Education and Aspirations: Evidence from Islamic and state schools in Pakistan and Nigeria Masooda Bano1 Abstract Drawing on survey and ethnographic data from Islamic and state schools in Pakistan and Nigeria, this paper questions assumed effect of attendance in Islamic schools on students’ levels of religiosity and aspirations. On both these counts, students from both the groups show broadly similar results. Modern-school girls in Pakistan do record a higher willingness to pursue a professional career. However, urban background, increased exposure to television, and more educated mothers appear more likely explanations for this difference than school effect. The Nigerian data supports this analysis. The findings suggest that increasing employment opportunities, access to media, and supporting female role models can make more Muslim females aspire for formal employment than interventions targeted at reforming Islamic norms. Key Words: Islamic schools, school-effect, aspirations, religiosity, female empowerment. Note: This is a very early draft of this paper. Circulation is limited to 2015 Oxford AALIMS Conference participants. Introduction Despite being conducive to empirical testing, the debate on whether religious schools effect moral socialisation of children remains divisive. The assertion that schools don’t just teach literacy and numeracy but also shape children’s moral worldview, has found support in some studies that have attempted to measure the effect of religious schools on students’ levels of religiosity (Barrett et al. 2007; Tritter 1992). Competing evidence instead emphasizes the effect of household religiosity levels both in shaping the preference for religious schools (Cohen-Zara and Sander 2008; Sander 2005) as well as the levels of religiosity (Hill 2011). Both sides of the argument have, however, largely been advanced drawing on data from Catholic or Jewish schools in the West. Madrasas (Islamic schools), which are routinely accused of radicalising Muslim youth, have not been subjected to such an analysis despite being ideally suited to it. This paper draws on survey data with female students from madrasas and state schools in Pakistan and northern Nigeria to assess whether time in madrasas or the family background seems to play a more important role in shaping students’ levels of religiosity and future aspirations. The results are surprising. In terms of levels of religiosity, measured through knowledge of Islamic rules, conviction in their fairness, desired levels of piety, and ritual practice, students from the two groups show very similar results. On two out of four questions aimed at measuring levels of ritual practice, madrasa students did show slightly higher results. But, drawing on 1 I will like to thank Abigail Barr for her assistance in developing the survey instrument and the research design. Uday Anand, Athena Sharma and Santiago I. Tort provided valuable research assistance. The study was developed under the ESRC/AHRC Ideas and Beliefs Fellowship (RES-071-27-0034) that I held between 2009- 2012. In Nigeria the survey was implemented through DFID Education Sector Support Programme in Nigeria (ESSPIN). Contribution from ODID research fund is also gratefully acknowledged. Lastly, the paper benefitted enormously from my many discussions with Imane Chaara. 1 ethnographic fieldwork, the paper shows group-effect is more likely explanation for that than madrasa education. Thus, the paper first of all shows that the assumed impact of madrasas on increasing students’ levels of religiosity in Muslim societies is potentially exaggerated. Second, the survey findings also challenge the assumed links between madrasa education and lowering of material aspirations. Though not necessarily framed in terms of aspirations, the assumption that madrasa education lowers students’ desire for material well-being and this-worldly achievements, dominates the literature on both male and female madrasa. It is routinely argued in the media and policy reports that madrasa education trains students to value otherworldly rewards thus making it easier for jihadi groups to mobilize madrasa students to sacrifice their life in jihad. Scholarship on female madrasas, which has seen a boom in recent years, echoes similar assertions about the impact of madrasa education on girls’ material aspirations. It is argued that madrasa education increases young women’s adherence to orthodox Islamic gender norms which when seen in light of liberal feminist framework don’t only appear out-rightly discriminatory2, they also seem to lower girls’ material aspirations by training them to value their role within the family over pursuit of professional career. Against these assertions, again the survey results from the two contexts are surprising: the data on material aspirations from the two set of student respondents shows little variation. There is, however, one striking difference in results between the two student groups in case of Pakistan: while showing similar material aspirations as madrasa students, modern school girls are more willing to opt for professional roles than madrasa students when presented with a dilemma where they are required to choose between a more orthodox or a more liberal interpretation of Islamic gender norms. This result is apparently inconsistent with the results on levels of religiosity where the girls from both the groups showed equally high results. While being convinced of the fairness of Islamic gender norms and aspiring for a high level of piety, school-girls are more willing to become economically empowered, compromise family time to secure job promotion and are more supportive of their husband’s decision to pursue non-Islamic modes of socializing with professional colleagues if that socialization is seen to be important for professional advancement. This desire to retain their religiosity but at the same time be flexible on the rulings to maximise opportunities for material advancement is an important difference between the two groups in Pakistan. What potentially might create this difference in the approaches of the two groups has important implications for advancing the gender and development programmes supported in many Muslim majority contexts. Most development agencies involved in such programmes acknowledge the need to be respectful of local norms yet encourage women to pursue paths that can trigger change within these norms that restrict their agency. The question then is that could this important difference in girls’ willingness to opt for professional careers or more liberal interpretations of Islam that enable women to maximize opportunities for material and social advancement be attributed directly to school type. The paper will show how family characteristics combined with urban/rural background, potentially become better predictor of this difference than madrasa education. Given the limitations of the sample size (see section on methodology) instead of attempting to run a regression analysis, the paper draws on four sources of data to 2 Examples include men having permission to have four wives, women getting half the share of inheritance, etc. 2 illustrate why factors other than school type appear more likely explanation for this difference. First, the paper is able to show important differences in the socio-economic and cultural background of the students in the two groups that are statistically significant and which in line with broader evidence in the literature can be argued to have a strong bearing on girls’ levels of religiosity and aspirations. Two, it compares responses of madrasa students between year 1 and year 4 (final year) because along with comparing across school-types, within institution comparison between early and final grades is widely used method to test whether time in school seems to have changed students’ values and aspirations. Comparing the responses from the first and final year madrasa students, thus, helps triangulate the results from comparison between the two school types. Such an analysis interestingly reveals that senior students in madrasas show a slightly higher willingness for professional roles than those in year 1 thereby suggesting a positive rather than negative impact of time in madrasa on girls’ willingness to adopt a professional career. Three, based on selected theological references and ethnographic fieldwork, the paper shows how even within orthodox Islamic position and in the education imparted in madrasas in Pakistan, motherhood is prioritized but pursuing a professional career is not seen to be forbidden3 in Islam. As part of Islamic history, women in madrasas read in detail about the wives of Prophet Mohammad, some of whom were professionally active or managed their own business. Thus, a better appreciation of the complex nature of Islamic theology and some familiarity with what actually is taught in madrasas, also helps understand why it is problematic to assume that time in madrasa will necessarily build negative attitudes towards pursuing professional career. There could potentially be other factors contributing to development of such an attitude. Fourth, using the evidence from Nigeria, we see that different results help establish the same position. Unlike the evidence from Pakistan, schools girls in northern Nigeria 4 do not show higher professional ambition than madrasa girls, but neither do we find any significant variation in the socio-economic and cultural backgrounds of the girls in the two types of schools in Kano that we see in Pakistan. Also, the level of opportunities available to girls the two country contexts are quite different, which further helps explain why certain dimensions of the socio-economic and cultural context rather than time in an Islamic school might better explain Muslim girls’ willingness to pursue economically empowering professional careers. The structure of the paper is as follows. Section 1 presents a brief review of the policy debates and academic literature linking madrasas within radicalisation or heightened levels of piety and confining of female agency. Section 2, defines religiosity and aspirations and reviews the dominant approaches in the literature to measure religiosity before presenting a rationale for choosing the four sub-measures of religiosity adopted in this study. Section 3 presents the methodology. Section 4 presents the results on levels of religiosity. Section 5 presents the results on levels of material aspirations including responses to the three vignettes developed to assess girls’ willingness to join professional roles and opt for more liberal interpretations of Islam. Section 6 highlights the nonmadrasas factors that seem to explore why college girls in Pakistan show more 3 Ethnographic fieldwork with madrasas in Pakistan and northern Nigeria shows that discuss is focused on understanding what professions are most appropriate for women rather than arguing that women should not work at all. Jobs requiring too much interaction with the other gender are discouraged; teaching positions at school, college or university level are highly encouraged. 4 The Islamic schooling system across northern Nigerian states has same historical origin. Thus, the structure of female madrasas in Kano is the same as in other northern Nigerian states. 3 willingness to adopt liberal positions. Section 7 triangulates these findings from other three sources of evidence. Section 8 concludes by situating these results in the broader debate on how to reform informal institutions so that they become more supportive of globally defined development goals. Muslim women, madrasas and Islamic norms Madrasas have been in the global spotlight since September 11 for their alleged links with Islamic militancy. The fact that some of the senior Taliban leaders were trained in madrasas operating in the borderland of Pakistan and Afghanistan made Pakistani madrasas a particular target of western policy efforts aimed at Islamic de-radicalization. Reforming madrasas has been an officially recognized part of the soft side of the US ‘war on terror’, which has mainly relied on use of military force including drones. Madrasa reforms have mainly focused on introducing modern subjects in madrasa curriculum with the view that it will increase madrasa students’ access to formal job market therefore reducing appeal for militant Islam. While the concerns around militancy have been highlighted more in case of male madrasas and have mainly been aired by western policy makers, media and academics from security studies, a rich academic literature drawing from more traditional social science disciplines, especially anthropology and politics, has emerged on growing number of institutional platforms that are today offering Islamic education to Muslim women. These institutions have emerged across the Muslim world since the 1970s and take many different forms. Many are informal operating as loosely structured study circles which meet weekly others are formally organized degree awarding educational institutions such as the female madrasas in Pakistan and their counter parts in northern Nigeria. While the interest in studying these movements has evolved more organically from within the academic community, many of the studies on these movements have indirectly ended up supporting the Islamic education and radicalisation thesis. Focused more on studying how participation in these movements impacts women’s approach to gender norms than on militancy, many of these studies argue that participation in these groups leads to higher emphasis on attaining religious piety, reduces desire to confront patriarchal power structures, and leads to higher appreciation for maternal roles over pursuing a profession. Women within these groups are also seen to justify controversial Islamic gender norms, such as men having permission to have four wives at any given time when women must secure a divorce before remarrying, women inheriting half the share of men, and two female witnesses required for every male witness in court disputes in matters of commerce. Such studies argue that Islamic education in female madrasas is very focused on making women idealize role of a mother, who by raising morally upright children builds the nation. In this line of reasoning, Islamic education is in general argued to downplay the importance of material wealth by emphasizing that Prophet was a simple man who lived a simple life. However, there is also a strong counter narrative in the literature on these institutions that based on similar nature of fieldwork draws very different conclusion about the impact of Islamic education on the girls’ attitudes and aspirations. This set of literature argues that though prioritizing maternal roles, participation in Islamic study circles or madrasas empowers Muslim women within the established structures of authority. These studies show that better knowledge of Islam makes these women learn many rights that Islam gives to the women enabling them to make more demands on their husbands. These women often are see to strategically use their newly acquired Islamic knowledge to 4 argue for increased authority within the household. Islamic education is also seen to improve the psychological well-being of the students. By increasing girls’ conviction that things are ultimately in the hands of God, Islamic education makes them feel less vulnerable to submit to any authority structure (formal or informal) that they do not believe is right. Finally, these studies also note that authors who argue that Islamic education leads to increased orthodoxy, ignore one important evidence that women following the same Islamic study group actually end up showing a lot of variation in choosing which specific Islamic interpretation to follow. These studies show that whether women choose to follow a more liberal interpretation or the orthodox one, often in seen to have a direct correlation with their socio-economic background and prior educational levels of the students. For instances, studies show that Islamic study circles catering to women from affluent families and having a professional background are led by preachers who themselves are modern educated and are able to reason how lslamic norms can fit the demands of modern time. These competing positions in the ethnographic studies on how women engage with the knowledge received within the Islamic study circles and how this education impacts their aspirations, worthy of further testing using survey methods; however, efforts of this nature remain limited. Survey based work on madrasas has increased since September 11, however, much of this work has focused on testing links between poverty and madrasa enrolment. Only a couple of studies have actually drawn on survey data to study the impact of madras education on social values and economic attitudes of the students. The closest to such attempts is the work of Asadullah and Chaudhry (2010). Looking at female graduates of registered secondary state schools and madrasas from rural schools in Bangladesh, they argue that madrasa students differ on attitudes related to working mothers, desired fertility, and higher education for girls even when controlling for individual, family and school traits. Further, in this paper exposure to female and younger teachers is associated with more favourable attitudes among graduates. Though the authors do not compare the two groups on levels of religiosity or material aspirations, this study comes closest to the effort being made in this paper. Theoretical overview Before we attempt to measure individual or collective levels of religiosity, it is important to discuss how we define it. There is a rich literature, especially within sociology of religion, dedicated to identifying the various components of religious behaviour that together arguably help determine individual or collective levels of religiosity. McAndrew and Voas (2011) in a paper titled Measuring Religiosity Using Surveys, argue that religion being very highly complex phenomenon is multidimensional in construct. Noting that there is a difference between religious affiliation (nominal association with religion by birth) and religious commitment (attitudes, behaviour and values) which can have varying degrees, they argue religiosity to be concerned with the latter. While it is agreed in the literature that quantification of religiosity is possible, there are no clear standards regarding which of the following aspects, preferred by different studies, should be measured: belief, practice, formal membership, informal affiliation, ritual initiation, doctrinal knowledge, moral sense, core values, or how your regard others. Consequently, studies attempting to measure the impact of school type on levels of religiosity have used different dimensions to measure students’ levels of religiosity largely shaped by what to the authors of those studies was the most convincing measure of religiosity in the given context. 5 This study chose to study religiosity by focusing on four dimensions: the degree of knowledge about core Islamic rulings, the degree of conviction in their fairness, aspirations to be close to God, and ritual practice. R1. Knowledge of Islam was measured by asking if the respondents think that the following rulings are part of Islam: men having the permission to have four wives, women having half the inheritance, and two witnesses required for one male witness in matters of commerce. R2. Belief in fairness of Islamic norms was tested by following up on each one of the three questions mentioned in R1 by asking if in their view by giving men that right, Islam gives women lower status than men. Affirmative answers were seen to question the fairness of Islamic law. R3. Aspiring to be close to God was measured by asking a number of questions within the section on aspirations that can help measure one’s desire for religious piety. Key dimensions covered were: aspirations to have time, places, and freedom to pray and pray regularly; to have the time, a place, and freedom to read the Quran and to read the Quran regularly; to have the time, freedom, and the means to do Hajj; to have religious virtue in eyes of those around you; to have religious virtue in her own eyes, and to have religious virtue in the eyes of Allah. R4. Ritual practice was measured by asking respondents if they performed a specific religious ritual and if yes then how frequently. Ritual practices covered included both obligatory Islamic rituals as well as some optional ones: five compulsory prayers plus the optional tahajjud, midnight prayer; frequency of reading the Quran; fasting in Ramadan, and levels of giving under the compulsory Islamic obligation of zakat as well as the optional forms of giving sadeeqa and kheerat. Response options ranged from never to daily. While a weighted average that could turn the responses to above questions into a religious index, has not yet been calculated, for purposes of conceptual reasoning, variable 2 and 3 are argued to be double the weight of variable 1 and 4 in determining a student’s level of religiosity. The rationale for this is that ritual practice is the most publicly observable aspect of one’s stated religiosity. It can therefore be prompted by many other motives, such as desire to express conformity with expected group norms especially in contexts where lack of observance of the rituals can lead to exclusion from the group. The need to differentiate between responses to publically observable rituals and private religiosity has therefore been emphasized routinely in the studies attempting to measure levels of religiosity (Barrett et al. 2007). In most Muslim societies where religion still retains a strong hold on the public sphere, the group pressure to take part in publically observable Islamic rituals becomes even more pronounced. Because in such a case, external pressure rather than internal conviction can become a plausible explanation for taking part in a publically observable ritual practice, this measure while included, is argued to be least reliable of people’s levels of religiosity than the other three. Variables 2 and 3 on the other hand asked about people’s inner beliefs and aspirations, which are publically unobservable and are therefore comparatively more true reflection of people’s inner convictions and desired levels of religiosity. In comparing the levels of religiosity between the two groups understudy, the paper thus conceptually places the highest weight on these two variables. Variable 1, which attempts to measures one’s 6 knowledge about Islam, can potentially be a useful measure of one’s levels of religiosity. However, since the questions aimed at testing the knowledge of girls about Islamic ruling presented three complex Islamic rulings quite simplistically, a no response could indicate not a lack of knowledge of that ruling but a disagreement with the way the ruling was summarized. Therefore, this variable again will be conceptually given less weight compared to variables 2 and 3 in developing a religiosity index. The second concept used in this paper that needs clarifying in the concept of aspirations. We need to be clear what we mean by it and why measuring difference in material or spiritual aspirations of students from madrasas and modern schools could be an important lens to understand the impact of madrasa education on girls’ future life outcomes. The study of aspirations, normally a focus of psychology, is gaining increasing currency in development. Aspirations are defined as forward-looking goals or targets (Locke and Lathan, 2002). Within development studies there is currently much interest in exploring the links between aspirations and poverty, with increasing number of papers arguing that lower aspirations lead to lower achievement. Bernard et al. (2014) note that ‘in economic terms, we might think of aspirations as bounds among individuals’ preferences, the limits of the choice sets which they consider as relevant for them and motivate their actions’. They further argue, ‘Once formed, our aspirations can function like other mental models in limiting the possible futures we consider by focusing our attention on some future options and filtering out others.’ Presenting the comprehensive review of the literature on aspirations from sociology, social psychology and economics, Bernard et al (2014) summarize four key features of aspirations. First, aspirations express goals or goal-orientations (or desired future endstates) that are relevant to well-being broadly defined. Second, aspirations evolve over time in response to life experience and circumstances. Authors have used different terms for the experiences that shape aspirations: intra-personal and extra personal environment, vicarious experience, and aspiration window to cite a few. In particular, social comparisons and learning from relevant others are important determinants of aspirations. Appadurai (2001) and Ray (2002) argue that individuals largely form aspirations by observing the outcomes of individuals whose behaviours they can observe and whom they can identify. Social psychologists also argue that aspirations and broader beliefs about self-efficacy are largely modelled on the experience of others in the immediate environment (Bandura 1977). This has been referred to as one’s ‘aspiration window.’ Third, as goals, aspirations are an important influence on behaviour (or actions) and thus attainment or outcomes. Aspirations motivate behaviour. They serve to mobilize and direct energy into action with respect to their objectives, thus providing motivate power for action. Many studies find significant impact of aspirations on choice and or attainment. Serneels and Dercon (2014) for example show that mothers’ aspirations matters for their children’s education outcomes. The paper observes a strong relationship: aspiring to one additional year of schooling lifts the grade achieved at age 15 by 1.8 years on average. The fact that in these studies aspirations are increasingly being linked to actual achievement and aspirations in turns seem to be influenced by one’s immediate reference group, the kind of schools one’s attend can thus have important influence on one’s aspirations and future life outcomes. If it is that madrasa education indeed ends up reducing women’s material aspirations and pursuit of economic empowerment then the impact of madrasas runs directly opposite to what development agencies are trying to do through their gender and empowerment programmes in countries such as Pakistan and 7 Nigeria. Raising women’s aspirations, making them economically empowered, making them challenge existing structures that deny them equal legal status, is at the heart of the gender and development efforts. Many development agencies are investing not just in education and jobs for women, they are actively funding major programmes aimed at changing attitudes not just of men but also of women by giving them awareness to aspire high and change the surroundings around them: DFID Pakistan is currently developing a £80 million programme targeted purely to changing gender norms. This programme is meant to complement DFID’s more traditional interventions aimed at increasing girls’ access to education, health and economic opportunities. It is due to this increased interest within development theory and practice on seeing aspirations as a critical first step towards shaping life’s ambitions and long term outcomes that this survey focused on comparing aspirations between the two groups. Methodology This paper presents an analysis of the effect of madrasa education on students’ levels of religiosity and aspirations based on results of a survey implemented in two Muslim majority contexts: Pakistan and northern Nigeria. The two countries present two important regions with strong Islamic influence: South Asia and West Africa. The two countries make a good comparative case because of the large number of Islamic schools they host. Pakistan has a rich network of madrasas and northern Nigeria has a large network of Tsangaya (Quranic) and Islamiyya schools. Both countries have also been suffering from radical and militant Islamic groups. The inspiration for this survey came from a larger ethnographic study that involved prolonged fieldwork with female Islamic study groups across these two contexts and Syria between 2007-2012 (Bano forthcoming). This comparative ethnographic fieldwork led to identification of five dimensions which could particularly benefit from further exploration through survey data: family background, psychological health, levels of religiosity, locus of control5, and future aspirations. This paper draws on the survey results on three of these dimensions— family background, levels of religiosity and future aspirations— to see if attendance in Islamic versus state schools correlates to any one’s religiosity levels or future aspirations and if yes then is it best attributed to school effect or whether the family background, when unpacked for the socio-cultural and economic variations, provides more plausible explanations for any difference in outcomes. The survey was implemented with girls between 10-14 grades who were in the age range of 16-20. They were selected from modern state school or colleges and Islamic schools. It is important to note here that the terms school and college are being used interchangeably in this paper because the same academic years are categorised differently in the Pakistani and Nigerian education system. In Pakistan, state school system only runs till Grade 10, and Grade 11 and 12 are classified as first two years of college. In Nigeria, Grade 11 and 12 are still part of the Senior School (SS). There is also a difference in the organization of female madrasas in the two countries. In Pakistan female madrasas mainly cater to girls after they have complete Grade 10 in state or private schools and offer a four-year degree programme. Madrasa education in Pakistan is thus mainly focused on providing education that is equivalent of Grade 11-14 5 Perhaps explain why the other two are not discussed here. 8 education. This meant in Pakistan, the survey had to be carried out with girls in madrasas and modern state run colleges. In case of Nigeria on the other hand, Islamiyya schools run parallel to state schools from primary to all the way to Senior Secondary (Grade 12). In case of Nigeria, it was therefore more appropriate to survey girls from final years of Senior Secondary classes across the two types of schools. The average age in the Nigeria sample was therefore younger as students were selected from Senior Secondary school grade. In case of Pakistan on the other hand the girls were selected up to Grade 14, i.e. the final year in a madrasa and a college. The reason sampling this slightly different age grouping in the two contexts in justified is that it is more accurately reflective of each country’s Islamic schooling landscape. A critical feature of the student sample selection was that students were selected from the base year as well as the final year in both the contexts. In case of Pakistan, students were selected from Year 11 and Year 14, i.e. first and last year of a madrasa and a modern college. In case of Nigeria, they were selected from Grade 9 and Grade 12, i.e. first and last year of an Islamiyya school and a state school. This was meant to help assess whether those in the senior years had absorbed the values of the given school. This methodological strategy has been popular within many other studies aimed at measuring impact of time in school on religiosity and attitudes. The selection of the madrasas and colleges was based on purpose sampling; students were selected randomly from the two school types using the school register. In Pakistan, girls were selected from two prominent madrasas and two prominent colleges in Lahore and Rawalpindi. Both are major cities in Punjab, the largest Pakistani provision. The selected institutions were seen to be good representative of their school type in that city. A total of 550 students were selected from across the four participating institutions. In Kano the logistical arrangements were facilitated through a development programme which allowed for easy access to a larger number of SS schools. The survey was implemented in a total of 9 state schools and nine Islamiyya schools in Kano. These schools were selected across three Local Government Authorities (LGAs): Fagge, Kumbosto and Albasu. In total, 1600 students were surveyed, majority however were from the state schools due to the bigger class sizes in state schools. The survey was implemented in a hall normally made available by the participating school, where all the selected students were seated and made to fill the questionnaire. The questions were read out aloud by a research assistant and students were required to mark their response on the questionnaire. Given the sample sizes, which particularly in case of Pakistan, were determined by practical considerations6, the contribution of this paper rests not in presenting a formal model. Instead, the focus is on establishing that contextual factors seem to have strong links to shaping religiosity levels and aspirations of girls in these two societies. These links, if further tested, could have important policy consequences. Results Madrasa education and religiosity 6 In Pakistan, access had to be secured individually from each participating school and madrasa thus making it difficult to cover a large number of institutions. 9 As outlined in the section on methodology, levels of religiosity were measured by focusing on four sub-variables. Table 1 lists responses to key questions asked to measure responses on these four sub-variables. Table 1. Levels of Religiosity for Madrasa and Modern Schools, Pakistan Madrasa Mean S.D. Modern School Mean S.D. X2 5.240566 3.547703 p-value a) Ritual Practice How many times do you pray in the day? 0.460430 1.482888 How often do you recite the Quran? 221.502 0.0000 171.059 0.0000 Do you fast during Ramadan? 1.009390 0.096672 1.097222 0.330124 14.4680 0.0000 Do you donate Zakat? 0.500000 0.501460 0.694657 0.461434 16.6577 0.0000 Do you donate Sadaqa? 0.965000 0.184241 0.901460 0.298589 7.0101 0.008 Do you donate Kheerat? Have you completed the Arabic recitation of the Quran (at least) one time? Have you read the Quran with translation? 0.876405 0.330048 0.900735 0.299568 0.6564 0.418 0.985916 0.118117 0.968532 0.174886 1.5719 0.210 2.500000 0.545777 2.261168 0.575809 7.1850 0.028 3.000000 0.000000 2.921502 0.356901 2.5704 0.277 2.942584 0.288489 2.770035 0.557286 17.6791 0.0000 2.976303 0.205653 2.431579 0.875962 70.3004 0.0000 0.104167 0.308709 0.069204 0.254241 0.7326 0.392 Do you think that, by giving women half the inheritance 0.080189 rights of men, Islam gives women lower status than men? 0.272228 0.056140 0.230598 1.1342 0.287 0.048544 0.215436 0.169118 0.375547 16.4767 0.200 0.990196 0.098771 0.996466 0.059444 0.000 0.999 0.995169 0.069505 0.964664 0.184954 0.0207 0.886 1.000000 0.000000 0.982270 0.132205 2.9238 0.087 0.980392 0.138990 0.931655 0.252793 0.0009 0.976 0.990244 0.098531 0.985612 0.119301 0.4823 0.487 1.000000 0.000000 0.982206 0.132436 2.8806 0.090 b) Religious Knowledge In your understanding, does Islam allow men to have four wives? In your understanding, does Islam give women half the inheritance rights of men? In your understanding, does Islam require two female witnesses for every one male witness in a court of law? c) Convictions on Fairness Do you think that, by allowing men to marry four wives, Islam gives women lower status than men? By giving female witnesses half the weight of male witnesses in a court of law, Islam gives women lower status then men? d) Aspired Levels of Closeness to God Do you aspire to have the time, a place, and freedom to pray and to pray regularly? Do you aspire to have the time, a place, and freedom to read the Quran and to read the Quran regularly? Do you aspire to have the time, freedom, and the means to do the Haaj? Do you aspire to have religious virtue in the eyes of those around her? Do you aspire to have religious virtue in her own eyes? Do you aspire to have religious virtue in the eyes of Allah? (1) chi-squared test 10 The table shows that on the two most important sub-measures of religiosity (conviction in fairness of Islamic gender norms, and aspiration to be close to God) girls from both the groups in Pakistan show equally high levels when tested at 5% level of significance. The two groups show slight differences in responses on the other two sub-variables (knowledge about Islam, and ritual practice). Put together the differences on these subvariables are, however, not significant, especially when we keep in view how these four sub-variables were ranked in the section on methodology. It is also important to note that on ritual practices the results are mixed. On levels of giving, college girls in fact show slightly higher score, while on saying regular prayers and undertaking reading of Quran, the madrasa girls do better. Ethnographic fieldwork carried out madrasas in both Pakistan and northern Nigeria will suggest that regular prayer and routine reading of the Quran in madrasas could most probably be linked to group-effect rather than necessarily being a reflection of higher levels of inner conviction in religion. Madrasas in Pakistan are boarding facilities where prayers and daily Quran reading is part of publicly visible rituals that all students are required to follow. To measure if madrasas education really translates into genuinely increased commitment to performance of publically observable Islamic rituals, the study needs to adopt a longitudinal approach where differences on ritual practice between girls from the two school types are measured four to five years after they have left school. Thus, looking across the sub-variables of levels of religiosity, it is difficult to argue that madrasa education leads to significantly higher levels of religiosity. On the two most critical measures of levels of religiosity, desire for closeness to good, and conviction in fairness of Islam, the girls in colleges show equally high scores. Madrasa education and aspirations The survey results on questioned aimed at measuring the impact of madrasa education on shaping material aspirations of the students in the two types of schools similarly do not record any significant difference. Despite studies recording the emphasis on simplicity placed in madrasa education, Table 2 shows that madrasa students showed similarly high material aspirations as college girls. The fact that college girls are on the average from slightly better economic background to begin with, will in fact suggest that by wanting as much as the college girls want, the madrasa girls are arguably showing even higher level of aspiration as they are trying to bridge a bigger gap. Table 2. Levels of Material Aspirations for Madrasa and Modern Schools, Pakistan Madrasa Mean S.D. Modern School Mean S.D. X2 1 p-value Material Aspirations Do you aspire to be married? 0.882927 0.322295 0.856140 0.351565 0.7436 0.389 How many sons do you aspire to have? (2) 1.703448 0.515377 1.605263 0.946797 1.3613 0.1742 How many rooms for living and sleeping do you aspire to? (2) How much money (per month) do you aspire to? (2) 4.521212 1.977273 5.488889 3.694398 -3.5514 0.0004 27,543.31 28,388.11 142,050.6 1,234,224 -1.5151 0.1309 How many cars/jeeps do you aspire to (for yourself or 1.473333 1.001319 2.018182 1.517428 -4.4402 0.0000 11 with your husband)? (2) Do you aspire to have a job of your own? 0.829787 0.376823 0.797153 0.402837 0.7802 0.377 (1) Chi-squared test (2) t-test Aspiration for professional roles Where the results from the survey do record a difference between the girls from the two school-types, and in quite interesting way, is on the three vignettes designed to make girls choose between an orthodox and liberal interpretation of an Islamic gender norm. These three vignettes presented three scenarios to the girls, which could potentially create a slight dilemma by making girls choose between a conservative and a more liberal interpretation of gender norms. Scenario 1 presented a dilemma where a woman had a choice between accepting a promotion that will be good for her career advancement but which would leave her less time to spend with her children. Scenario 2 presented a dilemma where a woman’s husband loses his job and she has to decide whether to help him by taking up a job herself or to stay at home and help what she can by trying to spend more economically. Scenario 3 created a dilemma where a husband whose career requires socializing with colleagues in clubs and parties, where alcohol is served and wives are expected to join, puts pressure on the wife to join him in these activities. The woman has to choose whether to support the husband and join him or leave him and follow a virtuous path. For each of these scenarios women were given four possible response options. These varied from no dilemma straight away accept the liberal norm, some dilemma but accept the liberal norm, some dilemma but do not accept the liberal norm, and no dilemma straight away reject the liberal norm. Table X. Dilemmas for Madrasa and Modern Schools, Pakistan Madrasa Mean S.D. Modern School Mean S.D. X2 1 p-value Dilemmas Career or husband and child? 3.408867 0.9674506 2.735714 1.158486 Contribute to household income or save money? 2.460396 1.041868 1.678571 0.9333937 70.7339 0.000 Go to events with husband or leave husband for a more 2.963158 virtuous path? (1) Chi-Squared test 1.020276 2.728261 1.024464 47.4094 0.0000 6.3368 0.096 The survey results show that on scenario 1 and 2, college girls in Pakistan showed a significantly higher willingness to opt for more progressive interpretations (See Table 3). Even on scenario 3 college girls showed higher willingness to pursue the more progressive position, i.e. to secure a job, but the difference in responses was not statistically significant. Here it is important to note that in all three cases, many madrasa girls also opted for liberal interpretations thereby highlighting the need to remember the diversity within each group type (see Figure 1). But, on the whole there was a significant difference between the two groups in willingness to opt for liberal interpretations of Islam that allow them to benefit from modern life opportunities, while retaining same conviction in fairness of orthodox Islamic norms and being equally keen on being close to God. 12 Figure 1. Answers to dilemmas for Madrasa and college students 13 Source: Own elaboration Can this important difference in approach towards engaging with modernity be attributed simply to madrasa education? The next section presents the counter-evidence. Evidence on household differences The school related cross-group difference in willingness to opt for liberal norms becomes more nuanced when we look at the data on the socio-economic and cultural background of the students. The data shows significant differences between the two groups on four counts, which keeping in view the broader literature on gender and development, can help explain the reason for different levels of willingness to follow the progressive interpretation. One, calculated on basis of reported income plus family asset ownership, college girls in general do seem to belong to a slightly better income group than madrasa students (See Table 4). Table 4. Household Socioeconomic Characteristics for Madrasa and Modern Schools, Pakistan Madrasa Mean S.D. a) Urban Background Are you a boarder, i.e., do you live in accommodation provided by the college/madrasa? b) Exposure to Television On average, how many hours of Cable TV do you watch in a day? c) Parent Background Type of education completed by father Type of education completed by mother Mother's Occupation d) Household Income Father's Income Mother's Income Modern School Mean S.D. t-stat1 p-value2 0.624390 0.485466 0.185714 0.389572 10.6656 0.0000 1.859903 1.479532 3.385417 1.597274 -10.9431 0.0000 3.971292 2.229665 1.179245 2.049657 1.452743 1.051266 5.758741 4.807018 1.538194 1.745524 1.748627 1.598758 10.1925 0.0000 -17.8593 0.0000 -3.0242 0.0026 15,572.5 15,500.0 15,764.2 8,346.7 112,904.9 104,892.9 549,381.4 459,035.4 -2.7759 -1.2599 d) Other Household Characteristics How many rooms (including bedrooms, living rooms and 4.164251 2.048461 6.062937 3.399106 -7.7084 drawing room) are/were there in this home How many people do you share a room with to sleep at 7.964286 4.752732 2.062500 1.463735 16.7482 night? How many of the following are used or owned by the people in your home? Fans 5.375000 3.291922 7.747253 4.452181 -6.5417 Clocks 3.333333 2.124927 5.311355 3.448430 -7.5946 Bed/Charpoi 8.778443 5.035299 6.091912 3.780266 5.9428 Electricity 0.995169 0.069505 1.000000 0.000000 -1.0000 Electric Iron 1.670391 1.079828 1.992537 1.151428 -3.0091 Flush Toilet 1.975155 1.548993 3.229008 2.764207 -5.9730 Pit Toilet 1.975610 1.903783 2.019608 1.573169 -0.1311 Tap Water 4.807143 3.815712 7.905213 5.187028 -6.4388 Bicycle 1.547368 1.412470 1.527027 0.943621 0.1238 Scooter or Motorcycl 1.491525 0.736656 1.620253 1.109356 -1.1566 Colour TV 1.544000 1.322900 2.083333 1.421589 -3.6345 Video/VCR/DVD 1.222222 0.559581 1.518135 1.750278 -1.9584 Telephone 1.472222 1.275844 1.550265 1.083661 -0.3441 0.0059 0.2148 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000 0.0000 0.3185 0.0028 0.0000 0.8961 0.0000 0.9017 0.2485 0.0003 0.0515 0.7323 14 Mobile Phone Tape Recorder/CD Pla Radio Gas/electric cooking Camera Refrigerator Air Conditioner Computer Car/Jeep Sewing Machine Washing Machine 3.585799 2.693694 5.313208 3.602926 -5.6976 0.0000 1.230769 0.581256 1.362162 0.717541 -1.3639 0.1756 1.326531 0.746876 1.437908 0.916444 -0.8574 0.3933 1.944882 1.454643 1.903084 1.219074 0.2744 0.7841 1.385714 0.728167 1.792453 1.506491 -3.0083 0.0029 1.370079 0.990456 1.620968 1.087974 -2.2443 0.0256 1.650000 1.166850 2.886364 2.527254 -4.6621 0.0000 1.307692 0.609049 1.737705 1.380662 -3.9438 0.0000 1.461538 0.859338 1.705128 1.176205 -1.2618 0.2137 1.560694 1.122219 1.434615 0.891592 1.2401 0.2159 1.456250 1.131854 1.467681 0.960032 -0.1065 0.9152 Two, there is significant difference in the rural and urban background of the girls in the two groups. Though based in the same cities, madrasas overwhelmingly cater to girls from rural areas, who were living in the madrasa provided boarding facilities. The schoolgirls were on the other hand from families living in those very cities. The cultural difference between the families of the two set of girls was also reflected in the high difference in language spoken at home: majority of school girls reported Urdu (the official language in Pakistan, mainly spoken in cities and within families which are more educated) as the language spoken at home; majority of madrasa girls reported Punjabi (provincial language spoken mainly in rural areas) as the main language spoken at home. Third, girls in schools reported having higher access to TV and watching more hours of TV than girls in madrasas where close to 40% reported watching zero hours of TV per week. Fourth, mothers of college girls in general had higher education qualification than those of madrasa girls. These findings when situated in findings from within the broader development literature present a strong ground for arguing that these differences in socio-economic and cultural background could potentially play a more important role in shaping girls’ willingness to opt for professional career and more liberal Islamic norms than the school type. 15 Figure 2. Family differences for madrasa and college students (clockwise 1-3) Source: Own elaboration Girls in major cities of Pakistan such as Lahore and Rawalpindi are much more likely to be exposed to female role models, which in the literature is seen to be important for encouraging women to pursue professional roles. Beaman et al. (2012) find that, in Indian villages where girls had female role models because the village was randomly assigned to reserve a seat on the village council for a woman, the gender gap in occupational aspirations declined among girls themselves and among parents. Similarly, the role of access to TV and cable TV network is increasingly argued to give women ideas to demand change. Chong et al. (2012) and Chong and La Ferrara (2009) showed that exposure to TV shows with strong female role models and smaller families in Brazil reduced fertility and increased divorce. Jensen and Ostre (2009) have shown that exposure to soap operas depicting urban women reduced fertility and domestic violence and altered beliefs about women’s autonomy in rural India. Other experimental studies find that providing concrete information about untapped opportunities can boost investment. Jensen (2012) finds that, in Indian villages randomly selected for recruitment visits publicising opportunities in call centres, young women increased their labour market participation and enrolment in relevant courses, delayed their marriage and their firs children, and reported higher career aspirations. Parents enrolled girls in school more and fed their daughters better. 16 Nigerian evidence So far we have discussed results from Pakistan, the results from Nigeria need separate treatment because unlike Pakistan, the survey data from Nigeria did not record any significant difference between the two groups on any comparative dimension. In case of Nigeria, even on knowledge of Islamic rules and ritual practice the girls in schools in Nigeria scored as high as girls in madrasas. There is also no difference in responses to the three vignettes that were designed to measure girls’ willingness to choose a more liberal interpretation of Islam (Data tables under preparation; to be added soon). School-girls in Nigeria show low willingness to accept the liberal norms as do the girls in madrasas. This overwhelmingly similarity in responses from the two groups in Nigeria also supports the argument that socio-economic and cultural contexts of the girls might be more important than Islamic education in shaping the levels of religiosity and future aspirations. Unlike Pakistan, there is no obvious difference in the socio-economic profile of the students in the two groups. These results might seem odd, but they become understandable when we remember that madrasas in Kano are organised as schools unlike Pakistan where they offer four years degree after completing Grade 10. Islamiyya schools in Kano run parallel to the state schools. The main difference between them and the state school is that they offer a more integrated curriculum with a high share of Islamic education than would be offered in the state schools. Since majority of Islamiyya schools are community-owned and offer better quality of education than state schools, it is understandable why the socio-economic and cultural background of the students in the two groups did not differ in Kano unlike Pakistan where going to madrasa means exiting the modern schooling system. This, however, does still raise the question as to why do girls from neither of the two groups in Kano record a significant choice in favour of liberal norms that allow for pursuit of professional career and increased financial independence. It is argued that the reason there might lie in the difference between the socio-economic conditions in Kano and the urban cities of Punjab where the survey was implemented. Lahore and Rawalpindi are much more affluent than Kano and also do better in terms of offering educational and professional opportunities for women. If we keep this difference in view then it is possible to argue that the lower willingness within women from both the school types in Kano to pursue more liberal Islamic norms again supports the argument that it is the socio-economic and culture context rather than Islamic education per se which shapes girls’ willingness to pursue a liberal or conservative interpretation of Islam. Reforming norms versus creating opportunities The evidence from both the contexts shows an overwhelming persistence of Islamic values. When it comes to conviction in Islamic faith and its fairness, even modern schools girls have expressed no doubt. Islam is seen as a just and fair religion. Most girls justified some of the controversial Islamic gender norms as being reflective of different biological make up of men and women. This high persistence of Islam raises a number of important points for policy as well as academic purposes. One, the high levels of religiosity even within the modern school girls in both the countries shows that modern education in itself does not necessarily reduce conviction in religious beliefs. Two, the fact that conviction in fairness in Islamic gender norms remains very high across the two groups, and modern school girls feel that they can opt for professional roles without compromising their desire to be pious, shows that discourse on Islam in the West often ignores the potential available within the Islamic tradition to modernize. Seen this way 17 the question becomes not why Islam in restrictive of female agency but what are the individual characteristics or societal conditions which make some Muslim women prefer to choose the restrictive interpretations and make others opt for a liberal outlook. Third, and most importantly, the evidence that desire for professional roles is higher within girls who are urban based, have better access to media, and have better educated mothers, would suggest that liberal interpretations of Islam are automatically likely to become more popular when the opportunities become available for women to engage with economic opportunities. Development agencies faced with the dilemma of addressing restrictive norms through direct interventions to reform them or through focusing on wider economic and social development (Duflo 2012; Klugman et al. 2014), could therefore benefit from focusing on the latter. References Appadurai, A. 2004. The Capacity to Aspire: Culture and the Terms of Recognition. In Rao, A. and Walton, M. (eds.) Culture and Public Action. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press. Asadullah, M. N. and Chaudhury, N. 2010. Religious Schools, Social Values, and Economic Attitudes: Evidence from Bangladesh. World Development. 38 (2): 205-217. Barrett, J. B., Pearson, J., Muller, C., and Frank, K, 2007. Adolescent Religiosity and School Contexts. Social Science Quarterly. 88: 4. Bernard, T. Dercon, S, Taffesse, S. 2011. Beyond Fatalism: An Empirical Exploration of SelfEfficacy and Aspirations Failure in Ethiopia. CSAE Working Paper. WPS/2014-2016. Bernard, T. Dercon, S, Orkin, K. Taffesse, S. 2014. The Future in Mind: Aspirations and Forward-Looking Behaviour in Rural Ethiopia. CSAE Working Paper. WPS/2014-2016. Duflo, E. 2012. Women Empowerment and Economic Development. Journal of Economic Literature. 50 (4): 1051-1079. Cohen-Zada, D. and Sander, W. 2008. Religion, Religiosity, and Private School Choice: Implications for Estimating the Effectiveness of Private Schools. Journal of Urban Economics. 64: 85-100. Hill, J. P. 2011. Faith and Understanding: Specifying the Impact of Higher Education on Religious Belief. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 50 (3): 533-551. Klugman, J., Hanmer, L., Twigg, S., Hasan, T., McCleary-Sills, J., Santamaria, J. 2014. Voice and Agency: Empowering Women and Girls for Shared Prosperity. Washington, DC: World Bank Group. World Bank. https://openknowledge.worldbank.org/handle/10986/19036 License: CC BY 3.0 IGO. McAndrew, S. and Voas, D. 2011. Measuring Religiosity Using Surveys. SQB Topic Overview. 18 Ntibagirirwa. S. 2009. Cultural Values, Economic Growth and Development. Journal of Business Ethics. 84: 297-311. Ray, D. 2002. Aspirations, Poverty and Economic Change. Paper presented at World Bank conference on Culture and Development. Washington D.C. Sander, W. 2005. Catholics and Catholic Schooling. Education Economics. 13: 257-268. Serneels, P. and Dercon, S. 2014. Aspirations, Poverty and Education: Evidence from India. Young Lives Working Paper 125. Oxford: Oxford University. Steffen, P. R., Clayton, S. and Swinyard, W. 2015. Religious Orientation and Life Aspirations. Journal of Religious Health. 54: 470-479. Tritter, J. 1992. An Education Change in Moral Values: Some Effects of Religious and State Schools on their Students. Oxford Review of Education. 18 (1): 29-43. 19
© Copyright 2024