and increased SCRs for familiar faces but no recognition on a priming task. Conclusions In conclusion, the investigation of covert face recognition in CP is relevant for a variety of reasons. First, it can represent a more sensitive tool than the traditional assessment of overt recognition for pinpointing the exact nature of face Neuropsychol Rev recognition deficit in different individuals. Second, it has the potential to shed further light on the exact role of different components within the human neural face recognition system. Moreover, it allows one to determine which apparently inaccessible information is still present within the visual system. This last aspect might be informative for the design of rehabilitation protocols aimed at improving face recognition skills. In fact, knowing that some information about face familiarity is still present, albeit not accessible, calls for the planning of interventions aimed at retrieving it. We can speculate that a cognitive rehabilitation protocol based on covert face recognition may focus on training the participants in discriminating between familiar and unfamiliar faces by using a vast sample of faces presented at different orientations (e.g., front and three quarter view). This strategy, based on the findings on the Forced choice familiarity task, would make CPs more confident in everyday life by potentially increasing the number of familiar faces they can recognize. Acknowledgments DR is supported by the Neuronale Koordination Forschungsschwerpunkt Frankfurt (NeFF), RP is supported by the ARC Centre of Excellence Grant CE110001021, and LS is supported by the European Research Council. References Anderson, M. J., & ter Braak, C. J. F. (2003). Permutation tests for multifactorial analysis of variance. Journal of Statistical Computation and Simulation, 78, 85–113. Avidan, G., & Behrmann, M. (2008). Implicit familiarity processing in congenital prosopagnosia. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2, 141–164. Avidan, G., & Behrmann, M. (2009). Functional MRI reveals compromised neural integrity of the face processing network in congenital prosopagnosia. Current Biology, 19, 1–5. Avidan, G., Hasson, U., Malach, R. L., & Behrmann, M. (2005). Detailed Exploration of Face-related Processing in Congenital Prosopagnosia: 2. Functional Neuroimaging Findings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1150–1167. Avidan, G., Tanzer, M., & Behrmann, M. (2011). Impaired holistic processing in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2541–2552. Barton, J. J. (2008). Structure and function in acquired prosopagnosia: Lesson from a series of 10 patients with brain damage. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2, 197–225. Barton, J. J., Cherkasova, M., & O'Connor, M. (2001). Covert recognition in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia. Neurology, 57, 1161–1168. Bate, S., Haslam, C., Tree, J. J., & Hodgson, T. L. (2008). Evidence of an eye movement-based effect in congenital prosopagnosia. Cortex, 44, 806–819. Bate, S., Haslam, C., Jansari, A., & Hodgson, T. L. (2009). Covert face recognition relies on affective valence in congenital prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 391–411. Behrmann, M., & Avidan, G. (2005). Congenital prosopagnosia: Faceblind from birth. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 180–187. Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., Marotta, J. J., & Kimchi, R. (2005). Detailed Exploration of Face-related Processing in Congenital Prosopagnosia: 1. Behavioral Findings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1130–1149. Bentin, S., Deouell, L. Y., & Soroker, N. (1999). Selective visual streaming in face recognition: Evidence from developmental prosopagnosia. Neuroreport, 10, 823–827. Bentin, S., Deouell, & Leon, Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 35–55. Bodamer, J. (1947). Die Prosop-agnosie. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervkrankheiten, 179, 6–53. Breen, N., Caine, D., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Models of face recognition and delusional misidentification: A critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 55–71. Catani, M., & Thiebaut de Schotten, M. T. (2008). A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex, 44, 1105–1132. Davis, J. M., McKone, E., Dennett, H., O’Connor, K. L., O’Kearney, R., & Palermo, R. (2011). Individual differences in face recognition ability are associated with social anxiety. PLoS One, 12. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028800. De Haan, E. H., Edward, H. F., & Campbell, R. (1991). A 15 year followup of a case of developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex, 27, 489–509. De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., Grossi, D., & Nichelli, P. (1991). Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex, 27, 213–221. Della Sala, S., & Young, A. W. (2003). Quaglino’s 1867 case of prosopagnosia. Cortex, 39, 533–540. Dobel, C., Bolte, J., Aicher, M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2007). Prosopagnosia without apparent cause: Overview and diagnosis of six cases. Cortex, 43, 718–733. Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2005). Dissociations of Face and Object Recognition in Developmental Prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 249–261. Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2006a). The Cambridge Face Memory Test: Results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic participants. Neuropsychologia, 44, 576–585. Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2006b). Developmental prosopagnosia: A window to content-specific face processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 166–173. Duchaine, B., Parker, H., & Nakayama, K. (2003). Normal recognition of emotion in a prosopagnosic. Perception, 32, 827–838. Duchaine, B., Germine, L., & Nakayama, K. (2007). Family resemblance: Ten family members with prosopagnosia and within-class object agnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 419–430. Eimer, M. (2000). Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 694–705. Eimer, M., Gosling, A., and Duchaine, B. (2012). Electrophysiological markers of covert face recognition in developmental prosopagnosia. Brain, doi:10.1093/brain/awr1347. Ewbank, M. P., Henson, R. N., Rowe, J. B., Stoyanova, R. S., and Calder, A. J. (2012). Different neural mechanisms within occipitotemporal cortex underlie repetition suppression across same and different-size faces. Cerebral Cortex, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs1070. Fox, C. J., Iaria, G., & Barton, J. J. (2008). Disconnection in prosopagnosia and face processing. Cortex, 44, 996–1009. Furl, N., Garrido, L., Dolan, R. J., Driver, J., & Duchaine, B. C. (2011). Fusiform gyrus face selectivity relates to individual differences in facial recognition ability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 1723–1740. Gainotti, G. (2007a). Different patterns of famous people recognition disorders in patients with right and left anterior temporal lesions: A systematic review. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1591–1607. Gainotti, G. (2007b). Face familiarity feelings, the right temporal lobe and the possible underlying neural mechanisms. Brain Research Reviews, 56, 214–235. Neuropsychol Rev Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 495–504. Gosling, A., & Eimer, M. (2011). An event-related brain potential study of explicit face recognition. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2736–2745. Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223–233. Humphreys, K., Avidan, G., & Behrmann, M. (2007). A detailed investigation of facial expression processing in congenital prosopagnosia as compared to acquired prosopagnosia. Experimental Brain Research, 176, 356–373. Jones, R. D., & Tranel, D. (2001). Severe developmental prosopagnosia in a child with superior intellect. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 265–273. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–4311. Lee, Y., Duchaine, B. C., Wilson, H. R., & Nakayama, K. (2010). Three cases of developmental prosopagnosia from one family: Detailed neuropsychological and psychophysical investigation of face processing. Cortex, 46, 949–964. Osternhout, P., & Holcomb, P. (1993). Event-related potentials and synctactic anomaly: Evidence of anomaly detection during the perception of continuous speech. Language & Cognitive Processes, 8, 413–437. Palermo, R., Willis, M. L., Rivolta, D., McKone, E., Wilson, C. E., & Calder, A. J. (2011). Impaired holistic coding of facial expression and facial identity in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1226–1235. Rivolta, D., Schmalzl, L., Coltheart, M., & Palermo, R. (2010). Semantic information can facilitate covert face recognition in congenital prosopagnosia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 1002–1016. Rivolta, D., Palermo, R., Schmalzl, L., & Coltheart, M. (2012a). Covert face recognition in congenital prosopagnosia: A group study. Cortex, 48, 344–352. Rivolta, D., Palermo, R., Schmalzl, L., & Williams, M. A. (2012b). An early category-specific neural response for the perception of both places and faces. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 45–51. Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., & Crommelinck, M. (2003). The functionally defined right occipital and fusiform “face areas” discriminate novel from visually familiar faces. NeuroImage, 19, 877–883. Rossion, B., Dricot, L., Goebel, R., & Busigny, T. (2011). Holistic face categorization in higher order visual areas of the normal and prosopagnosic brain: toward a non-hierarchical view of face perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 1–3. Rousselet, G. A., and Pernet, C. R. (2011). Quantifying the time course of visual object processing using ERPs: It’s time to up the game. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, doi:10.339/fpsyg.2011.00107. Rousselet, G. A., Pernet, C. R., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2011). Visual object categorization in the brain: What can we really learn from ERP peaks? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1–3. Schmalzl, L., Palermo, R., Green, M., Brunsdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2008). Training of familiar face recognition and visual scan paths for faces in a child with congenital prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 704–729. Thomas, C., Avidan, G., Humphreys, K., Jung, K., & Behrmann, M. (2009). Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 29–31. Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1994). Neuroanatomical correlates of electrodermal skin conductance responses. Psychophysiology, 31, 427–438. Tsukiura, T., Suzuki, C., Shigemune, Y., & Mochizuki-Kawai, H. (2008). Different contributions of the anterior temporal and medial temporal lobe to the retrieval of memory for person identity information. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 1343–1354. Valdes-Sosa, M., Bobes, M., Quinones, I., Garcia, L., ValdesHernandez, P. A., Iturria, Y., et al. (2011). Covert face recognition without the fusiform-temporal pathways. NeuroImage, 57, 1162– 1176. Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2011). Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: the case of Psi: Comment on Bem (2011). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 426–432. Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 291–298. Wilson, C. E., Palermo, R., Schmalzl, L., & Brock, J. (2010). Specificity of impaired facial identity recognition in children with suspected developmental prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 30– 45. Yardley, L., McDermott, L., Pisarski, S., Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2008). Psychosocial consequences of developmental prosopagnosia: A problem of recognition. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 65, 445–451.
© Copyright 2024